You are on page 1of 27

The Shape of the Sky: The INTRODUCTION

ARTICLE Art of Using Egocentric


Stereopsis in the Treatment
The digital age promises a quantum leap in
the nonsurgical treatment of strabismus.
New computerized vision therapy
of Strabismus technologies, 3D movies, and the soon-to-
explode field of virtual reality all promise
David L. Cook, OD, FCOVD, FAAO
advances in the presentation of stereoscopic
Private Practice, Marietta, Georgia
dichoptic displays technically impossible for
most of the history of strabismus therapy.
ABSTRACT
In a stereoscopic dichoptic display, we
A protocol for using stereopsis in the present each eye with a target that is unseen
treatment of strabismus is described. by the other eye. The two targets are slightly
Suitable for patients with either normal different just as the views from two eyes in
retinal correspondence (NRC) or the natural environment are slightly different.
anomalous binocular correspondence Sensory fusion allows the binocular illusion
(ABC), the approach emphasizes the that dichoptic targets in physically different
interactions between peripheral/egocentric positions are unified, creating the perception
and central/relative stereopsis in such dichoptic of depth and often encouraging the alignment
instrumentation as vectograms, digital of deviated eyes.
instrumentation, and 3D movies. The paper Clinicians have long used such displays in the
then discusses the transfer of appreciation treatment of strabismus. By the beginning of the
of stereopsis from such artificial dichoptic twentieth century, Javal1 in France and Worth2 in
stereoscopic presentations to consciousness England were already using mirror stereoscopes
of egocentric and relative stereopsis in natural and major amblyoscopes (modified mirror
seeing, emphasizing the art, rather than the stereoscopes) to produce dichoptic displays.
science, of treatment. These instruments, however, created a number
of challenges:
Correspondence regarding this article should be
1. 
A limited field of view excluded the
emailed to David L. Cook, OD, FCOVD, FAAO, at peripheral fusion that is largely responsible
cook2020@aol.com. All state­ ments are the author’s for maintaining alignment. As Brock,3 a
personal opinion and may not reflect the opinions of
the College of Optometrists in Vision Development, half a century later would write, “aversion
Vision Development & Rehabili­ tation or any institu­ to fusion seems to be limited mainly to
tion or organization to which the authors may be the macular and paramacular region. For
affiliated. Permission to use reprints of this article must
be obtained from the editor. Copyright 2016 College of this reason fusion training should begin
Optometrists in Vision Development. VDR is indexed in with the periphery and only gradually
the Directory of Open Access Journals. Online access is
available at www.covd.org. include the center.” The dichoptic targets
of stereoscopes and major amblyoscopes
Cook DL. The shape of the sky: the art of using ego­ were just too central.
centric stereopsis in the treatment of strabismus.
Vision Dev & Rehab 2016;2(4):211-37. 2. 
In approaching the sensory system, the
motor system was often ignored, the eyes
being allowed to remain in their strabismic
Keywords: 3D movie vision therapy, posture while the illusion of sensory
anomalous binocular correspondence, fusion occurred, combining targets that
dichoptic therapy, egocentric stereopsis, physically had nothing to do with one
optometric vision therapy, sphere of another.
attention, stereopsis, strabismus

211
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
3. For those adapted so that eye position that will revolutionize nonsurgical treatment
allowed an accurate perception of the of strabismus. This paper—based on my8
world free from the binocular illusions of forty years of experience in treating strabismic
diplopia and confusion, the true perception patients, years including the transition into the
of the instrument’s physically separated digital age—provides one strategy for how that
apertures was often more perceptually revolution could occur.
compelling than the illusions of blending
and depth created by dichoptic targets. Depth Perception
As Brock4 wrote, “the squinter … is apt to While allowing increased acuity, a spare eye,
resent the fact that he no longer can see and an expanded field of view, the principal
things as they really are and as he knows advantage of binocularity is enhanced depth
them to be.” Similarly, Flax5 commented perception, particularly the appreciation of
that strabismics “have learned to utilize stereopsis. There is little doubt on the importance
those aspects of the incoming eye signal of stereopsis. Levi, Knill, and Bavelier9 have
that are consistent with their logic and summarized the literature on how “stereopsis
with their language constructs.” matters” in the areas of driving, visually-guided
4. 
Binocular illusions learned in an hand movements, motor control, walking, visual
instrument that divorced the dichoptic feedback in control of movements, everyday
images from interactions with hands and activities and adaptations to changes in terrain.
body often did not transfer to natural If we divide space into three coordinates or
seeing. Aware of such challenges, Brock,6 dimensions, x-axis or width, y-axis or height, and
by the middle of the twentieth century, z-axis or depth, then depth perception is the
had developed the use of free-space ability to determine the position of objects on
anaglyphic stereoscopic displays. His the z- axis in free space … or to imagine a z-axis
stereo motivator7, as it came to be called, in photographic images or realistic art. Depth
projected stereoscopic images, which perception can include accurate objective
allowed the direct comparison of dichoptic behavior in quantifying of space. It can also
display illusions to real objects in a room include the subjective qualitative experience
and allowed fusion confirmed by balance itself. In an attempt to distinguish the quantitative
and movement. The lessons that Brock and qualitative aspects of depth perception,
taught can now be advanced by digital Vishwanath has provided the following attempt
technologies. Just as the increased safety at clarification:
of anesthesia and fellowship programs I define “visual perception of depth or

for the training of surgeons eclipsed 3-D structure” to generically refer to the
nonsurgical strabismus treatment in the perception of the third dimension, in
middle of the twentieth century, so will the pictures or real scenes, with one eye or
advent of digital technologies restore the two, regardless of the cues inducing it or
contributions of nonsurgical strabismus the qualitative impression associated with
treatment in the twenty-first century. it. This includes the perception of both
depth order and quantitative depth (e.g.,
Today’s digital instrumentation and blending the perception of 3-D shapes and locations
of monocular and binocular cues in 3D movies of objects in a 3-D pictorial space).
provide a welcome expansion of Brock’s “Stereoscopic vision” I take to literally
innovations. It is not the digital dichoptic tools mean “vision through a stereoscope” or,
by themselves, however, but the knowledge less literally, to refer to “binocular depth
of their use as a springboard to natural seeing perception”: the visual perception of depth
212
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
on the basis of binocular disparities. I reserve When viewed through a monocular aperture, for
the term “stereopsis” for the following: instance, photographs with good perspective
The characteristically vivid impression of may become so three-dimensionally vivid that
tangible solid form, immersive negative they approach the Greek stereos, meaning
space and realness … that obtains under “solid,” from which our term stereopsis is
certain viewing and stimulus conditions.10 derived. To capture this vividness, Vishwanath14
uses the term “monocular stereopsis” for the
Like Vishwanath we will use “depth perception. In this paper, however, to avoid
perception” in his first sense to include all z-axis confusion we will reserve the term stereopsis
appreciation, including that in pictures. We will, for binocular perception.
however, more traditionally, use “stereopsis” While the Verhoeff Stereopter and Howard
to include both correct binocular localization Dolman Test both test depth perception
of either natural or “dichoptic images,” naturally, the Titmus Stereo Fly and Wirt tests
emphasizing the subjective qualitative “vivid are both examples of artificial stereopsis. The
impression of tangible solid form and immersive Fly and Wirt test are both physically flat. Any
negative space and realness” or what Barry has depth is, again, an illusion occurring only in the
called “a palpable volume of empty space”11 If mind, not the apparatus, illusion being what
the truth of treatment emerges from the sum of often occurs when we transfer how we “do”
the perspectives, then additional perspectives vision in a familiar environment to how we “do”
on the division of depth perception may provide vision in a novel or artificial environment.
insights for understanding our treatment All dichoptic presentations, 3D movies
approach. included, are examples of stereoscopic illu­
sions, not natural stereopsis. The perception of
Monocular Versus Binocular Cues images springing off the movie screen is not to
Depth perception is most commonly divided be found in the physical movie theater but in
into depth from monocular cues versusdepth the minds of the viewers wearing the polarized
from binocular cues. After Adler,12 monocular glasses. As will be described below under
cues include interposition of one object in “Natural Stereopsis,” care must taken to ensure
front of another, customary size of objects, that dichoptic illusions, once appreciated
color and haziness of objects, convergence by those with strabismus, are transferred to
of lines to the vanishing point, shadows, and appreciation of stereopsis in natural seeing.
monocular parallax. To this list, we will add
“looming,” which, like motion parallax, also Central/Relative Versus Peripheral/
requires comparison across time, not just Egocentric Depth Perception
across space.13 Binocular cues, again according Burian15 provides a third highly profitable way
to Adler, include convergence, binocular to divide localization of depth: relative versus
accommodation, and stereopsis. egocentric. Suppose, for instance, a woman
raises two fingers at arm’s length before her
Artificial Versus Natural Depth Perception face and compares the fingers’ z-axis position
Another way to divide binocular depth in space. If attention is constricted to the two
perception is between artificial and natural targets, this is an example of relative depth
perception of depth. Photographs, realistic perception (RDP). Both the Verhoeff Stereopter
paintings, and 2D movies are all examples of and the Wirt test, for example, quantify RDP as
artificial depth. The mind creates the illusion the viewer tries to determine the relative position
of depth where none exists, transferring flat of targets. If however, in the two-finger example,
images into three dimensional perceptions. the viewer instead considers the distance of the
213
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
two fingers from her face and is aware of the If a patient’s interpupilary distance (PD) is
space in between her nose and both targets, six centimeters and the base out disparity of
egocentric depth perception (EDP) is being the target is six centimeters, the eyes will be
appreciated. During this heightened awareness converged to (and the fused image of the
of EDP—in my experience as a viewer and target should be localized) half way between
clinician—RDP often improves. As Vishwanath16 the patient and the physical location of the
has hypothesized, “stereopsis is a qualitative disparated target. If the patient’s PD is six
visual experience related to the perception centimeters and the target disparity is three
of egocentric spatial scale. Specifically, the centimeters the target should be perceived to
primary phenomenal characteristic of stereopsis float one fourth of the distance out between
(the impression of “real” separation in depth) target and patient. If, for the same patient,
is proposed to be linked to the precision with the target disparity is twice the PD the patient
which egocentrically scaled depth (absolute should perceive the target to float outward
depth) is derived.” Whether or not Vishwanath’s three fourths of the way from the target’s actual
hypothesis is true, the key to improving position and the patient’s face.
