You are on page 1of 11

Rhythm in Music: What is it? Who has it? And Why?

Author(s): John Bispham


Source: Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, Vol. 24, No. 2 (December 2006), pp.
125-134
Published by: University of California Press
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1525/mp.2006.24.2.125 .
Accessed: 17/11/2015 17:19

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

University of California Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Music
Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 23.235.32.0 on Tue, 17 Nov 2015 17:19:28 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Rhythm in Music 125

R HYTHM IN M USIC : W HAT IS IT ? W HO HAS IT ? A ND W HY ?

J OHN B ISPHAM (sometimes termed “analogous”) behaviors in the


Leverhulme Centre for Evolutionary Studies/ ethological literature (Fitch, in press; McDermott &
Centre for Music and Science, Hauser, 2005) and have attempted to account for the
University of Cambridge, United Kingdom adaptive strength of rhythm and entrainment in
the course of human evolution with reference to
mother–infant interaction (Dissanayake, 2000), coali-
THIS ARTICLE EXPLORES HUMAN rhythmic abilities and tion signaling (Hagen & Bryant, 2003; Merker, 2000)
behaviors within a framework of evolutionary theory and muscular bonding (McNeill, 1995). Furthermore,
highlighting the need for research in this area to be the principal proponents of these theories have tried to
grounded upon solid psychologically valid definitions rationalize an increased evolutionary drive toward these
of rhythm. A wide-ranging cross-species comparison of skills in humans by linking their hypothesis to increased
rhythmic or quasi-rhythmic behaviors is presented with human infant altriciality, female exogamy, or sociality.
a view to exploring possible homologies and homo- However, none of the sources cited give clear, psycho-
plasies to rhythm in human music. Sustained musical logically and behaviorally grounded, definitions of
pulse and period correction mechanisms are put for- rhythm and entrainment in music and are thus
ward as human-specific and music-specific traits. restricted in terms of the precise knowledge that can be
Finally hypotheses as to why these abilities may have inferred from the supporting evidence. The central aim
been selected for—and uniquely selected for—in the of this article is to identify, with reference to the avail-
course of human evolution are explored. able rhythm perception and performance literature,
those features of human rhythmic behavior that are
Received January 31, 2006, accepted August 22, 2006 shared with other animal and human behaviors and
those (if any) that are specific to—and thus key defin-
Key words: rhythm, ethology, evolution, entrainment, ing features of—musical rhythm and are hence likely to
action, emotion have evolved expressly for music.
Drawing upon psychological and neurophysiologi-
cal literature, I have suggested (Bispham, 2003) that
musical rhythmic behavior (MRB) be viewed as a con-

I
NTEREST IN THE EVOLUTION of human musical stellation of concurrently operating, hierarchically
capabilities and behaviors has increased and broad- organized, subskills including general timing abilities,
ened in recent years and has been the subject of smooth and ballistic movement (periodic and nonperi-
numerous and wide-ranging publications (e.g., Balter, odic), the perception of pulse, a coupling of action and
2004; Cross & Morley, in press; Wallin, Merker, & perception, and error correction mechanisms. Indeed, a
Brown, 2000). Existing evolutionary and comparative review of current evidence appears to support the idea
perspectives on rhythm and pulse in music have that all of these subskills share overlapping internal
pointed to possible homologous1 or homoplasic2 oscillatory mechanisms. As such all subskills, as well as
the resulting constellation, can be viewed as being
grounded in, and as having exaptively evolved from,
1
Homologous traits are present in two or more species due to fundamental kinesthetic abilities and modes of perceiv-
common descent. They may not be exactly the same but are inherited
from a common ancestor.
ing temporally organized events. I claim that viewing
2
MRB in this way provides an avenue for exploring use-
Homoplasic traits are similar traits that were not present in a ful comparisons with other types of human and animal
common ancestor and hence evolved independently. However,
correlations to other environmental or behavioural features can behavior as well as critically pointing toward putative
suggest parallel selection pressures that favour the evolution of any evolutionary continuities. Furthermore, this approach
similar trait. mirrors influential proposals for the study of the evolution

Music Perception VOLUME 24, ISSUE 2, PP. 125–134, ISSN 0730-7829, ELECTRONIC ISSN 1533-8312 © 2006 BY THE REGENTS OF THE UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA . ALL
RIGHTS RESERVED. PLEASE DIRECT ALL REQUESTS FOR PERMISSION TO PHOTOCOPY OR REPRODUCE ARTICLE CONTENT THROUGH THE UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA PRESS ’ S
RIGHTS AND PERMISSIONS WEBSITE , HTTP :// WWW. UCPRESSJOURNALS . COM / REPRINT I NFO. ASP. DOI: MP.2006.24.2.125

This content downloaded from 23.235.32.0 on Tue, 17 Nov 2015 17:19:28 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
126 J. Bispham

