You are on page 1of 8

European Journal of Neuroscience

European Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 36, pp. 2632–2639, 2012 doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08174.x

NEUROSYSTEMS

Experience-dependent brain plasticity after stroke: effect of


ibuprofen and poststroke delay

Jan A. Jablonka,1,2 Malgorzata Kossut,2 Otto W. Witte3 and Monika Liguz-Lecznar 2


1
Department of Animal Physiology, Faculty of Biology, Warsaw University, Warsaw, Poland
2
Department of Molecular and Cellular Neurobiology, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology, Warsaw, Poland
3
Hans Berger Department of Neurology, Jena University Hospital, Jena, Germany

Keywords: 2DG, inflammation, plasticity, rat, somatosensory cortex

Abstract
Despite indications that brain plasticity may be enhanced after stroke, we have described impairment of experience-dependent
plasticity in rat cerebral cortex neighboring the stroke-induced lesion. Photothrombotic stroke was centered behind the barrel cortex
in one cerebral hemisphere of rats. Plasticity of cortical representation of one row of vibrissae was induced by sensory deprivation of
all surrounding whiskers for 1 month, and visualized with [14C]-2-deoxyglucose autoradiography. In control rats deprivation resulted
in an enlargement of functional cortical representation of the spared row of vibrissae. After a focal stroke neighbouring the barrel
cortex, no plasticity of the spared row representation was found. Investigation of plastic changes with deprivation initiated 1 week and
1 month after stroke have shown that later poststroke onset of deprivation resulted in a partial recovery of cortical plasticity in the
barrel field. Western blot analysis of proinflammatory enzyme cyclooxygenase-2 (COX-2) expression revealed its strong upregulation
in the barrel cortex 24 h after stroke. When chronic treatment with the anti-inflammatory drug ibuprofen (10 mg ⁄ kg or 20 mg ⁄ kg)
accompanied deprivation, plasticity was restored. Ibuprofen applied before the ischemia also prevented the poststroke upregulation
of COX-2. The results strongly suggest that poststroke impairment of experience-dependent cortical plasticity is caused by stroke-
induced inflammatory reactions that subside with poststroke delay and can be at least partially ameliorated by pharmacological
treatment.

Introduction
Poststroke recovery requires rewiring of the brain by restorative neural rearrangement of the spared row representation could be observed
plasticity. Unfortunately, the mechanisms underlying this plasticity are (Jablonka et al., 2007). It is evident that in the poststroke cortex there
only poorly understood. Several results indicate that the poststroke must be factors that counterbalance the plasticity-supporting environ-
brain environment might be supportive for plasticity (Hagemann ment. It is also clear that these factors subside with time, as brain
et al., 1998; Que et al., 1999; Redecker et al., 2002; Blicher et al., plasticity after stroke is well documented.
2009; Butefisch, 2004). It has been suggested that a favourable time Brain infarct causes inflammatory responses at the injury site as
window for ‘stroke-induced brain plasticity’ opens shortly after stroke well as in the surrounding area (Schroeter et al., 2002; Wang et al.,
(Biernaskie et al., 2004; Maulden et al., 2005). 2007; Di Filippo et al., 2008). We hypothesized that such inflamma-
In contrast to the hypothesis suggesting facilitation of plasticity tory responses are a reason for the impairment of brain plasticity
after stroke (see for example Hsu & Huang, 1997; Murphy & Corbett, following stroke. Indeed, Candelario-Jalil (2008) showed that sup-
2009) we have shown that, following a cortical lesion, experience- pression of the inflammatory responses supports the spontaneous
dependent plasticity in the somatosensory cortex (S1) of rat is not recovery after stroke. Inflammation in the brain begins within a few
increased, but shows a pronounced depression (Jablonka et al., 2007). hours after stroke and persists for at least several weeks (Barone &
In these experiments, plasticity of cortical representation of vibrissae Feuerstein, 1999; Tan et al., 2003; Wang et al., 2007). It was recently
was evoked in the barrel field by clipping all but one row of vibrissae. reported that poststroke impairment of sensory learning is reduced by
In control rats this treatment results in enlargement of functional treatment with the anti-inflammatory drug ibuprofen (Greifzu et al.,
cortical representation of the spared row, as visualized by [14C]-2- 2011). Also, we have found that experience-dependent cortical
deoxyglucose (2DG) autoradiography. After a focal photothrombotic plasticity is rescued by inhibition of metalloproteases (Cybulska-
stroke neighbouring the barrel cortex, no experience-dependent Klosowicz et al., 2011).
The present experiments were designed to evaluate the time-related
dynamics of poststroke plasticity impairment and to examine whether
Correspondence: Malgorzata Kossut, as above. suppression of the inflammatory response in the poststroke brain can
E-mail: kossut@nencki.gov.pl
improve experience-dependent plasticity. In order to suppress inflam-
Received 17 November 2011, revised 27 April 2012, accepted 30 April 2012 matory responses after stroke, we chronically applied ibuprofen, a