stereopsis in the therapy room and in real space For the purposes of therapy, I imagine that
is very much linked to attention to EDP. egocentric localization and SILO are closely
In the therapy room, the closest clinicians related to the area of space the viewer selects
come to quantifying EDP is, perhaps, SILO for viewing. If, for instance in the above finger
(Smaller In and Larger Out), the perception example, the viewer selects only the two
in which dichoptic targets appear smaller fingers to view, there will be awareness of only
and closer with base out disparity causing central or relative stereopsis. If, however, the
convergence and larger and farther away with viewer selects all the space between his eyes
base in disparity causing divergence. While and the two fingers then both central RDP and
only stereoscopic or third degree dichoptic peripheral EDP will be perceived.
targets offer RDP, simultaneous perception “Opening appreciation of space” to allow
targets (first degree fusion), flat fusion targets for peripheral EDP/egocentric awareness of
(second degree targets) and stereoscopic (third depth can be accomplished in some cases by
degree fusion) targets all may be perceived to coaching to patient to be aware of the distance
float in space using EDP. When perception of (volume of air or space, not an introverted
spatial context is included, all targets may take estimate of feet or inches) between her nose
advantage of EDP! and the target. Other viewers are more likely
Measuring the accuracy of EDP is estimated to open up appreciation of space if coached
by comparing the position of the two eyes and to “try to see everything in the room at once,”
the actual location of each eye’s respective or if coached to be “simultaneously aware of
target (Figure 1). the right and left walls at the same time” or
“be aware of the length of the floor and ceiling
at the same time.” The words “be peripheral”
can be used, but only if the patient understands
that “being peripheral” includes awareness of
space, not just on the x-axis, but on all three
axes: x, y, and z.
The following proposed treatment approach
will provide examples of this principle.

214
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
TREATMENT information will now be confirmed by both
In our approach to using stereoscopic vision bifoveal superimposition and hand information.
in the treatment of strabismus, two general All three will now support perceptual fusion of
rules apply: 1) work with the eyes aligned the target.
or approximately aligned and 2) encourage Precise stereoscopic vision depends on
peripheral awareness. precise ocular alignment and gross peripheral
stereoscopic vision depends on at least
Alignment approximate alignment. In the words of Brock in
“Anomalous binocular correspondence his lecture notes, “It is the nature of the posture
(ABC) is diagnosed when the two eyes’ which determines the nature of the responses
foveal images are perceived in different visual … while the eyes are in strabismic posture, the
directions.”17 Unless the habitual angle of individual thinks strabismically, but the minute his
deviation is disturbed, the patient typically eyes are in a normal posture he ceases to think
perceives the objects related to foveal images strabismically and thinks the way we do.”20 Thus
in their correct position in space, as if the patient whether or not a patient has ABC, stereoscopic
knew where the eyes were pointing.18 The vision is likely to be appreciated when the eyes
perception is in agreement with what the body, are voluntarily aligned or at least approximately
including the hands, would confirm to be true. aligned to allow a peripheral fusion lock. Normal
If the right eye is looking straight ahead at a retinal correspondence (NRC) versus ABC need
monitor, and the left eye is turned toward a copy be considered when the eyes are deviated but
of War and Peace on the bookshelf to the left not when the eyes are aligned and stereoscopic
of the monitor, the exotropic patient with ABC vision is present. Even when the eyes are only
will use the right eye to perceive the monitor approximately aligned so that a slip of eight
straight ahead, which could be confirmed by or less prism diopters are observed on a
touch. The left eye will simultaneously see unilateral cover test, the clinician concentrates
War and Peace in its veridical (true) location not on reducing the degree of the slip but on
to the left, again confirmable by touch. When increasing the accuracy of localization, which
both monitor and book are fortuitously aligned in turn reduces the degree of the slip. Greater
with a visual axis, both monitor and book may accuracy of localization simply requires the eyes
simultaneously be perceived as clear, although to be more accurately aligned.
not necessarily equal. (As one patient described For many patients with intermittent exotropia,
his simultaneous perception, referring to dichoptic stereoscopic targets or hand-eye
the diminished perceptual state of his non- targets such as placing a pointer in a drinking
preferred, non-fixating eye, “My ‘big eye’ straw held parallel to the face, encourage
sees one thing, while my ‘little eye’ sees the alignment. Such stereoscopic demands may
other.” Such qualitative, perceptual differences prompt alignment at four or five inches in front
between the two eyes may help the strabismic of the nose even though stereoscopic vision and
patient keep track of the real locations of their binocular alignment are absent at traditional
perceived images.) testing distances. Similarly with esotropia, the
When ABC exists, the key to eliciting eyes will often align at some point in space closer
stereoscopic vision is to get the patient to the face. To determine this distance, Brock21
voluntarily pointing the two foveas in the same described a test in which a penlight is slowly
place at the same time so that sensory and motor moved towards a patient’s nose. Brock found
information will co-vary19 and come to coincide. three responses: 1) the patient actively takes
Or put another way, with both eyes voluntarily up and maintains binocular posture over some
pointed at the same target, oculomotor distance, exhibiting a “centration range.” 2) The
215
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
patient exhibits a “centration point” allowing stereoscopic vision, especially in esotropes
the penlight to rest at the intersection of his two prone to ABC, is peripheral awareness. Patients
eyes but making no effort to maintain binocular select an area to examine. The larger the area
posture when the penlight is moved on the simultaneously examined rather than examined
z-axis. 3) The patient exhibits neither a centration by sequential processing and saccadic eye
point nor range but actively avoids bi-fixating movements, the more “peripheral” the vision. It
the penlight. Rather as the penlight approaches, is convenient to think of the area simultaneously
the patient continues to over converge until, at examined as a “sphere of attention.” In actual fact,
some point, the patient actively diverges, again peripheral awareness includes simultaneously
to avoid bi-fixation. expanded awareness on all three axis: X, Y, and
When binocular alignment is more robust, Z. For ease of presentation, Figure 2 illustrates
stereopsis follows. Regarding the three groups a circle rather than a sphere of attention. The
Brock identified (above), peripheral stereoscopic circle includes the screen, the floating target,
vision was demonstrated within the centration the walls, and the distance between the patient
range among 90 percent of the first group, 53 and the screen. Simultaneously imagining a floor
percent of the second, and 13 percent of the and ceiling would create a sphere. The sphere
third—who, Brock suspected, align their eyes for may be enlarged by expanding attention on the
the stereo target, but not for the penlight. Brock X, Y, and or Z axis.
concluded, “3D appreciation is solely a function
of bi-fixation: When the eyes are not centered
on the object of regard, the latter cannot be
perceived stereoscopically. When they are
centered, 3D is rarely absent …”
With this in mind, whatever stereoscopic
testing and cover testing reveals at normal
testing distances during the exam is not
important. Those exotropes who align their
eyes at any distance for stereoscopic targets
and those esotropes, with or without ABC, with
gross stereoscopic vision somewhere within a
centration range are typically ideal candidates
for the for the therapy approach outlined below. Some patients do better attending to a
In my experience it is not uncommon to find single axis; some, by attending to two or three.
patients who could not perceive the stereo fly Peripheral awareness, or an expanded sphere
initially, but could perceive it after therapy. of attention, may be encouraged in any number
of ways:
Peripheral Awareness • Begin with large targets without central
As cited above, Brock suggested “aversion detail. Coach the patient to be aware of
to fusion seems to be limited mainly to the the outside of the target.
macular and paramacular region. For this reason • Hold the above-described large target
fusion training should begin with the periphery as close to the patient as the centration
and only gradually include the center.” Similarly, range permits and, again, coach the
both Ludlam22 and Flax23 have reported the use patient to be aware of the outside of
of binocular defocus to promote fusion. While the target. The closer a given target is,
defocus may prove an initial aid in allowing bi- the more peripheral it becomes. As the
fixation, another key to gaining alignment and distance from the target increases the
216
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
target becomes more central and requires encouraging the patient to look towards the
more accurate alignment. center of the screen rather than alternating
• 
Shake the target. The fovea responds attention. It may be easier for the patient to
best to stationary, high contrast, targets. see simultaneously the outer boundary of the
The peripheral retina responds to moving, background upon which the target appears
low contrast targets. Shaking targets, or is being projected: the outer boundary of
therefore, discourage foveal attention the vectogram holder, video monitor, or silver
to detail and encourage peripheral screen allowing the use of polarized targets.
awareness and appreciation of peripheral Divided awareness sometimes fails because,
stereoscopic targets (via peripheral retinal rather than open the sphere of attention, the
stimulation). patient merely alternates attention or becomes
• 
Blur the target. Again, as reported by simultaneously aware of two small areas at once
Flax and Ludlam, blur removes details without opening perception to include z-axis
that encourage central attention and awareness.
makes it easier for the patient to maintain
peripheral awareness. It is the patient’s Gestalt Awareness
attention to detail, not the details A second way to create peripheral aware­
themselves, however, which trigger ABC ness is to train the patient to see the screen as
and a loss of stereopsis. Blur is a tool a gestalt. One way to accomplish this gestalt
to help work around obsessive attention awareness is to ask the patient to look at the
to detail. As the ability to maintain unprinted back of a business card held several
peripheral stereopsis develops, blur may feet away. Most patients will perceive the
be slowly reduced. Indeed, when many business card as a business card instead of
esotropes first tap into the peripheral going central to see the card as a collection
system they report that central details of individual edges and corners. If one of the
are blurred. As peripheral stereopsis and patient’s eyes are covered and the card is slowly
a peripheral lock become more second moved toward his face, at some point you may
nature, patients generally learn to temper see the patient’s eye begin to move as he
their peripheral awareness allowing more begins to examine the parts of the card rather
and more central stereopsis and clarity than see the entire card at the same time. The
simultaneously. second this occurs, make the patient aware of
how it is different to see the whole card at once
Three other ways are generally useful to without moving his eyes. Move the card back
get the patient into the egocentric awareness out and have the patient, again, see the card
system: divided awareness, gestalt awareness, as a card. When the patient can see the card
and egocentric awareness. as a card, have the patient see the silver screen
as a silver screen, that is, see the entire screen
Divided Awareness simultaneously. When shifting into this mode of
Coach the patient to be simultaneously seeing, patients often report perceptual blurring
aware of the walls to the right and left of the of the 3D target and silver screen. Stereopsis
target or to be simultaneously aware of the is nevertheless elicited. As mentioned above,
floor and ceiling, thus “dividing” awareness. as more control of shifting from the central
If the patient is unable to simultaneously to peripheral mode is developed, it generally
select the two side walls to view, and instead becomes possible to achieve a balance between
looks first at one wall and then at the other, the two, allowing clear seeing of more central
then the peripheral vision may be elicited by targets in the presence of peripheral stereopsis.