of language (Hauser, Chomsky, & Fitch, 2002) and fits that are differentiable on the basis of their “rhythm
perfectly the predominant view in evolutionary theory class.”3 Hauser and McDermott (2003) draw attention
that complex behaviors such as music evolved in a to the important comparative point that “although
mosaic fashion, with individual components emerging human infants may be equipped with a capacity to dis-
or evolving independently or for independent reasons criminate languages on the basis of rhythmic cues, the
at times, and/or reforming with other components at presence of this ability in nonhuman primates that lack
other times. The following cross-species and intra- language suggests that it evolved for more general audi-
species comparative perspective, by focusing on indi- tory purposes” (p. 667). A more recent study shows that
vidual behavioral components, is implicitly set within cotton-top tamarins, like newborn babies (e.g., Ramus,
this constellation framework. 2002) and in contrast to 5-month-old infants (Nazzi,
Jusczyk, & Johnson, 2000), do not differentiate between
Periodic Production languages from within rhythmic classes (Tincoff et al.,
2005) supporting the idea that differentiation is based
In the introduction to The Origins of Music (Wallin on rhythmic cues. Additionally, similar results have
et al., 2000), the editors state that “most animals been found in experiments with trained rats, raising the
(including humans) have the ability to move in a possibility that the mechanism underlying our capacity
metric, alternating fashion” (p. 11). They contend that to discriminate languages based on rhythmic cues may
“what is special about humans is not their capacity to have evolved early within the mammalian clade (Toro,
move rhythmically but their ability to entrain their Trobalon, & Sebastián-Gallés, 2003).
movements to an external timekeeper, such as beating a While these studies are of interest to the perception of
drum” (p. 12). While this may well be correct it does not temporally structured events in humans their relevance
go far enough in distinguishing movements that are to rhythm perception in music is limited. Most impor-
metric and alternating from those that might be consid- tantly the rhythmic properties of language and music,
ered to be “rhythmic.” Crucially, musical rhythmic pro- despite evidence for some overlap (e.g., Patel & Daniele,
duction implies reference to an external timekeeper 2003), are not the same. Hauser and McDermott (2003)
and/or to an internally created and volitionally con- effectively argue for a continuum in which “music
trolled attentional pulse (e.g., Drake & Bertrand, 2001). places more focus on beat and timing of particular
As such, periodic production in nonhuman species can notes and language focuses on the overall frequency
only be considered as being homologous (and of direct contours and durations of particular phonemic clus-
relevance) to musical behaviors if its behavioral corre- ters” (p. 667). However, they do not address the possi-
lates are not explicable solely in terms of biomechanical bility that fundamental differences between rhythm in
efficiencies. Nevertheless, the difficulty of delineating language and music exist. Distinct differences are
the mechanisms underlying periodic behaviors adds arguably manifest in the particular nature of musical
support to the case for a physiologically grounded pulse where highly regular attentional pulses are sus-
approach to rhythmic behaviors and offers a possible tained over time and are mutually manifest to multiple
avenue for comparatively exploring timing mechanisms individuals. These points will be discussed in more
involved in motor output and rhythmic behaviors. detail below, but it is certainly the case that experiments
Changes to the human motor system, and particularly comparing human and primate rhythmic and tempo
the move toward bipedalism over evolutionary time, discrimination with musical materials could provide
have undoubtedly had profound impacts on rhythmic more insight in the future.
capabilities (e.g., Trevarthen, 1999). However, they are
not (contra Mithen, 2005) in themselves sufficient to Temporal Structuring in Communication
account for MRB in our species as they crucially do not
address the fact that rhythmic behavior implies an According to Owings and Morton’s (1998) model, ani-
ordering of output with reference to a sustained atten- mal communication occurs as a result of individuals
tional pulse (see Bispham, 2006). managing and assessing signals for the purpose of regu-
lating the behavior of others. Importantly, managers
Rhythm Perception and assessors (elsewhere—e.g., Seyfarth & Cheney,

Ramus, Nespor, and Mehler (1999) showed that, like 3


Differentiation is based on the proportion of time occupied by
human newborns, cotton-top tamarins have the capac- vowels and appears to concur with classical taxonomies of language
ity to discriminate between sentences from languages (e.g., Cutler, 1994).

This content downloaded from 23.235.32.0 on Tue, 17 Nov 2015 17:19:28 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Rhythm in Music 127

