You are on page 1of 18

CHAPTER 28

Social Intelligence

John F. Kihlstrom and Nancy Cantor

The term social intelligence was first used He did acknowledge that the Picture
by Dewey (1909) and Lull (1911), but the Arrangement sub test of the W AIS might
modern concept has its origins in E. L. serve as a measure of social intelligence
Thorndike's (1920) division of intelligence because it assesses the individual's ability
into three facets pertaining to the ability to to comprehend social situations (see also
understand and manage ideas (abstract intel- Rapaport, Gill, & Shafer, 1968; Campbell
ligence), concrete objects (mechanical intel- & McCord, 1996). In his view, however,
ligence), and people (social intelligence). In "social intelligence is just general intelli-
Thorndike's classic formulation: "By social gence applied to social situations" (1958,
intelligence is meant the ability to under- p. 75). This dismissal was repeated in
stand and manage men and women, boys Matarazzo's (1972, p. 209) fifth and final
and girls - to act wisely in human rela- edition of Wechsler's monograph, in which
tions" (p. 228). Similarly, Moss and Hunt social intelligence dropped out as an index
(1927) defined social intelligence as the "abil- term.
ity to get along with others" (p. 108). Vernon
(1933) provided the most wide-ranging defi-
nition of social intelligence as the "ability to Measuring Social Intelligence
get along with people in general, social tech-
nique or ease in society, knowledge of social Defining social intelligence seems easy
matters, susceptibility to stimuli from other enough, especially by analogy to abstract
members of a group, as well as insight into intelligence. When it came to measur-
the temporary moods or underlying person- ing social intelligence, however, Thorndike
ality traits of strangers" (p. 44). (1920) noted somewhat ruefully that "con-
By contrast, Wechsler (1939, 1958) gave venient tests of social inteJligence are
scant attention to social intelligence in the hard to devise .... Social intelligence shows
development of the Wechsler Adult Intelli- itself abundantly in the nursery, on the
gence Scale (WAIS) and similar instruments. playground, in barracks and factories and
SOCIAL INTELLIGENCE s6s
salesroom [sic], but it eludes the formal stan- and ideas, that differences in social intelli-
dardized conditions of the testing labora- gence tend to be swamped by differences in
tory. It requires human beings to respond abstract intelligence" (p. z8z).
to 1 time to adapt its responses1 and face, The inability to discriminate between
voice, gesture, and mien as tools" (p. 231). social intelligence and IQ, coupled with dif-
Nevertheless, true to the goals of the ficulties in selecting external criteria against
psychometric tradition, researchers quickly which the scale could be validated, led to
translated the abstract definitions of social declining interest in the GWSIT, and indeed
intelligence into standardized laboratory in the whole concept of social intelligence
instruments for measuring individual differ- as a distinct intellectual entity. Spearman's
ences in social intelligence (for thorough g afforded no special place for social intelli-
reviews of research published before 2ooo, gence, of course; nor was social intelligence
see Kihlstrom & Cantor, 2ooo; Landy, 2oo6; included, or even implied, in Thurstone's list
Taylor, 1990; Walker & Foley, 1973). of primary mental abilities.

The George Washington Social Social Intelligence in Guilford's Structure


Intelligence Test ofIntellect
The first of these was the George Washing- After an initial burst of interest in the
ton Social Intelligence Test (GWSIT; Hunt, GWSIT, work on the assessment and cor-
1928; Moss 1 1931; Moss, Hunt, Omwake, relates of social intelligence fell off sharply
& Ronning, 1927; for later editions, see until the 196os (Walker & Foley, 1973), when
Moss, Hunt, & Omwake, 1949; Moss, Hunt, this line of research was revived within the
Omwake, & Woodward, 1955). Like the context of Guilford's Structure of Intellect
Stanford-Binet Intelligence Test or WAIS, model of intelligence. Guilford postulated a
the GWSIT was composed of a number of system of at least 120 separate intellectual
subtests, which can be combined to yield an abilities, based on all possible combina-
aggregate score. Four subtests - Judgment tions of five categories of operations (cog-
in Social Situations, Memory for Names and nition1 memory, divergent production, con-
Faces, Observation of Human Behavior, and vergent production, and evaluation), with
Recognition of the Mental States Behind four categories of content (figural, sym-
Words- were employed in all editions of the bolic, semantic, and behavioral) and six
GWSIT. Subtests of Facial Expression and categories of products (units, classes, rela-
Social Information subtests were included tions, systems 1transformations, and implica-
in early editions but dropped from in later tions). Within this more differentiated sys-
editions, and a Humor subtest was added. tem, social intelligence is represented by the
Hunt (1928) originally validated the domain of behavioral operations. In con-
GWSIT through its correlations with adult trast to its extensive work on semantic and
occupational status, the number of extracur- figural content, Guilford's group addressed
ricular activities pursued by college stu- issues of behavioral content only very late
dents, and supervisor ratings of employees' in their program of research. Of the 30
ability to get along with people. However, facets of social intelligence predicted by the
some controversy ensued about whether structure-of-intellect model (5 operations x
social intelligence should be correlated 6 products), actual tests were devised for
with personality measures of sociability or only six cognitive abilities (O'Sullivan et al.,
extraversion. Most important, however1 the 1965; Hoepfner & O'Sullivan, 1969) and six
GWSIT came under immediate criticism for divergent production abilities (Hendricks,
its relatively high correlation with abstract Guilford, & Hoepfner, 1969).
intelligence. Thorndike and Stein (1937) O'Sullivan et al. ( 1965) defined the cat-
concluded that the GWSIT "is so heav- egory of behavioral cognition as represent-
ily loaded with ability to work with words ing the "ability to judge people" (p. 5l
566 JOHN F. KIHLSTROM AND NANCY CANTOR

