You are on page 1of 6

Review Biological

Psychiatry

Autism and the Social Brain: The First-Year


Puzzle
Mayada Elsabbagh and Mark H. Johnson

ABSTRACT
The atypical features of social perception and cognition observed in individuals with a diagnosis of autism have been
explained in two different ways. First, domain-specific accounts are based on the assumption that these end-state
symptoms result from specific impairments within component structures of the social brain network. Second,
domain-general accounts hypothesize that rather than being localized, atypical brain structure and function are
widespread, or hypothesize that the apparent social brain differences are the consequence of adaptations to earlier
occurring widespread changes in brain function. Critical evidence for resolving this basic issue comes from
prospective longitudinal studies of infants at risk for later diagnosis. We highlight selected studies from the newly
emerging literature on infants at familial risk for autism to shed light on this issue. Despite multiple reports of possible
alterations in brain function in the first year of life, overt behavioral symptoms do not emerge until the second year.
Our review reveals only mixed support, within this very early period, for localized deficits in social brain network
systems and instead favors the view that atypical development involving perceptual, attentional, motor, and social
systems precede the emerging autism phenotype.
Keywords: Autism, Brain development, Eye tracking, Imaging, Infant, Risk
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.02.019

Autism, a developmental disorder characterized by suboptimal centered on the attempt to identify precursors of the later
social and communication behavior, is commonly attributed to diagnosis. To date, many published studies have specifically
the atypical development of brain networks subserving social focused their investigations on social orienting and perception
perception and cognition. However, the fundamental question in infancy, on the assumption that such tasks will provide
of why the so-called social brain network is differentially the best predictive markers of later diagnosis. As such, many
affected remains unanswered. For this reason, over the last studies have adopted the general assumption that domain-
decade, several groups have begun the prospective study of specific deficits in infancy become increasingly compounded
infants who are at increased risk for developing autism (1–3). during postnatal development to result in the social impair-
Later-born siblings of children with autism are more likely to ments observed in diagnosed autism. Infant social brain
receive a diagnosis of autism themselves compared with network precursors are assumed to also be the origins of
infants with no family history of autism. Specifically, 20% the later emerging social, cognitive, and attention features
of these infants receive an autism diagnosis by 3 years of age, observed in this population (7).
and a further 20% may have other developmental difficulties or In this review, we question the “social-first” assumption
show subclinical aspects of the phenotype (4). Emerging prevalent in the literature on the grounds that the evidence in
results from studies of infant siblings have begun to provide support of this view is more mixed than has sometimes been
information about the developmental precursors and risk portrayed, and evidence from infants in the first year is more
processes that lead to the emergence of the autism phenotype consistent with subtle widespread atypicality across multiple
in toddlerhood. There is consensus that during the second brain systems. Our review of the literature on infants is largely
year of life many infants in whom autism is later diagnosed consistent with evidence from research on adults identifying
begin to be differentiated based on their behavior from infants neurocognitive differences in perceptual and cognitive sys-
at risk who do not receive a diagnosis and infants in low-risk tems in addition to the differences observed in social brain
control groups (1–3). networks.
Despite this progress in characterizing the emerging phe-
notype, what has been described as the first-year puzzle (1)
remains unresolved: within the earliest period of postnatal life, SOCIAL-FIRST ASSUMPTION
overt behavioral signs of later autism are subtle and variable Although autism is diagnosed on the basis of social and
and do not obviously map on the profile of atypicality seen communication impairment, the phenotype encompasses
later (5,6). Much of the research on infants at familial risk has much broader differences in perception and cognition. Several

SEE COMMENTARY ON PAGE

94 & 2016 Society of Biological Psychiatry.