217
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
Egocentric Awareness pencil tip to go outside the maze, the therapist
A third method to get the patient would alternately coach the child to “Trace
peripherally aware is to coach him to be inside the maze!” and “Trace outside the
simultaneously aware of all the space between maze!” Soon the child would take responsibility
his or her nose and the screen or target. The for tracing outside the maze and control the
key to such egocentric seeing is to perceive pencil tip to stay inside the maze. If a child
the space, not introvert to analytically estimate similarly had trouble with pursuit movements,
the distance of the space in feet or meters. taking his eyes off the therapist’s moving bead,
Other ways to encourage this awareness of the therapist would alternately coach the child
z-axis space is to place a dowel stick next to the to “Look away from the bead!” and “Look
target and have the patient, without moving towards the bead!” until the child knew when
his eyes to the side, be simultaneously aware he was looking at the bead.
of the entire length of dowel (a sort of “z-axis In the case of the quoit with the dot on the
gestalt awareness) and the target’s position wall, once the patient has stereopsis at one
compared to the dowel. If the target is close, distance, the therapist can coach the patient
a finger may be used. If the target is six feet to shift back and forth between central and
away, a six-foot dowel may be used. peripheral attention to teach the patient how
In the case of using the dowel stick, the to shift voluntarily into the stereoscopic mode.
following step by step sequence might be as The therapist can use the following instruction
follows: set stressing central versus egocentric, gestalt,
n Dowel close to the target or looping the or divided attention:
target, thus not requiring the patient to • Be aware of the distance between your
select too large of an area for viewing, nose and the dot (or be aware the silver
but not encouraging central awareness of screen as if it were a business card or be
the target aware of the two walls simultaneously—
n Dowel further to the side of the target, whatever instruction elicits stereopsis).
thus requiring the patient to open up Get the quoit to float out as far from the
peripheral vision a little further dot as you can.
n Dowel at the edge of the screen, thus • Now look at the very edge of the dot
encouraging the patient to simultaneously (preferable with a very slight indentation
see the whole screen created in it). Look at the tiny indentation
n Encouraging patient to see the float of in the dot. Make your vision small and
the target in relationship to the length of concentrate on the indentation. Can you
the floor and ceiling, or the length of the make the quoit go flat?
walls of the room. • Now be aware of the space between your
nose and the dot (etc.). Can you get the
For some, the use of the dowel fails because quoit to float outward again?
it occludes part of the projected vectogram
(e.g., quoit), thus creating a disparity between Repeat this sequence until the patient
monocular and binocular cues. can take responsibility for going central and
flattening the quoit and taking control to
Responsibility and Control go peripheral and get the quoit to float. As
The easiest way to make a patient take Birnbaum24 has described, patients often
responsibility for an error is to teach the patient attribute changes in perception to the
to make the error on purpose. If, for instance, apparatus rather than to themselves. Thus,
a child, while tracing a maze, kept allowing his to begin with, patients may perceive that the
218
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
quoit is floating or flat as if they have nothing action, vision, and language are often learned in
to do with the perception. When the central- unison—and, therefore, genuinely trusted.
peripheral alternation between responsibility In the case of unexperienced actions
and control procedure is complete, the patient (actions never directed by the visual process),
should take responsibility for making the quoit language again reduces to abstraction, analogy,
go flat and know that he is in control of the quoit or metaphor. The words are not understood
floating. Also, the patient’s learning to create a in the fullest perceptual sense any more than
lack of stereopsis on purpose generally allows a non-surgeon understands surgery in the
him or her to create stereopsis on purpose. The fullest perceptual sense. If, for instance, a seer
patient needs to understand that the float of the depends on sequential processing rather than
quoit is in the patient, not the quoit. simultaneously selecting larger areas of space
to view, then he may have no perceptual
Id
understanding of the difference between the
g en
nt
e rin tif
i ca
phrases “seeing a room with things in it” and
Ce t ion
“seeing individual things in a room.” He may
Vision be able to repeat or comment on the words,
ry
but the perceptual distinction between the two
An to
ti-
Gr - A udi phrases may elude him.
a vit ch
y
Sp ee Brock was aware of the restriction of language
for those unaccustomed to experiencing
Figure 3—Skeffington’s Four Circles stereopsis:
When stereoscopic fusion prevails, the

Language patient is conversant in our language and
Optometrists have noted language’s role in when stereoscopic perception is absent he
vision at least since the time that optometric speaks a foreign tongue which most of us
pioneer and educator A.M. Skeffington, as legend simply do not understand. For this selfsame
has it, captured the visual process by scribbling a reason we misinterpret his statements by
four-circle Venn diagram on a napkin. The circles applying them to our own mode of seeing
(see Figure 3) included “centering” (selecting instead of to his.25
an area of space for attention and meaning),
“identification” (the process by which one knows Consider an example or this breakdown
what things are), “anti-gravity” (sense of body of language. Suppose the patient is viewing
position/movement), and “speech/auditory” the quoit and dot target on the wall and the
(language/analysis of sound). Metaphorically therapist asks, “Which is closer, the dot or
speaking, vision emerges to become greater the quoit?” The patient replies, “The quoit.”
than the sum of the four circles for the purpose Asked, “If you were to walk down to the wall,
of deriving meaning and directing action. which would you come to first, the quoit or the
In abstract language, such as the phrase dot?” the patient again replies, “The quoit.”
“counting all the stars in the universe” real Similarly, when asked, “Where is the dot?”
action and vision are abandoned for analogy or the patient will correctly answer, “On the
metaphor. The metaphor may inspire action or wall.” But when asked, “Where is the quoit?”
mental images, but you can neither count nor the patient may answer, “On the wall.” This
accurately imagine all the stars in the universe. last response at first seems to contradict
In concrete, perceptual language, such as the the previous three, but actually the patient
phrase “picking up the marble,” language is is correct. In the illusion, the quoit may be
merged with vision to direct action; in fact, such perceived closer than the dot, but in actual
219
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
fact, both the quoit and dot are physically on note what your eyes feel like as you see that
the wall. If the patient were to walk down and illusion even though it conflicts with what your
touch them, his or her hand would confirm hands would normally tell you is true.” Even
the correctness of the response. if the patient has not voiced his concerns, he
One way to explain this behavior is that to often feels confused with the whole endeavor.
the strabismic patient, the question “Where?” The therapist’s communicating about the
means, “Where would my hands and body (not difference between eye information and hand
my eyes) tell me the target is?” This would information may be useful. At the very least,
be only natural if the patient, to correctly such communication signals to the patient that
understand the world, had come to ignore the therapist understands the patient’s dilemma
perceptions that conflict with his hands. Since between the illusions of binocular input and
dichoptic illusions conflict with hand-body input, habitual body action in habitual space. With
the patient ignores the stereopsis illusion, just such language-confused patients the therapist
as he ignores such illusions as luster, rivalry, or should continue with the “seem to be” rather
superimposition, any one of which could result than the “is” instruction sets.
in constant confusion and diplopia. Instead the Having considered some general sugges­
patient perceives what past actions have proved tions for dealing with expanding the size of
to be true. Visual illusions lie; the hands do the sphere of attention, taking responsibility
not. The response is not unlike asking a person and control, and dealing with language, we
to look at a photograph of a dog standing in consider the use of some actual dichoptic
front of a fire engine. To the question, “Which targets in developing stereopsis in strabismus.
is closer, the dog or the engine?” the viewer
would answer, “The dog.” But when asked to Vectographic Stereoscopic Vision
“touch the dog” and “touch the engine,” the Traditionally, at least since the time of
viewer would correctly touch both on the same Brock, red-green anaglyphs have been used
plane. The adapted strabismic is likely no more to create dichoptic stereoscopic vision in the
tricked by the quoit binocular-cues illusion treatment of strabismus. Since most patients,
than the photograph viewer is deceived by the in the author’s experience, have an easier
photograph monocular-cues illusion. time perceiving vectograms than anaglyphs,
To work around the language problem, this paper will stress vectographic and,
Brock suggested the following approach: with the exception of red-blue computer-
I, therefore try to explain to my squint
 generated randot stereograms, largely ignore
patients how their mode of sight differs from anaglyphic procedures, not that the training
ours and what strange visual falsifications tools suggested below could not be done
they have to expect before they may gain anaglyphically.
normal binocular vision.26
Quoit with Dot at Near
In the case of the quoit and dot the A great tool for eliciting stereoscopic
therapist might explain, “You’re right. Both the vision in esotropia is a quoit vectogram with a
quoit and dot are actually on the wall, but the nickel-sized dot on the clear plastic handheld
overhead projector and the glasses can make vectogram holder. While the quoits are
things seem different than they really are. dichoptic, one being seen by each eye, the dot
When I’m asking you where the quoit is, I’m is real. Both eyes are seeing the same dot as a
not asking where the quoit really is but where it point of reference.
seems to be. The quoit’s position is an illusion. When beginning, especially with a younger
It is not real. We are developing your ability to patient who is unfamiliar with the apparatus,
220
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
• Ask the patient to see the entire plastic
holder simultaneously as if it were a
business card (gestalt attention).
• Ask the patient to be aware of how much
space there is between the dot and his
Figure 4: Demonstration of nose (egocentric attention).
