You are on page 1of 12

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 1994227,197-209 NUMBEP.

2 (summER 1994)

TOWARD A FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS OF SELF-INJURY


BRIAN A. IWATA, MICHAEL F. DORSEY, KEITH J. SUIFER,
KENNETH E. BAUMAN, AND GINA S. RICHMAN
THE JOHN F. KENNEDY INSTITUTE AND
THE JOHNS HOPKINS UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF MEDICINE

This study describes the use of an operant methodology to assess functional relationships between
self-injury and specific environmental events. The self-injurious behaviors of nine developmentally
disabled subjects were observed during periods of brief, repeated exposure to a series of analogue
conditions. Each condition differed along one or more of the following dimensions: (1) play materials
(present vs absent), (2) experimenter demands (high vs low), and (3) social attention (absent vs
noncontingent vs contingent). Results showed a great deal of both between and within-subject
variability. However, in six of the nine subjects, higher levels of self-injury were consistently associated
with a specific stimulus condition, suggesting that within-subject variability was a function of
distinct features of the social and/or physical environment. These data are discussed in light of
previously suggested hypotheses for the motivation of self-injury, with particular emphasis on their
implications for the selection of suitable treatments.

The description, incidence and damaging effects ing principles (Bachman, 1972; Baumeister &
of self-injury, as well as numerous attempts to con- Rollings, 1976; Frankel & Simmons, 1976; John-
trol it, have been repeatedly documented in the son & Baumeister, 1978; Romanczyk & Goren,
literature. Self-injury is a bizarre and often chronic 1975; Schroeder, Schroeder, Rojahn, & Mulick,
form of aberrant behavior, the etiology of which is 1981; Smolev, 1971). However, some mixed find-
at best poorly understood. It poses serious risks to ings have been noted with almost all of the be-
those who engage in the behavior, and it represents havioral interventions. For example, although a
a formidable challenge to those who are responsible number of studies have shown that the reinforce-
for treating it. ment of incompatible or other behavior (DRI/
Most of the research on self-injury over the past DRO) reduced self-injury (Allen & Harris, 1966;
15 years has focused on discovering means for its Frankel, Moss, Schofield, & Simmons, 1976; Lo-
effective elimination. The greatest success has been vaas, Freitag, Gold, & Kassorla, 1965; Tarpley &
found using methods based on operant condition- Schroeder, 1979), others have reported poor results
with DRO/DRI (Corte, Wolf, & Locke, 1971;
This research was supported in part by Grant #000917- Measel & Alfieri, 1976; Young & Wincze, 1974).
15-0 from the Maternal and Child Health Service. We thank Extinction has been effective in some instances
Michael Cataldo for his support; Pamela Fabry, H. Richard
Waranch and Eric Ward for their valuable input during the (Jones, Simmons, & Frankel, 1974; Lovaas & Sim-
early stages of protocol development; Luis Aguerrevere, Pa- mons, 1969) but not in others (Corte et al., 1971;
tricia Davis, Rebecca Deal, Harvey Jacobs, John Parrish, Myers, 1975), and conflicting findings also have
Belinda Traughber, and Tim Wysocki for their assistance in
conducting the study; and Tom Thompson for his helpful been reported with both timeout (Adams, Klinge,
comments on a previous draft of the manuscript. & Keiser, 1973; Corte et al., 1971; Duker, 1975;
Reprinted from Analysis and Intervention in Develop- Solnick, Rincover, & Peterson, 1977), and over-
mental Disabilities, 1982, Vol. 2 pp. 3-20, with permis-
sion from Elsevier Science Ltd., "rhe Boulevard, Langford correction (Azrin, Gottlieb, Hughart, Wesolowski,
Lane, Kidlington OX5 1GB, United Kingdom. The original & Rahn, 1975; Foxx & Martin, 1975; Harris &
illustrations have been redrawn; the text has not been mod- Romanczyk, 1976; Measel & Alfieri, 1976).
ified. The only treatments that have been consistently
Reprint requests may be addressed to Brian A. Iwata,
Department of Psychology, The University of Florida, effective in treating self-injury are those based on
Gainesville, Florida 32611. punishment in the form of aversive stimulation
197
198 BRIAN A. IWATA et al.