with respect to "feelings, motives, thoughts, of many and diverse behavioral ideas,
intentions, attitudes, or other psychological these investigators labeled these divergent-
dispositions which might affect an individ- thinking abilities creative social intelligence.
ual's social behavior" (O'Sullivan eta!., p. 4). As with the behavioral cognition abilities
They made it clear that someone' s ability to studied by O'Sullivan et al. (1965}, the very
judge individual people was not the same as nature of the behavioral domain raised seri-
his or her comprehension of people in gen- ous technical problems for test development
eral, or "stereotypic understanding" (p. 5), in the behavioral domain, especially with
and bore no a priori relation to one's abil- respect to contamination by verbal (seman-
ity to understand oneself. Apparently, these tic) abilities. As might be expected, scor-
two aspects of social cognition lie outside ing divergent productions proved consider-
the standard structure-of-intellect model. ably harder than scoring cognitions, as in the
In constructing their tests of behavioral former case there is no one best answer,
cognition, O'Sullivan et a!. (1965) assumed and subjects' responses must be evaluated
that "expressive behavior, more particu- by independent judges for quality as well
larly facial expressions, vocal inflections, as quantity. Nevertheless, a factor-analytic
postures, and gestures, are the cues from study yielded six factors clearly interpretable
which intentional states are inferred" (p. 6). as divergent production in the behavioral
While recognizing the value of assessing domain, which were essentially indepen-
the ability to decode these cues in real- dent of both divergent semantic production
life contexts with real people serving as tar- and (convergent) cognition in the behavioral
gets, economic constraints forced the inves- domain.
tigators to rely on photographs, cartoons, A later study by Chen and Michael (1993),
drawings, and tape recordings (the cost employing more modern factor-analytic
of film was prohibitive); verbal materials techniques, essentially confirmed these find-
were avoided wherever possible, presum- ings - although Snyder and Michael (1983)
ably to avoid contamination of social intelli- had earlier found significant correlations
gence by verbal abilities. Their study yielded between some of these tests of social intelli-
six factors clearly interpretable as cogni- gence and tests of verbal and mathematical
tion of behavior, which were not contami- ability. A similar reanalysis of the O'Sullivan
nated by nonsocial semantic and spatial abil- eta!. (1965) data by Romney and Pyryt (1999)
ities. However, echoing earlier findings with found that all the tests loaded on a sin-
the GWSIT, later studies found substan- gle factor rather than the six independent
tial correlations between IQ and scores on factors predicted by Guilford's Structure of
the individual Guilford subtests as well as Intellect theory. In neither domain is there
various composite social intelligence scores much evidence for the ability of any of these
(Riggio, Messamer, & Throckmorton, 1991; tests to predict external criteria of social
Shanley, Walker, & Foley, 1971). Still, Shan- intelligence.
ley et a!. (1971) conceded that the corre- Tests of the remaining three structure-
lations obtained were not strong enough of-intellect domains (memory, convergent
to warrant Wechsler's assertion that social production, and evaluation) had not been
intelligence is nothing more than general developed by the time the Guilford pro-
intelligence applied in the social domain. gram came to a close. Hendricks et a!.
In one of the last test-construction efforts (1969) noted that "these constitute by far
by Guilford's group, Hendricks eta!. (1969) the greatest number of unknowns in the
attempted to develop tests for coping with [Structure oflntellect] model" (p. 6). How-
other people, not just understanding them ever, O'Sullivan et a!. (1965) did sketch out
through their behavior - what they referred how these abilities were defined. Conver-
to as "basic solution-finding skills in inter- gent production in the behavioral domain was
personal relations" (p. 3). Because success- defined as "doing the right thing at the right
ful coping involves the creative generation time" (p. 5), and presumably might be tested
SOCIAL INTELLIGENCE 567

by a knowledge of etiquette. Behavioral the batteries of cognitive and divergent-


memory was defined as the ability to remem- production measures devised by the Guil-
ber the social characteristics of people (e.g., ford group, Marlowe's ostensible measures
names, faces, and personality traits), while of social intelligence are all self-report scales,
behavioral evaluation was defined as the abil- whereas his measures of verbal and abstract
ity to judge the appropriateness of behavior. intelligence were the usual sorts of objec-
tive performance tests. The measurement of
individual differences in social intelligence
Convergent and Discriminant Validity
by means of self-report scales is a major
in Social Intelligence
departure from the tradition of intelligence
Following the Guilford studies, a num- testing, and it seems important to confirm
ber of investigators continued the attempt Marlowe's findings using objective perfor-
to define social intelligence and determine mance measures of the various facets of
its relation to general abstract intelligence. social intelligence.
Most of these studies explicitly employed
the logic of the multitrait-multimethod
matrix (MTMM; Campbell & Fiske, 1959), The Prototype of Social Intelligence
employing multiple measures of social and
nonsocial intelligence, and examining the Although social intelligence has proved dif-
convergent validity of alternative measures ficult for psychometricians to operational-
within each domain and their discriminant ize, it does appear to play a major role in
validity across domains (e.g., Sechrest & people's niiive, intuitive concepts of intel-
Jackson, 1961). For example, Day and his ligence. Sternberg and his colleagues asked
group showed that multiple measures of subjects to list the behaviors which they con-
social insight and social intelligence were sidered characteristic of intelligence, aca-
poorly correlated with academic intelli- demic intelligence, everyday intelligence,
gence (Jones and Day,1997; Lee, Wong, Day, and unintelligence; two additional groups of
Maxwell, & Thorpe, 2ooo; Lee, Day, Meara, subjects rated each of 250 behaviors from
& Maxwell, 2002; Wong, Day, Maxwell, & the first list in terms of how "characteris-
Meara, 1995). Weis and Suss (2007) obtained tic" each was of the ideal person possess-
similar results for measures of social under- ing each of the three forms of intelligence
standing and social knowledge, but not for (Sternberg, Conway, Ketron, & Bernstein,
social memory. 1981). Factor analysis of ratings provided by
Marlowe (1986) and his colleagues assem- laypeople yielded a factor of "social compe-
bled a large battery of personality mea- tence" in each context. Prototypical behav-
sures ostensibly tapping various aspects of iors reflecting social competence were these:
social intelligence. Factor analysis of these
instruments yielded five dimensions of social Accepts others for what they are; admits
intelligence: interest and concern for other mistakes; displays interest in the world at
people, social performance skills, empathic large; is on time for appointments; has
ability, emotional expressiveness and sen- social conscience; thinks before speaking
sitivity to others' emotional expressions, and doing; displays curiosity; does not
make snap judgments; makes fair judg-
and social anxiety and lack of social self-
ments; assesses well the relevance of infor-
efficacy and self-esteem. Factor scores on mation to a problem at hand; is sensitive
these dimensions of social intelligence were to other people's needs and desires; is frank
essentially unrelated to measures of verbal and honest with self and others; and dis-
and abstract intelligence. In evaluating stud- plays interest in the immediate environ-
ies like this, however, note that the apparent ment.
independence of social and general intelli-
gence may be at least partially an artifact Interestingly, a separate dimension of social
of method variance. Unlike the GWSIT and competence did not consistently emerge
568 JOHN F. KIHtSTROM AND NANCY CANTOR

