You are on page 1of 10

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/258280310

Client and Therapist Therapeutic Alliance, Session Evaluation, and Client


Reliable Change: A Moderated Actor-Partner Interdependence Model

Article  in  Journal of Counseling Psychology · November 2013


DOI: 10.1037/a0034939 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS
19 912

3 authors, including:

Dennis Kivlighan Cheri L Marmarosh


University of Maryland, College Park George Washington University
199 PUBLICATIONS   4,620 CITATIONS    53 PUBLICATIONS   1,216 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Introduction to Special Edition of IJGP I Edited on Attachment in Group Psychotherapy View project

Special Edition on Termination in Psychotherapy View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Cheri L Marmarosh on 26 March 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Counseling Psychology © 2013 American Psychological Association
2014, Vol. 61, No. 1, 15–23 0022-0167/14/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0034939

Client and Therapist Therapeutic Alliance, Session Evaluation, and Client


Reliable Change: A Moderated Actor–Partner Interdependence Model

Dennis M. Kivlighan Jr. Cheri L. Marmarosh


University of Maryland The George Washington University

Mark J. Hilsenroth
Adelphi University
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Actor–partner interdependence modeling (APIM; Kashy & Kenny, 2000) was used to study the early
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

therapeutic alliance in 74 clients being treated by 29 therapists to explore the relationship between the alliance
and treatment progress, while prioritizing the dyadic nature of the alliance. The APIM examines collaboration/
influence by modeling the impact of one dyad member’s alliance ratings on the other member’s session impact
rating (partner effects). In terms of the alliance, the results revealed significant client–actor effects for client
ratings of session depth and positivity as well as significant therapist–actor effects for therapist ratings of
session smoothness and positivity. For client-rated alliance, there were also significant client–partner effects
for therapist ratings of session depth. For clients who made a reliable change in treatment, an increase in
client-reported alliance was related to therapist reporting more arousal in the 3rd session. For clients who did
not make a reliable change in treatment, client-reported alliance was not related to therapist arousal.
Limitations of the study and implications of the findings are discussed.

Keywords: therapeutic alliance, session evaluation, actor–partner interdependence model

The therapeutic alliance is one of the most examined aspects of in depression and symptoms were .42 and .47, respectively,
the therapeutic relationship. In meta-analyzing the results of over whereas the effect size for the relationship between therapeutic
30 years of research, Horvath, Del Re, Flückiger, and Symonds alliance and dropout was .18. However, only the depression effect
(2011) found that the therapeutic alliance was a significant pre- size was significantly different from the dropout effect size.
dictor of therapy outcome, accounting for approximately 8% of the Changes in depression and symptoms may reflect the working
variance in treatment outcomes. Theoretically, the therapeutic al- aspect of treatment, whereas dropout may reflect the personal
liance should be differentially related to types of outcomes. In relationship between the client and therapist. For example, Tryon
differentiating the therapeutic alliance and the real relationship, and Kane (1995) found that therapist’s ratings of how well clients
Gelso (2011) stated that the therapeutic alliance captured the related at intake were related to the client terminating unilaterally.
working aspects of the therapist and client relationship, whereas Therefore, there may be an important theoretical and empirical
the real relationship reflected the personal, nonworking aspect of
distinction between outcomes that reflect the work of therapy and
this relationship. Therefore, the therapeutic alliance should show a
outcomes that reflect the personal relationship in therapy.
stronger relationship to outcomes that reflect the work of therapy
As noted above, however, there are few differential relation-
and a weaker relationship to outcomes that reflect the personal
ships between the therapeutic alliance and type of outcome prob-
aspects of therapy. Researchers have not used this work versus
personal distinction in their studies examining the relationship ably because outcome is usually measured at termination, which
between the therapeutic alliance and outcome. Although one of the may be too distal to capture the subtleties of the therapeutic
moderator analyses results in the Horvath et al. (2011) meta- alliance– outcome relationship (e.g., Hill & Lambert, 2004). Bo-
analysis hints at this distinction. These authors found that the effect swell, Castonguay, and Wasserman (2010) argued that looking at
sizes for the relationship between therapeutic alliance and change session outcome allows for a closer look at what comes between
process (the therapeutic alliance in a session) and long-term out-
come (the effects of a series of sessions or of a whole treatment).
In line with this observation, Owen, Quirk, Hilsenroth, and
This article was published Online First November 4, 2013. Rodolfa (2012) examined how the therapeutic alliance related to
Dennis M. Kivlighan Jr., Department of Counseling and Personnel session-level ratings of client intersession experience. They found
Services, University of Maryland; Cheri L. Marmarosh, Department of that the alliance was significantly related to therapeutic work
Psychology, The George Washington University; Mark J. Hilsenroth, The (“applying therapy”) between sessions but was unrelated to the
Derner Institute of Advanced Psychological Studies, Adelphi University.
personal therapy relationship (imagining dialogues with the ther-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Dennis
M. Kivlighan Jr., Department of Counseling and Personnel Services, 3214 apist) between sessions. In the present study, we build on the
Benjamin Building, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742. Owen et al. (2012) study by examining the relationship between
E-mail: dennisk@umd.edu the therapeutic alliance and session outcome.
15
16 KIVLIGHAN, MARMAROSH, AND HILSENROTH