ª 2012 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience ª 2012 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Effect of ibuprofen on poststroke plasticity 2633

nonselective cyclooxygenase (COX) inhibitor that crosses the blood– Aldrich, St Louis, MO, USA) was injected intravenously through a
brain barrier (Parepally et al., 2006). To verify the stroke-induced catheter implanted into the femoral vein and the skull surface was
inflammatory response we tested the expression of cyclooxygenase-2 illuminated for 20 min.
(COX-2) in the barrel cortex. COX-2 is a pro-inflammatory enzyme
that participates in glutamate excitotoxicity and cytotoxicity associated
with inflammation after stroke (Cherubini et al., 2005; Bidmon et al., Experimental groups
2000). The experiments with delayed onset of the deprivation after The following groups were used:
stroke were to test whether avoiding the period when inflammation is
strong would enhance the plastic change. (1) Animals with ibuprofen (a-methyl-4-(isobutyl)phenylacetic acid;
I1892; Sigma-Aldrich, St Louis, MO, USA) intraperitoneal
injections just before the stroke and every day for 1 month, and
Materials and methods whisker deprivation starting immediately after the stroke. Two
different doses of ibuprofen were applied – 10 mg ⁄ kg (n = 6) and
The experimental protocol was approved by the First Ethical
20 mg ⁄ kg. (n = 7).
Commission in Warsaw, Poland, and was in accordance with the
(2) Animals with deprivation starting with different delays after the
European Communities Council Directive of 24 November 1986
stroke – immediately after stroke (n = 7), 1 week after (n = 4) and
(86 ⁄ 609 ⁄ EEC).
28 days after (n = 5).
Male Wistar rats (230 g) received a photothrombotic stroke in the
(3) Control groups:
somatosensory cortex caudally and medially to the barrel field
intact + whisker deprivation (n = 7);
representation (Fig. 1). Sensory deprivation was performed by trim-
intact + whisker deprivation + 20 mg ⁄ kg ibuprofen injections
ming all the whiskers contralateral to the stroke except for row B. The
(n = 4);
trimming was repeated every second day for 1 month.
stroke only (n = 7).
(4) Animals for testing COX-2 expression in the brain
intact (n = 4)
Surgical procedure
stroke only (n = 5)
Photothrombotic stroke was inflicted as described previously (Jab- stroke + single ibuprofen injection (10 mg ⁄ kg) before stroke
lonka et al., 2007) under anaesthesia induced by a mixture of 3% (n = 4)
isoflurane in air. The body temperature was controlled and sustained at stroke + single ibuprofen injection (20 mg ⁄ kg) before stroke
36.5 ± 0.5 C. A catheter (0.75 mm) was inserted into the rat’s (n = 5)
femoral vein. An incision was made down the midline of the head and
the skull surface exposed and cleaned. A fibre-optic bundle mounted Animals were kept separately in plastic cages in a 12-h light–dark
on a cold light source was placed stereotaxically on the exposed skull cycle, at a temperature of  20 C, with free access to food and water.
bone, with its light centre (Ø = 1.5 mm) 4.5 mm posterior to bregma
and 4 mm lateral to the midline (aperture E ⁄ 2; 2750K, KL 1500 LCD;
Schott, Germany). Rose Bengal (0.4 mL, 10 mg ⁄ mL; 330000; Sigma- 2DG mapping
After a month of vibrissae deprivation, we performed 2DG functional
brain mapping. The animals were put in a restrainer and the whiskers
of the non-deprived side, except for row B, were cut close to the skin.
2DG, which was labelled with 14C (7 lCi ⁄ 100 g, specific activity
55 mCi ⁄ mmol; American Radiolabeled Chemical, St Louis, MO,
USA), was injected intramuscularly. The whiskers of rows B on both
sides of the snout were stroked in a rostrocaudal direction twice per
second with a hand-held mechanical stimulator. After 30 min of
stimulation, the rats were killed with an i.p. Vetbutal (0.4 mL ⁄ 100 g;
Biovet, Pulawy, Poland) injection and perfused with 4% paraformal-
dehyde (Sigma-Aldrich, St Louis, MO, USA) for 3 min. Then the
brain was removed and the cortex of both hemispheres flattened
between glass slides, frozen in heptane at )70 C and stored at this
temperature. Hemispheres were cut on a cryostat at )20 C into 40-
lm tangential sections, which were collected alternately on specimen
slides and cover slips. The sections on cover slips were immediately
dried and exposed on Kodak Mammography film for 2 weeks with a
set of [14C] standards (American Radiolabeled Chemicals, St. Louis,
MO). The remaining sections were stained for cytochrome oxidase
activity to identify the barrel field.

COX-2 expression analysis


The expression of COX-2 was analysed using the Western blot
Fig. 1. Localization of stroke-induced lesion, 30 days after stroke. Tangential
section across cortical layer IV stained for cytochrome oxidase activity. Arrow method in barrel cortex homogenates 24 h after stroke induction The
marks the stroke-induced lesion. Arrowheads, rows of barrels; A, anterior; P, tissue samples were taken from both hemispheres according to the
posterior. Scale bar, 1 mm. method described by Strominger & Woolsey (1987). The protein was

ª 2012 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience ª 2012 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 36, 2632–2639
2634 J. A. Jablonka et al.