Biological Psychiatry July 15, 2016; 80:94–99 www.sobp.org/journal ISSN: 0006-3223
Biological
Autism and the Social Brain Psychiatry

accounts, supported by research on children and adults with viewed as a subset of studies using looking behavior to
autism, have shifted the focus away from social impairment. assess cognition.
For example, the enhanced perceptual functioning account Early in postnatal life, the brain has basic orienting mech-
posits that autism is characterized by locally oriented visual anisms that support rapid attention to salient stimuli, partic-
and auditory perception and enhanced low-level discrimina- ularly those of social relevance (11). One such system is based
tion (8). More recently, a predictive coding account has been on a subcortical route and biases the human newborn to
applied to autism (9). Predictive coding suggests that higher attend toward faces (12). Various studies have allowed further
brain areas attempt to “explain” input from sensory brain areas specification of the key characteristics of faces preferentially
and then project these predictions down to lower areas, where attracting attention (13). These preferences are robust in the
the predicted sensory information is subtracted from the input. face of manipulation of low-level perceptual and motion
This account may explain how brain differences in autism features of the scenes. Although this putative system is based
lead to a bias favoring local over global processing. More on simple low-spatial frequency patterns characteristic of
specifically, hypersensitivity in visual or auditory processing faces, it is sufficient to bias the input to developing cortical
may underlie symptoms such as altered social engagement visual areas (14). In response, some of these cortical areas
and speech delay. These alternative accounts have also increase their specialization for processing faces and related
indicated that islands of enhancement, in addition to deficits, social stimuli, resulting in increasingly selective patterns of
may better characterize some aspects of the phenotype. cortical activation with increasing age.
Overall, these perspectives suggest that viewing autism in Beginning life with these rudimentary biases, infant social
terms of a focal impairment in the social brain is overly behavior does not simply mature over development giving
simplistic. A more complex pattern of interactions between rise to complex and well-developed social and communi-
social and perceptual systems may underlie the pattern of cation abilities in toddlers. Instead, social orienting reflects
symptoms or individual differences observed in this condition. experience-dependent changes across perceptual, cognitive,
These alternative accounts have received much less attention and social brain systems. The developmental course of
from the more recent research area examining early develop- fixations changes rapidly over development (15–17): newborn
ment in autism, which has more often continued to pursue the infants tend to fixate mainly around the edge of the face; later,
hypothesis of a social-first deficit. Studying the early emer- similar to adults, infants begin to fixate on the internal features
gence of autism signs and symptoms can help explain the of the face, such as eyes and mouth. Subsequently, they show
mechanisms underlying their development before symptoms a strong preference for the internal features of the face when
are amplified and complicated over the course of atypical they are watching their mother’s face when it displays
development. communicative expressions, such as maintained eye contact,
There are several good reasons why researchers studying smiling, speaking in infant-directed speech, and nodding.
infants at risk for autism have tended to focus on early markers Given this relatively mature literature on typical development,
of the social brain. First, social brain deficits are assumed to it is not surprising that hypotheses on the origins of autism
underlie key diagnostic features of the diagnosed syndrome, based on the social-first assumption have been so actively
and as such emerging atypical social behaviors (usually pursued.
observed in the second year) clearly precede the diagnostic
phenotype (1–3). Second, because of the limited attention
span of infants in their first year, researchers are forced ASSESSING THE SOCIAL-FIRST HYPOTHESES
to make pragmatic decisions about prioritizing particular Several groups have theorized that early deficits in social
domains for tests. Third, a mature literature on the typical information processing early in life may interfere with the
development of the social brain readily allows for the adoption emergence of developmental milestones that are critical for
of paradigms and theoretical ideas to be applied to at-risk social learning, such as shared attention (7,12,18,19). Accord-
populations. ing to this view, these cascading influences could preclude the
The social brain is a network of regions implicated in the typical development of sociocommunicative skills, eventually
processing of social information, including cortical areas leading to deficits in language acquisition, theory of mind, and
such as orbitofrontal cortex, superior temporal sulcus, understanding of others. In individuals with a diagnosis of
temporoparietal junction, and the fusiform face area as well autism, it may be possible to dissociate differences in social
as subcortical structures, such as amygdala and pulvinar orienting from differences in social reward systems (7). How-
(10). Although many aspects of this adult brain specialization ever, at the present time, it is impossible to investigate this
remain to be understood, much progress has been made in putative dissociation in infants ,12 months old, and so we do
understanding the developmental course of social percep- not differentiate these hypotheses in the current analysis.
tion and orienting. Because moving the eyes is the most Because deficits in social engagement are characteristic
important means of selecting visual input in infants and symptoms of autism, the most direct and parsimonious
adults, eye tracking is being increasingly used. Although eye hypothesis regarding the origins of these symptoms is impair-
movement measures have been used since the 1970s, ment or absence of rudimentary social orienting biases.
the advent of eye-tracking technology has substantially Retrospective studies looking back at the first 2 years of life
expanded the utility of these measures across multiple basic (20) show less orienting toward social stimuli and a reduced
and clinical research fields. Measuring cognitive phenomena response to name calling in the first years of life in infants in
through eye tracking requires a careful experimental design, whom autism is later diagnosed compared with infants in
and its use to measure “social brain” functions can be whom developmental delay is later diagnosed. Consistent with