(Birnbrauer, 1968; Corte et al., 1971; Dorsey, For several reasons, very little behavioral research
Iwata, Ong, & McSween, 1980; Sajwaj, Libet, & has focused on the environmental determinants of
Agras, 1974; Tanner & Zeiler, 1975). However, self-injury. First, in light of data from numerous
due to concerns regarding the appropriate and safe sources suggesting that self-injury is a learned phe-
use of "restrictive" or "intrusive" treatments (e.g., nomenon, behavioral researchers and clinicians gen-
ACFMR, 1971), it has been recommended that erally have dismissed the importance of etiology,
punishment be limited to those situations in which since the conditions that are necessary to develop
other interventions have failed (May, Risley, Twar- or maintain a response may be totally unrelated to
dosz, Friedman, Bijou, Wexler et al., 1975). It is the conditions that are sufficient to alter or eliminate
therefore important to conduct research that may it. Second, with respect to the initial development
eventually identify the limiting conditions of the of self-injury, functional analyses have been limited
various treatments for self-injury. It would be es- to animal studies (Holz & Azrin, 1961; Schaeffer,
pecially useful if these conditions were known prior 1970), since experimental attempts to induce self-
to initiating what otherwise might be an arbitrarily injury in humans when it does not already exist
determined and seemingly endless series of inter- would be regarded as unacceptable from the stand-
ventions. point of subject risk/benefit. Third, the apparent
Recent reviews (Carr, 1977; Johnson & Bau- severity of the behavior often suggests the need for
meister, 1978) have suggested that some of the immediate attention, thereby discouraging at-
treatment failures and inconsistencies reported tempts to identify features of the social and physical
throughout the literature may reflect a lack of un- environment that may serve to maintain self-injury
derstanding regarding the variables that either pro- (see Carr, Newsom, & Binkoff, 1976, for a notable
duce or maintain self-injury. In discussing a number exception).
of hypotheses for the motivation of self-injury, Carr Over the past two years, we have been working
(1977) indicated that the behavior may be rein- toward the development and refinement of an op-
forced through extrinsic sources (e.g., through pos- erant methodology whose application might prove
itive reinforcement such as attention, or negative useful in identifying the functional properties of
reinforcement such as the termination of demands), self-injury on a pretreatment basis. This article de-
or that the behavior itself may produce some form scribes and presents the results obtained with our
of intrinsic reinforcement (e.g., sensory stimulation, initial assessment protocol, in which subjects' be-
pain reduction). This conceptualization of self-in- havior was repeatedly observed across several well-
jury as a multiply controlled operant would indicate defined analogue environments. Similar approaches
that no single form of treatment can be expected have been used to examine the effects of physical
to produce consistent positive results, and it sug- aspects of the environment on behaviors such as
gests that one means of selecting a potentially ef- stereotypy (Adams, Tallon, & Stangl, 1980) and
fective treatment would consist of first determining pica (Madden, Russo, & Cataldo, 1980). In the
what events are currently maintaining the behav- present study, environmental events consisted of
ior.I both physical and social manipulations that might
differentially affect the occurrence of self-injury.
I
Punishment would be an exceptional case because its
effectiveness does not depend on its ability to alter a rein-
forcement contingency. Rather, punishment is effective due METHOD
to the fact that its "aversive" properties are sufficient to
overcome whatever source of reinforcement is maintaining Subjects and Setting
the behavior (Azrin & Holz, 1966). Given the types of Nine subjects participated in the study. All
stimuli that typically have been used as punishing events showed some degree of developmental delay, and
(e.g., electric shock, aromatic ammonia), it is not surprising
to find that punishment has been found to be the most were admitted for inpatient evaluation and/or
effective treatment for self-injury. treatment to The John F. Kennedy Institute, a
FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS OF SELF-INJURY 199
Table 1
Demographic Characteristics of Subjects

Sub- Age in Developmental Motor


ject Sex years level involvement Diagnosis Self-injury
I F 46/12 2-3/, yr Normal Mild to moderate mental re- Self-biting, head banging
tardation
2 M 510/12 8-12 mo Spastic cerebral palsy, Congenital rubella syndrome, Eye gouging, head banging
delayed profound mental retarda-
tion, blind, hearing deficit
3 M 13 8-10 mo Normal (restricted by Profound mental retardation, Ear pulling, head banging
arm restraints) Down's syndrome
4 M 68/12 10-15 mo Normal Profound mental retardation, Head banging, head hitting
autistic-like behavior
5 M 131/12 2-3 yr Poor ambulation, ab- Severe to profound mental Face slapping, head banging,
normal gait, delayed retardation, Rubenstein- hand biting
Taybi syndrome
6 M 17/l2 6-9 mo Delayed Developmental delay, cra- Hand mouthing
niosynostosis
7 M 172/12 15-24 mo Mild cerebral palsy, de- Congenital rubella syndrome, Head hitting, head banging,
layed profound mental retarda- arm biting, self-choking,
tion hair pulling
8 M 49/12 2-14 mo Delayed Profound mental retardation, Head hitting, head banging
Down's syndrome
9 M 37/12 6-12 mo Cerebral palsy, left Profound mental retardation Head hitting, head banging
hemiplegia, delayed