2003—”senders” and “receivers”) need not display (Arcadi, Robert, & Boesch, 1998; Arcadi, Robert, &
any awareness of their own or other’s intentionalities, Mugurusi, 2004).
but can simply be predisposed, through processes of Whether or not “song”—defined as complex learned
evolution and conditioning, to behave in certain vocalization—in songbirds (Marler, 2000), cetaceans
ways. In this model signals are seen to be indicative (e.g., humpback whales; Payne, 2000), and pinniped
of an animal’s internal state and endow their species (e.g., Sjare, Stirling, & Spencer, 2003; Van Parijs,
actions with a degree of predictability through 2003) can be considered as analogies to human song is
their “motivational-structural” significance (deriv- currently under debate in the literature. McDermott
ing from the correlations between the types of signals and Hauser (2005) argue that human and animal song
emitted and the motivational states of individuals). are neither homologous or homoplasic on the basis that
For the purposes of this discussion temporal struc- animal song occurs only under highly restricted con-
turing in communication is marked by aspects where texts of courtship or territorial defense; that song is
the temporal structure of events acts as a commu- functional in these contexts and never occurs for pure
nicative signal according to Owings and Morten’s enjoyment or the enjoyment of others; and that in most
model. Repeated signaling in itself is thus not consid- singing species only males sing and show unique neural
ered relevant if it is explicable in terms of redun- adaptations for song. Fitch (2006) counters this pro-
dancy. Further caution is also required as what may posal, arguing that analogy is a property of mechanisms
appear to be temporal patterning may simply be the not dependent upon context or inferred adaptive func-
epiphenomenal outcome of conflicting signaling tion; contrasting “pure enjoyment” (a proximate causal
functions (see Owings and Morton’s, 1998, discus- explanation) with communicative function (an ulti-
sion of Tungara frogs, pp. 6–7). mate adaptive explanation) is to conflate two separate
One way in which temporal structuring is functional levels of biological explanation (Tinbergen, 1963); and
in animal communication is that distinctive temporal that sex-specificity of a trait within a species need not
patterns can be used as a source of individuality for disqualify it from analogy with a similar sex-shared trait
sounds not individually marked by the vocal tract. in other species. Fitch concludes that “none of
Screams in rhesus macaques are a well-studied exam- [McDermott and Hauser’s] arguments provide com-
ple. They have clear communicative functionality and, pelling grounds for rejecting the traditional analogy
although the nature of the scream production results in between human and animal song.” (p.184)
a loss of timbral individuality (Rendall, Owren, & Although Fitch’s arguments are valid, they do not
Rodman, 1998), individuals have been shown to provide evidence for song being analogous to human
respond differentially depending on the identity of the song. The range of behaviors mentioned raise impor-
caller (Ghazanfar, Flombaum, Miller, & Hauser, 2001). tant and interesting issues regarding possible analogies
Other examples of temporal structuring in communi- to vocal learning, modes of perceiving temporally
cation that have been viewed as relevant to human structured events and sequencing of complex motor
music are animal “drumming” and “song.” A range of actions. Nevertheless, it is doubtful if any of the above
species including palm cockatoos (Wood, 1988), wood- behaviors can be considered fully analogous to MRB in
peckers (Dodenhoff, Stark, & Johnson, 2001), and kan- humans. Importantly, all of these behaviors are not
garoo rats (Randall, 1997) exhibit “drumming” on only contextually distinct but also distinct in terms of
hollow objects in their environments in communica- the mechanisms involved. No evidence exists demon-
tive, territorial or mate attraction contexts. However, strating that actions are guided with reference to an
bimanual drumming in gorillas (Schaller, 1963), chim- internally manifest pulse and as such the temporal fea-
panzees (Goodall, 1986), and bonobos (unpublished tures can be explained as resulting simply from biome-
data mentioned in Fitch, in press) are putatively of even chanical characteristics or from learned and stored
greater relevance due to phylogenetic proximity and motor output sequences. Furthermore, these behaviors
possible homologies to human drumming in terms of display no relationship to the temporal structure of
the motor skills employed. Interestingly in chim- external stimuli nor coordination between the
panzees, where drumming is exhibited predominantly actions—overt or perceptual—of two or more individ-
on buttress roots by the dominant male in displays that uals based on a mutually manifest and regularly struc-
are augmented by vocal pant-hooting, aspects of the tured temporal framework. In other words two
drumming have been correlated with resting periods features that are critical to MRB—entraining mecha-
and future direction in group foraging, and have been nisms and interpersonal interaction—appear to be
shown to be both individually and culturally distinct absent in the examples given.

This content downloaded from 23.235.32.0 on Tue, 17 Nov 2015 17:19:28 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
128 J. Bispham

Ecological Engagement These duets are generally thought to be involved in ter-


ritorial advertisement and strengthening of pair bonds.
An entraining process has been proposed as the root or Existing rationales for gibbon duetting include the
facilitator of human and animal temporal perception advertising of pair strength to potential competitors, as
(Jones, 1976; Jones & Boltz, 1989). According to this well as the hypothesis that pair bonds involve a degree
view, internal oscillatory mechanisms are attuned to of learning and time investment and, henceforth,
external cues allowing us to build expectations for the increase the cost of partner desertion (Geissmann &
timing of future events (future-directed attending) and Ordeldinger, 1999). This is supported by the observa-
to interact efficiently with our environment. This per- tion that duetting becomes increasingly well coordi-
spective has considerable theoretical value and its rele- nated over time and by one case documenting a certain
vance to rhythm in music is well established (e.g., Large & amount of adjustment in the duet structure with a part-
Jones, 1999). Indeed, the likely existence of internal oscil- ner exchange. Although as yet not understood, the
latory mechanisms that are shared between different mechanisms used to achieve coordination between
domains of human behavior and cognition strongly sug- individuals are clearly not fully analogous to MRB in
gests that entrainment in music constitutes an evolution- humans with no evidence of a pulsed framework being
ary exaptation of more generally functional mechanisms employed. However, observation of the duets does sug-
for future-directed attending to temporally structured gest that, as is the case in human music, strong relation-
events (Bispham, 2003). Nevertheless there are crucial ships exist between the rate of hoots, levels of
additional features that must be accounted for in respect kinesthetic movement and degrees of emotional excite-
of musical rhythmic behaviors. These include the cre- ment in both animals. As such it could be argued that
ation of a mutually manifest interactive framework for the pairs are interactively affecting each other’s emo-
communication based upon a sustained “musical” pulse, tional and physiological state within a context of social
period correction mechanisms, and a coupling of action bonding. This in itself marks these duets as being par-
with perception. These will be discussed in the section on ticularly relevant and partially analogous to MRB in
musical rhythmic behaviors. humans.