One of the most widely used measures of session outcome is the The APIM has two types of effects: actor effects and partner
Session Evaluation Questionnaire (SEQ; Stiles & Snow, 1984), effects. Actor effects describe the relationship between a partici-
which measures session outcome on the dimensions of Depth, pant’s own predictor and her or his outcome (e.g., the therapist’s
Smoothness, Positivity, and Arousal. Several studies have exam- therapeutic alliance and the therapist’s session evaluation). Partner
ined the relationship between the Depth and/or Smoothness di- effects describe the relationship between a participant’s own pre-
mensions of session evaluation and the therapeutic alliance. Kiv- dictor and her or his partner’s outcome (e.g., the therapist’s ther-
lighan (2007) found that counselors’ and clients’ ratings of the apeutic alliance and the client’s session evaluation). The APIM
alliance were related to their own ratings of session depth and also specifies two correlations: one describes the dependency
smoothness. Kim, Ng, and Ann (2009) only measured session between clients’ and therapists’ therapeutic alliance ratings and the
depth and found a strong relationship between therapeutic alliance other describes the dependency between clients’ and therapists’
and session depth for Asian American clients. However, Gelso et session evaluation ratings. In the APIM, interpersonal influence is
al. (2005) found no relationship between client-rated therapeutic defined as a significant partner effect, whereas mutual influence is
alliance and client-rated session depth and smoothness. defined as both partner effects being significant.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Only one study (Mallinckrodt, 1993) examined the relationship Kivlighan (2007) argued that therapeutic alliance studies rarely
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

between therapeutic alliance and all four dimensions of session statistically examine collaboration or mutual influence because the
outcome operationalized in the SEQ. In this complex study, alli- combined contributions of both therapists’ and clients’ therapeutic
ance and session outcome were rated by both counselors and alliance ratings are not assessed in the same study. In the Horvath
clients at different points in treatment. The one consistent finding et al. (2011) meta-analysis, there were 112 independent effects for
in this study was that early client ratings of the therapeutic alliance client-rated therapeutic alliance, but only 23 independent effects
were significantly and positively related to client-rated session for therapist-rated therapeutic alliance. Horvath et al. did not
depth at early, mid, and late treatment. No other consistent rela- report how many of the outcomes examined involved the cli-
tionships between therapeutic alliance (either client- or therapist- ents’ or the therapists’ perspective. It is likely, however, that
rated) and session outcome (either client- or therapist-rated) most of the studies involved only client ratings of outcome.
emerged. There is some evidence, however, that specific SEQ Therefore, the therapeutic alliance– outcome literature appears
scales may capture either the work or personal aspect of session to involve mostly an examination of the clients’ perceptions of
outcome. In a large study examining the relationship between both the therapeutic alliance and treatment outcome (client–
session evaluations and session impacts, Stiles et al. (1994) found actor effects).
that Depth and Arousal were more strongly related to work- Only two published studies used the APIM to examine the
oriented impacts (Understanding and Problem Solving) and that therapeutic relationship in individual counseling. Gelso et al.
Smoothness and Positivity were more strongly related to the rela- (2012) found that clients’ ratings of the real relationship were
tionship oriented impact (Relationship). Given these relationships related to their own rating of outcome (a client–actor effect) and to
we hypothesized that: their therapist’s ratings of outcome (a client–partner effect). In the
study that is the most relevant for the present research, Kivlighan
Hypthothesis 1: Client and therapist ratings of the therapeutic (2007) used the APIM to examine the relationship between clients’
alliance will be significantly and positively related to their and therapists’ ratings of the working alliance and their ratings of
ratings of session Depth and Arousal. session Depth, a session’s perceived power or value, and Smooth-
ness, a session’s comfort or pleasantness (Stiles & Snow 1984). He
The Therapeutic Alliance: Collaboration found that clients’ and therapists’ ratings of the therapeutic alli-
ance were significantly related to their own ratings of session
and Mutual Influence
Depth and Smoothness (significant client– and therapist–actor
There appears to be a consensus across theoreticians and re- effects). He also found that therapists’ ratings of the therapeutic
searchers that collaboration, working together, and reciprocity are alliance were significantly related to clients’ ratings of session
the core features of the therapeutic alliance (Hatcher & Barends, Depth (a therapist–partner effect).
2006; Horvath et al., 2011; Horvath & Symonds, 1991). Research- One purpose of the present study was to replicate and extend
ers can measure perceptions of collaboration and reciprocity or Kivlighan’s (2007) study in several important ways. We replicate
model collaboration and reciprocity statistically. This perception the earlier study by using the APIM to once again examine the
versus statistical distinction is similar to the objective versus relationship between clients’ and therapists’ ratings of the working
subjective distinction made by researchers examining similarity in alliance and their ratings of session Depth and Smoothness. As
groups (e.g., Dunlop & Beauchamp, 2011). Most research on the noted above, the SEQ, however, includes two additional subscales:
therapeutic alliance involves perceptions of collaboration and rec- Arousal, the experience of excitement or vitality after a session,
iprocity, relying on the wording of the therapeutic alliance inven- and Positivity, the experence of positive emotions after a session,
tory to get at the client’s and therapist’s perceptions of collabora- but these subscales were not examined in Kivlighan (2007). There-
tion and reciprocity. Some items on therapeutic alliance fore, we extend the Kivlighan (2007) study by examining the
inventories do assess perceptions of collaboration (e.g., “My ther- Positivity and Arousal dimensions of session evaluation as rated
apist and I are working towards mutually agreed-upon goals”); by both clients and therapists.
however, other items assess individual perceptions (e.g., “I believe As noted above, the vast majority of alliance research has
my therapist likes me”). Alternatively, collaboration and reciproc- involved an examination of client–actor effects (i.e., the client’s
ity can be assessed using the actor–partner interdependence model rating of alliance and the client’s rating of either treatment or
(APIM; Ledermann & Kenny, 2012). session outcome), yet very few of these studies have controlled for
CLIENT AND THERAPIST ALLIANCE 17