Quoit Behind Dot

While any of these approaches may be


useful in eliciting EDP (egocentric depth
perception), the patient’s responses often give
clues whether he is in the relative/central or
egocentric/peripheral mode. For instance,
the therapist holds the clear-plastic holder with when the quoit is at 3 (base out disparity), the
dot in one hand within the centration range patient may report that the quoit is in front, but
(see Figure 4) and, with the other hand, places at C (base in disparity) report, “Now the dot is in
both superimposed right and left quoit slides front.” This response suggests that the patient
physically first in front of the holder and then is attending to RDP (relative depth perception)
behind it, asking the patient if the quoit is in rather than EDP. The perceived z-axis position
front or behind the dot. When the patient has of the dot should not be moving because the
demonstrated an understanding of “in front true position of the dot is not changing. The
of” and “behind,” the therapist inserts the two perceptual position of the moving quoit should
quoit slides into the holder, centering them be changing. If the patient is in the egocentric
around the dot, and disparates the right and mode, he should perceive the quoit moving
left slides repeatedly between C and 3. Note in front of and behind the dot compared to
here that the therapist is not trying to build himself. Asking the patient to attend to the
ranges, which would cause the eye to deviate space between his nose and the dot, generally
and could invite an ABC response. Rather the shifts him into the egocentric mode, stabilizes
therapist instructs the patient to watch the dot the absolute position of the dot, and allows the
with aligned eyes, disparates the quoits, and patient to perceive the quoit moving in front of
asks the patient about the perceived relative and behind the dot.
positions of the dot and rope. Once peripheral awareness has been trained
The area of space selected for viewing may to maintain grossly aligned eyes through some
be increased by using a number of approaches range of z-axis movement, the goal is to make
suggested in the above peripheral vision the target more central. The closer the holder
section: is to the patient’s face, the more peripheral the
•  Shake the two quoits compared to quoit is and generally the more accurate the
the dot even while moving the quoits localization. As the vectogram holder is moved
relative to each other between 3 and C out farther away from the patient’s nose, the
(peripheral retinal stimulation). quoit and dot become a more central target.
• Ask the patient to look towards the dot Also if the eyes are not diverging as the target
but to be aware of the shaking quoit out recedes, the angle of strabismus is increasing
of the corner of his eye (peripheral retinal relative to the dot. Such factors cause the
stimulation). perception of the position of the quoit to
• Ask the patient to be aware of the whole become confused. Thus, the patient is definite
room while judging if the quoit is in front about the quoit’s position when it is near. As the
of or behind the dot (divided attention). dot is moved farther away and into the “zone
221
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
of confusion,” the patient is no longer certain wall but, having entered the zone of confusion,
of the quoit’s position. Rather than being able will no longer be certain of the quoit’s position.
to circle the quoit with a finger or pointer As outlined above, a number of instruction sets
accurately, the patient gives vague reports and variations of physical parameters can be
such as, “the quoit seems to be somewhere used to manipulate peripheral awareness and
in front of the dot.” Also, for a given base-out elicit the patient’s perception of stereopsis and
disparity, the farther the dot is moved away correct stereo-localization:
from the patient, the greater the amount of • Start closer to the wall. Coach the patient
float the quoit should appear to have relative to “look toward the dot, but keep your
to the dot. The therapist knows the zone of attention focused on the entire ring at
confusion has been reached when, as the dot the same time.”
is gradually moved farther from the patient, • Gradually move farther away until certain
the patient begins to perceive the relative localization is lost.
distance between the quoit and the dot to be •  At a distance where the patient has
stabilizing or even decreasing, as if the quoit is some perception of the ring’s float, work
now retreating back closer to the dot. “responsibility and control,” alternately
having the patient go central to lose the
Quoit with Dot on Wall float and then go peripheral to regain
Using an overhead projector, the therapist the float.
projects the quoits with the dot target onto • Cover one of the patient’s eyes and lose
a silver screen that does not depolarize the the EDP. Uncover the patient’s eye and
light. This target is half natural (the dot is regain fusion and EDP.
seen simultaneously by both eyes, as are the • Have the patient look at his finger held a
contents of the room) and half dichoptic (each few inches in front of his face. Then have
eye is exposed to a different ring). The therapist the patient return his attention to the
positions the patient so that both eyes may be screen, become peripheral, and again
grossly aligned on the projected dot. If this see the float.
requires the patient to move too close to the • Coach the patient as follows:
screen, then the shadow of the patient’s head n “Be aware of the right and left walls at
can serve as the dot. the same time.” (divided attention)
As the patient views the dot, the therapist n “Be aware of the floor and ceiling at
disparates the quoits between C and 3, base in the same time.” (divided attention)
and base out (again the therapist is not building n “ Be aware of the distance between
ranges, but introducing disparities so that your nose and the dot.” (egocentric
each eye’s slightly different perspective elicits attention)
stereopsis). The patient is coached to increase n “ Be aware of how long the floor is
the size of his or her sphere of awareness and between yourself and the dot.”
to report if the quoit appears to be in front of (egocentric attention)
or behind the dot. After correct stereoscopic n “See the entire silver screen simultane­
vision is achieved with the patient standing ously as if it were a big silver business
closer to the dot on the wall, and the patient card.” (gestalt attention)
has a certainty of where the quoit is floating, n “ Constrict your attention to the very
the therapist can coach the patient as far away edge of the dot and make the quoit go
from the wall as he can maintain certainty of the flat on the wall. Now, see the side walls
quoit’s location. Past that distance, the patient (entire screen, the space between you
may be able to report that the quoit is off the and the dot—whichever works) and
222
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
make the quoit float.” (Responsibility may interfere with the perception of
and control) stereopsis due to the conflict between
• Arrange the room physically to increase monocular and binocular depth percep­
appreciation of stereopsis in the following tion cues when the dowel overlaps a
ways: portion of the quoit.
n Adjust the overhead projector to blur n H
ave the patient use a shorter, lighter,
the quoit. Gradually reduce the blur as dowel stick to tap the longer dowel stick
stereopsis and correct localization can at a point even with the perceived ring.
be maintained. Thus if the patient perceives the quoit
n Shake the quoit to encourage peri­ as being close to the wall, he will tap
pheral awareness (peripheral retinal the one stick against the other at that
stimulation). distance, etc.
n Place a chair between the patient
and the dot and have the patient be Projected Spirangle
peripherally aware (as described above). Stage I—Peripheral Stereoscopic Vision
Compare the location of the quoit Once the patient can alternately voluntarily
to the chair back in order to increase choose to appreciate or not appreciate
z-axis awareness. At first, the patient’s stereopsis with correct localization several
constricted sphere of awareness may feet away from the wall, we are ready to try
require the chair to be aligned directly the spirangle vectogram. To begin with, the
with the quoit. As the size of the therapist adjusts the overhead projector to
sphere of awareness increases, allowing blur the projected spirangle until its form is
simultaneous awareness of the chair vague and its letters and other details are
and ring, the therapist can move the beyond recognition. Encouraging peripheral
chair further to the side to increase awareness, the therapist shakes the vectogram
x-axis awareness. and encourages the patient to discern if the
n Align more than one chair along outside or the inside of the spirangle is closest.
the patient’s z-axis to extend z-axis When the patient is able to see the float, the
awareness, stereopsis, and correct therapist adds the responsibility and control of
localization. central-peripheral alternation until the patient
n Arrange two chairs between the patient can control the appreciation of stereopsis and
and the screen, but place one to the localization with the blurred target. Then the
right side and one to the left side of therapist reduces the blur until the patient
the screen. Coach the patient to be perceives the target to begin to flatten. The
simultaneously aware of the two chairs, therapist blurs the target just enough to
the dot, and the floating quoit. restore the EDP and coaches the patient to
n Have the patient hold a long dowel use peripheral awareness—especially gestalt
stick between himself and the dot attention of the complete silver screen—to keep
on the wall. Have the patient try to the spirangle floating, even as blur is slowly
see the entire length of the dowel reduced. Indeed, the goal of the spirangle
simultaneously. With the aid of the dowel is to teach the patient to use peripheral
stick, while the therapist continues to awareness to maintain the target’s float despite
shake the ring, the float can become the introduction of central detail. Apart from
quite dramatic. Alternatively, for some the central-peripheral attention procedures
patients, awareness of the side wall can encouraging responsibility and control, digital
be more useful: Recall that the dowel instrumentation is often more helpful in the
223
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
development of peripheral attention in the Digital Stereoscopic Vision
presence of central stereopsis. For digital therapy, we use the Computer
Orthoptics® Liquid Crystal Automated Vision
Stage II—Central Stereoscopic Vision Therapy System (VTS4).27 While the images of
Once the patient has good appreciation the VTS4 are dichoptic, the VTS4 procedures
of spirangle stereopsis with the vectogram can be performed to stress real images in
perfectly clear, the therapist can direct the the real world as well. When, for instance, to
patient’s attention to the alphabet within the increase peripheral awareness and egocentric
conical structure of the target, with lines which stereopsis, attention is directed to awareness of
spiral outward from center to periphery. Each of the instrument framing the screen and images,
these larger letters is in a circle and surrounded both eyes are seeing the same frame.
by a square, as shown in Figure 5 (not to be While the projected quoit vectogram has
confused with the tiny letters with suppression certain advantages over the VTS4—being easier
controls at the spirangle’s center and end). to blur without lenses and being better suited for
working responsibility-and-control for alternate
central-peripheral awareness—the VTS4 has a
number of advantages over projected quoits:
• The liquid crystal glasses are flashing
rapidly between the right and left eyes’
images, making it difficult not to perceive
the stereopsis—a feature which makes
Depending on whether the circle has base it more difficult to voluntarily flatten
out, base in, or no disparity relative to its the stereopsis with central awareness.
square, the circle should be perceived as closer, (The rapidly changing stimulus is similar
farther, or in the same plane as the square in impact to shaking the quoits.) This
(respectively). With letter A, for instance, the persistence of stereo nevertheless
circle containing the letter should be perceived provides a boon for many of those who
in the same plane as the square; with letter cannot otherwise perceive stereoscopic
B the circle should be perceived closer than vision.
the square; with letter C the circle should be • Unlike the projected quoits, the VTS4
perceived behind the square. allows the patient to get very close to
As the patient searches for the alphabet the screen without occluding the target,
and perceives if the circle is flat, in front of, thus the VTS4—like the quoit with dot at
or behind the square, the therapist needs to near—allows a more peripheral target at
coach to patient to continue to see the entire a distance where the esotropic patient’s
spirangle floating out in space. Thus the eyes are closer to being aligned.
procedure combines EDP and RDP, as well as • The VTS4 provides a wide variety of
spatial language. targets, some more peripheral, some
Not all patients who can perceive some more central, all of which can be varied in
degree of float on the quoit will be able to size to make them even more central.
perceive the float on the spirangle. Not all • The VTS4 also allows an excellent bridge
patients who perceive the float on the spirangle for coming to appreciate 3D TV, it being
will be able to perceive the relative position possible to connect a Blu-ray 3D Player to
of the circles and squares. Thus it is possible the same computer system.
for patients to develop decent EDP while still
lacking central RDP.
224
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
The best targets for initially eliciting the absolute depth of the target floating
stereoscopic vision are the ring, the dog and ring, out into the room.
and the robot and ring. The same techniques • 
As stereoscopic fusion improves, add
that were used for the projected quoits may covering and uncovering one eye or
be used for eliciting peripheral/egocentric looking back and forth between a near
localization for these VTS4 targets: finger and the screen.