Temporally Structured Duetting Interactions Synchronous Chorusing

In about 6% of bird species pairs combine to produce While impressive group coordination is displayed in,
coordinated duets. In some species two birds coordi- for example, schools of fish, periodically structured
nate alternating calls with such precision that it is group synchrony of sound and/or action is rare in the
impossible to tell that two birds are involved from the animal kingdom. Documented cases include biolumi-
auditory signal alone (e.g., plain wren; Mann, Marshall- nescent flashing in fireflies, claw waving in fiddler crabs,
Ball & Slater, 2003). Duetting is most common in the and “chorusing” in Neotropical katydid and some frog
tropics and is thought to relate to the fact that birds species (see Greenfield, 1994). In all cases the synchrony
there frequently hold year-round territories and use the is based on either sight or sound, is achieved either by
duets as a form of territorial advertisement (Farabaugh, advanced signaling or through phase correction mech-
1982). This in turn is associated with birds that form anisms, and occurs only within a narrow temporal
long-term monogamous pair bonds. Another feature range. The context is always that of male sexual display
that may be associable with duetting is sexual with synchronicity either being an indirect outcome of
monomorphism (similarity between the sexes). all participants desiring to signal first, a cooperative
Whether or not the coordination is achieved by entrain- effort to maximize output and hence attract females to
ment or, for example, synchronous commencing of their group, or as a means of predator avoidance.
fixed action patterns is not known but presents an Merker (2000) argues that, as periodic group syn-
interesting avenue for future research. chronicity is restricted to male sexual display in the ani-
A particularly interesting case is pant hoot duets in mal kingdom, it is likely to have played a similar role at
gibbons (Hylobates spp.). Geissmann (2000, p. 103) some point in recent (from 6 to 7 million years ago)
explains that “Gibbons produce loud song bouts that human evolution and supports this view with reference
are mostly exhibited by mated and monogamous pairs. to female exogamy in humans and chimpanzees.
Typically, mates combine their partly sex-specific reper- However, this view does not sufficiently account for a
toire in relatively rigid, precisely timed, and complex lack of contextual, physiological, or behavioral correla-
vocal interaction to produce well-patterned duets.” tions between the examples given and synchronized

This content downloaded from 23.235.32.0 on Tue, 17 Nov 2015 17:19:28 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Rhythm in Music 129

behaviors in humans. In contrast to MRB in humans, are unable to entrain their actions to a pulsed signal
correction mechanisms in the animal examples are (McAuley, Jones, Johnstone, & Miller, 2006) suggests
restricted to phase correction mechanisms to individu- that (a) precedes (b) ontogenetically (and possibly
ally specific quasi-eigenfrequencies with no evidence phylogenetically) and is psychologically and/or physi-
for period correction (see below for a discussion on ologically less complex. Effectively, there are features
these correction mechanisms), the temporal structure of rhythm in music that are contextually and mecha-
of signaling is exclusively periodic (i.e., no temporal nistically distinct and hence cannot be explained as
structuring around the base periodicity), and is, in all having evolved with relation exclusively to nonmusical
cases, restricted to a single modality. Additionally the behaviors.
context of male sexual display does not correlate to the
breadth of contexts in which MRB is evident in Musical Rhythmic Behavior
humans.
Based on the discussions leading up to this point and
Nonmusical Human Interaction with reference to the available psychological and neuro-
physiological literature in this area I propose two inter-
Musicality is part of a natural drive in human sociocul- connected features of MRB that are both human-specific
tural learning which begins in infancy. (Trevarthen, and music-specific: musical pulse and period correction.
1999/2000, p. 194) As such they can be viewed as having evolved specifically
for music and for reasons that are not wholly explicable
In general terms, rhythmic behaviors and abilities through analogy to other human or animal behaviors.
pervade all human social interactions; regularities
combined with social knowledge provide a mutually Musical Pulse
manifest framework for interaction. Furthermore,
correlations between aspects of temporal structuring Although examples of unpulsed music do exist (see
in music and language (Patel & Daniele, 2003) sug- Clayton, 1996) and may seem to contradict the exis-
gest some overlap in the mechanisms employed. One tence of pulse being a universal feature of music, we can
way of looking at this is to postulate that interper- safely state that pulse is a highly salient feature of musi-
sonal entrainment is the key rhythmic feature in cal experience worldwide. Arom (1991) goes as far as to
human interactions. According to this view interper- define music as “a succession of sounds capable of giv-
sonal entrainment is a multifaceted and prevalent ing rise to a segmentation of time during which it flows
feature of all human interactions ranging along a in isochronous units . . . there can only be music inas-
spectrum from (a) a loose, subconscious use of pulse much as it is measured and ‘danceable.’” As described
as a framework for interpersonal/turn-taking interac- above, an internally generated and/or externally guided
tions in, for example, mother–infant or linguistic attentional pulse is a well-modeled and widely accepted
interactions (e.g., Cutler, 1994; see Clayton, Sager, & feature of temporal perception in which perceived reg-
Will, 2004) with deviations from expectancy used for ularities build expectations as to the timing of future
affective/communicative purposes to (b) a strict events (Jones, 1976). Musical pulse, however, would
adherence to pulse (groove) in group behavior and appear to be distinct in that it is maintained over time
synchronicity of output where participants are aware and is perceived unambiguously, or at related hierarchi-
of the pulse framework and desire to maintain a cal levels (London, 2004), by enculturated individuals
degree of temporal stability and group-coordination (Stobart & Cross, 2000). Furthermore, the production
(e.g., music and dance). An alternative possibility is or perception of a musical pulse involves engagement of
that the appearance of pulse in nonmusical interac- the motor system in a particular way that enables an
tion does not depend on entrainment mechanisms individual, at least potentially, to manage both fine and
similar to those employed in music and is the result of gross temporal control in ballistic and smooth move-
organizing actions in relation to short-term and con- ments (Janata & Grafton, 2003; Thaut, McIntosh, &
stantly interrupted pulses and expectancies based on Rice, 1997). Production and/or entrainment to a musi-
temporal cues and experience. Whichever interpreta- cal pulse putatively involve internal periodic oscillatory
tion turns out to be correct, contrasting evidence that mechanisms overlapping with motor-coordination
infants perceive pulse (Hannon & Johnson, 2005) and (Bispham, 2003) and provide a mechanism to affect and
interact with reference to temporal regularities regulate levels of physiological arousal (Husain,
(Malloch, 1999/2000) but that children as old as four Thompson, & Schellenberg, 2002).