in ratings made by a group of experts on components, may well be somewhat inde-


intelligence. Rather, the experts' dimen- pendent of each other; but the actual rela-
sions focused on verbal intelligence and tion among various intellectual abilities is an
problem-solving ability, with social compe- open, empirical question.
tence expressly emerging only in the ratings Answering this question, of course,
of the ideal "practically intelligent" person. requires that we have psychometrically ade-
Perhaps these experts shared Wechsler's clis- quate instruments for assessing social intel-
missive view of social intelligence. ligence. This brings us back to our starting
Similar stuclies were conducted by point - the question of how social intelli-
Kosmitzld and John (1993), and by Schnei- gence is to be 1neasured. Future investiga-
der, Ackerman, and Kaufer (1996), and tors who wish to make the attempt might be
obtained similar results. In the Schneider well advised to begin with the intuitive con-
et al. study, factor analysis revealed seven cept of social intelligence held in the mind of
dimensions of social competence that were the layperson. When Alfred Binet was given
essentially uncorrelated with measures of the task of devising an intelligence test for
quantitative and verbal/reasoning ability. French schoolchildren, he began by examin-
On the basis ofthesefinclings, Schneider eta!. ing the kinds of things that they were asked
concluded that "it is time to lay to rest any to do in school. If a new generation of psy-
residual notions that social competence is chometricians undertakes the task of assess-
a monolithic entity, or that it is just gen- ing social intelligence, they might well begin
eral intelligence applied to social situations" by looldng at how that construct is repre-
(p. 479). As with Marlowe's (1986) study, sented in the mind of real people engaged in
however, the reliance on self-report mea- the ordinary course of everyday living. After
sures of social intelligence compromises this all, social intelligence is a social construct,
conclusion, which remains to be confirmed not just an academic one.
using objective performance measures of the
various dimensions in the social domain.
Social intelligence played little role in The Development of Social Intelligence
Sternberg's early componential view of
human intelligence (e.g., Sternberg, 1977), While the psychometric research just
which was intended to focus on reasoning reviewed has focused - though not quite
and problem-solving skills as represented exclusively - on normal adults, there is
by traditional intelligence tests. However, also a long-standing interest in social intel-
social intelligence is explicitly represented ligence among developmental psychologists
1
in Sternberg s more recent triarchic view of (for a review, see Greenspan & Love, 1997) -
intelligence (e.g., Sternberg, 1988), accord- particularly among those psychologists con-
ing to which intelligence is composed of cerned with the assessment, treatment, and
analytical, creative, and practical abilities. rehabilitation of children (and adults) with
Practical intelligence is defined in terms of developmental disorders such as mental
problem-solving in everyday contexts and retardation and autism.
explicitly includes social intelligence (Stern-
berg & Wagner, 1986). According to Stern-
Mental Retardation
berg, each type of intelligence reflects the
operation of three different kinds of compo- Of course, social intelligence has always
nent processes: performance components, played a role in the assessment of men-
which solve problems in various domains; tal retardation. This psychiatric cliagnosis
executive metacomponents, which plan and requires not only evidence of subnormal
evaluate problem solving; and knowledge- intellectual functioning (i.e., IQ < 70) but
acquisition components, by which the first also demonstrated evidence of impairments
two components are learned. For Sternberg, in "c01nmunication, self-care, home liv-
these abilities, and thus their underlying ing, social and interpersonal skills, use of
SOCIAL INTELLIGENCE 569

community resources self-direction func-


1 1
calculated as social age divided by chrono-
tional academic skills, work, leisure, health, logical age). The Vineland has been recently
and safety" (American Psychiatric Associa- revised (Sparrow, Balla, & Cicchetti, 1984),
tion, 1994, p. 46). In other words, the diag- but its adequacy as a measure of social intel-
nosis of mental retardation involves deficits ligence is compromised by the fact that
in social as well as academic intelligence. linguistic functions, motor skills, occupa-
Furthermore, the wording of the diagnos- tional skills, and self-care and self-direction
tic criteria implies that social and academic are assessed as well as social relations. As
intelligence are not highly correlated - it an alternative, Taylor (1990) has proposed
requires positive evidence of both forms of a semistructured Social Intelligence Inter-
impairment meaning that the presence
1
view covering such domains as social mem-
of one cannot be inferred from the presence ory, moral development, recognition of and
of the other. response to social cues, and social judgment.
While the conventional diagnostic crite- However, Taylor concedes that such an
rion for mental retardation places primary interview, being idiographically constructed
emphasis on IQ and intellectual function- to take account of the individual's particu-
ing, Greenspan and Love (1997) argued that lar social environment, cannot easily yield
it should emphasize social and practical numerical scores by which individuals can
intelligence instead. To this end, they pro- be compared and ranked. More impor-
posed a hierarchical model of social intelli- tant than ranking individuals, &om Taylor's
gence. In this model, social intelligence con- point of view, is identifying areas of high
sists of three components: social sensitivity, and low functioning within various environ-
reflected in role-taking and social inference; ments experienced by the individual, and
social insight, including social comprehen- determining the goodness of fit between the
sion, psychological insight, and moral judg- individual and the environments in which
ment; and social communication! subsum- he or she lives.
ing referential communication and social
problem solving. Social intelligence, in turn,
Autism
is only one component of adaptive intel-
ligence (the others being conceptual intel- Another group of developmental disabili-
ligence and practical intelligence), which in ties, autistic spectrum disorders, also invokes
turn joins physical competence and socioemo- the concept of social intelligence. Kanner's
tional adaptation (temperament and char- (1943) classic description of autism portrays
acter) as the major dimensions of personal children who do not seem to be capable
competence broadly construed. Greenspan of engaging in normal social behavior or
and Love did not propose specific tests for of maintaining normal social relationships,
any of these components of social intelli- and the diagnostic criteria specified in the
gence but implied that they could be derived Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Men-
from experimental procedures used to study tal Disorders (DSM-IV; American Psychi-
social cognition in general. atric Association, 1994) emphasize deficits
All this is well and good, but while the cri- in social relations: impairments in nonverbal
terion for impaired intellectual functioning behavior, failures to develop peer relation-
is clearly operationalized by an IQ threshold, ships, lack of spontaneous sharing and other
there is as yet no standard by which impaired aspects of social reciprocity; impairments
social functioning - impaired social intelli- in communication, including an inability to
gence - can be determined. The Vineland initiate or sustain conversations or social
Social Maturity Scale (Doll, 1947) was an imitative play; and stereotyped patterns
important step in this direction: This instru- of behavior, including inflexibility in var-
ment yields aggregate scores of social age ious behavioral routines. All of these fea-
(analogous to mental age) and social quo- tures suggest that autism is characterized
tient (by analogy to the intelligence quotient, not just by social withdrawal and language
57° JOHN F. KIHLSTROM AND NANCY CANTOR

impairment but by a specific impairment moral reasoning, while obviously related to