• Being aware of the entire room to the
sides of the instrument (divided attention) The patient works these peripheral ring
• Being aware of the distance between targets until maximum float is obtained using
nose and target (egocentric attention) either rectangular screen frame gestalt attention
• Being aware of the rectangular gestalt of or egocentric attention. Next the airplane-
the screen frame as if it were a business alphabet target and then the dolphin-fish target
card (gestalt attention) may be used to better combine RDP and EDP.
• Pressing the Function 11 key to cause the The therapist fully increases the size of the
ring to rapidly oscillate (peripheral retinal airplane-alphabet target and has the patient
stimulation). remain aware of the screen frame gestalt until
the patient can see the target’s blue background
In addition, a number of other techniques closer and further than the screen frame as the
may be used to facilitate appreciation of target is varied between base-out and base-in
stereoscopic vision: disparity. It is often the egocentric localization
• Making the ring larger or smaller to of the blue background that allows the patient
optimize appreciation of stereoscopic to overcome the central details encouraging a
vision at a given distance. A ring that is return to strabismic posture and loss of RDP.
larger than the patient’s ability to open As with the circle targets, the dowel stick
up side vision is ineffective. A ring that is or neoprene toy sword may be placed closer
too small triggers strabismic posture and to individual letters of the airplane-alphabet
adaptations. target to encourage perception of egocentric
• Placing both hands on the screen, one on localization. The patient should be encouraged
each side of the ring, being aware of both to perceive simultaneously the entire length
hands at the same time, and being aware of the sword or dowel, that is, should be
of the length of both arms. encouraged to see the gestalt of the sword
• Placing the tip of a dowel (or neoprene rather than running eyes up and down its length
foam sword, to protect the screen surface) to build a sequential perception of the sword;
near the ring or other target to facilitate sequential perception frequently defeats both
appreciation of stereoscopic vision without EDP and RDP.
the need to fully expand the sphere of The fish target may be used the same as the
attention and open up peripheral vision. alphabet target, again seeing the position of the
Once the stereopsis may be appreciated pale green background compared to the gestalt
with the sword close to the target, the of the frame edge. Whereas the alphabet target
therapist positions the dowel increasingly matches monocular and binocular cues, with
farther to the side until the patient can letter size corresponding to letter distance (so
open up side vision enough to use the the patient’s responses of depth can be based
rectangular screen frame gestalt to elicit entirely on monocular cues), the fish target
egocentric localization. The goal is for the does not necessarily match size and distance.
patient to be able to perceive not only Thus it requires stereoscopic appreciation and
the relative depth within the target, but gives a more reliable guide of the patient’s
225
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
binocular illusion status. (The stationary ‘Figure-8 My therapists and I typically begin with the
Racetrack’ vectogram also varies size and depth second track of Madagascar III. We stop the
independently.) movie with the animated television set in the
As digital stereo targets can be seen at scene flying out at the patient. We prompt the
greater distances from the monitor, their patient to be aware of the outside of the screen
appreciation can be combined with such and appreciate the egocentric position of the
traditional movement activities as leg swings animated television set in space. We next stop
while balancing on one foot, walking rails, the action of the film with the monkey in the
balance boards, or infinity walk28 —all without wig applying makeup. Again, we encourage
loss of correct localization. awareness of the edge of the screen and
perceiving the egocentric position of the
Computer-generated Randot Stereograms monkey’s lips. Finally, we stop the movie when
With strabismics who are truly intermittent, the pillow explodes into a cloud of floating
their eyes being precisely aligned rather than feathers, and we train the patient on being
grossly aligned when straightened, computer- aware of how attention to the outside of the
generated randot stereograms can be used. As screen gets the feathers to float out further.
designed, the randot stereograms stress RDP, a Once the patient has seen the animated
smaller square being perceived to float outward flying television, the monkey applying lipstick,
from a larger rectangle. When these randot and the floating feathers in 3D, we replay track
targets are used, however, care must be taken 2 without stopping it, encouraging awareness
not to forget EDP. If the patient “gestalts” the of the outside of the screen and perception
rectangular edge of the computer screen, as of the RDP and EDP in the scenes without
practiced during previous digital procedures, stopping the action. We then jump ahead to
the patient should perceive SILO of the larger track 12, again stopping the action throughout
rectangular background moving behind the the circus scene, getting the patient aware of
screen and in front of the screen even as the all the 3D moments before replaying the scene.
smaller square floats relative to the rectangle. While doing this, we encourage the use of all
The larger rectangular target should float the tools learned in the therapy room during
into the screen and get larger during base-in vectographic and digital presentations:
vergence and float outward and get smaller • The dowel stick or neoprene sword can
during base-out vergence. In this way randot be inserted in the scenes to encourage
stereograms can also be used to integrate 3D perception while still allowing a
RDP and EDP, which can later be transferred to constricted sphere of attention. Ideally,
natural conditions, as we will see in the “Natural by the time 3D television is used, the
Stereopsis” section below. patient has already learned to expand
the sphere of attention to include the
3D TV gestalt of the screen edge and expected
The VTS4 has any number of targets EDP for the movie scenes.
working evermore central stereopsis. These • The patient can stand at greater distances
targets can and should be used to develop from the screen.
stereopsis further, but once RDP and EDP can • The patient can cover an eye and
be maintained on the airplane and dolphin uncover it to compare the stereopsis
targets without triggering strabismic posture while also learning to regain fusion after
and spatial confusion, 3D movies can be used dissociation.
for further practice in both RDP and EDP, both
in office and at home.
226
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
The same principles can be applied to epiphenomenon, for instance, because it adds
any other 3D movie that makes use of both no propulsion to the bee’s flight. The term is
crossed and uncrossed disparities. When popular in a philosophy that proposes that
working 3D television movie perception the visual consciousness adds nothing to visual
therapist needs to be aware of the times when action. The argument is that since actions—
maximum base out disparity is encountered to like a tennis pro’s swing—come before any
be sure it is properly perceived by the patient. conscious analysis of the action, consciousness
Wikipedia provides a “List of 3D Films.”29 The lags behind the action,31 consciousness (that is,
list tells if the movie was filmed in 3D or filmed conscious free will) is too slow to affect action. It
in 2D and converted to 3D. Converted movies may be true that consciousness played no part
typically contain little base out disparity with in the reflex action. However, whether conscious
which to check localization. Most animated films exploration may have played an active part in
are filmed in 3D and have both BI and BO, or learning the swing is not considered. In the same
crossed and uncrossed, disparity. way, even though consciousness of stereopsis
Once the patient demonstrates accurate 3D may follow rather than lead visual action it does
movie perception in the office, the therapist can not necessarily exclude conscious exploration
assign 3D TV movie viewing at home and 3D of stereopsis as a tool for teaching comfortable
movie viewing in the theater. An adult patient alignment. Despite a century of disagreements
can be trained to ask herself periodically, “Am between those promoting the surgical
I aware of the border of the screen.” “Am I versus nonsurgical tools for the treatment of
aware of the distance between my nose and the strabismus,32 my experience with vision therapy
screen?” “How far off the screen are the images certainly supports this second view.
popping?” If the patient is a child, a parent can
be trained to ask the same questions. Mechanical versus Localization Alignment
Physiologically speaking, there is more than
Natural Stereopsis one way to align an eye. Maddox, in 1893,
The reason we work dichoptic stereo classified vergences as tonic, accommodative,
perception is not to familiarize the patient with disparity, or psychic (such as voluntary and
binocular illusions, which may have no benefit proximal convergence).33 Lenses altering
beyond entertainment in the office or movie accommodation can be used to alter
theater. We use stereoscopic vision procedures vergences. In the treatment of strabismus, I find
to train improved localization, which the patient it convenient to divide non-accommodative
may use to encourage alignment and improve vergences into two practical classifications:
performance. Unless, however, this awareness mechanical vergence and localization vergence.
is carried beyond the confines of the therapy What I am calling mechanical alignment is the
room, the treatment may be for naught. It is, use of vergences to avoid diplopia (disparity)
therefore, useful to transfer what the illusions and encourage voluntary alignment (psychic).
teach in the therapy room to real stereopsis in I can, for instance, voluntarily cross my eyes
the real world. and control the separation of diplopic images
Despite the advantages of stereopsis, even in space. This method, however, requires that I
this goal is not beyond criticism. The surgeons learn to divorce accommodation from vergence
Von Noorden and Campos,30 for instance, have if I am to see clearly with crossed eyes (absolute
written, “Stereopsis is an epiphenomenon of presbyopia helps).
normal binocular vision.” An epiphenomenon Most exotropes and many esotropes similarly
is a byproduct of an action that adds no can voluntarily align their eyes, using voluntary
benefit to the action. The buzz of a bee is an convergence or divergence. The problem is
227
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
that while using such mechanical vergences Thus each time the patient is about to insert
strabismics are often not comfortable without the stick in the straw, the therapist reminds
considerable training, including the use of base- the patient to “see the distance between your
in and base-out prisms to train convergence and nose and the straw” or asks, “Can you see how
divergence, as well as monocular and binocular much air there is between your nose and the
accommodative rock to ensure full flexibility straw?” In this way the therapist is encouraging
between accommodation and vergences. z-axis-egocentric awareness in the appreciation
Patients using mechanical vergence expend of stereopsis to help trigger alignment. The
concentration and effort that often results in goal is to turn localization vergence into the
stress. Unless the desire for cosmetically aligned primary strategy and mechanical vergence
eyes outweighs the inconvenience of the into a secondary strategy for fine-tuning ocular
stress, mechanical vergence alignment is often alignment if necessary.
abandoned.
I propose a second approach to align eyes: Brock String
localization. Localization vergence similarly relies Physiological diplopia can be an artifact
on both disparity and psychic vergence, but in of extending the z-axis sphere of attention,
a different way. Here, disparity is used in the causing the object fixated to be seen single
creation of stereopsis rather than the avoidance and objects aligned closer and further to be
of diplopia. Here, the eyes are aligned where seen double. If a Brock string—a several-feet-
the patient knows the target to be compared or-more-long string with three beads on it—is
to self. Thus EDP plays a major role. While extended outward from the patient’s face, and
mastering the exercises designed to increase the patient extends z-axis awareness to see
accommodative and vergence flexibility and the entire length of the string simultaneously,
comfort remains as useful as ever in achieving physiological diplopia may create the illusion
alignment, if the patient learns to utilize of two strings crossing each other to form an X,
localization from EDP, then alignment is far less the intersection of which occurs at the point of
stressful. With this goal in mind, we turn to the bi-fixation, the distance at which the patient’s
art of transferring dichoptic stereoscopic vision eyes are converged. (As discussed previously,
to natural stereopsis. the sphere of attention, also allowing the
physiological diplopia illusion, can often
Binocular Stick and Straw be expanded, depending on the patient’s
When performing the binocular stick and idiosyncrasies, using simultaneous gestalt
straw procedure, the patient inserts a pickup awareness of the string length, egocentric
stick in a drinking straw held parallel to his/ awareness of the distance between nose and
her face. The procedure encourages alignment string end, or divided awareness of the walls to
and perception of stereopsis for its completion. either side of the string.)