This content downloaded from 23.235.32.0 on Tue, 17 Nov 2015 17:19:28 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
130 J. Bispham

Period Correction The concluding sections discuss hypotheses concern-


ing the possible functionality of MRB in human evolu-
Sensorimotor synchronization is of paramount impor- tion. While largely conjectural they are intended to
tance to MRB and requires—even in cases of synchro- exploit the strength of an evolutionary perspective in
nizing with strictly periodic signals—some form of highlighting gaps in our understanding and in guiding
corrective mechanism without which timing errors due us toward productive areas for future research.
to internal timekeeping and motor variance (Wing &
Kristofferson, 1973) would accumulate and lead to a
Why?
loss of synchrony (Hary & Moore, 1985). Furthermore,
musical pulse is subject to interactive involuntary fluc-
A wealth of persuasive arguments for music being an
tuations due to motor variance, features of individual
adaptive trait (Huron, 2001), strong evidence against a
style (Collier & Collier, 2002) as well as deliberate
sexual selection hypothesis (Brown, 2004), the fact that
expressive and structurally motivated modulations of
musical pulse and period correction cannot be
tempo and microtimings (e.g., Iyer, 2002; Palmer,
accounted for by theories relating to mother–infant
1997). On the basis of over a century and a quarter of
interactions (e.g., Dissanayake, 2000), the inherently
timing data, as well as more recent neuroimaging stud-
interactive and social nature of period correction as
ies (e.g., Stephan et al., 2002), two interacting correc-
well as the social contextualization (Gregory, 1997) and
tion mechanisms are widely accepted to be
social embeddedness (Tolbert, 2001) of music strongly
independently operational in music: phase correction
suggest that music (and subsequently MRB) is (or was)
and period correction (see Repp, 2005). In short, phase
functional at the group level and in group ceremony. A
correction adjusts for asynchronies between the last
hypothesis of this nature with regard to music generally
response and stimulus events assuming an unchanged
was proposed by Roederer (1984), who states that “the
period whereas period correction modifies the next tar-
role of music in superstitious or sexual rites, religion,
get interval on the basis of discrepancies between the
ideological proselytism and military arousal clearly
timekeeper interval and the last or last few interstimu-
demonstrates the value of music as a means of estab-
lus intervals, thus altering the period of the attentional
lishing behavioral coherency in masses of people.”
musical pulse. By enabling an interactive and sustained
Cross (2001) builds on this by arguing that music’s
coupling of overt and/or covert action and perception,
ubiquity and efficacy in encounters with the numinous
these two mechanisms form the basic temporal frame-
are best accounted for by reference to proto-music’s
work for real-time interpersonal musical behaviors.
“floating intentionality”4 (see Cross, 2005). With refer-
Phase correction mechanisms can be supposed to be
ence to Sperber’s (1996) notion of relevant mysteries,5
operational in all activities involving future-directed
Cross explains that music, like religious ideas, is distin-
attending where expectations are constantly updated
guished from everyday beliefs by its paradoxicality and
based upon asynchronies between attentional pulses
relevance, by its broad applicability and ambiguity. He
and stimulus events. In contrast period correction is
concludes that “by virtue of these attributes music may
almost by definition functional specifically within the
thus be particularly appropriate as a means of amplify-
framework of a sustained musical pulse. As Repp (2004)
ing, exemplifying or reinforcing in the course of ongo-
states, “It is likely that period correction is a specifically
ing experience just these attributes of belief that are
human ability [and] is a manifestation of the more gen-
interpretable as religious; music’s indeterminacy may
eral human ability to set the tempo of a rhythmic activ-
suit it for use as a means of pursuing and perhaps even
ity at will.” Importantly, and in contrast to phase
parsing the numinous” (p. 37). Crucially the creation of
correction, which seems to represent independent
collective alternate realities or beliefs is prospectively
processes of largely automatic action control, period
correction is facilitated by or incurs conscious aware-
4
ness of the tempo change and can thus be interpreted as The key point that Cross is making by ascribing transposable
a representation of intentional cognitive control (Repp, intentionality to music is that meaning in music is both culturally
constructed and individually flexible (depending on, for example,
2001). This is supported by a recent study into the experience, social status or mood). In other words, it retains a high
effects of intention, attention and awareness, on adap- level of ambiguity and can mean different things to different people
tation to tempo changes in sensorimotor synchroniza- while remaining collectively meaningful.
tion (Repp & Keller, 2004), which shows that period 5
Defined as situations where beliefs or mental representations arise
correction is strongly dependent on all three variables which are contradictory but are each separately related to (and hence
whereas phase correction depended only on intention. relevant in respect of) other mental representations and beliefs.

This content downloaded from 23.235.32.0 on Tue, 17 Nov 2015 17:19:28 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Rhythm in Music 131