Biological Psychiatry July 15, 2016; 80:94–99 www.sobp.org/journal 95


Biological
Psychiatry Autism and the Social Brain

this finding, experimental studies with children in whom autism (28), it will be important to examine whether infants who later
is already diagnosed show that the children orient less to develop autism prefer speech over nonspeech early in life
social stimuli, such as people and voices, compared with similar to typically developing infants (30). Preliminary findings
nonsocial stimuli, such as objects and sounds, relative to of risk group differences in the selectivity of responding to
children of the same age and developmental level (20). One human vocal sounds have been reported, although diagnosis
possibility is that the increased interest in objects is a outcomes are yet available (31).
consequence of decreased interest in people. Despite the lack of clear evidence for frank impairment in
Researchers have tested the same hypothesis in infants at social systems in the first year of life, infants at risk have
risk in whom autism was later diagnosed, using designs provided a window into the full range of developmental
ranging from controlled experiments to less structured obser- pathways and outcomes observed in this population. Hetero-
vational studies in different home and laboratory contexts (2). geneity in these pathways is increasingly the focus in studies
Contrary to predictions from the social-first hypotheses, in characterizing older children with a diagnosis of autism, and
many studies, infants who went on to develop autism as its origins remain unexplained (32). For example, in regard to
toddlers initially showed the typical social responses expected the wide range of variability in language outcomes in autism,
in the first year of life. These infants were indistinguishable on research on infant siblings has provided unique opportunities
a wide range of social orienting measures during the first year: to study the origins of this variability. Merin et al. (17) initially
orienting to faces embedded among distractors (21,22), gaze tested the hypothesis that infants within the first year showed
to faces and objects (22), shared affect and overall social similar amounts of eye and mouth fixations when an adult was
responsiveness to an examiner (22,23), parent-reported social interacting with the infants but increased fixations of the eyes
engagement and communication skills (24,25), and quality of relative to mouth in the “still-face” period, during which the
parent-child play interactions (26). In contrast, almost all of adult suddenly froze, became expressionless, and stopped
these studies report more frequency and clarity of risk signs in interacting with the infants. In the same study, infants at family
the second year of life. Taken together, these studies provide, risk of autism did not differ as clearly in their scanning, but a
at best, mixed support for the social-first hypothesis. small subgroup of infants at risk looked more to the mouth
Other studies focused on the bias to fixate eyes early on in relative to the eyes. A follow-up study with a larger group
life. These studies are motivated by a large body of evidence (including the infants in the previous study) found that more
suggesting specific and early emerging differences in proc- mouth relative to eye fixations did not relate specifically to
essing of eye gaze. Specifically, despite mixed evidence from autism but did relate to individual differences in expressive
older children and adults (15), reduced fixation to eyes has language as assessed later in toddlerhood (16).
been proposed as a key feature of the emerging autism Another study with an independent cohort replicated and
phenotype. The hypothesis that infants who later develop extended these findings (15). In complex scenes requiring
autism orient differently to the eyes has been tested more more endogenous control of attention, increased scanning of
recently using diverse stimuli and observational contexts. the mouth relative to the eyes in the first year of life was
Here, too, in some studies, infants initially exhibit typical associated with superior expressive language in toddlerhood.
modulation of fixations to different face regions, in particular As in the previous study, this relationship held across both
to eyes (15,16); they also show little differences in precursors typically and atypically developing groups (i.e., infants in
to joint attention, including frequency of eye contact (22,23) whom autism or other developmental conditions were later
and reflexive gaze following (27). When trajectories of fixation diagnosed). Moreover, the association is highly sensitive to
to different facial features were examined longitudinally, specific viewing contexts (complex scenes where there are
several studies showed a decline in looking toward the eyes competing cues) and to a narrow developmental window
by both typically developing infants and infants at risk within the first year (15). As such, these studies suggest
(15,16,28). This decline occurs because infants begin to continuity between the developmental mechanisms observed
redistribute their attention away from the eyes and to other in autism with the mechanisms found in typical development.
facial features, seeking cues that are relevant for learning and Studying visual fixations early in life is only one approach
further development (15,16). However, one of these studies, for testing social-first hypotheses. Other hypotheses are
which followed infants from 2 months of age, reported a based on the idea of localizable deficits in structures within
steeper slope of decline in looking to the eyes in the subgroup the cortical social brain network. By this view, behavioral
of infants who later developed autism (29). Prominent inter- symptoms emerge on a developmental timetable consistent
individual variability was observed in these early trajectories, with the maturation of the regions concerned. Given chal-
where, paradoxically, some infants in the at-risk group who lenges in imaging brains of infants, there is a much smaller
later developed autism showed greater fixation to eyes than literature on functional brain development. Although, as
control subjects when tested in the first months after birth. discussed earlier, young infants who later develop autism do
Taken together, these findings suggest that contrary to the not show clear evidence of atypical overt behavior in these
predictions generated by the social-first hypotheses, infants in areas, initial evidence from near-infrared spectroscopy studies
whom autism is later diagnosed may begin life with the near- indicates that infants with familial risk show attenuated
typical biases to fixate faces and eyes. responses to social stimuli within the first year of life in
Orienting to auditory social stimuli, such as speech or other posterior temporal lobe regions (31). Infants who later receive
vocal sounds, has been less systematically investigated to an autism diagnosis also show atypical neural responses to
date. Because an impairment in such mechanisms was eye gaze shifts within the first year of life recorded over the
suggested to underlie later difficulties in language acquisition temporal lobes (33).