in the abilities that underlie effective social social reasoning and to reasoning in general,
interaction. constitutes a separate domain of reasoning
Specifically, it has been proposed that that might follow its own unique princi-
autistic children and adults lack a "theory ples, developmental trajectory, and the like.
of mind" (Wellman, 1990) by which they This does not rule out a role for emo-
can attribute mental states to other peo- tional processes, but it keeps social cogni-
ple and reflect on their own mental lives tion at the center of the study of moral
(Baron-Cohen, 1995; Baron-Cohen et a!., reasoning
1993; see also Tager-Flusberg, 2007). This According to social-cognitive domain the-
hypothesis brought the problem of assess- ory (Turiel, Killen, & Helwig, 1987; Smetana,
ing social intelligence in disabled popula- 2oo6), morality is only one of several aspects
tions (including mental retardation as well of the social world about which children
as autism) directly in contact with a liter- and adults acquire knowledge, and about
ature on the development of social cogni- which they engage in reasoning, judgment,
tion in normal children. Still, Bruner and and decision making. The "conventional"
Feldman (1993) have argued that deficits domain of social knowledge has to do with
in social cognition, such as those seen in norms of social behavior that vary from one
autism, are actually secondary to deficits in context to another. The "personal" domain
general cognitive functioning. The funda- has to do with our understanding of individ-
mental question endures: Is social cognition ual persons as psychological entities, includ-
a separate faculty from nonsocial cognition? ing the attributions that we make for our
Is social intelligence anything different from own and others' behaviors, and our ability
general intelligence applied to the social to infer meaning in social situations. The
domain? "moral" domain concerns universally appli-
cable and obligatory concepts of harm, wel-
fare, fairness, and rights. Most of the focus in
Moral Reasoning
social-cognitive domain theory has been on
Another trend contributing to revived inter- the moral domain and on children's develop-
est in social intelligence was the upsurge ing the ability to understand moral concepts
of interest in moral reasoning following the and render judgments of right and wrong.
publication of Kohl berg's Piagetian theory As a developmental theory, social-cognitive
of moral reasoning (e.g., Kohlberg, 1963). domain theory assumes that social-cognitive
As Turiel (2oo6) notes, Piaget himself had abilities are heterogeneous -that children's
viewed moral reasoning within the wider (and adults') abilities to reason about the
context of the child's knowledge and judg- social world and the trajectory of their devel-
ment of social relationships. So, just as opment may well differ from one domain
Thorndike raised tbe question of how social to another. But for present purposes, social-
intelligence related to academic intelligence, cognitive domain theory offers an alterna-
the Piaget-Kohlberg trend raised the ques- tive description of the domains in which
tion of how age differences in moral rea- children and adults apply distinctively social
soning were related to social reasoning in intelligence.
general. One answer is that they do not
relate much at all, because moral judg-
ments are based on unconscious, intuitive The Fall and Rise of Social Intelligence
processes that are based more on emotion
than reason; in this view1 the reasons we Reviewing the literature published up to
give for our judgments are little more than 1983, Landy (2oo6) characterized the search
after-the-fact rationalizations (e.g., Greene, for social intelligence as "long, frustrating,
Sommerville, Nystrom, Darley, & Cohen, and fruitless." Certainly it has been long and
2001; Haidt, 2001). Another approach is that frustrating. Decade by decade, Landy traces
SOCIAL INTELLIGENCE 57 1

a record of "disappointing empirical results exceptional cases, individuals who possess


and substantial theoretical criticism" (p. 82). extraordinary levels of ability in one domain
This record did not, however, diminish the against a background of normal or even
enthusiasm of both basic and applied social impaired abilities in other domains (alter-
psychologists for the concept of social intel- natively, a person may show extraordinarily
ligence. Landy's review essentially stopped low levels of ability in one domain against a
at 1983, and for good reason - for very soon background of normal or exceptionally high
events were to give social intelligence a new levels of ability in others). In addition, Gard-
lease on life. ner postulated several other signs suggesting
different types of intelligence. Among these
are identifiable core operations, coupled with
The Theory of Multiple Intelligences
experimental tasks that permit analysis of
The milestone event here was the theory these core operations and psychometric tests
of multiple intelligences proposed by Gard- that reveal individual differences in the abil-
ner (1983, 1993, 1999; Walters & Gardner, ity to perform them. In addition to experi-
1984). Unlike Spearman and other advocates mental and psychometric evidence, Gardner
of general intelligence, Gardner proposed (1983) also assumes that qualitatively differ-
that intelligence is not a unitary cognitive ent forms of intelligence will show distinctive
ability but that there are seven (and per- developmental histories, in terms of differ-
haps more) quite different kinds of intelli- ent developmental trajectories, from infancy
gence, each hypothetically dissociable from through adolescence and adulthood to old
the others, and each hypothetically associ- age - and, perhaps, different evolutionary
ated with a different brain system. While pathways as well. Finally, Gardner argues
most of these proposed intelligences (lin- that each form of intelligence is encoded in a
guistic, logical-mathematical, spatial, musi- unique symbol system by which the ability in
cal, and bodily-kinesthetic) are "cognitive" question can be manipulated and transmit-
abilities somewhat reminiscent of Thur- ted by a culture. For social intelligence, this
stone's primary mental abilities, two are is, at least in part, the language of traits- the
explicitly personal and social in nature. thousands of terms that we use to describe
Intrapersonal intelligence is the ability to gain each other's mental states, but which do not
1
access to one S own internal emotional life, apply to nonsentient objects (e.g., Allport &
and interpersonal intelligence is the ability to Odbert, 1937).
notice and make distinctions among other Gardner did not offer any new tests of
individuals. social intelligence, nor did he provide com-
Although Gardner's (1983) multiple intel- pelling evidence that his multiple intelli-
ligences are individual-differences con- gences were really qualitatively different
structs, in which some people or some diag- from each other. But in the context of a
nostic groups are assumed to have more growing interest in cognitive neuroscience,
of these abilities than others, Gardner does and a growing inclination among psychol-
not rely on the traditional psychometric ogists to take neurobiological data as the
procedures - scale construction, factor gold standard of what is psychologically
analysis, multitrait-multimethod matrices, "real," claims for a neuropsychological dis-
external validity coefficients, and so on- for sociation between interpersonal intelligence
documenting individual differences. Rather, and other forms of intelligence (e.g., that
his preferred method is a somewhat impres- damage to the prefrontal cortex can selec-
sionistic analysis based on a convergence of tively impair intrapersonal and interper-
signs provided by eight different lines of evi- sonal intelligence while leaving other abil-
dence - chief among which are isolation by ities intact) gave new life to the notion that
brain damage, such that one form of intel- social intelligence can be distinguished from
ligence can be selectively impaired, leav- linguistic, logical-mathematical, and spatial
ing other forms relatively unimpaired; and intelligence.
572 JOHN F. KIHLSTROM AND NANCY CANTOR