isolated using a Total Protein Extraction Kit (Millipore Billerica, MA, analysis for comparison of the experimental groups was carried out
USA) and protein concentration was measured in each probe with with Kruskal–Wallis one-way anova (sigma plot software; Systat
Pierce BCA Protein Assay Kit (Thermo Scientific Rockford, IL, Software, San Jose, CA, USA). For post hoc pairwise comparison the
USA). Ten micrograms of total protein from each sample was loaded Tukey–Kramer test was used.
onto 9% SDS-PAGE gel and run at 100 V for 3 h. Proteins were
transferred onto Immobilon P membrane (Millipore Billerica, MA,
USA). Nonspecific binding was blocked by incubating membranes in Results
10% nonfat milk for 2 h at room temperature and then membranes Stroke-induced lesion
were incubated overnight at 4 C with polyclonal epitope-specific
rabbit antibody against COX-2 (no. DLN-15526; 1 : 200; Dianova, The extent of cortical lesion 1 month after the stroke was defined on
Hamburg, Germany) and mouse antiactin monoclonal antibody cytochrome oxidase-stained sections. The lesion was situated pos-
(MAB1501; 1 : 1000; Millipore Billerica, MA, USA). After washing teromedially to the barrel field (Fig. 1; marked with arrow) in the
in Tris-buffered saline with Tween, membranes were incubated with posterior parietal cortex and extended through all cortical layers,
biotinylated antirabbit secondary antibody (1 : 200; Vector Labora- tapering off in layer VI. The mean lesion volume in animals killed
tories, Burlingame, CA, USA) and biotinylated antimouse secondary 1 month after the stroke was 0.8 ± 0.3 mm3 and the mean distance of
antibody (1 : 200; Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA, USA), the stroke-induced lesion edge from the B1 barrel edge was
followed by horseradish peroxidase-linked avidin (Vector). Blots were 1200 ± 208 lm. The infarct was confined to the cortical tissue and
visualized enzymatically with diaminobenzidine as a substrate [SIG- in any case it did not included the subcortical white matter. Ibuprofen
MAFAST 3,3¢-diaminobenzidine (DAB) tablets; Sigma]. had no influence on the volume of the lesion core as the volume of the
lesion in treated animals was 0.6 ± 0.3 mm3 vs. 0.8 ± 0.3 mm3 in the
stroke group without the treatment (unpaired t-test – t6 = 1.39,
P = 0.18).
Data analysis The lesion was reflected by the reduced intensity of 2DG uptake
The infarct volume was estimated 1 month after stroke in a separate (Fig. 2; marked with asterisk). We estimated the extent of the dip in
group of rats (n = 7), on sections cut in the coronal plane stained for metabolic activation. No significant differences were observed
cytochrome oxidase and examined with a Nikon Eclipse 80i between groups. The region of lowered metabolism was more
microscope equipped with a digital camera (Evolution VF; MediaCy- extensive than the lesion and amounted to 1.2 ± 0.3 mm3 in rats
bernetics Bethesda, MD, USA). In order to obtain the lesion volume, without pharmacological treatment, 1 ± 0.4 mm3 in rats with
the lesion area was measured in every third section (40-lm sections). 10 mg ⁄ kg ibuprofen and 1 ± 0.5 mm3 in rats receiving 20 mg ⁄ kg
The lesion volume was calculated by multiplying the lesion area by ibuprofen.
the distance between the sections and summing the results.
The autoradiograms were analyzed by a computer image analysis
program (MCID Res Inc., Ontario, Canada). The software enables 2DG labelling of row B cortical representations
display of an image of a stained section (from which the autoradio- Bilateral stimulation of row B vibrissae during 2DG brain mapping
gram was obtained) and an adjacent cytochrome oxidase-stained enhanced isotope incorporation in the barrel fields of both hemi-
section which was superimposed on the autoradiogram so that the spheres. The functional representation of row B whiskers was visible
relations of the barrel field and the position of the stroke could be as a band of increased OD situated over histological row B barrels in
accurately determined (Fig. 1). The width of the 2DG-labelled cortical layer IV, but discernible on the autoradiograms from layer II to VI
representation of row B whiskers was measured on every section at (Jablonka et al., 2007). Unilateral deprivation of all vibrissae except
three locations across the row. Pixels with 2DG uptake intensity row B resulted in significant (50%) widening of the spared row B
exceeding the mean of the surrounding cortex by > 15 % were representation in the contralateral hemisphere, as compared to control
considered labelled representations. Results, tested first for relatively row B representation in the non-deprived hemisphere (1196 ± 119 vs.
equal widening within layers, were averaged for all sections (layers II– 798 ± 34 lm, paired t-test – t6 = 8.67, P = 0.0001; Figs 2 and 5).
VI) of one hemisphere. The significance of the differences in labelling The stroke did not influence the width of row B representations in
of row B representation between the brain hemispheres was calculated non-deprived animals, either in the lesioned hemisphere or in the
with paired two-tailed Student’s t-tests. Comparisons of row B intact (806 ± 74 and 803 ± 67 lm respectively; paired t-test –
representation width between experimental groups were made sepa- t6 = 0.14, P =0.9).
rately for control and experimental hemispheres with one-way anova.
The statistical analysis for control hemispheres did not revealed any
significant differences between experimental groups (anova – F7,39 =
Effect of poststroke delay on deprivation-induced plasticity
1.76, P = 0.12). Statistical analysis of differences between experimental
hemispheres revealed significant effects of experimental situation on the Whisker deprivation that started immediately after the stroke did not
width of the row B cortical representations (anova – F7,39 = 14, result in significant widening of the spared row B representation
P = 0.003). Results of post hoc comparisons (Tukey–Kramer multiple (856 ± 107 vs. 889 ± 67 lm; paired t-test – t3 = 8.58, P = 0.34;
comparisons test) between respective groups are presented in the results. Figs 2 and 5). When we postponed the deprivation until 1 week after
Western blots were densitometrically quantified using ImageJ the stroke, the spared row B whiskers cortical representation was
software (NIH, Bethesda, MD, USA). Absolute optical density (OD) significantly wider than the non-deprived side row B representation
was normalized to the ODs of the corresponding bands of actin (20%; 961 ± 37 vs. 798 ± 24 lm; paired t-test – t6 = 8.67, P = 0.003;
loading control and expressed as relative abundance in arbitrary units. Fig. 5A). However, it did not differ significantly from that in either
For the statistical analysis, ratios of COX-2 band intensities in non-deprived animals after stroke or rats deprived immediately after
experimental hemispheres to respective control hemispheres were stroke (961 ± 37, 806 ± 74 and 898.8 ± 67 lm respectively; Tukey–
calculated and compared between experimental groups. The statistical Kramer test – F7,39 = 14, P = 0.23; Fig. 3) Thus, 1 week after the