partner effects. Despite the lack of controlling for partner effects, Hypothesis 4: There will be significant client–partner effects
we believe that the alliance studies and alliance theory support the of therapeutic alliance on session depth and arousal when
existence of actor effects. On the basis of our analysis of work- clients report a reliable change and nonsignificant client–
oriented versus personal-oriented outcomes, we made the follow- partner effects when there is no reliable change.
ing hypotheses:
The APIM, depicted in Figure 1, contains two actor effects: (a)
Hypothesis 2a: Clients’ ratings of alliance will be significantly client alliance and session evaluation and (b) therapist alliance and
related to clients’ ratings of session Depth and Arousal, when session evaluation; two partner effects: (a) client alliance and
controlling for therapists’ partner effects. therapist session evaluation and (b) therapist alliance and client
session evaluation; and the effect for the reliable change index
Hypothesis 2b: Therapists’ ratings of alliance will be signifi-
(RCI). The model also contains four interaction effects: (a) client–
cantly related to therapists’ ratings of session Depth and
actor by RCI, (b) therapist–actor by RCI, (c) client–partner by
Arousal, when controlling for clients’ partner effects.
RCI, and (d) therapist–partner by RCI. The APIM accounts for the
nesting of clients and therapists within dyads by specifying a
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Eugster and Wampold (1996) found that clients and therapists


correlation between clients’ and therapists’ session evaluations.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

take different factors into account when making judgments about


session outcome because of their differing role perspectives. Sul-
livan (1954) also described differences in the therapist’s and Method
client’s roles in terms of the therapist’s role as a participant
observer. As a participant observer, the therapist observers the Participants
client’s behavior and participates as a significant other in the
counseling relationship. This participant observer role can be seen Clients. This study is an extension of Pesale, Hilsenroth, and
in Lent et al.’s (2006) suggestion that therapists’ session evalua- Owen (2012). There were 74 clients receiving individual psycho-
tions are related to their own experiences of the counseling rela- therapy in the Psychodynamic Psychotherapy Treatment Team at a
tionship and their inferences about their client’s experiences of the community outpatient psychological clinic and provided written
counseling relationships. Therefore, as a participant observer, informed consent for this research. For a complete description of
counselors’ session evaluations should be related to their own and the clients, see Pesale et al. (2012).
their client’s perceptions of the alliance. The client’s role, how- Therapists. Twenty-nine (14 male, 15 female) third- or
ever, in counseling is to be a full participant in the counseling fourth-year doctoral students enrolled in an American Psycholog-
relationship. Therefore, clients tend to be more self-focused, using ical Association-approved Clinical Psychology Ph.D. program
their perceptions of the alliance as evidence for deciding their conducted the psychological assessment and psychotherapy. One
evaluations of sessions. The Kivlighan (2007) study did not sup- of these therapists was a racial/ethnic minority, and the rest were
port this theoretical model, because he found partner effects only Caucasian. Five therapists saw one patient, eight therapists saw
for the therapists. However, the Gelso et al. (2012) study did reveal two patients, 11 therapists saw three clients, two therapists saw
significant client–partner effects. Despite these contradictory find- four clients, and one therapist saw five clients. See Pesale et al.
ings, we base our hypotheses concerning differences between (2012) for a description of these therapists.
clients and counselors in terms of the participant and observer Treatment. Clients first received a psychological evaluation
roles, and we specifically hypothesized that: from a therapeutic model of assessment (see Hilsenroth, 2007, for