96 Biological Psychiatry July 15, 2016; 80:94–99 www.sobp.org/journal


Biological
Autism and the Social Brain Psychiatry

It is likely that these atypical early responses originating At the present time, the largest body of work supporting
from regions of the cortical social brain contribute to the later claims on the nature of the social brain network comes from
emerging social behavioral difficulties. However, as discussed studying adults. Much less is understood regarding the infant
subsequently, it is also possible that superior temporal sulcus brain, but an emerging literature on structural imaging with
and other nexus (or critical hub) regions of cortical social brain infants at risk for autism may begin to address this knowledge
are particularly vulnerable to generalized differences in the gap. Findings to date have not lent support for specific
efficiency or quality of neural processing across large swathes differences in the social brain network. For example, one
of cortex. study reported enlarged total cerebral volumes and excess
extra-axial fluid within the first year of life in infants who later
developed autism. By the second year, the same infants still
FROM SOCIAL-FIRST HYPOTHESES TO had larger total cerebral volumes but typical levels of extra-
WIDESPREAD ATYPICALITY axial fluid (40). Another study with an independent cohort
More recent accounts of several psychiatric conditions have reported that the corpus callosum more specifically may be
shifted the focus from localizable deficits affecting isolated enlarged in infants who develop autism within the first year of
structures or networks to the atypical functioning of large- life but that differences are no longer apparent by the age of 2
scale networks and whole-brain connectivity. As such, it is not years (41). In contrast, functional analysis in the same cohort
enough to account for sparing or impairment in response to showed that posterior brain network structures, on the whole,
specific socially relevant information such as faces or eyes, show reduced efficiency, including regions involved in proc-
but instead we need to consider how information is integrated essing auditory and visual stimuli, language, and nonlinguistic
across multiple brain regions and in response to a wide range social stimuli (42).
of stimuli and task demands. Methods other than magnetic resonance imaging in infants
Comprehensive reviews of the broad range of early markers at risk lend further support to altered overall network con-
identified in infants at risk within the first year of life raise nectivity in the first year of life. First, converging measures of
substantial challenges to the social-first hypotheses (1,2,6). overall brain connectivity patterns have documented differ-
These include the presence of head lag when infants are ences in infants at risk as a group within the first year, albeit
pulled to sit, reflecting delayed motor maturation (34); higher without these differences being examined directly in relation to
level of perceptual sensitivity (24); production of fewer middle a later autism diagnosis. These differences include increased
consonant types and fewer late consonant types (35); and central and temporal gamma oscillatory activity (43) and
slower attention disengagement, reflecting generally poor altered trajectory of multiscale entropy derived from electro-
attentional flexibility [rather than specific to socially relevant encephalography (44). By the start of the second year, these
information (36) and overall lower activity level (23)]. The wide differences become characteristic of the emerging phenotype,
range of these atypicalities appears more consistent with where infants who later develop autism show elevated phase-
domain-general differences during the first year. lagged alpha-frequency electroencephalography connectivity
In the light of these domain-general and widespread (45). However, these connectivity differences are not unique to
effects, how can we account for differentially severe conse- social brain structures but appear to reflect altered connec-
quences for social behavior in autism? Theoretical accounts tivity across large swathes of cortex.
suggest that delayed or atypical specialization of function and A few behavioral studies have examined how the social
associated microstructure can occur as a result of a change in brain networks become deployed for more cognitively
the fidelity of neural processing in cortical circuits during demanding tasks that require high-resolution temporal inte-
development and will likely result in delayed or atypical gration. One example of this pattern mentioned earlier is the
patterns of specialization (37). However, mild impairment that high degree of modulation of fixations to different face regions
reduces the fidelity of processing only slightly may have as a function of complex versus simple social scenes;
significant consequences primarily for brain regions associ- performance in the complex scene only was associated with
ated with complex, dynamic, and less predictable stimuli— language outcomes (15). Another study found few differences
features that typically characterize the social world (5). in face scanning across multiple conditions except when the
It has been proposed that regions such as the fusiform face actress started speaking (46). The authors suggested that
area, superior temporal sulcus, and temporoparietal junction speech introduces additional task demands, which dispropor-
become recruited into the cortical social brain network during tionally disrupts attention to the inner features of the face in
development precisely because their patterns of primary the group who later develop autism (46). Infants who later
inputs and outputs make them suitable as a “nexus” region developed autism in this study also looked overall less at the
for integrating across sensory channels with high temporal scenes as a whole, suggesting that they interacted differently
precision (38). For example, Carter and Huettel (38) argued with the task and raising the possibility that this performance
that the temporoparietal junction is frequently active in social difference is attributed to multiple social and nonsocial brain
cognitive tasks because it is located at the anatomic con- networks.
vergence of several different critical pathways of information Finally, despite being indistinguishable in orienting toward,
processing, and the overlap and integration of these different or scanning of, socially relevant information, studies have
streams lead to novel secondary computations, such as suggested that infants who later develop autism may rely
decision making within a social context. It is not surprising on alternative different neural mechanisms to achieve this
that evidence shows that these regions can be differentially performance. For example, one study found similar patterns of
impaired in adults with autism (39). visual fixations to dynamic gaze cues (eyes looking toward