functional at the group level by endowing the actions of ticular primate species) appear to be not able or moti-
others with a degree of predictability (Shennan, 2002). vated to employ musical pulse or period correction
A challenge for future research will be modeling the mechanisms? One possibility is that the adaptive strength
distinct functionality of rhythm in these contexts. or functioning of MRB is dependent on other species-
Arguably the most likely hypothesis is that MRB is pri- specific characteristics. These may include the ability to
marily rooted in providing a temporal framework, col- sustain attention to events—active or imaginary—and
lective emotionality, a feeling of shared experience, and volitionally control actions (Norman & Shallice, 1980)
cohesiveness to group activities and ritualistic cere- the ability to rehearse events in working memory
monies. Effects of tempo on arousal levels (Husain (Baddeley, 1997); joint intentionality and the ability
et al., 2002), the consistent use of music in altering and/or motivation to manage the cognitive environment
states (Thayer, Newman, & McLain, 1994), and the clear of others in communication (Sperber & Wilson, 1986;
relationship between rhythmic behaviors and physical Tomasello, Carpenter, Call, Behne, & Moll, 2005); and
action (Janata & Grafton, 2003) suggest that musical complex emotions. Alternatively, increased infant altri-
pulse is functional in regulating emotions and motiva- ciality, expanding group size, and increased sociality in
tional states by means of affecting states of action- the hominine lineage (Dunbar, 1996; Joffe, 1997) may
readiness (Frijda, 1986; Frijda & Zeelenberg, 2001). A have uniquely provided selection pressures toward the
crucial point to make at this stage is that, contrary to the use of affective and contingent modes of communica-
focus of research undertaken in the area of music and tion, providing a necessary stepping-stone to MRBs.
emotion (e.g., Juslin & Sloboda, 2001), music can be
viewed as being functional in regulating emotions and Conclusion
as communicating strategies for the regulation of emo-
tion rather than as raw emotional expression per se A comparative perspective on musical rhythmic behav-
(Joel Swaine, personal communication). In this frame- iors suggests that musical pulse and period correction
work period correction mechanisms could provide the are unique to humans and to the context of music and
means for expanding a strategy for self-regulation to can thus be supposed to have evolved specifically for
one that functions in co-regulating and achieving a music. Additionally this article demonstrates the value
convergence of emotional and motivational state with of a clear model of rhythm in music as a constellation of
regularities providing joint focus, redundancy, and partially shared and partially specific abilities as well as
attentive efficiency as well as a feeling of shared experi- the need for the study of rhythm to be positioned
ence. These features may in turn generate nonconflict- within a wider framework of human cognition and
ing modes of appraisal and interpretation of ritualistic behavior.
and symbolic acts and representations in ceremonial
activities and enable or support the creation of joint Author Note
belief systems. Supporting this hypothesis will require
working within a framework that allows us to concep- I would like to thank Ian Cross and Joel Swaine, who
tually relate issues of music, rhythm, emotional self and kindly discussed contents of the article with me and
co-regulation, action, attention, and functionality at the offered many invaluable and insightful comments and
individual and/or group level. suggestions.
An equally pertinent and potentially informative ques-
tion is, why us? When such simple acts as marching Address correspondence to: John Bispham, Leverhulme
together in time have been hypothesized to create feel- Centre for Evolutionary Studies, University of Cambridge,
ings of group bondedness, solidarity, and cohesion Fitzwilliam Street, Cambridge CB2 1QH, UK. E-MAIL
(McNeill, 1995), why is that other social animals (in par- jcb59@cam.ac.uk

References

A RCADI , C., R OBERT, D., & B OESCH , C. (1998). Buttress A RCADI , C., R OBERT, D., & M UGURUSI , F. (2004). A
drumming by wild chimpanzees: Temporal patterning, comparison of buttress drumming by wild chimpanzees
phrase integration into loud calls, and preliminary from two populations. Primates, 45, 135–139.
evidence for individual distinctiveness. Primates, 39, A ROM , S. (1991). African polyphony and polyrhythm: Musical struc-
505–518. ture and methodology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

This content downloaded from 23.235.32.0 on Tue, 17 Nov 2015 17:19:28 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
132 J. Bispham