Hypothesis 3: Clients’ ratings of alliance (client–partner ef-


fects) will be significantly related to therapists’ session eval-
uations (greater Depth and Arousal), when controlling for
clients’ actor effects.

Therapeutic Alliance, Session Evaluations,


and Outcome
It is clear that therapeutic process differs when clients experi-
ence a more or less successful outcome (e.g., Gelso, Kivlighan,
Wine, Jones, & Friedman, 1997). Therefore, it is important to
examine how the actor–partner relationships described above vary
as a function of the client’s outcome status. We predict that the
client–partner effect described in Hypothesis 3 will be stronger
when the client has a better outcome. Specifically, we expected
that client symptom change would moderate the relationship be-
tween clients’ alliance ratings and therapists’ session evaluations.
This is because theory and research suggest that those therapists Figure 1. Actor–partner interdependence model for client and therapist
who are better at monitoring the state of their clients’ alliances (as alliance and client and therapist session evaluation and client reliable
evidenced by a significant partner effect for client alliance ratings) change. Reliable change index (RCI) is calculated from the Global Severity
will be more successful (Richards, 2011). Therefore, we made the Index of the Brief Symptom Inventory. The intercorrelations among the
final hypothesis: five predictor variables are not depicted in the figure.
18 KIVLIGHAN, MARMAROSH, AND HILSENROTH

details). The clinician who carried out the psychological assess- 0.30 (SD ⫽ 0.31), and test–retest reliability using an outpatient
ment also conducted the psychotherapy sessions. Psychotherapy sample was .90. The mean pretreatment GSI for the present sample
consisted of once- or twice-weekly sessions of short-term psy- was 1.06 (SD ⫽ 0.59).
chodynamic psychotherapy (see Hilsenroth, 2007) and was open- Reliable change (RC) was calculated using formulas described
ended. Blagys and Hilsenroth (2000) describe the key features of in Jacobsen and Truax (1991) and Jacobson, Roberts, Berns, and
this treatment model. McGlinchy (1999). A client was classified as having made an RC
if her or his RCI was greater than 1.96 (coded as 1) and as not
having made an RC if her or his RCI was less than or equal to 1.96
Measures
(coded as 0).
The SEQ (Stiles et al., 1994; Stiles & Snow, 1984). The SEQ
is a measure of in-session psychotherapy process that consists of Procedure
24 bipolar adjective scales rated from 1 (e.g., weak) to 7 (e.g.,
powerful). It is separated into two sections, each consisting of 12 Clients filled out the BSI prior to starting treatment. Client and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