Biological Psychiatry July 15, 2016; 80:94–99 www.sobp.org/journal 97


Biological
Psychiatry Autism and the Social Brain

and away from the infant), but an atypical event-related has centered on potentially boosting early emerging social
potential response to the same gaze cues characterized the skills (51), and similar approaches to attention and executive
subgroup of infants who developed autism as toddlers (33). systems are possible (52). With increasing refinement, such
Although infants who later developed autism were indistin- models may begin to target specific developmental mecha-
guishable in face-sensitive components correlated with initial nisms, highlighting the extent to which they are involved in
visual response and analysis (P100 and N290), they were later emerging symptoms.
atypical in components reflecting integration of top-down and
bottom-up information (33). Direct measurement of brain
structure and function early in life suggests widespread ACKNOWLEDGMENTS AND DISCLOSURES
atypicality of emerging neural networks in autism that differ- EM acknowledges salary support from the Bourgeois Foundation and Brain
entially affects brain computations necessary for an under- Canada and the Azrieli Foundation Grant No. BC_Azrieli_MIRI_3388. MHJ
standing of the social world. acknowledges funding from the United Kingdom Medical Research Council,
MQ: Transforming Mental Health PsyIMPACT Programme, and Innovative
Medicines Initiative joint undertaking under Agreement No. 11530, resour-
ces of which are composed of financial contributions from the European
SOLVING THE FIRST-YEAR PUZZLE: FUTURE
Commission Seventh Framework Programme Grant No. FP7/2009‐2013
DIRECTIONS and European Federation of Pharmaceutical Industries and Associations
The findings reviewed here provide support for a potential companies in kind contribution.
resolution to the first-year puzzle (1); widespread and diffuse
effects on brain connectivity have partial and mild effects on
multiple systems during the first year, but their effects are only
ARTICLE INFORMATION
clearly revealed when more complex multisensory integration From the Department of Psychiatry (ME), McGill University, Montreal,
Quebec, Canada; and Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development (MHJ),
and high temporal resolution processing becomes important
Birkbeck, University of London, London, United Kingdom.
for age-appropriate typical behaviors. Key nexus regions of Address correspondence to Mayada Elsabbagh, Ph.D., Department of
the social brain that depend on high temporal resolution Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, McGill University, 1033 Pine Avenue West,
integration may be particularly vulnerable to the mildly Montreal, Quebec H3A 1A1, Canada; E-mail: mayada.elsabbagh@mcgill.ca.
degraded signal processing. From this point, a child may Received Mar 21, 2015; revised Feb 5, 2016; accepted Feb 18, 2016.
adapt his or her behavior to his or her own processing
capabilities, by focusing attention and interests on domains REFERENCES
that are more predictable than the increasingly complex social 1. Elsabbagh M, Johnson MH (2010): Getting answers from babies about
environment (5). autism. Trends Cogn Sci 14:81–87.
Three research directions may support further testing of 2. Jones EJH, Gliga T, Bedford R, Charman T, Johnson MH (2014):
alternatives to the social-first hypothesis. First, it could be Developmental pathways to autism: A review of prospective studies of