B ADDELEY, A. (1997). Human memory: Theory and practice F ITCH , T. (2006). The biology and evolution of music: A
(revised edition). Howe: Psychology Press. comparative perspective. Cognition. 100(1), 173–215.
B ALTER , M. (2004). Seeking the key to music. Science, 306, F RIJDA , N. (1986). The emotions: Studies in emotion and social
1120–1122. interaction. New York: Cambridge University Press.
B ISPHAM , J. (2003). An evolutionary perspective on the human F RIJDA , N., & Z EELENBERG , M. (2001). Appraisal: What is the
skill of interpersonal musical entrainment. Unpublished MPhil dependent? In R. J. Davidson, P. Ekman, & K. R. Scherer
dissertation, University of Cambridge. (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion (pp. 141–155). Oxford:
B ISPHAM , J. (2006). “Music” means nothing if we don’t know Oxford University Press.
what it means. Lead review of The singing Neanderthals: The G EISSMANN , T. (2000). Duet songs of the siamang, Hylobates
evolution of music, language, mind and body by S. Mithen. syndactylus: I. Structure and organisation. Primate Report, 56,
Journal of Human Evolution, 50, 587–593. 103–123.
B ROWN , S. (2004). Evolutionary models of music: From G EISSMANN , T., & O RDELDINGER , M. (2000). The relationship
sexual selection to group selection. In F. Tonneau & N. S. between duet songs and pair bonds in siamangs Hylobates
Thompson (Eds.), Perspectives in ethology 13: Behavior, syndactylus. Animal Behaviour, 60, 805–809.
evolution and culture (pp. 231–281). New York: Plenum G HAZANFAR , A., F LOMBAUM , J., M ILLER , C., & H AUSER , M.
Publishers. (2001). The units of perception in the antiphonal calling
C LAYTON , M. (1996). Free music: Ethnomusicology and the behavior of cotton-top tamarins (Saginus oedipus): Playback
study of music without metre. Bulletin of the School of experiments with long calls. Journal of Comparative
Oriental and African Studies, 59, 323–332. Physiology, A, 187, 27–35.
C LAYTON , M., S AGER , R., & W ILL , U. (2004). In time with the G OODALL , J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of
music: The concept of entrainment and its significance for behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
ethnomusicology. ESEM Counterpoint, 1, 1–45. G REENFIELD, M. (1994). Cooperation and conflict in the
C OLLIER , G., & C OLLIER , J. (2002). A study of timing in two evolution of signal interactions. Annual Review of Ecological
Louis Armstrong solos. Music Perception, 19(3), 463–483. Systems, 25, 97–126.
C ROSS , I. (2001). Music, cognition, culture and evolution. G REGORY, A. (1997). The roles of music in society: The
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 930, 28–42. ethnomusicological perspective. In D. Hargreaves & A. North
C ROSS , I. (2005). Music and meaning, ambiguity and evolution. (Eds.), The social psychology of music (pp. 123–140). Oxford:
In D. Miell, R. MacDonald, & D. Hargreaves (Eds.), Musical Oxford University Press.
communication (pp. 27–43) Oxford: Oxford University Press. H AGEN , E., & B RYANT, G. (2003). Music and dance as a
C ROSS , I., & M ORLEY, I. (in press). Music in evolution and coalition signaling system. Human Nature, 14(1), 21–51.
evolutionary theory: The nature of the evidence. In S. H ANNON , E., & J OHNSON , S. (2005). Infants use meter to
Malloch & C. Trevarthen (Eds.), Communicative musicality: categorize rhythms and melodies: Implications for musical
Narratives of expressive gesture and being human. Oxford: structure learning. Cognitive Psychology, 50, 354–377.
Oxford University Press. H ARY, D., & M OORE , G. (1985). Temporal tracking and
C UTLER , A. (1994). The perception of rhythm in language. synchronisation strategies. Human Neurobiology, 4, 73–77.
Cognition, 50, 79–81. H AUSER , M., C HOMSKY, N., & F ITCH , T. (2002). The faculty of
D ISSANAYAKE , E. (2000). Antecedents of the temporal arts in language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve?
early mother-infant interaction. In N. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Science, 298, 1569–1577.
Brown (Eds.), The origins of music (pp. 389–410). Cambridge, H AUSER , M., & M C D ERMOTT, J. (2003). The evolution of the
MA: MIT Press. music faculty: A comparative perspective. Nature
D ODENHOFF, D., S TARK , R., & J OHNSON , E. (2001). Do Neuroscience, 6, 663–668.
woodpecker drums encode information for species H URON , D. (2001). Is music an evolutionary adaptation? Annals
recognition? Condor, 103, 143–150. of the New York Academy of Science, 930, 43–61.
D RAKE , C., & B ERTRAND, D. (2001). The quest for universals H USAIN , G., T HOMPSON , W., & S CHELLENBERG , E. (2002).
in temporal processing in music. Annals of the New York Effects of musical tempo and mode on arousal, mood, and
Academy of Science, 930, 17–27. spatial abilities. Music Perception, 20, 151–171.
D UNBAR , R. (1996). Grooming, gossip and the evolution of I YER , V. (2002). Embodied mind, situated cognition, and
language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. expressive microtiming in African-American music. Music
FARABAUGH , S. (1982). The ecological and social significance of Perception, 19(3), 387–414.
duetting. In D. E. Kroodsma, E. H. Miller, & H. Ouellet (Eds.), JANATA , P., & G RAFTON , S. (2003). Swinging in the brain:
Acoustic communication in birds (pp. 85–124). New York: Shared neural substrates for behaviours related to sequencing
Academic Press. and music. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 682–687.

This content downloaded from 23.235.32.0 on Tue, 17 Nov 2015 17:19:28 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Rhythm in Music 133