ª 2012 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience ª 2012 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 36, 2632–2639
Effect of ibuprofen on poststroke plasticity 2635

Fig. 2. Effect of stroke upon experience-dependent plasticity. Digitized pseudocoloured autoradiograms of tangential cortical sections at the level of layer IV
presenting the 2DG labelling following stimulation of row B vibrissae in intact controls, in rats with all vibrissae except row B removed for 30 days (deprivation),
and in rats with stroke and deprivation. Arrows point to labelling corresponding to stimulated row B representations. Asterisk marks the stroke-induced lesion; Au,
auditory cortex; x, air bubble artefact.

Fig. 3. High magnification of 2DG-labelled row B representation in the poststroke groups, on digitized autoradiograms of tangential sections of the cortex at
different poststroke time points. Asterisk marks the stroke-induced lesion.

stroke, the plasticity-impeding factors were less effective but still 877 ± 86 lm; paired t-test – t5 = 5.79, P = 0.002; Fig. 4). However,
present. The extent of plastic change in that group was smaller than in the width of labelling was not significantly different from that in the
the deprived controls without stroke (20% vs. 50%) and the width of group with stroke but without ibuprofen. The extent of the plastic
spared-row representations amounted to 961 ± 37 and change was smaller than in the deprived rats without stroke
1196 ± 119 lm respectively (Tukey–Kramer test – F7,39 = 14, (1029 ± 63 vs. 1196 ± 120 lm; Tukey–Kramer test – F7,39 = 14,
P = 0.009; Fig. 5A). P = 0.03; Fig. 5B).
When deprivation started 1 month after stroke the spared-row Injections of a higher dose (20 mg ⁄ kg) of ibuprofen had a stronger
representation was enlarged by 25% (1001 ± 153 lm in the deprived influence on cortical remodelling, as the spared-row representation
vs. 797 ± 88 lm in the non-deprived hemisphere; paired t-test – widened by 33% (1067 ± 63 vs. 805 ± 70 lm; paired t-test –
t4 = 4.93, P = 0.008; Fig. 5A). This did not differ from deprivation in t6 = 11.3, P = 0.00003; Fig. 4). The width of spared-row representa-
the group delayed by 1 week (1001 ± 153 and 961 ± 37 lm respec- tion in that group did not differed significantly from the width of
tively; unpaired t-test – t7 = 0.51, P = 0.63; Fig. 3) and still differed spared-row representation in the control group with deprivation
significantly from that observed in deprived rats without stroke (the (1067 ± 63 and 1196 ± 120 lm; Tukey–Kramer test – F7,39 = 14,
widths of spared-row representations were 1001 ± 153 and P = 0.12; Fig. 5B) and was significantly greater than in the group with
1196 ± 119 lm, respectively; Tukey–Kramer test – F7,39 = 14, stroke and without ibuprofen (1067 ± 63 vs. 806 ± 74 lm; Tukey–
P = 0.006; Fig. 5A). Thus, even after a long poststroke delay the Kramer test – F7,39 = 14, P = 0.001; Fig. 5B).
extent of the plasticity change in cortical representation did not reach
the control value.
Poststroke expression of COX-2
The Western blot analysis revealed a single immunoreactive band of
Effect of ibuprofen 70 kDa which corresponded with COX-2 protein (Fig. 6). In control
Ibuprofen by itself did not influence plasticity in healthy animals. Rats intact animals COX-2 expression was low and no interhemispheric
without stroke, subjected to deprivation and treated with ibuprofen differences were found, giving a value of 1.14 ± 0.26 for the
(20 mg ⁄ kg), had the same enlargement of spared whiskers represen- interhemispheric COX-2 expression ratio. In stroke animals without
tation (54%) as control animals (50%). The width of the spared row B ibuprofen treatment the expression of COX-2 in the hemisphere
representation was 1140 ± 41 vs. 740 ± 23 lm in the non-deprived contralateral to the stroke was similar to that in control tissue, whereas
hemisphere (paired t-test – t3 = 39.91, P = 0.00003; Fig. 5B). in the affected hemisphere COX-2 expression increased compared to
After stroke, daily ibuprofen injections at a dose of 10 mg ⁄ kg the contralateral barrel cortex, giving a stroke to control hemisphere
resulted, in whisker-deprived rats, in a statistically significant increase ratio > 2· higher than in control animals (2.99 ± 0.32 vs. 1.14 ± 0.26,
in the width of the spared-row cortical representation compared to the respectively; anova – F17,3 = 33.92; P = 0.004). A single injection of
contralateral hemisphere (17% of widening; 1029 ± 63 vs. the lower dose of ibuprofen (10 mg ⁄ kg) before the stroke induction did

ª 2012 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience ª 2012 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 36, 2632–2639
2636 J. A. Jablonka et al.