pediatric hospital affiliated with TheJohns Hopkins amination by a physician, as well as other diagnostic
University School of Medicine. Interviews and di- consultations (e.g., neurological, audiological, vi-
rect observations conducted prior to admission in- sual). The purpose of the examination was to assess
dicated that each subject exhibited moderate to high current physical status and to rule out organic fac-
rates of self-injurious behavior. Demographic in- tors that might be associated with or exacerbated
formation for each subject is provided in Table 1. by self-injury. Potential subjects who presented an
Sessions were conducted in 3.Om by 3.Om therapy immediate risk of severe physical damage due to
rooms, equipped with tables and chairs, a variety self-injury were not induded in the study. Second,
of games and toys, and either floor carpeting or a each subject's physician recommended a criterion
mat. Each therapy room was adjoined to a 3.Om (expressed in terms of either degree of injury or
by 1.5m observation room via a one-way mirror. level of responding or both) for terminating ob-
servation sessions due to physical risk. Physicians
Human Subjects Protection and nurses observed sessions intermittently in order
In order to assess the differential effects of en- to assess subjects' self-injury as it occurred and, if
vironment on self-injury, the present study required necessary, to modify the criterion. Third, if a sub-
that subjects be allowed to engage in self-injurious ject's physical condition or level of responding met
behavior while free from mechanical, physical or the criterion for terminating a session, (s)he was
chemical restraint. All procedures were reviewed removed from the therapy room, self-injury was
and approved by a human subjects committee, and interrupted via brief physical or mechanical re-
the following safeguards were employed to reduce straint, and a physician or nurse examined the sub-
the risk of physical damage as a function of self- ject and either approved continuation or recom-
injury exhibited during the observation sessions. mended postponement of the sessions. Fourth,
First, each subject received a complete medical ex- following each set of four observation sessions, sub-
200 BRIAN A. IWATA et al.
Table 2
Observer Definitions of Subjects' Self-injury

Response Definition Subjects


Ear pulling & gouging Closure of fingers, fingernails or hand on ear with a pull- 3
ing or digging motion
Eye gouging Any contact of any part of hand within the ocular area 2
Face slapping Forecful contact of the open hand with the face 6
Hair pulling Closure of the fingers and thumb on hair with a pulling 7
motion away from the head
Handmouthing Insertion of one or more fingers into the mouth 6
Head banging Forceful contact of the head with a stationary environ- 1-9
mental object
Head hitting Forceful contact of the hand with any part of the head 4, 5, 7, 9
Neck choking Forceful dosure of both hands around the neck 7
Self-biting Closure of the upper and lower teeth on the flesh of any 1, 5, 7
portion of the body

jects were routinely examined by a nurse who noted listing of the specific self-injurious responses ob-
any changes in physical status as a result of self- served for each subject, along with operational def-
injury. Finally, each subject's case was reviewed at initions used in collecting data.
least weekly in both departmental case conferences During each session, an observer recorded the
and interdisciplinary rounds. occurrence or nonoccurrence of self-injurious be-
In light of the above procedures, it was felt that havior from the observation room during contin-
the degree of risk to which subjects were exposed uous, 10-sec intervals (Powell, Martindale, & Kulp,
was no greater (and perhaps considerably less) than 1975). Interval changeovers were signalled by a
that found in their natural environment. During cassette tape containing pre-recorded prompts. The
the course of the study, subjects often engaged in dependent variable of interest consisted of the per-
self-injury to the extent that minor bleeding or centage of intervals during which one or more self-
swelling occurred; however, at no time did subjects injurious responses were scored, and was calculated
require any medical care due to their self-injury by dividing the number of positively scored inter-
other than routine deaning and/or topical dressing vals by the total number of intervals, and multi-
by a nurse. On three occasions, a session was ter- plying by 100.
minated prematurely for subject 7, due to an ex-
tremely high rate of forceful head banging against Interobserver Agreement
the floor of the observation room. However, self- Two observers independently scored responses
injury was never severe enough to require the ter- during 35% of the sessions (the range for individ-
mination of a session for other subjects, and no uals was 17% to 67%). Overall, occurrence, and
subject was ever exciuded from participation in nonoccurrence reliability percentages were calculat-
sessions due to residual effects of accumulated self- ed on an interval-by-interval basis by dividing the
injury. number of agreements by the number of agree-
ments plus disagreements, and multiplying by 100
Response Definitions and Measurement (Bailey & Bostow, 1979; Hawkins & Dotson,
Observations conducted prior to and upon ad- 1975). Overall, occurrence, and nonoccurrence
mission indicated that all subjects engaged in two agreement averaged 96.8%, 82.8%, and 91.7%,
or more self-injurious topographies, with head respectively. Individual means and ranges for each
banging the most prevalent. Table 2 contains a subject are presented in Table 3. Lower agreement
FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS OF SELF-INJURY 201

Table 3
Interobserver Agreement Data

Percent of
observations
for which
observer Overall Occurrence Nonoccurrence
agreement
Subject was measured X% Range X% Range X% Range
1 26.6 99.8 98-100 84.3 50-100 99.7 97-100
2 67.0 88.0 63-100 80.4 41-100 63.2 27-100
3 32.0 98.8 88-100 95.0 75-100 97.8 83-100
4 17.0 96.3 94-99 86.8 78-95 80.0 39-99
5 25.0 99.7 97-100 93.5 75-100 99.3 98-100
6 17.0 100 100 100 100 100 100
7 61.3 92.4 73-100 72.9 21-100 88.8 61-100
8 45.0 97.0 91-100 85.5 43-100 97.5 90-100
9 30.3 99.2 96-100 46.9 0-100 99.0 95-100