J OFFE , T. (1997). Social pressures have selected for an extended PATEL , A., & DANIELE , J. (2003). An empirical comparison of
juvenile period in primates. Journal of Human Evolution, 32, rhythm in language and music. Cognition, 87, 35–45.
593–605. PAYNE , K. (2000). The progressively changing songs of
J ONES , M. (1976). Time, our lost dimension: Toward a new humpback whales: A window on the creative process in a wild
theory of perception, attention, and memory. Psychological animal. In N. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The
Review, 83, 325–335. origins of music (pp. 135–150). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
J ONES , M., & B OLTZ , M. (1989). Dynamic attending and R AMUS , F., N ESPOR , M., & M EHLER , J. (1999). Correlates
responses to time. Psychological Review, 96, 459–491. of linguistic, rhythm in the speech signal. Cognition, 73,
J USLIN , P., & S LOBODA , J. (2001). Music and emotion: Theory 265–292.
and research in affective science. Oxford: Oxford University Press. R AMUS , F. (2002). Language discrimination by newborns:
L ARGE , E., & J ONES , M. (1999). The dynamics of attending: Teasing apart phonotactic, rhythmic, and intonational cues.
How people track time-varying events. Psychological Review, Annual Review of Language Acquisition, 2, 85–115.
106, 121–156. R ANDALL , J. (1997). Species-specific footdrumming in
LONDON , J. (2004). Hearing in time: Psychological aspects of kangaroo rats: Dipodomys ingens, D. deserti, D. spectabilis.
musical meter. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Animal Behaviour, 54, 1167–1175.
M ALLOCH , S. (1999/2000). Mothers and infants and R ENDALL , D., OWREN , M., & R ODMAN , P. (1998). The role of
communicative musicality. Musicae Scientiae, Special Issue: vocal tract filtering in identity cueing in rhesus monkey
Rhythm, Musical Narrative, and the Origins of Human (Macaca mulatta) vocalizations. Journal of the Acoustical
Communication, 29–58. Society of America, 103, 602–614.
M ANN , N., M ARSHALL -B ALL , L., & S LATER , P. (2003). The R EPP, B. (2001). Phase correction, phase resetting, and phase
complex song duet of the plain wren. Condor, 105, 672–682. shifts after subliminal timing perturbations in sensorimotor
M ARLER , P. (2000). Origins of music and speech: Insights synchronization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
from animals. In N. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown Perception and Performance, 27, 600–621.
(Eds.), The origins of music (pp. 31–48). Cambridge, MA: R EPP, B. (2004). Comments on “Rapid motor adaptations to
MIT Press. subliminal frequency shifts during syncopated rhythmic
M C AULEY, J., J ONES , M., J OHNSTONE , H., & M ILLER , N. sensorimotor synchronization” by M. Thaut & G. Kenyon.
(2006). The time of our lives: Life span development of Human Movement Science, 21, 61–78.
timing and event tracking. Journal of Experimental R EPP, B. (2005). Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of the
Psychological Genetics, 135, 348–367. tapping literature. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12,
M C D ERMOTT, J., & H AUSER , M. (2005). The origins of music: 969–992.
Innateness, uniqueness and evolution. Music Perception, R EPP, B., & K ELLER , P. (2004). Adaptation to tempo changes in
23(1), 29–60. sensorimotor synchronisation: Effects of intention, attention,
M C N EILL , W. (1995). Keeping together in time: Dance and drill and awareness. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,
in human history. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. 57, 499–521.
M ERKER , B. (2000). Synchronous chorusing and human R OEDERER , J. (1984). The search for a survival value of music.
origins. In N. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The Music Perception, 1, 350–356.
origins of music (pp. 315–327). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. S CHALLER , G. (1963). The mountain gorilla. Chicago: University
M ITHEN , S. (2005). The singing Neanderthals: The origins of of Chicago Press.
music, language, mind and body. London: Weidenfeld and S EYFARTH , R., & C HENEY, D. (2003). Signalers and receivers in
Nicolson. animal communication. Annual Review of Psychology, 54,
NAZZI , T., J USCZYK , P., & J OHNSON , E. (2000). Language 145–173.
discrimination by English-learning 5-month-olds: Effects of S HENNAN , S. (2002) Genes, memes and human history. London:
rhythm and familiarity. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, Thames and Hudson.
1–19. S JARE , B., S TIRLING , I., & S PENCER , C. (2003). Structural
N ORMAN , D., & S HALLICE , T. (1980). Attention to action: variation in the songs of Atlantic walruses breeding in the
Willed and automatic control of behavior. CHIP 99, University Canadian High Arctic. Aquatic Mammals, 29, 297–318.
of California, San Diego. S PERBER , D. (1996). Explaining culture. Oxford: Blackwell.
OWINGS , D., & M ORTEN , E. (1998). Animal vocal S PERBER , D., & W ILSON , D. (1986). Relevance: Communication
communication: A new approach. Cambridge: Cambridge and cognition. Oxford: Blackwell.
University Press. S TEPHAN , K., T HAUT, M., W UNDERLICH , G., S CHICKS , W.,
PALMER , C. (1997). Music performance. Annual Review of T IAN , B., T ELLMANN , L., S CHMITZ , T., H ERZOG , H.,
Psychology, 48, 115–138. M C I NTOSH , G., S EITZ , R., & H OMBERG , V. (2002).

This content downloaded from 23.235.32.0 on Tue, 17 Nov 2015 17:19:28 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
134 J. Bispham

Conscious and subconscious sensorimotor TOMASELLO, M., C ARPENTER , M., C ALL , J., B EHNE , T., &
synchronization—prefrontal cortex and the influence of M OLL , H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions:
awareness. NeuroImage, 15, 345–352. The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioural and Brain
S TOBART, H., & C ROSS , I. (2000). The Andean anacrusis? Sciences, 28, 675–691.
Rhythmic structure and perception in Easter songs of TORO, J., T ROBALON , J., & S EBASTIÁN -G ALLÉS , N. (2003). The
Northern Potosí, Bolivia. British Journal of Ethnomusicology, use of prosodic cues in language discrimination tasks by rats.
9(2), 63–94. Animal Cognition, 6(2), 131–136.
T HAUT, M., M C I NTOSH , G., & R ICE , R. (1997). Rhythmic T REVARTHEN , C. (1999/2000). Musicality and the intrinsic
facilitation of gait training in hemiparetic stroke motive pulse: Evidence from human psychobiology and
rehabilitation. Journal of Neurological Sciences, 151, 207–212. infant communication. Musicae Scientiae, Special Issue:
T HAYER , R., N EWMAN , J., & M C L AIN , T. (1994). The self- Rhythm, Musical Narrative, and the Origins of Human
regulation of mood: Strategies for changing a bad mood, Communication, 155–215.
raising energy, and reducing tension. Journal of Personality VAN PARIJS , S. (2003). Aquatic mating in pinnipeds: A review.
and Social Psychology, 67, 910–925. Aquatic Mammals, 29, 214–226.
T INBERGEN , N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology. WALLIN , N., M ERKER , B., & B ROWN , S. (Eds.). (2000). The
Zeitschrift fuer Tierpsychologie, 20, 410–433. origins of music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
T INCOFF, R., H AUSER , M., T SAO, F., S PAEPEN , G., R AMUS , F., W ING , A., & K RISTOFFERSON , A. (1973). The timing of
& M EHLER , J. (2005). The role of speech rhythm in language interresponse intervals. Perception & Psychophysics, 13,
discrimination: Further tests with a non-human primate. 455–460.
Developmental Science, 8(1), 26–35. WOOD, G. (1988). Further field observations of the palm
TOLBERT, E. (2001). Music and meaning: An evolutionary story. cockatoo Probosciger aterrimus in the Cape York Peninsula,
Psychology of Music, 29, 89–94. Queensland. Corella, 12, 48–55.

This content downloaded from 23.235.32.0 on Tue, 17 Nov 2015 17:19:28 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like