Fig. 5. Effect of deprivation on cortical representation plasticity


(mean ± SD). (A) Influence of the onset of induction of poststroke plasticity.
(B) Influence of different doses of ibuprofen on the poststroke plasticity. The
differences between hemispheres are marked with asterisks (*). Dots (•) mark
differences in relation to experimental hemisphere of intact group with
deprivation. Diamonds (¤) refer to the relations between experimental
hemispheres of particular groups. •P < 0.05; **,••,¤¤P < 0.01; ***,•••P < 0.001.

COX-2 levels in somatosensory cortex 24 h after ischemia, an effect


that was abolished with application of ibuprofen prior to the stroke
induction. We excluded the possibility that the ibuprofen by itself
influenced plasticity (Fig 5).
Partial whisker deprivation leads to enlargement of the cortical
Fig. 4. High magnification of digitized autoradiograms of tangential sections representation of the spared whiskers at the expense of the
of the cortex showing the effect of ibuprofen on plasticity of row B representation of the removed whiskers. This cortical map reorga-
representation in the poststroke groups. nization involves CaMKII and CRE and requires NMDA receptor-
dependent induction of long-term potentiation (Glazewski et al.,
2000; Barth et al., 2000). It also involves depression which is similar
not change the COX-2 expression in barrel cortex neighbouring the to long-term depression and weakening of intracortical synaptic
infarct area. The interhemispheric ratio of COX-2 expression was connections (Drew & Feldman, 2009). Activity of intracortical
similar to that in the stroke group (2.81 ± 0.35 vs. 2.99 ± 0.32; anova inhibitory circuits is altered in the whisker-deprived representation
– F17,3 = 33.92; P = 0.001). However, increasing the ibuprofen dose (Li et al., 2009). Loss and growth of dendritic apical branches is
to 20 mg ⁄ kg significantly attenuated the augmentation of COX-2 enhanced, and their spines undergo rearrangement (Tailby et al.,
expression in the injured hemisphere to give a ratio value of 1.5 ± 0.39, 2005). Within days, axons on both excitatory and inhibitory neurons
which did not differ significantly from the control values (Fig. 6). elongate in the direction of the neighbouring cortex deprived of
sensory input (Marik et al., 2010). We found that stroke strongly
affected this type of cortical plasticity. With a focal stroke in the
Discussion vicinity of the barrel field, we observed a complete loss of use-
Our results demonstrate that, following stroke, experience-dependent dependent remodelling of cortical representations in the afflicted
plasticity of cortical vibrissae representations was impaired. This hemisphere (Figs 2 and 3). The effect was not due to impairment of
impairment of plasticity decreased when the poststroke onset of stimulation-driven 2DG uptake in the lesioned hemisphere because
deprivation was delayed. The plasticity may be restored by application we found previously that, 1 month after stroke, the 2DG labelling of
of ibuprofen, showing that the poststroke impairment of brain cortical row B representation in non-deprived animals was the same
plasticity is caused at least partially by stroke-induced inflammatory in the hemisphere with and without stroke; it also did not differ from
reactions in the brain. This thesis is supported by the results of COX-2 the extent of labelling in controls without the stroke (Jablonka et al.,
expression Western blot analysis, which revealed an elevation in 2007).

ª 2012 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience ª 2012 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 36, 2632–2639
Effect of ibuprofen on poststroke plasticity 2637