percentages were obtained during sessions in which sign that used a multielement manipulation (Bar-
subjects exhibited either extremely high or extreme- low & Hayes, 1979; Sidman, 1960; Ulman &
ly low levels of responding. Sulzer-Azaroff, 1975). Subject 1, who served as a
pilot, was exposed to three of the four conditions.
Staff Training Eight sessions (two per condition) were conducted
All observers and experimenters who participat- each day, with four sessions occurring in the morn-
ed in the study had previous coursework and ex- ing and four in the afternoon. The order of pre-
perience in the use of behavioral interventions with sentation for each series of four sessions was deter-
developmentally disabled children. In addition, mined by random drawing. Each session lasted for
specific training activities were employed to ensure 15 min, with the exception of the three occasions
that staff could reliably observe behavior and re- noted earlier. For those conditions requiring the
spond appropriately during sessions in which they presence of an experimenter in the room with a
served as an experimenter. Each staff member re- subject, at least three different persons were trained
ceived written instructions describing the observa- to conduct sessions for each subject, and were ro-
tion procedure and experimental protocol. After tated to control for experimenter-specific effects.
reading and reviewing these materials with an ex- Within each series of conditions, experimenters were
perienced staff member, a new staff member was changed between sessions, and subjects were briefly
assigned to conduct informal observations, reli- removed from the room.
ability observations, and primary data observations Social disapproval. The experimenter and sub-
for approximately five sessions each. Persons serving ject entered the therapy room together, where a
as experimenters (i.e., those conducting sessions) variety of toys were available on a table and the
did so only after demonstrating competence as an floor, within easy reach of the subject. The exper-
observer. At least one of the authors was present imenter directed the subject to "play with the toys"
during each session and provided feedback regard- while the experimenter "does some work." If the
ing compliance with the procedures as needed. subject had questionable receptive language or poor
hearing, the experimenter initially placed the sub-
Experimental Conditions ject in physical contact with the toys. The experi-
Eight of the nine subjects were exposed to each menter then sat in a chair across the room and
of four different conditions in an experimental de- assumed the appearance of reading a book or mag-
202 BRIAN A. IWATA et al.