infants at risk. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 39:1–33.
argued that infant sibling research to date has remained
3. Falck-Ytter T, Bölte S, Gredebäck G (2013): Eye tracking in early
descriptive of the natural history of prodromal and emerging autism research. J Neurodev Disord 5:28.
autism in infancy. Moving forward, the substantial body of 4. Ozonoff S, Young GS, Carter A, Messinger D, Yirmiya N, Zwaigen-
research on the typical development of the social brain can be baum L, et al. (2011): Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders: A
put to use with more targeted hypothesis to clarify underlying Baby Siblings Research Consortium study. Pediatrics 128:e488–e495.
mechanisms. For example, a few studies have directly inves- 5. Johnson MH, Jones EJH, Gliga T (2015): Brain adaptation and
alternative developmental trajectories. Dev Psychopathol 27:
tigated early visual (47) and auditory (48) mechanisms in
425–442.
infants at risk for autism, providing rare evidence for enhance- 6. Gliga T, Jones EJ, Johnson MH (2014): Low noise in autism: Cause or
ment of processing as potentially underlying later social out- consequence? Autism 19:369–370.
comes in toddlerhood (47). 7. Chevallier C, Kohls G, Troiani V, Brodkin ES, Schultz RT (2012): The
Second, a key issue for this alternative perspective will be social motivation theory of autism. Trends Cogn Sci 16:231–239.
to dissociate the later behavioral symptoms of autism that 8. Mottron L, Dawson M, Soulieres I, Hubert B, Burack J (2006):
Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update, and eight
reflect an ongoing pathology of brain processing from behav-
principles of autistic perception. J Autism Dev Disord 36:27–43.
ioral symptoms that are adaptive responses. For example, 9. Lawson RP, Rees G, Friston C (2014): An aberrant precision account
some infant brain structure and function atypicalities correlate of autism. Front Hum Neurosci 8:302.
best with later measures of repetitive behaviors (45), suggest- 10. Adolphs R (2009): The social brain: neural basis of social knowledge.
ing that other behavioral features of autism may reflect later Annu Rev Psychol 60:693–716.
adaptations as an atypical brain seeks to fit the environmental 11. Klein JT, Shepherd SV, Platt ML (2009): Social attention and the brain.
Curr Biol 19:R958–R962.
niche of human development (5). Analyses of emerging
12. Johnson MH (2005): Subcortical face processing. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:
structural and functional connectivity will be critical for this 766–774.
endeavor (49). Our review indicates that relying on a model of 13. Farroni T, Menon E, Johnson MH (2006): Factors influencing new-
focal deficit to find predictors of autism may not be sufficient borns’ preference for faces with eye contact. J Exp Child Psychol 95:
to capture the full range of developmental pathways and 298–308.
outcomes observed in this population. 14. Johnson MH (2013): The paradox of the emerging social brain. In:
Banaji MR, Gelman SA, editors. Navigating the Social World: What
Finally, another approach to testing key issues about the
Infants, Children, and Other Species Can Teach Us. Oxford: Oxford
social-first hypothesis is to target social or other brain systems University Press, 6–10.
through early intervention before the onset of diagnostic 15. Elsabbagh M, Bedford R, Senju A, Charman T, Pickles A, Johnson MH
symptoms (50). For example, one prodromal intervention (2013): What you see is what you get: Contextual modulation of face