bipolar scales. Factor analysis revealed two evaluation indices— therapist SEQ and therapeutic alliance ratings were collected from
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Depth and Smoothness—and two postsession mood indices— either the third or fourth treatment session. Clients were informed,
Positivity and Arousal (Stiles et al., 1994; Stiles & Snow, 1984). both verbally and in writing, that their therapists would not have
Previous research has revealed alphas ranging from .74 to .91 for access to their responses on these session measures. The mean
these four SEQ subscales using a subset of the current participants number of sessions in this sample was 31 (SD ⫽ 22), and the
(Ackerman, Hilsenroth, Baity, & Blagys, 2000). The validity of the post-BSI was given at termination. Further details of the measures,
SEQ has been demonstrated by significant correlations between methodology, and procedures used are described more fully else-
SEQ scales and measures of therapist quality, session impact, and where (Hilsenroth, 2007).
global session evaluations (Stiles et al., 1994).
Combined Alliance Short Form–Patient Version (CASF-P; Data Analysis
Hatcher & Barends, 1996). The CASF-P is a client-rated alli-
We used path analysis within a structural equation modeling
ance measure consisting of 20 items rated on a 7-point scale where
framework to analyze the APIM (Kenny, Mannetti, Pierro, Livi, &
1 ⫽ never, 2 ⫽ rarely, 3 ⫽ occasionally, 4 ⫽ sometimes, 5 ⫽
Kashy, 2002). All variables were mean-centered before creating
often, 6 ⫽ very often, and 7 ⫽ always. Hatcher and colleagues
interaction terms.
(Hatcher & Barends, 1996; Hatcher, Barends, Hansell, & Gut-
freund, 1995) reported evidence on both the construct and incre-
mental validity of this scale with regard to outcome. In addition, Results
both Ackerman et al. (2000) and Clemence, Hilsenroth, Ackerman, The means and standard deviations for therapists’ and clients’
Strassle, and Handler (2005) reported convergent validity data ratings of alliance, session Depth, session Smoothness, Positivity,
with related measures of psychotherapy process as well as criterion and Arousal are displayed in Table 1. Clients’ mean GSI posttreat-
validity with regard to the prediction of treatment outcome using a ment scores (0.67, SD ⫽ .62) were significantly smaller than their
sample of clients at the same university-based clinic as the clients pretreatment scores (1.01, SD ⫽ .58), t(73) ⫽ ⫺5.45, p ⬍ .0001.
in the present study, as well as a coefficient alpha of .91. Of the 74 clients, 47 (63.51%) did not make a reliable change
Working Alliance Inventory–Therapist Version (WAI-T; (RCI ⬍ 1.96), and 27 (36.48%) did make a reliable change (RCI ⬎
Horvath & Greenberg, 1989). The WAI-T is a therapist-rated 1.96). Because there was a good deal of variability in the number
alliance measure. The Total scale score for the WAI-T used in this of treatment sessions, we examined the relationship between num-
study was derived from a recent psychometric adaptation (Hatcher, ber of sessions completed and client reliable change. The point
1999) using responses from two samples. The first was a national biserial correlation for number of treatment sessions and reliable
sample consisting of practicing therapists’ ratings on one patient change was .06 (p ⫽ .61). Therefore, reliable change was not
from their current practice (N ⫽ 251). The second was a clinical confounded with sessions completed.
sample consisting of 63 therapists who completed ratings on 259 Because therapists saw more than one client, we initially used a
different patients. Previous research has revealed alphas ranging two-level hierarchical linear modeling (Raudenbush, Bryk,
from .75 to .86 (Hatcher, 1999), and examining a subset of the Cheong, & Congdon, 2005) analysis to test for therapist effects for
current participants, coefficient alphas range from .74 to .91
(Clemence et al., 2005). Ratings on the WAI-T are reported on the
same 7-point scale as described for the CASF-P, ranging from 1 Table 1
(never) to 7 (always). Means and Standard Deviations for Therapists’ and Clients’
Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI; Derogatis, 1993). The BSI Alliance Ratings and Session Evaluations Early in Treatment
is a 53-item self-report inventory that assesses symptom distress in
Clients Therapists
a number of different domains/problem areas using a Likert scale
ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely) and was collected at Variable M SD M SD
pre- and posttreatment. The psychometric properties, reliability, Alliance 6.15 0.63 5.75 0.44
and validity of this measure, as well as a description of specific Depth 5.51 0.87 5.02 0.911
symptom subscale scores and a summary score, the Global Sever- Session Smoothness 4.86 1.12 4.65 1.00
ity Index (GSI), are provided in the manual (Derogatis, 1993). The Positivity 4.92 1.08 5.23 0.92
Arousal 3.97 1.08 4.48 1.13
mean GSI for a normal population (N ⫽ 719, nonpatients) was
CLIENT AND THERAPIST ALLIANCE 19

the session evaluation variables. Therapists who only saw one 14


client were excluded in these tests of therapist effects on client and
12
therapist SEQ ratings. We conducted eight empty models to de-
termine the variance in SEQ ratings accounted for by therapists. 10

Therapist Arousal
The variance estimates from these eight models were used to
8 No
calculate intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs). The ICCs for Reliable
client session evaluation ratings were Depth (.06; p ⫽ .10), 6 Change
Reliable
Smoothness (.10; p ⫽ .08), Positivity (.17; p ⫽ .02), and Arousal Change
4
(.002; p ⫽ .42). The ICCs for therapist session evaluation ratings
were Depth (.07; p ⫽ .09), Smoothness (.01; p ⫽ .21), Positivity 2
(.06; p ⫽ .10), and Arousal (.05; p ⫽ .12). Because the therapist
0
variance for only one of the SEQ ratings was significant, we did
Low Client Therapeutic Alliance High Client Therapeutic Alliance
not model therapists in our main analyses.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Data were analyzed using full information maximum likelihood Figure 2. Interaction of client alliance and client reliable change in
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