This explosion of interest in emotional


Emotional Intelligence
intelligence probably has much to do with
The idea of social intelligence also got a what might be called the "affective counter-
boost from arguments in favor of individual revolution" in psychology- the feeling that,
differences in emotional intelligence, defined since the cognitive revolution of the 1950s
as "the ability to monitor one's own and oth- and 196os, psychology had gone overboard
ers' feelings, to discriminate among them, in emphasizing epistemology and needed to
and to use this information to guide one's pay more attention to feelings and desires.
thinking and action" (Salovey & Mayer, Certainly there is little reason to think that
1990, p. 189; see also Mayer, Roberts, & emotional intelligence is a clearer concept
Barsade, zoo8; Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, than social intelligence, or any easier to mea-
zooS; Salovey & Grewal, 2005). Emotional sure (Murphy, 2oo6). Whatever the reason,
intelligence subsumes four component abil- the upsurge of interest in emotional intel-
ities: the ability to perceive emotions in ligence seems to have carried social intel-
oneself and others; to use emotions in the ligence along with it, so that we can look
service of thinking and problem solving; forward to a revival of research interest in
to understand emotions and the relations this topic.
among them; and to manage emotions in
oneself and others. Emotional intelligence
Social Neuroscience
and social intelligence are not the same
thing: There is nothing particularly social All the more so, perhaps, now that Goleman
about snake phobia, and there are many (2oo6) has done for social intelligence what
aspects of social cognition where emotion he did earlier for emotional intelligence. The
plays little or no role. But, as the listing premise of Goleman's book is that rewarding
of the component abilities indicates, emo- social relationships are the key to happiness
tion is frequently evoked in a social context, and health (roughly half of the book reviews
so emotional intelligence and social intelli- research on the social psychology of health)
gence do share a sort of family resemblance. and that the key to rewarding social rela-
The idea of emotional intelligence was tionships is social intelligence. Therefore,
popularized by Daniel Goleman in a series we need new tools for the assessment of
of books (e.g., Goleman, 1995) and quickly individual differences in social intelligence,
caught on in both academic and applied psy- but - more to the point - we need edu-
chology. A search of the Psyclnfo database cational programs that will enable people
reveals that before 1990, only three items to learn how to increase their emotional
had the phrase "emotional intelligence" in intelligence and therefore to be happier and
their title or abstract, compared to 253 for healthier, as well as wiser. Whereas Gardner
"social intelligence." For the decade 1990- had postulated a single social intelli-
1999, emotional intelligence had 77 such gence, or perhaps two (intrapersonal and
items, compared to 97 for social intelligence. interpersonal intelligence), Goleman argues
But for the decade 2000-2009, emotional for a highly differentiated set of social intelli-
intelligence garnered 1,838 items (this is not a gences, grouped under two major headings.
misprint), compared to 289 for social intelli- Social awareness (corresponding to the "self-
gence. Whereas Thorndike (1920) postulated awareness~~ domain of emotional intelli-
social intelligence as the third member of a gence) includes the ability to perceive other
triad of intelligences, along with mechani- people's internal mental states, to under-
cal and abstract intelligence, it seems possi- stand their feelings and thoughts, and to
ble that, as suggested by Mayer, "Emotional comprehend the demands of complex social
intelligence could be ... the replacement situations. It includes modules dedicated to
member of the triumvirate where social primal empathy, empathic accuracy, attune-
intelligence failed" (quoted in Goleman, mont, and social cognition. Social facility, or
2006, p. 330). relationship management (corresponding to
SOCIAL INTELLIGENCE 573

the "self-management" domain), "builds on common psychometric notions such as intel-


social awareness to allow smooth, effective ligence testing, the intelligence quotient, and
interactions" (p. 84) and includes interaction the like. As originally coined by E. L.
synchrony, self-presentation, influence, and Thorndike (1920) and pursued in the studies
concern for others. reviewed so far, social intelligence referred to
Goleman provocatively characterizes the person's ability to understand and man-
previous work on social intelligence as a age other people, and to engage in adap-
"scientific backwater" (p. 330) in need of tive social interactions. In its less common
total rethinking. Taking a key from Gard- meaning, intelligence has to do with a body
ner (1999; Walters & Gardner, 1984), who of information and knowledge. This second
relied more on neuropsychology than on meaning is implicated in the titles of certain
psychometrics, as well as the doctrine of government organizations, such as the Cen-
modularity as it has developed in contem- tral Intelligence Agency in the United States,
porary cognitive and social neuroscience and its British counterparts MI-5 and MI-6.
(Fodor, 1983; Kihlstrom, in press), Gole- Both meanings are invoked by the concept
man hypothesizes that social intelligence is of social intelligence. But from Thorndike
mediated by an extensive network of neu- and Guilford to Gardner and Goleman, and
ral modules, each dedicated to a particu- beyond, social intelligence research and the-
lar aspect of social interaction. But more ory has been predicated almost exclusively
than that, Goleman asserts that "new neu- on what might be called the "ability view."
roscientific findings have the potential to On the other hand, Cantor and Kihlstrom
reinvigorate the social and behavioral sci- have offered an alternative "knowledge
ences," just as ''the basic assumptions of view" of social intelligence that refers simply
economics ... have been challenged by the to the individual's fund of knowledge about
emerging 'neuroeconomics,' which studies the social world (Cantor & Kihlstrom, 1987,
the brain during decision-making" (p. 324). 1989; Kihlstrom & Cantor, 1989, 2ooo). In
Perhaps this prediction will come true. At contrast to the ability view of social intel-
the same time, however, it is a matter of ligence, the knowledge view does not con-
historical fact that the real revolution in ceptualize social intelligence as a trait, or
economics - the advances that garnered group of traits, on which individuals can
the Nobel Prizes - flowed from observa- be compared and ranked on a dimension
tional field studies (e.g., Simon, 1947, 1955) from low to high. Rather, the knowledge
and paper-and-pencil questionnaires (Kah- view of personality begins with the assump-
neman, 2003; Tversky & Kahneman, 1974). tion that social behavior is intelligent - that
But even if cognitive and social neuroscience it is mediated by what the person knows
do not prove to be the saviors of social intel- and believes to be the case, and by cogni-
ligence (or of cognitive and social psychol- tive processes of perception, memory, rea-
ogy in general), Goleman's list of social- soning, and problem solving, rather than
intelligence abilities is as good a place as being mediated by innate reflexes, condi-
any to start developing a new generation of tioned responses, evolved genetic programs,
instruments for assessing social intelligence. and the like. Accordingly, the social intelli-
gence view construes individual differences
in social behavior - the public manifesta-
The Knowledge View of Social tions of personality - to be the product
Intelligence of individual differences in the knowledge
that individuals bring to bear on their social
Intelligence, as defined in standard dictionar- interactions. Differences in social knowl-
ies, has two rather different meanings. In edge cause differences in social behavior, but
its most familiar meaning, intelligence has it does not make sense to construct measures
to do with the individual's ability to learn of social IQ. The important variable is not
and reason. It is this meaning that underlies how much social intelligence the person has
574 JOHN F. KIHLSTROM AND NANCY CANTOR

but rather what social intelligence he or she her ability to carry out the actions required
possesses- what the individual knows about to achieve control over the events in a situ-
himself or herself, other people, the situa- ation. Although Rotter (1966) proposed an
tions in which people encounter each other, individual-differences measure of internal
and the behaviors they exchange when they versus external locus of control, it would
are in them. never occur to Bandura to propose a nomo-
thetic instrument for measuring individ-
ual differences in generalized self-efficacy
The Evolution of Cognitive Views
expectations. The important consideration
of Persotullity
is not whether an individual is relatively high
The social intelligence view of personality or low in self-perceptions of competence, or
has its origins in the social-cognitive tradi- even actual competence, but rather whether
tion of personality theory, in which con- the person believes that he or she is com-
strual and reasoning processes are central to petent to perform a particular behavior in
issues of social adaptation. Thus, Kelly (1955) some particular situation.
characterized people as naive scientists gen- The immediate predecessor to the social-
erating hypotheses about future interper- intelligence view of personality is Mischel's
sonal events based on a set of personal con- (1968, 1973) cognitive social-learning recon-
structs concerning self, others, and the world ceptualization of personality. Although
at large. These constructs were idiographic sometimes couched in behaviorist language,
with respect to both content and organiza- an emphasis on the subjective meaning of the
tion. Individuals might be ranked in terms situation marked even Mischel's 1968 theory
of the complexity of their personal con- as cognitive in nature. Since that time, Mis-
struct systems, but the important issue for chel has broadened his conceptualization of
Kelly was knowing what the individual's personality to include a wide variety of dif-
personal constructs were. Beyond complex- ferent constructs, some derived from the
ity, the idiosyncratic nature of personal con- earlier work of Kelly, Rotter, Bandura, and
struct systems precluded much nomothetic others reflecting the importation into per-
comparison. sonality theory of concepts originating in the
While Kelly's theory was somewhat icon- laboratory study of human cognitive pro-
oclastic, similar developments occurred in cesses. From Mischel's (1973) point of view,
the evolution of social learning theories of the most important product of cognitive
personality. The initial formulation of social development and social learning is the indi-
learning theory (Miller & Dollard, 1941), a vidual's repertoire of cognitive and behav-
combination of Freudian psychoanalysis and ioral construction competencies -the ability to
Hullian learning theory, held that person- engage in a wide variety of skilled, adaptive
ality was largely learned behavior and that behaviors, including both overt action and
understanding personality required under- covert mental activities. These construction
standing the social conditions under which competencies are as close as Mischel gets to
it was acquired. However, the slow rise the ability view of social (or, for that matter,
of cognitive theories of learning soon lent nonsocial) intelligence.
a cognitive flavor to social learning the- On the other hand, the importance of
ory itself (Bandura & Walters, 1963; Rotter, perception and interpretation of events in
1954). Bandura (1973) argued for the acqui- Mischel's system calls for a second set of
sition of social knowledge through precept person variables, having to do with encod-
and example rather than the direct experi- ing strategies governing selective attention
ence of rewards and punishment, and later and personal constructs - Kelly-like cate-
(1986) he distinguished between the out- gories that filter people's perceptions, mem-
come expectancies emphasized by Rotter ories, and expectations. Then, of course,
and expectancies of self-efficacy - the indi- following Rotter and Bandura, Mischel
vidual's judgment or belief concerning his or also stresses the role of stimulus-outcome,
SOCIAL INTELLIGENCE 575