azine. Attention was given to the subject contingent the experimenter repeated the instruction, modeled
upon each episode of self-injury (either a single the correct response, and waited an additional 5
response or a rapid burst of responses), and took sec. If no response occurred at that point, the ex-
the form of statements of concern and disapproval perimenter repeated the instruction and physically
(e.g., "Don't do that, you're going to hurt your- guided the subject through the response, using the
self'; "Look at your hand, don't hit yourself'; least amount of contact necessary to complete it.
etc.), paired with brief physical contact of a non- Appropriate modification and/or elimination in the
punitive nature (e.g., hand on shoulder). All other first two steps occurred for subjects with auditory
responses exhibited by the subject were ignored. or visual deficits. Social praise was delivered upon
This condition was designed to approximate one completion of the response, regardless of whether
type of reinforcement contingency that might main- or not modeling or physical guidance were required,
tain self-injury. In the natural environment, es- and the next trial was begun. Contingent upon the
pecially in institutional settings having low staff- occurrence of self-injury at any time during the
to-client ratios, self-injury often produces much session, the experimenter immediately terminated
emotional behavior and attention from caregivers, the trial and turned away from the subject for 30
while other behavior receives relatively little atten- sec, with an additional 30-sec change-over delay
tion (Frankel & Simmons, 1976; Lovaas et al., for repeated self-injury. While such a consequence
1965; Lovaas & Simmons, 1969; Risley, 1968). for self-injury might resemble an extinction pro-
Thus, statements of concern and social disapproval cedure, it was actually designed to assess whether
paired with physical contact contingent upon self- or not self-injury was maintained through negative
injury may maintain the behavior via inadvertent reinforcement as a result of escaping or avoiding
delivery of positive reinforcement. demand situations (Carr, 1977; Carr et al., 1976;
Academic demand. Educational activities ap- Jones, Simmons, & Frankel, 1974; Measel & Al-
propriate for each subject were selected on the basis fieri, 1976; Wolf, Risley, Johnston, Harris, & Al-
of a special education evaluation conducted upon len, 1967).
admission, or from an individual education pro- Unstructured play. As in the two previous con-
gram plan obtained from the subject's current school ditions, an experimenter and subject were present
or institutional placement. Examples of the edu- in the room. No educational tasks were presented,
cational tasks included: placing plastic rings on a and a variety of toys were available within the
peg, stacking wooden blocks or placing them in a subject's reach. Throughout the session, the exper-
bucket, putting pieces in a wooden puzzle, thread- imenter maintained dose proximity to the subject
ing large plastic beads on a string, grasping and (i.e., within im when both were seated), allowed
holding small objects, and touching various body the subject to engage in spontaneous isolate or co-
parts upon request. The tasks were judged to have operative toy play or to move freely about the room,
a low probability of occurrence, in that subjects and periodically presented toys to the subject with-
never completed them spontaneously. In addition, out making any demands. The experimenter de-
the tasks were apparently difficult for subjects to livered social praise and brief physical contact con-
perform even when physically guided. tingent upon appropriate behavior-the absence of
During the academic session, the experimenter self-injury-at least once every 30 sec. Self-inju-
and subject were seated at a table, and the exper- rious behavior was ignored, unless its severity reached
imenter presented learning trials using a graduated, the point where the session was terminated. This
three-prompt procedure (Homer & Keilitz, 1975; condition served as a control procedure for the
Tucker & Berry, 1980). The experimenter initially presence of an experimenter, the availability of
gave a verbal instruction and allowed the subject potentially stimulating materials, the absence of
5 sec to initiate a response. If, after the 5 sec, the demands, the delivery of social approval for ap-
subject failed to initiate an appropriate response, propriate behavior, and the lack of approval for
FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS OF SELF-INJURY 203
self-injury. Additionally, it was designed to serve jects 4, 7, and 9, respectively, during the Alone
the function of an "enriched environment" (Hor- condition). For this reason, the condition means for
ner, 1980), in which relatively little self-injury might individual subjects are also portrayed graphically
be expected to occur. in standard deviation units above or below a sub-
Alone. The child was placed in the therapy room ject's overall mean. Thus, Figure 1 provides a sum-
alone, without access to toys or any other materials mary of both absolute level and relative variability
that might serve as external sources of stimulation. of subjects' self-injurious behavior.
The purpose of this condition was to approximate Several differences can be seen in the present
a situation that would be considered "impover- data. First, the level of responding varied widely
ished" or "austere" from a social and physical across subjects, with the overall mean percent of
standpoint (Homer, 1980). There is growing evi- intervals of self-injury ranging from a low of 4.5%
dence to suggest that self-stimulatory behavior is (subjects 1 and 9) to a high of 91.2% (subject 6).
motivated through self-produced reinforcement of Second, considerable variability was observed with-
a sensory nature (Rincover, 1978; Rincover, Cook, in subjects across the different experimental con-
Peoples, & Packard, 1979), and it is possible that ditions. The within-subject variability was evident
self-injury may be similarly maintained (Carr, 1977; regardless of a subject's overall level of responding.
Dorsey, Iwata, Reid, & Davis, in press; Favell, For example, subjects 1 and 6, who displayed
McGimsey, & Schell, 1982; Parrish, Aguerrevere, markedly different overall levels of self-injury, both
Dorsey, & Iwata, 1980; Rincover & Devany, 1982). showed variable responding across conditions. Third,
If so, one might expect to observe higher levels of within-subject patterns of responding did not ap-
self-injury in situations where minimal amounts of pear related to the overall level of self-injury. For
stimulation are provided by the environment. example, subjects 3 and 9, both of whom displayed
The above procedures continued until: (1) ap- relatively little self-injury, differed with respect to
parent stability in the level of self-injury was ob- the condition in which self-injury was found to be
served, (2) unstable levels of responding persisted the greatest.
in all conditions for 5 days, or (3) 12 days of sessions In spite of the above differences, the data provide
were completed. The length of subject participation information regarding specific conditions that may
in this study averaged 8 days (range = 4-1 1), affect self-injury, and the results shown in Figure
while the total number of sessions run per subject 1 suggest five general patterns of responding for
averaged 30 (range = 24-53). the present subjects. The first pattern was charac-
terized by a relatively low level of self-injury during
the Unstructured play condition. For all of the eight
RESULTS subjects exposed to this condition (subject 1 was
Figure 1 summarizes the results for the nine exduded), self-injury during Unstructured play was
subjects. For each subject, the numerical data in at or below their overall mean level, and four of
Figure 1 indicate the overall mean percent of in- the subjects (subjects 2, 4, 5, 9) showed less self-
tervals of self-injury and its standard deviation, and injury during Unstructured play when compared
means for the separate experimental conditions. to any of the other conditions. A second pattern
These data allow for an examination of overall was reflected in the data for subjects 4, 6, 7, and
responding between subjects, as well as condition- 9. For these individuals, self-injury was greatest
by-condition comparisons within subjects. How- during the Alone condition, in which access to
ever, in light of the rather large differences observed external sources of stimulation was minimized. This
in subjects' overall level of self-injury, it is difficult pattern is most dearly evident in subject 4's data.
to make condition-by-condition comparisons be- However, subjects 6 and 9, whose overall level of
tween subjects on the basis of absolute data alone self-injury differed considerably, also displayed more
(e.g., 81.3% vs 44.4% vs 8.9% self-injury for sub- self-injury during the Alone condition. A third pat-
204 BRIAN A. IWATA et al.

O~ cadNt So AcacervtSocial Academic


Alone Play Aloe* Pla ! Alone Pl;ay
2- + A- + + ~ ~2. 2 ' -t
, 1- mi 12.3 1* 1A 'I 17.0
gg188 ThP .0
80_8, 77.0
P.N., r1a. 7.
.
.i1.1' R
*'0
0
-' I _..
-1 . -
I/I
Adc-t" 0Z'h'-ml--'.'-u-
O.
{rb04
I

56.3 ' i-1 1.


I

1
.....