If mechanical vergence is the primary strategy If for instance the string beads are placed at
being used, the patient voluntarily aligns four inches, ten inches, and forty-eight inches
his eye on the straw while inserting the stick from the patient’s nose, and the patient’s eyes
into the straw. If instead, the patient is to use are bi-fixating the middle bead to create the
localization vergence, he is coached to expand normal illusion, the patient will perceive the
the sphere of attention to include the previously bi-fixated bead as single and the beads closer
learned z-axis awareness to judge the distance and farther double. If alternation is artificially
between self and straw. The awareness helps suspended by command, and the esotropic
trigger alignment, reducing the effort typically patient with ABC is bi-fixating the ten-inch
needed to produce mechanical vergence. bead, he can typically be encouraged to behold
228
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
the physiological diplopia illusion inside the bi- seeing. Furthermore, while esotropes often
fixation point, but he perceives no such illusion avoid bi-fixation when their eyes are diverging,
at greater distances (typically perceiving a approaching from the near side of the target, bi-
single string perpendicular to his face beyond fixation is frequently better tolerated when the
the bi-fixated bead). Such a patient will perceive eyes are converging, approaching from farther
two strings converging into the ten-inch bead to closer, possible because the esotrope has
and a single string in its true position beyond no previous experience inside of the centration
the point of bi-fixation. Similarly, the patient point leading to adaptation and avoidance of
will see the four-inch bead inside the bifixation bi-fixation.
point as double and the forty-eight inch bead Since the bead will appear single, even if
beyond the bi-fixation point as single. Thus the the esotrope over-converges, care must be
patient sees a Y rather than an X. taken that convergence halts, the three strings
The goal of the Brock string, however, is intersect, and bi-fixation occurs exactly at the
not to illicit the physiological-diplopia illusion. bead. Once such bi-fixation has been mastered,
Apart from the string being an excellent appreciation of egocentric and relative depth
tool for the development of z-axis gestalt perception may be added by performing a
awareness, the goal of the string is to use modification of the Brock string procedure.
the illusion to provide feedback for bi-fixation
while expanding the sphere of attention along 3D Stick and String
the z-axis. Note that the perception of the The Brock string is modified into a 3D
strings intersecting as a ‘Y’ is as useful for this procedure by having the patient, while
feedback as the perception of the intersection maintaining bi-fixation, be aware of the length
as an ‘X’! Thus there is no reason for teaching of the strings running into the bead, or be
the esotrope with ABC to see double beyond aware of the amount of space between the
the point of bi-fixation — which could translate patient’s nose and the bead. The bead is now
into seeing double in the real world — when substituted for the straw in the above-described
it is not necessarily useful to do so. Instead stick-and-straw procedure. The patient holds
esotropes can be trained to bi-fixate a bead a pickup stick parallel to his face and several
once they have already been trained to diverge inches to the side of the bead. While the patient
as far as the strabismus allows (with free space maintains egocentric localization and perceives
jump vergences and peripheral awareness). The the three strings intersecting at the bead, he
instructions for working with the Brock string uses the stick to touch the side of the bead just
while perceiving a ‘Y’ intersection are: as in the stick-and-straw procedure, where he
• Throw the eyes maximally outward to placed the stick inside the straw. To accomplish
make the three strings meet as far behind this, both EDP and RDP are coordinated,
the bead as possible. the egocentric stereo forming a base for the
• Then converge just until the bead relative-stereoscopic stick-andbead placement.
appears single, with the three strings Once the patient is learning to coordinate
intersecting at it. eye position, stereoscopic perception, and
hand movement by simultaneously bi-fixating
Developing physiological diplopia inside the the bead, perceiving the illusion of three
centration point typically offers little problem, so strings meeting at the bead, and accurately
long as the patient is not alternating to maintain directing the stick from the side of the bead to
strabismic posture. The esotrope’s visual world contact the bead, the therapist begins varying
is beyond, not inside of the bi-fixation point, the distance of the bead from the patient’s
so the diplopia does not transfer to everyday nose, placing the bead at various distances
229
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
within the patient’s centration range. Thus if appreciation of stereopsis outside the therapy
the patient’s range of bi-fixation were between room.
two and ten inches, the therapist would place If you hold a hand mirror before your face,
the bead at two inches then ten inches, then you may count a few of your eyelashes in the
six inches, etc. At the ten-inch distance, the reflection. If instead of concentrating on such
patient would get the three strings to intersect central vision, you open your side vision and
at the bead before making the placement. At become simultaneously aware of the gestalt
closer distances, the therapist would have the of the mirror frame encircling the glass, you
patient diverge behind the bead and slowly should become aware of the actual location
converge until the two images of the bead just of your reflected face floating as far behind
came together. This completed, the patient the glass as you are in front. At this point,
would again open up egocentric attention and instead of being locked in RDP and noting
make the stick placement. that the reflection of the tip of your nose is
The above procedure can be repeated with closer than the reflection of your cheeks, you
the string slanted to the right, left, up, and have extended your sphere of attention to
down compared to the previous straight-ahead become aware of EDP and the position of
position. We want the patient to be adept at bi- your reflection compared to actual space.
fixation, EDP, RDP and eye-hand coordination When working with strabismus, the therapist
in all fields of gaze where binocularity may exist. may use this stereoscopic phenomenon in the
same way that the quoit and dot were used.
Vertical Stick and Straw By encouraging the patient to “see the room
Vertical stick and straw procedure is just to both sides of the mirror” or “see the entire
an extension of binocular stick and string. In mirror frame at once” or “see the space between
this activity, however, instead of using a bead you and the mirror frame” the therapist can
on the string, the patient uses a vertically-held coach divided, gestalt, or egocentric awareness,
drinking straw aligned parallel to the face. As whichever best expands the sphere of attention
with the bead, when the patient’s eyes are and result in EDP. To work responsibility and
aligned beyond the straw, the patient should control, the therapist can encourage the patient
perceive the illusion of the straw being double. to alternate between center and periphery,
If the patient has already mastered control of constricting awareness to a single eyelash and
vergences using the string, the therapist now then expanding awareness to the rim of the
coaches the patient slowly to converge until mirror frame and the egocentric position of the
the two straws just came together, again being reflection in space, some distance beyond the
careful not to allow convergence nearer than mirror frame.
the bi-fixation of the straw. The patient, holding While working the hand-held mirror pro­
the stick vertically above the bi-fixated straw, cedure, the therapist can check for accurate
lowers the stick into the straw. As with the location of the reflected image by moving a
string, the placements would be repeated at hand toward the patient from well behind the
all distances that the patient can look beyond mirror. The patient is coached to simultaneously
the straw, double the straw, and converge to (without shifting his eyes back and forth) be
fuse the straw before making the vertical stick aware of his reflection and your hand and note
placement. when the hand and reflected image are at
the same distance from his face. The absolute
Hand Mirror Stereopsis position of the reflected image should change
Mirrors provide another means for manipu­ as the distance of the mirror from the patient’s
lating the sphere of attention to transfer face changes. To check the localization of the
230
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
mirror image, the therapist simply has to observe If when asked to see the frame gestalt,
if the reflected image is as far behind the mirror the patient moves his eyes about the frame to
as the patient’s face is in front of the mirror. If view it sequentially rather than simultaneous­
not, the therapist should coach the patient to ly, the therapist should encourage him to look
expand his sphere of attention, using whichever toward his reflection and see the entire frame
method works best. Inaccurate localization simultaneously out of the corner of his eyes. As
signals that the sphere of attention is contracted explained in the gestalt awareness section, he
or that the patient’s eyes are not aligned. In the should view the gestalt of the frame just as he
case of an esotrope, the therapist can move the would view the gestalt of a business card. This
mirror closer to the patient’s face to secure more awareness should not be difficult if it has already
accurate alignment. been worked extensively with dichoptic vecto­
Because the mirror can be held mere inches graphic and digital images.
in front of the face, the procedure is useful for If the patient is successful at shifting into the
esotropia as well as exotropia. The expansion of egocentric system, he should see his reflection
the sphere of attention can also be manipulated spring back as far behind the frame and glass as
by changing the mirror size, larger mirrors he is standing in front of the mirror. If not, (and
requiring a greater expansion. Similarly, the the patient’s eyes are aligned or approximately
closer a given size mirror is to the face, the aligned) it is likely the patient is still processing
more peripheral the patient must become to centrally (or his eyes are not really aligned). If
be simultaneously aware of the entire frame the patient is not able to expand his sphere of
encircling the glass. attention out as far as the edge of the mirror
frame, have him approach the mirror and
Wall Mirror Stereopsis place his hands at shoulder width on the glass.
The following mirror stereopsis procedure Have him wiggle the fingers of both hands
is useful for intermittent exotropes when their simultaneously and be simultaneously aware of
eyes are aligned. It is also useful for intermit­ both hands’ finger movement while watching
tent esotropes, or esotropes with approximate­ the reflection of his face behind the mirror.
ly aligned eyes and peripheral fusion. Such Ask the patient if he can now see his reflection
alignment or partial alignment should already behind the glass of the mirror compared to
have been taught using dichoptic images as his hands. This modification allows the patient
explained in the above sections before at­ to open up his sphere of attention less and
tempting this procedure. still shift into the peripheral mode of seeing.