behavior-outcome, and self-efficacy expect- into how people form impressions of per-
ancies. Also in line with Rotter's theory, sonality. Research on person perception, in
Mischel notes that behavior will be gov- turn, led to an inquiry into the implicit the-
erned by the subjective values associated with ories of personality that provide the cogni-
various outcomes. A final set of relevant tive basis for impression formation. Specifi-
variables consists of self-regulatory systems cally, Cronbach argued that one's implicit
and plans, self-imposed goals and conse- theory of personality consisted of his or
quences that govern behavior in the absence her knowledge of "the generalized Other"
(or in spite) of social monitors and exter- (1955, p. 179) - a mental list of the impor-
nal constraints. These variables are more tant dimensions of personality and estimates
in line with the knowledge view of social of the mean and variance of each dimen-
intelligence. sion within the population, as well as esti-
mates of the covariances among the several
dimensions. Cronbach argued that this intu-
Social Intelligence as Social Knowledge
itive knowledge might be widely shared and
Following Winograd (1975) and Anderson could be acquired as a consequence of social-
(1976), Cantor and Kihlstrom (1987) clas- ization and acculturation processes; but he
sified social intelligence into two broad also assumed that there would be individual
categories: declarative social knowledge, and cultural differences in this knowledge,
consisting of abstract concepts and specific leading to individual and group differences
memories, and procedural social knowledge, in social behavior. Studies of impression
consisting of the rules, skills, and strategies formation, implicit personality theory, and
by which the person manipulates and trans- later, causal attributions1 social categories,
forms declarative knowledge and translates scripts, and person memories provided
knowledge into action. Following Tulving the foundation for the social-intelligence
(1983), the individual's fund of declarative analysis of personality structures and
social knowledge, in turn, can be broken processes.
down further into context-free semantic Following Kelly (1955) and Mischel (1973),
social knowledge about the social world in Cantor and Kihlstrom ( 1987) accorded social
general and episodic social memory for the concepts a central status as cognitive struc-
particular events and experiences that make tures of personality. If the purpose of per-
up the person's autobiographical record. ception is action, and if every act of per-
Similarly, procedural knowledge can be sub- ception is an act of categorization (Bruner 1

classified in terms of cognitive and motoric 1957), the particular categories that orga-
social skills. These concepts, personal mem- nize people's perception of the social world
ories, interpretive rules1 and action plans assume paramount importance in a cogni-
are the cognitive structures of personality. tive analysis of personality. Some of these
Together, they constitute the expertise that concepts concern the world of other people
guides an individual's approach to solving and the places we encounter them: knowl-
the problems of social life. edge of personality types, social groups, and
The cognitive architecture of social intel- social situations. Other concepts concern
ligence will be familiar from the literature on the intrapersonal world: the kinds of peo-
social cognition (for an overviews, see Fiske ple we are, both in general and in par-
& Taylor, 2007)- a literature that, interest- ticular classes of situations, and our the-
ingly, had its beginnings in early psychome- ories of how we got that way. Some of
tric efforts to measure individual differences these conceptual relations may be univer-
in social intelligence. For example, Vernon sal, and others may be highly consensual
(1933) argued that one of the characteristics within the individual's culture; but, as Kelly
of a socially intelligent person was that he (1955) argued, some may be quite idiosyn-
or she was a good judge of personality - a cratic. Regardless of whether they are shared
proposition that naturally led to inquiries with others, the individual's conceptual
576 JOHN F. KIHLSTROM AND NANCY CANTOR

knowledge about the social world forms a or wrong: Are smart people also friendly?
major portion of his or her declarative social How do you know when a person is happy
knowledge. or sad? Is it proper to laugh at a funeral?
Another important set of declara- In this way, it is possible, at least in princi-
tive social knowledge structures represents ple, to evaluate the accuracy of the person's
the individual's autobiographical mem- social knowledge and the effectiveness of his
ory (Kihlstrom, 2009). In the context of or her social behaviors. However, as noted at
social intelligence, autobiographical mem- the outset, the social intelligence approach
ory includes a narrative of the person's own to personality abjures such rankings of peo-
actions and experiences, but it also includes ple (Cantor, zoo3). Rather than asking how
what he or she has learned through direct socially intelligent a person is, compared
and vicarious experience about the actions to some norm, the social intelligence view
and experiences of specific other people, and of personality asks what social intelligence
the events that have transpired in particular a person has, which he or she can use
situations. In addition, every piece of con- to guide his or her interpersonal behav-
scious autobiographical memory is linked to ior. In fact, the social intelligence approach
a mental representation of the self as the to personality is less interested in assessing
agent or patient of some action, or the stimu- the individual's repertoire of social intelli-
lus or experiencer of some state (Kihlstrom, gence than in seeking to understand the gen-
Beer, & Klein, zooz). eral cognitive structures and processes out
On the procedural side, a substantial por- of which individuality is constructed, how
tion of the social intelligence repertoire con- these develop over the life course of the indi-
sists of interpretive rules for making sense vidual, and how they play a role in ongoing
of social experience: for inducing social social interactions. For this reason, Cantor
categories and deducing category member- and Kihlstrom (1987, 19B9; Kihlstrom & Can-
ship, making attributions of causality, infer- tor, 1989) have not proposed any individual-
ring other people's behavioral dispositions differences measures by which the person's
and emotional states, forming judgments of social intelligence can be assessed.
likability and responsibility, resolving cog- Although the social intelligence view of
nitive dissonance, encoding and retrieving personality diverges from the psychometric
1
memories of our own and other people s approach to social intelligence on the matter
behavior, predicting future events, and test- of assessment, it agrees with some contem-
ing hypotheses about our social judgments. porary psychometric views that intelligence
Some of these procedures are algorithmic is context-specific. Thus, in Sternberg's
in nature, while others may entail heuristic (1988) triarchic theory, social intelligence
shortcuts (Nisbett & Ross, 1980). Some are is part of a larger repertoire of knowledge
enacted deliberately, while others may be by which the person attempts to solve the
evoked automatically, without much atten- practical problems encountered in the phys-
tion and cognitive effort on our part (Bargh, ical and social world. According to Cantor
1997; but see also Kihlstrom, zooS). They are and Kihlstrom (1987), social intelligence is
all part of our repertoire of procedural social specifically geared to solving the problems
knowledge. of social life, and in particular managing the
life tasks, current concerns (Klinger 1977), or
personal projects (Little, zoos) that people
Sodal Intelligence in Life Tasks
select for themselves, or that other people
It should be clear that from the knowledge impose on them from outside. Put another
view of social intelligence, the assessment way, one's social intelligence cannot be eval-
of social intelligence has quite a different uated in the abstract but only with respect
character than it does from the ability view. to the domains and contexts in which it is
From a psychometric point of view, the exhibited and the life tasks it is designed
questions posed have answers that are right to serve. And even in this case, 11 adequacy"
SOCIAL INTELLIGENCE 577