are the most detrimental to the experience-dependent plasticity. When


the deprivation began 7 days after the stroke, the plastic change in the
representation of the spared whiskers was partially re-established,
although still significantly smaller than in the rats without stroke.
Greifzu et al. (2011) did not observed any beneficial effect of
poststroke delay upon monocular deprivation-induced ocular domi-
nance shift. However, in the same animals, a poststroke delay of 1 or
2 weeks improved visual acuity and contrast sensitivity which had
been impaired by the stroke.
Our previous results, which demonstrated considerable remapping of
cerebral cortex after the stroke, indicated that the heightened activation
of many cortical regions, activity not evoked by stimulation of vibrissae,
persisted up to 4 weeks after stroke onset, while 2 months after stroke
the interhemispheric equilibrium stabilized (Jablonka et al., 2010). We
observed here that 3 weeks difference in the onset of deprivation
between the groups (1 week to 1 month delay) influenced the experi-
ence-dependent plasticity improvement by just a few per cent, similarly
to the effect of the 1-week delay. Apparently, the factors impeding
plasticity after stroke recede partially before the 7th poststroke day and
thereafter until the 28th day do not ameliorate remarkably. One may
therefore assume that disorganized brain activity disturbs the proper
brain response to the plasticity-inducing experience. Perhaps even
longer poststroke delay of deprivation would restore the plasticity of
cortical representation to the control value.
On the other hand, the immediate pathologies, such as the poststroke
blood supply restriction and reperfusion (Schaller & Graf, 2004), energy
failure and decrease in ion pump activity (Hossmann, 1999), spreading
depression (Dietrich et al., 1994), metabolic changes and oedema
(Marmarou, 2007) are detrimental to plasticity. Stroke-induced inflam-
mation, by itself or by some indirect influences (Wang et al., 2008; Di
Fig. 6. Influence of unilateral photothrombotic stroke and ibuprofen treatment Filippo et al., 2008), may also impair experience-dependent cortical
upon the COX-2 expression in adjoining rat barrel cortex. Top panel shows reorganization. Our results showed that 24 h following injury a
examples of the blots. The position of protein markers (kDa) is indicated at the
proinflammatory enzyme COX-2 was highly upregulated in the
left of the panel. A unique immunoreactive band, corresponding to a protein of
70 kDa, was detected with anti-COX-2 antibody in all homogenates. Lower somatosensory cortex of the lesioned hemisphere. Earlier, Ahmad
bands of 43 kDa show immunostaining for actin, used as a loading control. et al. (2009) showed increased expression of COX-2 protein 4 and 24 h
Bottom panel shows quantification of COX-2 expression changes in somato- after middle cerebral artery occlusion, and Bidmon et al. (2000)
sensory cortex after photothrombotic stroke and ibuprofen treatment. Bars observed a similar effect after cerebral cortical photothrombosis. Also,
represent the mean ± SD ratio of COX-2 expression in somatosensory cortex of
experimental (with infarct) hemisphere to control hemisphere. **P < 0.01; similarly to the above-mentioned papers, the increase we observed was
***
P < 0.001. confined to the hemisphere which had suffered the stroke. Based on the
literature data we tested COX-2 expression 24 h after induction of
ischemia, as at later survival times it was found to return to control
The position of the lesion may be important for creating a pro- values (Bidmon et al., 2000; Strauss et al., 2000).
plastic milieu at a distant but strongly connected site (Carmichael The anti-inflammatory drug ibuprofen partially re-established the
et al., 2005). We did not examine this aspect in our experiments. The poststroke plasticity. Western blot analysis revealed that ibuprofen
lesioned region is very sparsely connected with the barrel field (Fabri also had a strong effect upon COX-2 expression. A single application
& Burton, 1991). We concentrated here on diffuse non-directed effects of the higher ibuprofen dose (20 mg ⁄ kg) prevented the COX-2
of stroke on cortical plasticity, expecting a facilitatory effect (Dohle upregulation in the vicinity of the infarct 24 h after the stroke. The
et al., 2009). Instead, we found a strong suppression of experience- lower dose (10 mg ⁄ kg) was ineffective in attenuating COX-2
dependent plasticity in a region neighbouring the stroke, suggesting a expression. However, with chronic application of ibuprofen at the
generalized reduction of ability to modify cortical representations in lower dose, a small increase in plasticity of the peri-infarct barrel
the vicinity of cortical damage. Recently, influence of the same kind of cortex was observed when the results from deprived and control
stroke on the ocular dominance plasticity in mice has been observed hemispheres were compared. This change was not large enough to be
(Greifzu et al., 2011). Ocular dominance plasticity shares mostly the significantly different from that observed in rats who had undergone
same cellular mechanisms as the S1 experience-dependent remodel- stroke without ibuprofen. The higher ibuprofen dose was sufficient to
ling (Buonomano & Merzenich, 1998). It can be assumed that Greifzu restore experience-dependent plasticity in the barrel cortex.
et al. (2011) observed the same phenomenon, the impairment of Ibuprofen might interact directly as a nonselective inhibitor of
experience-dependent cortical representation plasticity after focal cyclooxygenases or by influencing transcription factors – nuclear
stroke in the vicinity of the visual cortex, as we observed in the receptor protein peroxisome proliferator-activated receptors (PPARs;
barrel field of rats and mice (Jablonka et al., 2007; Cybulska- Kojo et al., 2003). Both cyclooxygenases and PPARs may influence
Klosowicz et al., 2011). the expression of MMP-9 metalloproteinase (Wang et al., 2008; Yin
In contrast with the results of Greifzu et al. (2011), we found that et al., 2011), which is important both for the integrity of the blood
the changes in brain physiology appearing immediately after the stroke brain barrier and for structural plasticity mechanisms participating in

ª 2012 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience ª 2012 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 36, 2632–2639
2638 J. A. Jablonka et al.