With the patient several feet in front of a Providing that the patient is simultaneously
wall mirror, have him examine the features of attending to both hands and the reflection of
his face—nose, eyes, chin, eyebrows. While his face rather than sequentially attending to
performing these central observations, most hands and face, he should appreciate EDP.
patients will be aware of the details of their Another way to open the sphere of atten­
faces, not the position of their reflections in the tion is to place two strips of masking tape
mirror. To expand the patient’s sphere of at­ on the mirror, one strip to each side of the
tention and get him into the EDP system, have patient’s reflection. The patient then watches
the patient shift attention to perceive the ge­ his reflection while being simultaneously
stalt of the mirror frame and be aware of how aware of both strips. This will often allow
far his reflection is behind the frame and glass. the patient to shift into the peripheral mode
Most patients will instantly see their reflections and perceive EDP. The tapes are especially
spring behind the glass. useful if the patient is practicing using a wide

231
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
bathroom mirror at home when shaving or and plays a game with his son, chasing him
applying makeup. around the room, and warning him to beware
Once the patient has opened his sphere of “the claw.” These scenes inspire a useful
of attention to become aware of the distance therapy procedure:
his reflection is behind the mirror glass, the If you raise your hand with its palm vertical
therapist can stand behind and to the side of to your face and then curl your fist so your
the mirror and have the patient move up or fingers and thumb are all pointed at your
back until the patient’s reflection is beside the eyes, you will be viewing “the claw.” If you
therapist. To accomplish this, the mirror must view this claw first with one eye, then the
either be freestanding or positioned next to other, then both, you will see that each
a doorway or hall so the therapist can stand eye perceives a different image and when
in the next room. The therapist then moves both eyes are opened simultaneously
closer or farther behind the mirror, and the these two different images are combined
patient, using EDP, can move closer and farther into a stereoscopic perception in which the
in front of the mirror to continue to perceive the fingers appear longer as they perceptually
image of his reflection beside the therapist. spring out closer to your face. This I call,
Once good localization has been obtained, “Claw Stereopsis.”
the patient can become aware of mirror stere­
opsis in routine life whenever a mirror is used. If the patient places “the claw” in front of
his face, directs both eyes at the nearest
Mirror Near-far Jumps or Mirror Cover/ fingertip, becomes aware of how far that
Uncover/Recover fingertip is in front of his nose, and extends
Once the patient has demonstrated his vision to simultaneously be aware of
accuracy in perceiving the mirror image, the the length of the fingers and position of
procedure can be combined with near-far his palm, the stereopsis will become even
jump vergences or cover/uncover/recover. more apparent.
With near far jumps, the patient looks at
his finger two or three inches in front of his J ust as a wall mirror can provide a stereo
nose and then looks back into the mirror to target to give feedback for alignment in
locate his image behind the glass. In the case distance, “the claw” can provide a natural
of cover/uncover/recover, the patient covers stereo target to encourage alignment at
one eye and imagines that his image is on near. The claw can be combined with near-
the glass. (Indeed, most patients looking with far jump vergences, using the mirror in the
one eye covered can image the reflection to distance and the claw at near. An esotrope
be anywhere, either on or behind the glass.) can similarly use claw stereopsis by holding
The patient then uncovers the eye, aligns it, his curled fist within his centration range
and becomes aware of his reflection’s correct to compare one-eyed and two-eyed depth
egocentric position behind the glass. Both perception.
cover/uncover/recover and near-far jumps
are continued a few minutes at a time over Yoked Prism Walk
a number of sessions until the procedure is Yoked prisms are a powerful tool for building
effortless. EDP. Mismatching eye and body information,
yoked prisms often bypass habitual, automatic
Claw Stereopsis patterns of perception and trigger exploration.
In the movie Liar Liar, Jim Carry plays the The patient’s attention shifts to answer two
part of a father who turns his fist into “a claw” questions: 1) “Where is my body compared to
232
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
the world?” (2) “Where is the world compared encouraging the patient to note how much of
to my body?” Answering these questions the room he can see at one time, if things look
can enhance both egocentric awareness and taller or shorter, if he feels taller or shorter, if
simultaneous perception of peripheral space. things are consistently warped or unevenly
This is true whether or not the patient is warped, some areas being constricted, some
strabismic. Getzell has discussed the expansion being expanded. The patient might note how
of the sphere of attention (although under a the room changes if he nods his head, how it
different name) in non-strabismic patients.34 feels to walk, if he can simultaneously perceive
Once the patient has learned to voluntarily the two sides of the room or simultaneously
align his eyes, or align them enough to allow behold the floor and the ceiling. The therapist
a peripheral fusion lock, the yoked prism encourages the patient to note anything that
procedure is especially useful as a bridge to might extrovert him into the room and shift him
egocentric awareness during natural seeing. into the egocentric system. Again the therapist
The therapist positions the patient at the end of can ask if the length of the floor, simultaneously
a room and has him look across the room and viewed, seems to be slanting up or down.
describe what is seen. If the patient is in the The patient walks the length of the room
central mode, he will likely mention windows, and back. The therapist then adjusts the yoked
chairs, tables, cabinets, words on a poster, etc. prisms to base up. Now, if the patient is in
If he is in the egocentric mode, he may say the egocentric system rather than the central
something like “a long room” or “a big room.” system, when he looks to the end of the room
Thus in the central, sequential mode, the and achieves gestalt awareness of the entire
patient sees the particulars of the stuff in the length of the floor, the floor should appear to
room. In the egocentric, simultaneous mode, slant downward. The patient’s walking to the
the patient sees a room, with stuff in it, the stuff end of the room and back, and the questions
all seen as one with its position in the room. directing attention are repeated.
Once the patient has observed the room, With the patient again standing at the end
he dons the 15-20 ^ yoked prism glasses base of the room, as at the beginning of the exercise,
down. The therapist asks the patient to look to the therapist again removes the patient’s
the end of the room and try to see the entire yoked prisms and asks the patient to examine
length of the floor simultaneously—the patient the room and perceive if the room looks any
is thus coached to develop gestalt awareness different. If necessary, the therapist can prompt
of the floor the same way he previously the patient, “Does it seem as if you are seeing
developed gestalt awareness of the projected the entire room at the same time? Most
vectograph silver screen, or gestalt awareness patients will have shifted into the egocentric
of the rectangular edge of the 3D television system to experience simultaneous perception
screen, or gestalt awareness of the rectangular of the full room and will now be seeing, rather
edge of the wall mirror. than individual things in the room, a room with
Once the patient has simultaneous gestalt things in it, each thing having its individual
awareness the full length of the floor, the position in the larger room.
therapist asks whether the floor appears to be
slanting upwards or downwards. If the patient Free Space Egocentric Stereopsis
is in the egocentric mode, the floor will appear After the patient has used both vectographic
to be slanting upwards. If the patient is in the and digital dichoptic images voluntarily to shift
central mode, the floor may appear to be level or between central and egocentric seeing as
floating downwards. In either case, the therapist described in previous sections of this paper,
has the patient walk the length of the room, and after the patient has walked with the yoked
233
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
prism and experienced the perceptual shift into system, enter a hallway and instead of being
egocentric seeing, the skill can be transferred to aware of all the space in the hallway are aware
natural seeing. of the exit sign, or a particular doorway or wall
While the patient is standing at one end of ornament. If instead, the patient shifts into the
the room, the therapist stands half a dozen feet egocentric, simultaneous system, all the air in
in front of the patient and has the patient direct the hallway becomes the center of simultaneous
attention at the therapist’s nose, eyebrows, exploration; the patient should perceive the
etc. to get the patient into the central mode. hallway in perspective, the dimensions of the
Then the therapist asks the patient instead to hallway’s end appearing smaller in size than the
be aware of the space in between the patient’s dimension of the walls, floor and ceiling directly
nose and the therapist’s face and also to be surrounding the patient.
aware of the rest of the room behind the To help with this perceptual shift, the therapist
therapist. (Again, seeing space is not to be accompanies the patient down the hallway
confused with calculating distance, comparing and encourages him to achieve simultaneous
it to a picture of a ruler in the mind.) At this gestalt awareness of the full length of the floor,
point in therapy, after the patient has already monitoring the patient’s eyes for telltale vertical
experienced opening and closing the sphere of saccades that would signal that the patient is
awareness, the patient should be aware of the looking at the nearer and farther parts of the
shift of perspective, which comes when shifting floor for sequential processing rather than using
into the where-is-it, egocentric, peripheral, and the peripheral/egocentric system.
simultaneous system from the central, what is it, The therapist can encourage the patient to
sequential system. The patient should now be perceive simultaneously the two walls of the
aware of the whole room simultaneously, rather hallway and how they appear to flow backwards
than just observing the details of the therapist’s as the therapist and patient walk. The therapist
face. The patient, if asked, should be aware that can encourage the patient to see simultaneously
the therapist has a position in the larger room. If the gestalt of the floor and ceiling. The hallway
the therapist moves, the patient should be able can be used for the yoked base-up or base-
to perceive that the therapist’s position in the down prism walks as the patient achieves gestalt
room changes, being one third or one half the awareness of the air bounded by the length of
way across the room. Again, if the patient’s eyes walls, floor and ceiling. The goal of this therapy
are darting about the room, the therapist should is to remind the patient to have aligned eyes,
remind the patient to look toward the therapist create peripheral awareness, and shift into the
and to be simultaneously aware of the rest of EDP system whenever he enters a hallway.
the room.
The goal of the procedure is to enable a
patient to, at any time, be able to think about
the gestalt of space between his nose and any
intermediate or distant object and to shift into
the egocentric system. Once this becomes
second nature, the patient can use egocentric
awareness as a feedback for alignment.

Hallway Stereo
Another tool for evoking egocentric
awareness is a hallway. Patients who habitually
limit perception to the central, sequential
234
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
Doorway Stereo alignment of eyes. Taking advantage of this, the
In this activity the therapist serves as a central therapist and patient can take a walk outside,
target framed by a doorway. The room behind the therapist coaching the patient to observe
the therapist is visible beyond the doorway (see simultaneously the gestalt of the shape of the
Figure 6). The therapist stands in front of the patches of blue sky and bordering objects
door frame, facing the patient. First, the therapist and the subsequent improvement in the
encourages the patient to constrict attention to relative stereopsis. The procedure is complete
the individual features of the therapist’s face. if whenever the patient walks or drives, he
Next, the therapist encourages the patient to experiences the joy of seeing the shape of the
be aware of the distance between his nose and sky, extroverting attention, and enhancing EDP
the therapist and to see how the wall behind the and alignment.
doorway seems to spring back from the doorway
as the room fills with space. CONCLUSION
Another way to elicit this 3D perspective is Even without touching upon the newest uses
to have the patient open up his side vision to of virtual reality,35 the digital age promises a
achieve gestalt awareness of the door frame revolution in the nonsurgical care of strabismus.
and to see how much space there is between But what is the goal of such treatment? Most
the frame and the far wall in the room beyond would agree on “aligned eyes with clear, single
the doorway. Again, the goal of the procedure is binocular vision.” The meanings of the “clear”
to teach the patient to shift into the egocentric and “single” in the goal are fairly straightforward,
system whenever he approaches a doorway. being almost universally considered in ophthalmic
The awareness of space can then be used for care, even if only crudely approximated to check
feedback concerning alignment. for neurological integrity. But what exactly does
“binocular vision” mean?