cannot be judged from the viewpoint of the autobiographical memory. They also formu-
external observer but must come from the late specific plans for action and monitor
point of view of the particular person whose their progress toward their goals, taking spe-
life tasks are in play. cial note of environmental factors that stand
Life tasks provide an integrative unit in the way and determining whether the
of analysis for studying the interaction actual outcome meets their original expecta-
between the person and the situation (Can- tions. Much of the cognitive activity in life-
tor & Fleeson, 1994; Cantor & Harlow, 1994; task problem solving involves forming causal
Cantor, Kemmelmeier, Basten, & Prentice, attributions about outcomes and in sur-
2002; Cantor & Langston, 1989; Cantor & veying autobiographical memory for hints
Malley, 1991}. They may be explicit or about how things might have gone differ-
implicit, abstract or circumscribed, univer- ently. Particularly compelling evidence of
sal or unique, enduring or stage-specific, the intelligent nature of life-task pursuit
rare or commonplace, poorly defined or well comes when, inevitably, plans go awry or
defined. Whatever their features, they give some unforeseen event frustrates progress.
meaning to the individual's life and serve to Then, the person will map out a new path
organize his or her daily activities. They are toward the goal or even choose a new goal
defined from the subjective point of view compatible with a superordinate life task
of the individual: They are the tasks that Intelligence frees us from reflex, tropism,
the person perceives himself or herself as and instinct in social life as in nonsocial
"working on and devoting energy to solv- domains.
ing during a specified period in life" (Cantor
& Kihlstrom, 1987, p. 168). First, life tasks are
articulated by the individual as self-relevant, QUO VADIS?
time-consuming, and meaningfuL They pro- It is possible that the concept of social intel-
vide a kind of organizing scheme for the ligence has outlived its usefulness and will be
individual's activities, and they are embed- supplanted by emotional intelligence. Alter-
ded in the individual's ongoing daily life. natively, it is possible that neuroscientific
And they are responsive to the demands, analyses will give new life to the study of
structure, and constraints of the social envi- social intelligence, as they promise to do
ronment in which the person lives. Life in other areas of psychology. On the other
tasks are often willingly undertaken, but hand, perhaps we should abandon the "abil-
they can also be imposed on people from ity" model of social intelligence completely,
outside, and the ways in which they are along with its psychometric emphasis on
approached may be constrained by socio- developing instruments for the measuring of
cultural factors. Unlike the stage-structured individual differences in social competencies
views of Erikson and his popularizers, how- of various sorts- tests intended to rank peo-
ever, the social-intelligence view of person- ple, and on which some people must score
ality does not propose that everyone at a high and others must score low. Instead of
particular age is engaged in the same sorts focusing on how people compare, perhaps we
of life tasks. Instead, periods of transition, should focus on what people know, and how
when the person is entering into new insti- they bring their social intelligence to bear
tutions, are precisely those times when indi- on their interactions with other people, on
vidual differences in life tasks become most the tasks life has set for them, and on the
apparent. tasks they have set for themselves. In this
The intelligent nature of life-task pur- way, we would honor the primary idea of the
suit is clearly illustrated by the strategies cognitive view of social interaction, which
deployed in its service. People often begin to is that interpersonal behavior is intelligent,
comprehend the problem at hand by simu- based on what the individual knows and
lating a set of plausible outcomes, relating believes - no matter how smart or stupid
them to previous experiences stored in it may appear to other people.
580 JOHN F. KIBLSTROM AND NANCY CANTOR

Matarazzo, J. D. (1972). Wechsler's measurement Romney, D. M., & Pyryt, M. C. (1999). Guilford's
and appraisal of adult intelligence (5th ed.). Bal- concept of social intelligence revisited. High
timore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. Ability Studies, 10, 137-199.
Mayer, J. D. 1 Roberts, R. D., & Barsade, S. Rotter, J. B. (1954). Social learning and clinical psy-
G. (2oo8). Human abilities: Emotional intel- chology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
ligence. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 507- Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for
536. internal versus external control of reinforce-
Mayer, J. D. 1 Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. ment. Psychological Monographs, 8o(1, Whole
(2oo8). Emotional intelligence: New ability No. 6o9).
or eclectic traits? American Psychologist, 63, Salovey, P., & Grewal, D. (2005). The science of
5°3-5 17· emotional intelligence. Current Directions in
Miller, N. E., & Dollard, J. H. (1941). Social learn- Psychological Science, 14(6), 281-285.
ing and imitation. New Haven, CT: Yale Uni~ Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional
versity Press. intelligence. Imagination, Cognition, and Per-
Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. sonality, g, 185-211.
New York, NY: Wiley. Schneider, R. J., Ackerman, P. L., & Kanfer, R.
Mischel, W. (1973). Toward a cognitive social (1996). To "act wisely in human relations":
learning reconceptualization of personality. Exploring the dimensions of social compe-
Psychological Review, So, 252-283. tence. Personality & Individual Differences, 21,
Moss, F. A. (1931). Preliminary report of a study of 469-482.
social intelligence and executive ability. Public Sechrest, L., &Jackson, D. N. (1961). Social intel-
Personnel Studies, 9, 2-9. ligence and the accuracy of interpersonal pre-
Moss, F. A., & Hunt, T. (1927). Are you socially dictions. Journal of Personality, 29, 167-182.
intelligent? Scientific American, 137, 108-uo. Shanley, L. A., Walker, R. E., & Foley, J. M.
Moss, F. A., Hunt, T., & Omwake, K. T. (1949). (1971). Social intelligence: A concept in search
Manual for the Social Intelligence Test, Revised of data. Psychological Reports, 29, 1123-1I32.
Form. Washington, DC: Center for Psycholog- Simon, H. A. (1947). Administrative behavior.
ical Service. New York, NY: Macmillan.
Moss, F. A., Hunt, T., Omwake, K. T., & Ron- Simon, H. A. (1955). A behavioral model of ratio-
ning, M. M. (1927). Social Intelligence Test. nal choice. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 6g,
Washington, DC: Center for Psychological 99-n8.
Service. Smetana, J. G. (2oo6). Social-cognitive domain
Moss, F. A., Hunt, T., Omwake, K. T., & Wood- theory: Consistencies and variations in chil-
ward, L. G. (1955). Manual for the George dren's moral and social judgments. In M.
Washington University Series Social Intelligence Killen & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of
Test. Washington, DC: Center for Psycholog- moral development (pp. 1I9-153). Mahwah, NJ:
ical Service. Erlbaum.
Murphy, K. R. (Ed.). (2oo6). A critique of emo- Snyder, M., & Cantor, N. (1998). Understanding
tional intelligence: What are the problems and personality and social behavior: A function-
how can they be fixed? Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. alist strategy. In D. T. Gilbert & S. T. Fiske
Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed.,
Strategies and shortcomings in social judgment. Vol. 2, pp. 635-679). Boston, MA: McGraw-
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. HilL
O'Sullivan, M., Guilford, J. P., & deMille, R. Snyder, S. D., & Michael, W. B. (1983). The
(1965). The measurement of social intelli- relationship between performance on stan-
gence. Reports from the Psychological Labora- dardized tests in mathematics and reading to
tory, University of Southern California, No. 34- two measures of social intelligence and one of
Rapaport, D., Gill, M. M., & Schafer, R. (1968). academic self-esteem of primary school chil-
Diagnostic psychological testing (Rev. ed.). New dren. Educational and Psychological Measure-
York, NY: International Universities Press. ment, 43 1 1141-1148.
Riggio, R. E., Messamer, J., & Throckmorton, B. Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V.
(1991). Social and academic intelligence: Con- (1984). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale. Cir-
ceptually distinct but overlapping constructs. cle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
Personality & Individual Differences, 12, 695- Spearman, C. (1927). The abilities of man. New
7oz. York, NY: Macmillan.
SOCIAL INTELLIGENCE 581