estimation in EQS. Model fit statistics are not reported because the predicting therapist arousal.
APIM model is saturated. Table 2 displays the results of the APIM
analyses for alliance and session evaluation. There were significant
client–actor effects for session Depth (b ⫽ .59, p ⬍ .05) and for
Positivity (b ⫽ .47, p ⬍ .05). When clients had higher alliance made a reliable change at the end of treatment, clients’ alliance
ratings, they also had higher session depth and positivity ratings. ratings and therapists’ arousal ratings early in treatment also in-
The therapist–actor effects for session Smoothness (b ⫽ .76, p ⬍ creased.
.05) and session Positivity (b ⫽ .75, p ⬍ .05) were significant. The APIM variables accounted for 17% (p ⬍ .05) of the
When therapists had higher alliance ratings, they also had higher variance in clients’ ratings of session Depth and 14% (p ⬍ .05) of
session Smoothness and Positivity ratings. None of the therapist– the variance in therapists’ ratings of session Depth. Clients’ and
partner effects were significant. The client–partner effect for ses- therapists’ ratings of session Depth were not significantly corre-
sion Depth (b ⫽ .46, p ⬍ .05) was significant. When clients had lated (r ⫽ .14, p ⬎ .05). The APIM variables accounted for 10%
higher alliance ratings, their therapists rated the sessions as deeper. (p ⬍ .05) of the variance in clients’ ratings of session Smoothness
There was a significant relationship between clients’ early treat- and 15% (p ⬍ .05) of the variance in therapists’ ratings of session
ment Arousal ratings and client end-of-treatment reliable change Smoothness. Clients’ and therapists’ ratings of session Smooth-
(b ⫽ ⫺.64, p ⬍ .05). When clients had higher arousal ratings early ness were not significantly correlated (r ⫽ ⫺.04, p ⬎ .05). The
in treatment, they were less likely to subsequently show a reliable APIM variables accounted for 13% (p ⬍ .05) of the variance in
change. Clients’ early treatment alliance ratings and client reliable clients’ ratings of Positivity and 26% (p ⬍ .05) of the variance in
change interacted to predict therapist early treatment arousal (b ⫽ therapists’ ratings of Positivity. Clients’ and therapists’ ratings of
1.10, p ⬍ .05). The form of this interaction is displayed in Figure Positivity were not significantly correlated (r ⫽ ⫺.01, p ⬎ .05).
2. The simple slope for clients who made a reliable change was The APIM variables accounted for 16% (p ⬎ .05) of the variance
0.90 (p ⬍ .05). The simple slope for clients who did not make a in clients’ ratings of Arousal and 12% (p ⬍ .05) of the variance in
reliable change was ⫺0.04 (p ⬎ .05). As seen in the figure, when therapists’ ratings of Arousal. Clients’ and therapists’ ratings of
therapists had clients who did not make a reliable change at the end Arousal were not significantly correlated (r ⫽ ⫺.04, p ⬎ .05). In
of treatment, there was no relationship between client alliance all four APIM analyses, clients’ and therapists’ ratings of the
ratings and therapist arousal early in psychotherapy. When clients alliance were not significantly correlated (r ⫽ ⫺.10, p ⬎ .05).

Table 2
Regression Coefficients, Standard Errors, and t Statistics for the Actor–Partner Interdependence Model for Client and Therapist
Alliance and Client and Therapist Session Evaluation With Partner-Moderated Actor and Partner Effects

Therapist Reliable change


Client alliance alliance index (RCI) Client ⫻ RCI Therapist ⫻ RCI
Variable b t b t b t b t b t

Client Depth .59 3.18 .12 .43 .02 .12 ⫺.13 ⫺.42 .09 .21
Therapist Depth .46 2.31ⴱ .33 1.11 .04 .17 ⴚ.37 ⴚ1.09 .37 1.00
Client Smoothness .34 1.35 .59 1.61 .28 1.09 .09 .22 ⴚ.93 ⴚ1.61
Therapist Smoothness .19 .87 .76 2.43ⴱ .35 1.53 ⴚ.64 ⴚ1.76 ⫺.27 ⫺.53
Client Positivity .47 1.97ⴱ .11 .30 .33 1.33 .16 .40 .08 .14
Therapist Positivity .22 1.19 .75 2.70ⴱ .23 1.18 ⴚ.32 ⴚ1.02 .53 1.22
Client Arousal ⫺.08 ⫺.32 .31 .90 ⫺.64 ⫺2.65ⴱ .37 .94 .73 1.34
Therapist Arousal ⴚ.04 ⴚ.17 .16 .43 .20 .76 1.01 2.41ⴱ ⫺.08 ⫺.14
Note. RCI ⫽ Global Severity Index of the Brief Symptom Inventory–reliable change index coded 0 for no reliable change attained and 1 for reliable
change attained. Bold type ⫽ partner effects; regular type ⫽ actor effects.