The Shape of the Sky Does binocularity mean that when we are
A blue sky possesses a shape, sometimes placed in artificial conditions using dichoptic
bounded merely by the horizon, sometimes images we all see the same illusions, perceptions
chiseled at the edges by mountains, trees, that occur only in our minds rather than
buildings, telephone poles, even launching accurately mirroring the world before us? Do we
sites and Ferris wheels. Within, the patches of properly perceive first, second, and third degree
blue sky are often divided by the fleecy white fusion, physiological diplopia, luster, or rivalry if
of clouds or feathered wings of birds. Outside our eyes are fortuitously aligned with the targets,
our office, for instance, in one direction the diplopia if they are not? Or is binocular vision
trees cut a v-shaped patch of blue. In the other normal only if it allows us—without resorting to
direction, the trees form a crooked blue figure sequential processing—to be instantly aware of
of sky pierced by a white church steeple. When an object’s identity and position in space and to
the therapist coaches the patient to use gestalt answer the questions “What is it?” and “Where
awareness to behold the shape of the blue is it?” When we see an object in the room, are
sky, the patient may shift into the egocentric we simultaneously aware of that object’s position
system; the surrounding trees and telephone compared to the room and ourselves without
poles may leap into perspective, each being the need to assemble the individual features of
seen in its proper order of depth. the scene like pieces in a puzzle, one fixation at
Thus the gestalt awareness needed to see a time?
the shape of the sky affords the strabismic, or The gift of binocular vision is not necessarily
other vision therapy patient, with yet another to be found in the numbers and the illusions but
chance for appreciation of EDP to reward the in performance and the joy found in the visual
235
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
consciousness of Barry’s “palpable volume of language by past action, only then are we freed
visual space,” and Vishwanath’s “characteristically to embrace the silent reality of space. Only then,
vivid impression of tangible solid form, immersive do we truly begin to dance with the dimensions
negative space and realness.” This paper has of the world. Today’s digital technologies,
offered for the treatment of strabismus some added to breakthroughs in our understanding of
procedures whose success supports Vishwanath’s egocentric depth perception, are making it ever
theory of the role of egocentric awareness in the more possible for strabismic patients to see the
perception of stereopsis. shape of the sky.
EDP therapies—and there are many, many
approaches, some described for nonstrabismic Conflicts of Interest
visual anomalies by Getzell36 —reward alignment The author has no financial interests in any
with an awareness of threedimensional space of the products mentioned in this paper.
while lessening the stress and discomfort
of purely mechanical voluntary alignment. Acknowledgments
Coaching heightened “gestalt,” “egocentric,” I’d like to thank my mentors Bill Ludlam,
and “divided” attention extends the “sphere of Nathan Flax, Arnie Sherman, Martin Birnbaum,
attention” to allow enhanced EDP and provide Elliot Forrest, and Irwin Suchoff. I’d also like to
an escape from the perturbations of being thank Robert Sanet, Samantha Slotnick, and
locked into a central identification system… Susan Barry for insights and inspiration. Finally,
conceivably evolved to answer the question I would like to thank the Optometric Extension
“Will it eat me?” Program Foundation for sponsoring my many
In addition to building flexibility of mech­ strabismus seminars around the nation, allowing
anical vergences, therapy should address me to learn from the questions of the hundreds
the localization vergences inspired by EDP. of doctors and therapists who attended.
Both vergence modes are helpful in achieving
alignment and fusion. This paper has considered REFERENCES
one such treatment protocol available to augment 1. 1 Javal É. Manuel théorique et pratique du strabisme.
the reader’s current treatment approach, Paris: Libraire De L’ Academie de Medecine, 1896.
allowing strabismic and non-strabismic patients 2. Worth C. Squint: it’s causes, pathology, and treatment.
London: John Bale, Sons & Danielsson, Ltd., 1903.
alike to thrill at egocentric depth perception.
3. Brock FW. Pitfalls in orthoptic training of squints. Optom
When this perceptual shift occurs, it can Weekly 1941: 32 (42):1185-1189.
be dramatic, even profound. While the shapes 4. Brock FW. Conditioning the squinter to normal visual
of objects often reduce to little more than habit. Optom Weekly 1941: 32 (28): 793-804.
reflections of habitual sight, the shape of space 5. Flax N. A functional approach to strabismus. Transcript
is always novel and, like creative visualization, San Jose Vision Training Seminar 1968, Aug 24-25: 36-37.
invites a leap into liquid visual consciousness. 6. For an excellent summary of Brock’s contributions see
Barry SR. The work & wisdom of Dr. Fredrick W. Brock.
Exploration and fresh, active seeing in the Journal of Behavioral Optometry 2011: 22 (3): 59-63.
moment replace habitual, automatic, stimulus- 7. Brock Fw. Brock technique of strabismus training
response, stale perception carved by repetitive instruction manual. Keystone View Company. https://goo.
action, often in a past too distant to recall. gl/lN6Gs0
In the words of philosopher Alva Noé, 8. Because this paper stresses the art rather than the
science of strabismus treatment, I will use the essay form,
“Consciousness is not something that happens employing the subjective first person rather than the
to us. It is something we do or make. Better: it objective third person.
is something we achieve. Consciousness is more 9. Levi DM, Knill DC, Bavelier D. Stereopsis and amblyopia:
like dancing than it is like digestion.”37 When A mini-review. Vision Res 2015: 114 (9) 17-30.
we escape the enchantment of details bound to
236
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016
10. Vishwanath D. Toward a new theory of stereopsis. 33. Morgan MW. The Maddox classification of vergence eye
Psychological Review 2014: 121 (2) 153. movements. Am J of Optom & Physio Optics 1980: 57(9):
11. Barry SR. Fixing My Gaze: A Scientist’s Journey into Seeing 537-539.
in Three Dimensions. New York: Basic Books, 2009:94. 34. Getzell has described a pervasive disorder that he calls,
12. Moses RA. Adler’s Physiology of the Eye, Fifth Edition. The “tunneling,” which he defines as “a form of exclusive
C.V. Mosby Company, 1970: 672-682. concentration” whose treatment teaches patient’s to
“hang on to space.” The current paper’s EDP would qualify
13. Slotnick, Samantha. Personal correspondence. as “hanging onto space,” and a constricted sphere of
14. Vishwanath D: 152. attention would qualify as tunneling—although “closeting”
15. Burian HM, von Noorden GK. Binocular Vision and Ocular would be closer to the truth. Getzell’s reflections helped
Motility. Saint Louis: The C.V. Mosby Company, 1974:34. inform many of my own explorations into the treatment of
strabismus. See Getzell JH. Tunneling—a pervasive vision
16. Vishwanath D: 151. disorder. Optom Vis Perf 2014: 2(1): 13-16.
17. Schor C. Perceptual-Motor Computational Model of 35. Our initial experience in using the Vivid Vision virtual
Anomalous Binocular Correspondence. Optometry and reality technology in our clinic shows great promise for
Visual Science 2015: 92(5): 544-550. expanding the sphere of attention. Although Vivid Vision
18. For a review of motor factors in binocular correspondence is being marketed primarily for the treatment ofamblyopia,
see, Cook DL. Considering the ocular motor system in the its potential for strabismus therapy appears good, the
treatment of anomalous retinal correspondence. J Am greatest handicap being that the patient’s objective eye
Optom Assoc 1984: 55 (2): 109-117. position cannot be monitored during therapy. Still, the
19. Covariation is a change in correspondence which panoramic virtual world relieves the mismatch generally
accompanies a change in strabismic angle. See Hallden found between dichoptic images and the surrounding
U. Fusional Phenoma in Anomalous Correspondence. physical world. The technology’s greatest benefit, however,
Uppsala, Almquist and wiksells Boktryckeria, 1952: 5-93. comes when the virtual reality headset is removed and
patients find it easier to perceive EDP during the therapies
20. Quoted in Barry Sr. Ibid. detailed in the Natural Stereopsis section of this paper.
21. Brock FW. Visual training—Part III. Optom Weekly 1957: 48 Again, it is not just the instrument, but knowing how to
(20): 977-982. bridge the gap into natural seeing that is important. See
22. Ludlam WM, in Borish IM (Ed). Clinical Refraction, 3rd ed. https://www.seevividly.com.
Chicago: Professional Press, 1970: 1303-1304. 36. Getzell JH. Vision therapy procedures for tunneling.
23. Flax N. Ibid. Optom Vis Perf 2015: 3(4): 303-217.

24. Birnbaum MH. The role of the trainer in visual training. J 37. Noé A. Out of Our Heads: Why You Are Not Your Brain,
Am Optom Assoc 1977: 48 (8): 1035-1039. and Other Lessons from the Biology of Consciousness.
New York: Hill and Wang, 2009: xii.
25. Brock FW. Conditioning the squinter to normal visual
habits. Optom Weekly 1941: 32 (28): 793-804.
26. Brock FW. Conditioning the squinter to normal visual AUTHOR BIOGRAPHY:
habits. Optom Weekly 1941: 32 (29): 819-824. David Louis Cook, OD, FCOVD, FAAO
27. https://goo.gl/JUyzD1 Marietta, Georgia
28. Sunbeck D. Infinity walk. https://goo.gl/jRp7Y4
An Academy diplomate in binocular
29. https://goo.gl/IQIZDX vision and perception, Dr. Cook has
30. von Noorden GK, Compos EC. Binocular Vision and Ocular written on, treated, and lectured about
Motility, Sixth Edition. Saint Louis: Mosby, Inc., 2002:298. strabismus for over 35 years. He is also
the author of When Your Child Struggles,
31. Cook DL. Biomythology: the Skeptic’s Guide to Charles
Visual Fitness, Biomythology, and the novel The Anatomy of
Darwin and the Science of Persuasion. Bloomington,
Blindness.
IN:Authorhouse, 2016: 334-337.
32. Cook DL. Seeing is believing. Vision Dev & Rehab 2016:
2(2): 97-100.

237
Vision Development & Rehabilitation Volume 2, Issue 4 • December 2016

You might also like