Sternberg, R. J. (1977). Intelligence, information Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment
processing, and analogical reasoning: The com- under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Sci-
ponential analysis of human abilities. Hillsdale, ence, 185, 1124-1131.
NJ: Erlbaum. Vernon, P. E. (1933). Some characteristics of the
Sternberg, R. J. (1988). The triarchic mind: A new good judge of personality. Journal of Social
theory of intelligence. New York, NY: Viking. Psychology, 4, 42-57.
Sternberg, R. J. Conway, B. E., Ketron, J. L., Walker, R. E., & Foley, J. M. (1973). Social intel-
& Bernstein, M. (1981). People's conceptions ligence: Its history and measurement. Psycho-
of intelligence. Journal of Personality & Social logical Reports, 33, 839-864.
Psychology, 41, 37-55. Walters, J. M., & Gardner, H. (1986). The the-
Sternberg, R. J., & Wagner, R. (Eds.). (1986). ory of multiple intelligences: Some issues and
Practical intelligence: Nature and origins of com- answers. In R. J. Sternberg & R. Wagner
petence in the everyday world. Cambridge, UK: (Eds.), Practical intelligence: Origins of compe-
Cambridge University Press. tence in the everyday world (pp. 163-182). Cam-
Tager-Flusberg, H. (2oo7). Evaluating the theory- bridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
of-mind theory of autism. Current Directions Wechsler, D. (1939). The measurement and
in Psychological Science, 16, 311-315· appraisal of adult intelligence. Baltimore, MD:
Taylor, E. H. (1990). The assessment of social Williams & Wilkins.
intelligence. Psychotherapy, 27, 445-457· Wechsler, D. (1958). The measurement and
Thorndike, E. L. (1920). Intelligence and its use. appraisal of adult intelligence (4th ed.). Balti-
Harper's Magazine, 140, 227-235· more: Williams & Wilkins.
Thorndike, R. L., & Stein, S. (1937). An eval- Weis, S., & Suss, H.-M. (2007). Reviving the
uation of the attempts to measure social search for social intelligence - A multitrait-
intelligence. Psychological Bulletin, 34, 275- multimethod study of its structure and con-
285. struct validity. Personality and Individual Dif
Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. ferences, 42(1), 3-14-
New York, NY: Oxford UniVersity Press. Wellman, H. M. (1990). Thechild'stheoryofmind.
Turiel, E. (2oo6). The development of moral- Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
ity. In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R. M. Winograd, T. (1975). Frame representations and
Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: the procedural-declarative controversy. In D.
Social emotional, and personality development Bobrow & A. Collins (Eds.), Representation
(6th ed., Vol. 3, pp. 789-857). Hoboken, NJ: and understanding: Studies in cognitive science
Wiley. (pp. 185-21o). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Turiel, E., Killen, M., & Helwig, C. (1987). Moral- Wong, C.-M. T., Day, J. D., Maxwell, S.
ity: Its structure, functions, and vagaries. In J. E., & Meara, N. M. (1995). A multitrait-
Kagan & M. Lamb (Eds.), The emergence of multimethod study of academic and social
morality in young children. Chicago, IL: Uni- intelligence in college students. Journal of Edu-
versity of Chicago Press. cational Psychology, 87, 117-133·
The Cambridge Handbook
of Intelligence

Edited by
ROBERT J. STERNBERG
Oklahoma State University

SCOTT BARRY KAUFMAN


New York University

CAMBRIDGE
UNIVERSITY PRESS
CAMBRIDGE UNIVERSITY PRESS
Cambridge, New York, Melbourne, Madrid, Cape Town,
Singapore, Sao Paulo, Delhi, Tokyo, Mexico City
Cambridge University Press
32 Avenue of the Americas, New York, NY 10013~2473, USA
www.cambridge.org
Information on this title: www.cambridge.org/9780521739115

©Cambridge University Press 2011

This publication is in copyright. Subject to statutory exception


and to the provisions of relevant collective licensing agreement'i,
no reproduction of any part may take place without the written
permission of Cambridge University Press.

First published 2011

Printed in the United States of America

A catalog record for this publication is available from the British Library.

Library of Congress Cataloging in Publication data

The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence I [edited by] Robert]. Sternberg, Scott Bany Kaufman.
p. em.- (Cambridge Handbooks in Psychology)
Includes bibliographical references and index.
ISBN 978~o-521~518o6~z -ISBN 978-0-521-739ll-5 (pbk.)
1. Intellect. 2. Human information processing.
L Sternberg, Robert J. (Robert Jeffrey), 1949- II. Kaufman, Scott Barry, 1979- III. Title. IV. Series.
BF431.Cz6837 2011
153·9-dC22 2010049730

ISBN 978-o-521-51806-2 Hardback


ISBN 978-o-szJ-73911-5 Paperback

Cambridge University Press has no responsibility for the persistence or accuracy of URLS for external or third-party
Internet Web sites referred to in this publication and does not guarantee that any content on such Web sites is, or
will remain, accurate or appropriate.

You might also like