98 Biological Psychiatry July 15, 2016; 80:94–99 www.sobp.org/journal


Biological
Autism and the Social Brain Psychiatry

scanning in typical and atypical development. Soc Cogn Affect 33. Elsabbagh M, Mercure E, Hudry K, Chandler S, Pasco G, Charman T,
Neurosci 9:538–543. et al. (2012): Infant neural sensitivity to dynamic eye gaze is
16. Young GS, Merin N, Rogers SJ, Ozonoff S (2009): Gaze behavior and associated with later emerging autism. Curr Biol 22:338–342.
affect at 6 months: predicting clinical outcomes and language 34. Flanagan JE, Landa R, Bhat A, Bauman M (2012): Head lag in infants
development in typically developing infants and infants at risk for at risk for autism: A preliminary study. Am J Occup Ther 66:577–585.
autism. Dev Sci 12:798–814. 35. Paul R, Fuerst Y, Ramsay G, Chawarska K, Klin A (2010): Out of the
17. Merin N, Young GS, Ozonoff S, Rogers SJ (2006): Visual fixation mouths of babes: Vocal production in infant siblings of children with
patterns during reciprocal social interaction distinguish a subgroup ASD. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 52:588–598.
of 6-month-old infants at-risk for autism from comparison infants. 36. Elsabbagh M, Fernandes J, Jane Webb S, Dawson G, Charman T,
J Autism Dev Disord 37:108–121. Johnson MH (2013): Disengagement of visual attention in infancy is
18. Schultz RT (2005): Developmental deficits in social perception in associated with emerging autism in toddlerhood. Biol Psychiatry 74:
autism: The role of the amygdala and fusiform face area. Int J Dev 189–194.
Neurosci 23:125–141. 37. Rubenstein JLR, Merzenich MM (2003): Model of autism: Increased ratio of
19. Dawson G (2008): Early behavioral intervention, brain plasticity, and excitation/inhibition in key neural systems. Genes Brain Behav 2:255–267.
the prevention of autism spectrum disorder. Dev Psychopathol 20: 38. Carter RM, Huettel SA (2013): A nexus model of the temporal–parietal
775–803. junction. Trends Cogn Sci 17:328–336.
20. Elsabbagh M, Johnson MH (2007): Infancy and autism: Progress, 39. Lombardo MV, Chakrabarti B, Bullmore ET, Baron-Cohen S (2011):
prospects, and challenges. Prog Brain Res 164:355–383. Specialization of right temporo-parietal junction for mentalizing and its
21. Elsabbagh M, Gliga T, Pickles A, Hudry K, Charman T, Johnson MH relation to social impairments in autism. Neuroimage 56:1832–1838.
(2013): The development of face orienting mechanisms in infants at- 40. Shen MD, Nordahl CW, Young GS, Wootton-Gorges SL, Lee A, Liston
risk for autism. Behav Brain Res 251:147–154. SE, et al. (2013): Early brain enlargement and elevated extra-axial fluid in
22. Ozonoff S, Iosif A-M, Baguio F, Cook IC, Hill MM, Hutman T, et al. infants who develop autism spectrum disorder. Brain 136:2825–2835.
(2010): A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs 41. Wolff JJ, Gerig G, Lewis JD, Soda T, Styner MA, Vachet C, et al.
of autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 49:256–266; e252. (2015): Altered corpus callosum morphology associated with autism
23. Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, Rogers T, Roberts W, Brian J, Szatmari P over the first 2 years of life. Brain 138:2046–2058.
(2005): Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. Int J 42. Lewis JD, Evans AC, Pruett JR, Botteron K, Zwaigenbaum L, Estes A,
Dev Neurosci 23:143–152. et al. (2014): Network inefficiencies in autism spectrum disorder at 24
24. Clifford SM, Hudry K, Elsabbagh M, Charman T, Johnson MH (2012): months. Transl Psychiatry 4:e388.