-1 -1 '-
4.5|
Si
| =

SD l' o
,
-2 JU SD2.
W71 51 5.6m.5
M-
SD1

81.3 2I 36.7 2- j

/..
1-
,1-
IA. 18.5 99.5
Z 0 Ia
lw 159'
0
F 23.3 87.7 9.5 90-t
<1 /2.
5..
2 1 7.2 -1 7.0 4.5. -1 i

-Ml- 36.1 LMA16.7 -F.1 . 91


S4
!. | s20@ -2.0 au SD-sl 7.6. | 'iSD= 14:6|
c ..I

*t i

/
I .I A

I
;
2e

/A.
244.4 1t '1 -
8.9
U
F .
mm -
20.3
m /m
P-O.o
132
17.6
WI .d% -a.. 1m
22.3 22.2 21.6 2.8 1.10
1-3 2 " - -.

-~ ~ M-29.4 x Mml 7.5 1.50


SI~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~
-2 J A=2
I.
1.
So21@1i l/ ^1 -2 SD n, .SD= 0.2 ( -2 i SD3.86 I

Figure 1. Summary data for subjects 1-9 across experimental conditions. The numbers to the right of each graph show
a subject's overall mean percent of intervals of self-injury and its standard deviation, while the numbers above/below each
bar represent the mean percent of intervals of self-injury per condition. The solid/open bars portray each condition mean
in standard deviation units above/below the subject's overall mean.

tern of results was suggested by the data for subjects curred most often during the Social disapproval
1 and 3. Both of these individuals exhibited little condition. Finally, the data for subjects 2 and 8
or no self-injury during all but one of the condi- showed an undifferentiated pattern, in that they
tions-the High demand situation. Subject 5 ex- exhibited either very high (subject 2) or similar
emplified a fourth pattern in which self-injury oc- (subject 8) amounts of self-injury across two or
FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS OF SELF-INJURY 205
CHILD 1 CHILD 2
100

m
CO
CO
-J

w
I-
z 10 20 30 10 20 30 40
co
0
IL CHILD 4
0 100

80- 80.
z
LU
0
60- 60 -
a-
40 40.

20 20.

J
10 20 30
SESSIONS
Figure 2. Percentage of intervals of self-injury for subjects 1, 2, 4, and 5 across sessions and experimental conditions.

more conditions. Subject 6's data might also be high levels of self-injury across all experimental
considered an example of undifferentiated respond- conditions.
ing merely because he exhibited very high levels of
self-injury across all conditions, even though the
greatest amount was seen during the Alone con-
DISCUSSION
dition. Present results indicate that the occurrence of
Figure 2 presents session-by-session data for four self-injury varies considerably, both between and
subjects whose results are characteristic of different within individuals. More importantly, the data show
response patterns. Subjects 1, 4, and 5 exhibited that within-subject variability is not merely a ran-
higher levels of self-injury during the Academic dom process. In six of the nine subjects, higher
demand, Alone, and Social disapproval conditions, levels of self-injury were consistently associated with
respectively, while subject 2 engaged in relatively a specific stimulus condition. These results provide
206 BRIAN A. IWATA et al.

direct empirical evidence that self-injury may be a approval condition. This finding was rather sur-
function of different sources of reinforcement (Carr, prising in light of the fact that social attention often
1977), a finding that has significant implications has been suggested as a likely source of reinforce-
for treatment. ment for self-injury. However, in situations where
In four of our subjects, self-injury was relatively it can be determined that clients engage in self-
high during the Alone condition, suggesting a form injury for the attention that it produces, extinction
of self-stimulation as a motivational variable. As- (ignoring), timeout, and DRO would seem to be
suming that this analysis is a correct one, knowledge the most effective treatments.
of the specific reinforcing event provided by self- Three of the subjects showed either undifferen-
injury would greatly enhance the effectiveness of tiated patterns or high levels of self-injury across
reinforcement procedures designed to reduce the all stimulus conditions. Although it is impossible
behavior. For example, on several occasions, we to determine what may have accounted for these
have noticed that visually impaired clients engage results, several possibilities appear likely. Each of
in eye-poking that probably intensifies visual stim- these subjects was either quite young or profoundly
ulation. Intervention for such individuals might retarded, and it is possible that the different con-
include the use of bright flashing lights, massage ditions were not dearly discriminable to them. Al-
to the ocular area, etc., that is delivered contingent ternatively, the behavior may have been a function
upon the absence of self-injury, or produced by a of variables that were not controlled in the present
response that is incompatible with self-injury (Fa- study. Finally, self-injury in these individuals may
vell et al., 1982). Alternatively, effective extinction represent a response that serves multiple func-
procedures may not require the withholding of so- tions-providing stimulation when little is avail-
cial consequences but, instead, the elimination or able, producing attention from others, and termi-
attenuation of sensory stimulation derived from the nating undesirable situations. The latter possibility
response (e.g., Dorsey et al., in press; Rincover, is most significant in that it suggests the need for
1978; Rincover et al., 1979; Rincover & Devany, different treatments applied to the same individual,
1982). depending upon the situation in which self-injury
Two subjects exhibited more self-injury during is observed.
the Academic sessions, where the behavior func- Although clear differences were observed in a
tioned to briefly terminate demands made by an majority of our subjects, the present data must be
experimenter. The pattern of behavior shown by regarded as limited in two respects. First, our meth-
these subjects resembles that described by Carr et odology did not control for very subtle aspects of
al. (1976) who were able to reduce self-injury by contingencies that may affect behavior. For ex-
including non-demand periods (reading entertain- ample, assuming that the attention provided during
ing stories to the subject) during a demand con- the Social disapproval condition serves a reinforcing
dition. The use of "guided compliance" trials, in function, the reinforcement is provided on a very
which a client's self-injury is followed by physical frequent basis.2 The Alone condition differed from
assistance in completing the desired academic re- the Social attention condition in at least two re-
sponse and continuation of the session until a per- spects: it not only represented a condition of stim-
formance criterion is reached, might also be effective
in "extinguishing" the negative reinforcement pro-
vided through escape responding. On the other
2
The use of these schedules raises the question of whether
or not procedures in this study could have contributed to
hand, a typical extinction technique-the contin- the development of self-injury in our subjects. Data indicating
gent withdrawal of attention-would be expected an increasing function across time would have suggested that
to strengthen the behavior. learning or acquisition was taking place. However, only the
data for subject 2 showed any increase across sessions, and
Only one subject in the present study showed it can be seen (Figure 2) that this subject's self-injury was
higher levels of self-injury during the Social dis- occurring at high levels during initial sessions.
FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS OF SELF-INJURY 207