the reorganization of dendritic spines (Wang et al., 2008). The MMP- Barone, F.C. & Feuerstein, G.Z. (1999) Inflammatory mediators and stroke: new
9-induced impairment of poststroke plasticity changes has recently opportunities for novel therapeutics. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab., 19, 819–834.
Barth, A.L., McKenna, M., Glazewski, S., Hill, P., Impey, S., Storm, D. & Fox,
been demonstrated in mice, in the same experimental model of K. (2000) Upregulation of cAMP response element-mediated gene expres-
photothrombotic stroke and experience-dependent plasticity changes sion during experience-dependent plasticity in adult neocortex. J. Neurosci.,
(Cybulska-Klosowicz et al., 2011). Thus, one of the potential 20, 4206–4216.
mechanisms for the beneficial effect of ibuprofen on poststroke Bidmon, H.J., Oermann, E., Schiene, K., Schmitt, M., Kato, K., Asayama, K.,
plasticity might involve counteracting the unfavourable effects of Witte, O.W. & Zilles, K. (2000) Unilateral upregulation of cyclooxygenase-2
following cerebral, cortical photothrombosis in the rat: suppression by MK-
MMP-9 upregulation after stroke. 801 and co-distribution with enzymes involved in the oxidative stress
Recent results on mice show no influence of ibuprofen on the size cascade. J. Chem. Neuroanat., 20, 163–176.
of focal photothrombotic stroke and no influence on decreased ocular Biernaskie, J., Chernenko, G. & Corbett, D. (2004) Efficacy of rehabilitative
dominance plasticity after stroke, but a pronounced improvement in experience declines with time after focal ischemic brain injury. J. Neurosci.,
24, 1245–1254.
visual acuity and contrast sensitivity in ibuprofen treated mice Blicher, J.U., Jakobsen, J., Andersen, G. & Nielsen, J.F. (2009) Cortical
(Greifzu et al., 2011). The discrepancy between our results concerning excitability in chronic stroke and modulation by training: a TMS study.
the effect of ibuprofen on cortical experience-dependent remapping in Neurorehabil. Neural Repair, 23, 486–493.
the barrel cortex, and those shown by Greifzu et al. (2011) in the Buonomano, D.V. & Merzenich, M.M. (1998) Cortical plasticity: from
visual cortex, despite the similar model of the focal photothrombotic synapses to maps. Annu. Rev. Neurosci., 21, 149–186.
Butefisch, C.M. (2004) Plasticity in the human cerebral cortex: lessons from the
stroke, may well be due to the difference in drug treatment onset normal brain and from stroke. Neuroscientist, 10, 163–173.
(30 min before in our model and just after the stroke in the Greifzu Candelario-Jalil, E. (2008) Nimesulide as a promising neuroprotectant in brain
et al. experiment) and ⁄ or the ibuprofen dose we used (the highest ischemia: new experimental evidences. Pharmacol. Res., 57, 266–273.
dose we used was 20 mg ⁄ kg whereas Greifzu et al. (2011) treated Carmichael, S.T., Archibeque, I., Luke, L., Nolan, T., Momiy, J. & Li, S.
(2005) Growth-associated gene expression after stroke: evidence for a
mice with 60 mg ⁄ kg). Also, in our brain mapping experiments the rats growth-promoting region in peri-infarct cortex. Exp. Neurol., 193, 291–311.
were not anesthetized, while deep anaesthesia was used by Greifzu Cherubini, A., Ruggiero, C., Polidori, M.C. & Mecocci, P. (2005) Potential
et al. (2011) for optical recording of ocular dominance changes. As markers of oxidative stress in stroke. Free Radic. Biol. Med., 39, 841–852.
anesthesia dampens the brain’s metabolic response (Shapiro et al., Cybulska-Klosowicz, A., Liguz-Lecznar, M., Nowicka, D., Ziemka-Nalecz,
1978), small increases in ocular dominance shift might have been M., Kossut, M. & Skangiel-Kramska, J. (2011) Matrix metalloproteinase
inhibition counteracts impairment of cortical experience-dependent plasticity
missed by the experimenters. Some other factors might also have been after photothrombotic stroke. Eur. J. Neurosci., 33, 2238–2246.
of some importance – the experimental animals (mice vs. rats), cortical Di Filippo, M., Tozzi, A., Costa, C., Belcastro, V., Tantucci, M., Picconi, B. &
area tested (S1 vs. V1), different durations of deprivation (4 weeks for Calabresi, P. (2008) Plasticity and repair in the post-ischemic brain.
whiskers vs. 2 weeks monocular). Neuropharmacology, 55, 353–362.
Dietrich, W.D., Feng, Z.C., Leistra, H., Watson, B.D. & Rosenthal, M. (1994)
It is important to stress here that our experimental design did not Photothrombotic infarction triggers multiple episodes of cortical spreading
address the issue of recovery of function. We examined only cortical depression in distant brain regions. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab., 14, 20–28.
remodelling in response to changed sensory input. Our results Dohle, C.I., Eysel, U.T. & Mittmann, T. (2009) Spatial distribution of long-
demonstrate a decrease in experience-dependent plasticity in the term potentiation in the surround of visual cortex lesions in vitro. Exp. Brain
cortex neighbouring a focal cortical stroke and its partial reestablish- Res., 199, 423–433.
Drew, P.J. & Feldman, D.E. (2009) Intrinsic signal imaging of deprivation-
ment after application of an anti-inflammatory drug, ibuprofen. In the induced contraction of whisker representations in rat somatosensory cortex.
acute phase, stroke reduces plastic remodelling of the cortex; with Cereb. Cortex, 19, 331–348.
longer poststroke interval plasticity is partially restored. We probably Fabri, M. & Burton, H. (1991) Ipsilateral cortical connections of primary
observed a sequential action of several factors interfering with somatic sensory cortex in rats. J. Comp. Neurol., 311, 405–424.
Glazewski, S., Giese, K.P., Silva, A. & Fox, K. (2000) The role of alpha-
plasticity that diminished with increasing poststroke survival time. CaMKII autophosphorylation in neocortical experience-dependent plasticity.
The injection of the anti-inflammatory drug may act on one or several Nat. Neurosci., 3, 911–918.
of these factors; this requires further studies. However, even when the Greifzu, F., Schmidt, S., Schmidt, K.F., Kreikemeier, K., Witte, O.W. &
deprivation was applied 1 month after the stroke, the plastic change in Lowel, S. (2011) Global impairment and therapeutic restoration of visual
the cortical representation was not more extensive than in the control plasticity mechanisms after a localized cortical stroke. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
USA, 108, 15450–15455.
rats without stroke. No pro-plastic effect of stroke was observed in our Hagemann, G., Redecker, C., Neumann-Haefelin, T., Freund, H.J. & Witte,
experience-dependent plasticity model. O.W. (1998) Increased long-term potentiation in the surround of experi-
mentally induced focal cortical infarction. Ann. Neurol., 44, 255–258.
Hossmann, K.A. (1999) The hypoxic brain. Insights from ischemia research.
Adv. Exp. Med. Biol., 474, 155–169.
Acknowledgements Hsu, K.S. & Huang, C.C. (1997) Characterization of the anoxia-induced long-
The research was supported by the Polish–German Cooperation Program in term synaptic potentiation in area CA1 of the rat hippocampus. Br. J.
Neuroscience grant no. S006 ⁄ -P-N ⁄ 2007 ⁄ 01 and Polish Ministry of Science and Pharmacol., 122, 671–681.
Higher Education grant no. 0133 ⁄ P01 ⁄ 2010 ⁄ 70. We thank Renata Zakrzewska Jablonka, J.A., Witte, O.W. & Kossut, M. (2007) Photothrombotic infarct
Marcin Kazmierczak and Ewa Nosecka for excellent technical assistance. impairs experience-dependent plasticity in neighboring cortex. NeuroReport,
18, 165–169.
Jablonka, J.A., Burnat, K., Witte, O.W. & Kossut, M. (2010) Remapping of the
somatosensory cortex after a photothrombotic stroke: dynamics of the
Abbreviations compensatory reorganization. Neuroscience, 165, 90–100.
14
2DG, C 2-deoxyglucose; COX, cyclooxygenase. Kojo, H., Fukagawa, M., Tajima, K., Suzuki, A., Fujimura, T., Aramori, I.,
Hayashi, K. & Nishimura, S. (2003) Evaluation of human peroxisome
proliferator-activated receptor (PPAR) subtype selectivity of a variety of
anti-inflammatory drugs based on a novel assay for PPAR delta(beta). J.
References Pharmacol. Sci., 93, 347–355.
Ahmad, M., Zhang, Y., Liu, H., Rose, M.E. & Graham, S.H. (2009) Prolonged Li, P., Rudolph, U. & Huntsman, M.M. (2009) Long-term sensory deprivation
opportunity for neuroprotection in experimental stroke with selective selectively rearranges functional inhibitory circuits in mouse barrel cortex.
blockade of cyclooxygenase-2 activity. Brain Res., 1279, 168–173. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 106, 12156–12161.