p ⬍ .05.
20 KIVLIGHAN, MARMAROSH, AND HILSENROTH

Discussion also found significant client–partner effects. In their study, thera-


pists’ evaluations of treatment outcome were related to their cli-
ents’ ratings of the real relationship. It is hard to see why the
Actor Effects
Kivlighan (2007) study did not reveal significant client–partner
As hypothesized, clients’ alliance ratings were related to their effects for the alliance. All three studies used similar measures and
ratings of session Depth but unexpectedly also to their ratings of similar client and therapist populations. It will be important for
Positivity. Also contrary to our expectations, therapists’ alliance future research to examine the hypothesis concerning the effects of
ratings were related to their ratings of session Smoothness and the therapist’s role as a participant observer. Given the results of
Positivity. The findings for client Depth and for therapist Smooth- this study and of Gelso et al. (2012), it seems that therapists’ are
ness replicate results of Kivlighan (2007). However, he also found affected by their clients’ view of the early therapy relationship
that therapists’ alliance ratings were significantly related to their (either the alliance or the real relationship).
ratings of Depth and that the clients’ alliance ratings were related It is also interesting to note that there were no client–partner
to their ratings of Smoothness. Across the two studies, clients effects for session Smoothness, Arousal, and Positivity. Therefore,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

appear to consistently associate the therapeutic alliance with the the client’s perception of the alliance did not relate to her or his
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

work-oriented outcome of session Depth. However, their therapist therapist’s perceptions of the session’s comfort or his or her
consistently associated the therapeutic alliance with the personal- positive emotions or experience of excitement or vitality after a
oriented outcome of session smoothness. These results confirm session. We expect that for therapists, Depth may be related to the
Eugster and Wampold’s (1996) findings that clients and therapists working aspect of psychodynamic treatment (i.e., exploratory and
take somewhat different factors into account when making judg- expressive), whereas Arousal, Positivity, and Smoothness may be
ments about the session. related to comfort in treatment. If this is the case, then it seems that
As far as we can tell, this was the first study to examine actor therapists pay attention to the client’s alliance to gauge the work
effects for alliance and session Positivity and Arousal. Contrary to that can be or is being accomplished.
our hypothesis for both therapists and clients, their own ratings of
alliance were related to their own ratings of Positivity but not to Moderation Effects
their own ratings of Arousal. It seems that both therapists and
clients associate a strong therapeutic alliance with the experience We hypothesized that those therapists who were better at mon-
of positive emotions after a session. However, neither party asso- itoring the state of their clients’ alliances would be more successful
ciates the therapeutic alliance with the experience of excitement or (Richards, 2011); therefore, client–partner effects would be stron-
vitality after a session. There were, however, important moderated ger when clients made a reliable change than when the clients did
therapist–partner effects that are discussed below. It is important to not make a reliable change. We found support for this hypothesis,
note that all of these significant actor effects were found when but only in the case of Arousal. When clients did not make a
controlling for partner effects. Therefore, it appears that a partic- reliable change in treatment, there was no significant relationship
ipant’s alliance rating is related in important ways to her or his between the client’s therapeutic alliance and the therapist’s rating
evaluation of the session. The evaluations of sessions for clients of Arousal early in treatment. The significant relationship between
and therapists have important convergences (therapeutic alliance a client’s therapeutic alliance and her or his therapist’s Arousal
and positivity) and divergences (client alliance with depth and only occurs when clients’ make a reliable change across treatment.
therapist alliance with smoothness). In examining the significant interaction, it is important to keep
in mind that the therapeutic alliance and session evaluation ratings
come from early treatment, whereas the reliable change can only
Partner Effects
be calculated after the client terminates treatment, on average
Our partner hypotheses were based on therapist and client role 27–28 weeks after the alliance and session evaluation ratings. For
differences. As noted above, Sullivan (1954) described the thera- clients who will eventually make a reliable change, when the client
pist’s role as that of a participant observer. As a participant rates the early session therapeutic alliance as stronger, the therapist
observer, the therapist observers the client’s behavior and partic- ends that session feeling more arousal. For clients who will even-
ipates as a significant other in the counseling relationship. There- tually not make a reliable change, their ratings of the therapeutic
fore, therapists’ session evaluations should be affected by their alliance are unrelated to their therapist’s arousal. Therefore, it is a
own and their clients’ ratings of the alliance. The client’s role in good prognostic indicator if in an early session, the therapist
counseling, especially in early sessions, is, to the extent possible, responds with invigoration or excitement to clients experiencing
to be a full participant in the counseling relationship. As partici- higher levels of the therapeutic alliance. And, as per the procedures
pants, clients are typically attuned to only their own view of the of this study, clients were informed, both verbally and in writing,
alliance. Therefore, we hypothesized that there would be signifi- that their therapists would not have access to their responses on
cant client–partner effects but not significant therapist–partner these session measures. So neither party had access to each other’s
effects when examining work-oriented session outcomes (Depth scores on these process measures, and such sentiments were only
and Arousal). discernable from the dyadic interactions during the session.
We found one significant client–partner effect: Therapists’ ses- Why would therapists’ excitement in relationship to higher
sion Depth ratings were related to their clients’ therapeutic alliance alliance ratings very early in treatment be related to clients’ even-
ratings. This is in contrast to Kivlighan (2007), who found that tual improvement in treatment? Stiles et al. (1994) wondered
therapists’ alliance ratings were related to their clients’ ratings of whether the “energizing impact of psychotherapy” (p. 184) could
session Depth. Our results are similar to Gelso et al. (2012), who be related to novice clinicians being able to apply what they had
CLIENT AND THERAPIST ALLIANCE 21