Temperament in the first 2 years of life in infants at high-risk for autism 43. Bosl W, Tierney A, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson C (2011): EEG complex-
spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 43:673–686. ity as a biomarker for autism spectrum disorder risk. BMC Med 9:18.
25. Hudry K, Chandler S, Bedford R, Pasco G, Gliga T, Elsabbagh M, et al. 44. Elsabbagh M, Volein A, Csibra G, Holmboe K, Garwood H, Tucker L,
(2013): Early language profiles in infants at high-risk for autism et al. (2009): Neural correlates of eye gaze processing in the infant
spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 44:154–167. broader autism phenotype. Biol Psychiatry 65:31–38.
26. Wan MW, Green J, Elsabbagh M, Johnson M, Charman T, Plummer F 45. Orekhova EV, Elsabbagh M, Jones EJH, Dawson G, Charman T,
(2012): Quality of interaction between at-risk infants and caregiver at Johnson MH (2014): EEG hyper-connectivity in high-risk infants is
12-15 months is associated with 3-year autism outcome. J Child associated with later autism. J Neurodev Disord 6:40.
Psychol Psychiatry 54:763–771. 46. Shic F, Macari S, Chawarska K (2014): Speech disturbs face scanning
27. Bedford R, Elsabbagh M, Gliga T, Pickles A, Senju A, Charman T, in 6-month-old infants who develop autism spectrum disorder. Biol
et al. (2012): Precursors to social and communication difficulties in Psychiatry 75:231–237.
infants at-risk for autism: Gaze following and attentional engagement. 47. Gliga T, Bedford R, Charman T, Johnson MH; BASIS Team (2015):
J Autism Dev Disord 42:2208–2218. Enhanced visual search in infancy predicts emerging autism symp-
28. Kuhl PK, Coffey-Corina S, Padden D, Dawson G (2005): Links toms. Curr Biol 25:1727–1730.
between social and linguistic processing of speech in preschool 48. Guiraud JA, Kushnerenko E, Tomalski P, Davies K, Ribeiro H, Johnson
children with autism: Behavioral and electrophysiological measures. MH; BASIS Team (2011): Differential habituation to repeated sounds
Dev Sci 8:F1–F12. in infants at high risk for autism. Neuroreport 22:845–849.
29. Jones W, Klin A (2013): Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2– 49. Vértes PE, Bullmore ET (2014): Annual research review: Growth connec-
6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature 504:427–431. tomics—the organization and reorganization of brain networks during nor-
30. Vouloumanos A, Werker JF (2007): Listening to language at birth: mal and abnormal development. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 56:299–320.
Evidence for a bias for speech in neonates. Dev Sci 10:159–164. 50. Green J, Dunn G (2008): Using intervention trials in developmental
31. Lloyd-Fox S, Blasi A, Elwell CE, Charman T, Murphy D, Johnson MH psychiatry to illuminate basic science. Br J Psychiatry 192:323–325.
(2013): Reduced neural sensitivity to social stimuli in infants at risk for 51. Green J, Charman T, Pickles A, Wan MW, Elsabbagh M, Slonims V,
autism. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 280:20123026. et al. (2015): Parent-mediated intervention versus no intervention for
32. Szatmari P, Georgiades S, Duku E, Bennett TA, Bryson S, Fombonne infants at high risk of autism: A parallel, single-blind, randomised trial.
E, et al. (2015): Developmental trajectories of symptom severity and Lancet Psychiatry 2:133–140.
adaptive functioning in an inception cohort of preschool children with 52. Wass S, Porayska-Pomsta K, Johnson MH (2011): Training attentional
autism spectrum disorder. JAMA Psychiatry 72:276. control in infancy. Curr Biol 21:1543–1547.

Biological Psychiatry July 15, 2016; 80:94–99 www.sobp.org/journal 99

You might also like