ª 2012 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience ª 2012 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 36, 2632–2639
Effect of ibuprofen on poststroke plasticity 2639

Marik, S.A., Yamahachi, H., McManus, J.N., Szabo, G. & Gilbert, C.D. (2010) characterization of inflammatory responses. J. Neurosci. Methods, 117, 43–
Axonal dynamics of excitatory and inhibitory neurons in somatosensory 49.
cortex. PLoS Biol., 8, e1000395. Shapiro, H.M., Greenberg, J.H., Reivich, M., Ashmead, G. & Sokoloff, L.
Marmarou, A. (2007) A review of progress in understanding the pathophys- (1978) Local cerebral glucose uptake in awake and halothane-anesthetized
iology and treatment of brain edema. Neurosurg. Focus, 22, E1. primates. Anesthesiology, 48, 97–103.
Maulden, S.A., Gassaway, J., Horn, S.D., Smout, R.J. & DeJong, G. (2005) Strauss, K.I., Barbe, M.F., Marshall, R.M., Raghupathi, R., Mehta, S. &
Timing of initiation of rehabilitation after stroke. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil., Narayan, R.K. (2000) Prolonged cyclooxygenase-2 induction in neurons and
86, S34–S40. glia following traumatic brain injury in the rat. J. Neurotrauma, 17, 695–711.
Murphy, T.H. & Corbett, D. (2009) Plasticity during stroke recovery: from Strominger, R.N. & Woolsey, T.A. (1987) Templates for locating the whisker
synapse to behaviour. Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 10, 861–872. area in fresh flattened mouse and rat cortex. J. Neurosci. Methods, 22, 113–
Parepally, J.M., Mandula, H. & Smith, Q.R. (2006) Brain uptake of 118.
nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs: ibuprofen, flurbiprofen, and indo- Tailby, C., Wright, L.L., Metha, A.B. & Calford, M.B. (2005) Activity-
methacin. Pharm. Res., 23, 873–881. dependent maintenance and growth of dendrites in adult cortex. Proc. Natl.
Que, M., Schiene, K., Witte, O.W. & Zilles, K. (1999) Widespread up- Acad. Sci. USA, 102, 4631–4636.
regulation of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors after focal photothrombotic Tan, K.T., Lip, G.Y. & Blann, A.D. (2003) Post-stroke inflammatory response:
lesion in rat brain. Neurosci. Lett., 273, 77–80. effects of stroke evolution and outcome. Curr. Atheroscler. Rep., 5, 245–251.
Redecker, C., Wang, W., Fritschy, J.M. & Witte, O.W. (2002) Widespread and Wang, Q., Tang, X.N. & Yenari, M.A. (2007) The inflammatory response in
long-lasting alterations in GABA(A)-receptor subtypes after focal cortical stroke. J. Neuroimmunol., 184, 53–68.
infarcts in rats: mediation by NMDA-dependent processes. J. Cereb. Blood Wang, X.B., Bozdagi, O., Nikitczuk, J.S., Zhai, Z.W., Zhou, Q. & Huntley,
Flow Metab., 22, 1463–1475. G.W. (2008) Extracellular proteolysis by matrix metalloproteinase-9 drives
Schaller, B. & Graf, R. (2004) Cerebral ischemia and reperfusion: the dendritic spine enlargement and long-term potentiation coordinately. Proc.
pathophysiologic concept as a basis for clinical therapy. J. Cereb. Blood Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 105, 19520–19525.
Flow Metab., 24, 351–371. Yin, K.J., Deng, Z., Hamblin, M., Zhang, J. & Chen, Y.E. (2011) Vascular
Schroeter, M., Jander, S. & Stoll, G. (2002) Non-invasive induction of focal PPARdelta protects against stroke-induced brain injury. Arterioscler.
cerebral ischemia in mice by photothrombosis of cortical microvessels: Thromb. Vasc. Biol., 31, 574–581.

ª 2012 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience ª 2012 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and Blackwell Publishing Ltd
European Journal of Neuroscience, 36, 2632–2639

You might also like