ulus deprivation, but also eliminated access to all injury. Both of these features (operational definition
social attention. In light of the fact that an operant of the environment and limited duration) should
response may occur at higher rates during the initial increase the likelihood that the present methodol-
stages of extinction than during a CRF condition, ogy, or one similar to it, could be incorporated into
a high level of self-injury during the Alone con- the design of most intervention research. Procedures
dition might not be maintained by reinforcement for minimizing risks to subjects were also carefully
of a self-stimulatory nature, but by the withdrawal considered, and provide a model for screening and
of social reinforcement. Thus, our methodology monitoring that might be considered essential in
does not isolate conclusively the exact nature of the research of this type. In particular, the independent
contingency responsible for maintaining self-injury, monitoring system was seen as a safeguard to ex-
and we foresee the need for constructing an ex- perimenters as well as subjects, and should be em-
tended series of conditions that progressively ana- ployed whenever possible.
lyzes variables such as reinforcement schedules. A The major focus of the present study was on the
second limitation can be found in the incomplete- identification of variables that are associated with
ness of our analysis. For example, if subjects exhibit (and may serve to maintain) the occurrence of self-
self-injury primarily in demand situations, a re- injury. However, it is important to note that lower
duction of self-injury following a reversal of the levels of self-injury were consistently associated with
apparent contingency operating in that environment the control condition, which included the avail-
(i.e., the elimination of escape as a consequence for ability of toys, the relative absence of demands,
self-injury) would provide stronger evidence that and reinforcement for behavior that was generally
the behavior was, in fact, maintained through neg- incompatible with self-injury. This finding is con-
ative reinforcement. Furthermore, a comparison of sistent with previous data (Homer, 1980) sug-
that technique to one whose use is unrelated to the gesting that physical and social characteristics as-
concept of negative reinforcement for self-injury sociated with an "enriched environment" may
(e.g., timeout, DRO) would provide the ultimate produce a number of beneficial outcomes, including
test of the clinical utility of the assessment procedure reductions in self-injury. In addition to enrichment,
in selecting effective treatments. Although no treat- in cases where individuals exhibit few adaptive be-
ment data are included in this study, all of the haviors, successful treatment of self-injury may in-
present subjects were provided a therapy program clude the active shaping and/or reinforcement of
following the completion of their assessment. Dur- specific appropriate responses, such as toy play
ing the course of that treatment, we have conducted (Favell et al., 1982).
several types of intervention analyses, the results of In summary, the present study offers a meth-
which have been very encouraging in cases where odology for examining the multiple effects of en-
self-injury was dearly differentiated during the as- vironment on the occurrence ofself-injury. Whether
sessment period. or not it will contribute to a more thorough un-
In addition to the above limitations, several dis- derstanding of the etiology of self-injury remains
tinctive features of the present study are worth to be seen.3 However, it is dear that improvements
noting. The use of environments that may not close- are needed in our approach to the treatment of self-
ly resemble naturalistic situations was based on our injury. The present results suggest that it may be
experience that it is often difficult to either precisely
identify or control naturally occurring events related I The present
study does not address the issue of environ-
to self-injury. Also, by using well-defined analogue mental versus physiological determinants of self-injury. How-
environments, it was possible to limit subjects' in- ever, assuming that there may be a physiological basis for
clusion in the study to an amount of time no greater the development or maintenance of self-injury, research of
the present type should suggest ways to reduce the effects of
than that of a typical baseline period, yet provide environmental variance when conducting biobehavioral in-
data on a number of variables that may affect self- vestigations.
208 BRIAN A. IWATA et al.

possible to empirically identify variables that affect Corte, H. E., Wolf, M. M., & Locke, B. J. (1971). A
self-injury prior to implementing lengthy treatment comparison of procedures for eliminating self-injurious
behavior of retarded adolescents. Journal of Applied
conditions. If so, we can no longer afford to conduct Behavior Analysis, 4, 201-213.
clinical research in which the baseline data provide Dorsey, M. F., Iwata, B. A., Ong, P., & McSween, T. E.
information regarding behavior in a single invariant (1980). Treatment of self-injurious behavior using a
water mist: Initial response suppression and generaliza-
situation, or to make treatment decisions based on tion. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 13, 343-
a "best guess" as to what might constitute the most 353.
effective means of intervention. Dorsey, M. F., Iwata, B. A., Reid, D. H., & Davis, P. A.
(in press). Protective equipment: Continuous and con-
tingent application in the treatment of self-injurious be-
REFERENCES havior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis.
Duker, P. (1975). Intra-subject controlled time-out (social
Accreditation Council for Facilities for the Mentally Retarded. isolation) in the modification of self-injurious behavior.
(1971). Standards for residential facilities for the Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, 19, 107-112.
mentally retarded. Chicago: Joint Commission on Ac- Favell, J. E., McGimsey, J. F., & Schell, R. M. (1982).
creditation of Hospitals. Treatment of self-injury by providing alternative sensory
Adams, G. L., Tallon, R. J., & Stangl, J. M. (1980). En- activities. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental
vironmental influences on self-stimulatory behavior. Disabilities, 2, 83-104.
American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 85, 17 1-17 5. Foxx, R. M., & Martin, E. D. (1975). Treatment of scav-
Adams, K. M., Klinge, V., & Keiser, T. W. (1973). The enging behavior (coprophagy and pica) by over-correc-
extinction of a self-injurious behavior in an epileptic tion. Behavior Research and Therapy, 13, 153-162.
child. Behavior Research and Therapy, 11, 351-356. Frankel, F., Moss, D., Schofield, S., & Simmons,J. Q. (1976).
Allen, K. E., & Harris, F. R. (1966). Elimination of a Case study: Use of differential reinforcement to suppress
child's excessive scratching by training the mother in self-injurious and aggressive behavior. Psychological Re-
reinforcement procedures. Behavior Researrh and Ther- ports, 39, 843-849.
apy, 4, 79-84. Frankel, F., & Simmons, J. Q. (1976). Self-injurious be-
Azrin, N. H., Gottlieb, L., Hughart, L., Wesolowski, M. havior in schizophrenic and retarded children. American
D., & Rahn, T. (1975). Eliminating self-injurious be- Journal of Mental Deficiency, 80, 512-522.
havior by educative procedures. Behavior Research and Harris, S. L., & Romanczyk, R. G. (1976). Treating self-
Therapy, 13, 101-111. injurious behavior of a retarded child by over-correction.
Azrin, N. H., & Holz, W. C. (1966). Punishment. In W. Behavior Therapy, 7, 235-239.
K. Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior: Areas of research Hawkins, R. P., & Dotson, V. A. (1975). Reliability scores
and application. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. that delude: An "Alice in Wonderland" trip through the
Bachman, J. A. (1972). Self-injurious behavior: A behav- misleading characteristics of interobserver agreement scores
ioral analysis. Journal ofAbnormal Psychology, 80, 211- in interval recording. In E. Ramp & G. Semb (Eds.),
224. Behavior analysis: Areas of research and application.
Bailey, J. S., & Bostow, D. E. (1979). Research methods Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.
in applied behavior analysis. Tallahassee, FL: Copy Gra- Holz, W. C., & Azrin, N. H. (1961). Discriminative
fix. properties of punishment. Journal of the Experimental
Barlow, D. H., & Hayes, S. C. (1979). Alternating treat- Analysis of Behavior, 4, 225-232.
ments design: One strategy for comparing the effects of Horner, R. D. (1980). The effects of an environmental
two treatments in a single subject. Journal of Applied 'enrichment" program on the behavior of institution-
Behavior Analysis, 12, 199-210. alized profoundly retarded children. Journal of Applied
Baumeister, A. A., & Rollings, J. P. (1976). Self-injurious Behavior Analysis, 13, 473-49 1.
behavior. In N. R. Ellis (Ed.), International review of Horner, R. D., & Keilitz, I. (1975). Training mentally
research in mental retardation. New York: Academic retarded adolescents to brush their teeth. Journal of Ap-
Press. plied Behavior Analysis, 8, 301-309.
Birnbrauer, J. (1968). Generalization of punishment ef- Johnson, W. L., & Baumeister, A. (1978). Self-injurious
fects-A case study. Journal ofApplied Behavior Anal- behavior: A review and analysis of methodological details
ysis, 1, 201-211. of published studies. Behavior Modification, 2, 465-
Carr, E. G. (1977). The motivation of self-injurious be- 484.
havior: A review of some hypotheses. Psychological Bul- Jones, F. H., Simmons, J. Q., & Frankel, F. (1974). An
letin, 84, 800-816. extinction procedure for eliminating self-destructive be-
Carr, E. G., Newsom, C. D., & Binkoff, J. A. (1976). havior in a 9-year-old autistic girl. Journal of Autism
Stimulus control of self-destructive behavior in a psy- and Childhood Schizophrenia, 4, 214-250.
chotic child. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 4, Lovaas, 0. I., Freitag, G., Gold, V. J., & Kassorla, I. C.
139-152. (1965). Experimental studies in childhood schizophre-

You might also like