learned with clients. It is certainly possible that when novice decreased client arousal. Clients who experienced a reliable
therapists perceived a stronger alliance, they became excited be- change reported feeling less aroused after these early sessions.
cause they felt able to implement the techniques that they had This fits with the notion of clients feeling more introspective.
learned. Pesale and Hilsenroth (2009) found that therapists’ use of
psychodynamic techniques was significantly related to clients’
Limitations
perceptions of Depth ratings early in the therapy process. In prior
studies, therapists also rated more Depth when they used more There are several limitations to consider when drawing conclu-
psychodynamic or interpersonal techniques compared with when sions about the results. One of the most significant limitations is
they used more cognitive-behavioral techniques (Stiles, Shapiro, & the reliance on a correlational design that inhibits us from making
Firth-Cozen, 1988). In addition, dynamically oriented therapists causal inferences about the direction of influence in the relation-
may be more aroused by therapies that emphasize emotional ship between working alliance and session evaluations. We are not
engagement, exploration of underlying thoughts and needs, and the sure whether the alliance influences the session experiences or
linkage of those internal experiences to the therapy relationship. whether session experiences influence the alliance. In the future,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Therefore, a series of events may be happening in early sessions collecting repeated measures of the alliance, session experience,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

for clients who make a reliable change. First, when their dynam- and symptom change during the course of treatment would allow
ically oriented therapists perceive that the client has a stronger us to explore how these factors influence one another over time. In
therapeutic alliance, the therapist becomes aroused and excited addition to the correlational design, we only focused on a relatively
because she or he sees this strengthened alliance as an opportunity small sample of clients in psychodynamic treatment. Although the
to use dynamic and interpersonal techniques. Or, conversely, the sample is sufficient for the statistical analysis used, we are not
therapist used these interventions with apparently good response certain that our findings would generalize to another population
on the part of the client, thus leading to an increase in therapist felt of clients being treated with a different form of psychotherapy.
efficacy. The stronger alliance coupled with the presumed use of None of the clients were randomly selected, and the majority of
these techniques leads to the therapist seeing the session as deeper. the clients were Caucasian females. The therapists were all
Future research could test whether this sequence of events does graduate trainees, and we are not certain whether the results
happen in more successful treatments. would replicate with a more seasoned group of clinicians.
Another possible explanation for the positive relationship between We tested for therapist nesting and found significant nesting for
client therapeutic alliance and therapist Arousal in cases in which the only clients’ ratings of positivity. It is important to note, however,
client makes a reliable change is that therapists may be aroused by the that the test for nesting was conducted on a fairly small sample,
emotional experience of being with clients who are doing the work of which may not have been large enough to detect what Kenny,
counseling. Therapists consider that their clients are working at a deep Kashy, and Bolger (1998) called “consequential nonindepen-
level when clients express unacceptable feelings (Greenberg & Sa- dence.” In other words, there may have been nesting effects in our
fran, 1987). They would argue that it is the experiential learning, of data, but our tests were not powerful enough to detect it. Therefore,
being cared for and valued by the therapist during these moments of the results of the APIM should be viewed cautiously and hopefully
vulnerability that facilitates change. In essence, change in symptoms replicated in larger samples. Finally, we presumed that therapists
occurs when clients are being more emotionally vulnerable in the would be able to assess their clients’ view of the alliance. How-
therapy. Relatedly, Fosha (2000) described the experience of “alive- ever, we do not know how well our therapist could detect their
ness” and “realness” that is experienced when people are able to client’s perceptions of the therapeutic alliance.
express these vulnerable affects in therapy. Fosha argued that thera- Another limitation is that alliance and session evaluation were
pists can rely on their own internal experience to determine whether measured only in an early session. It will be important for future
their clients are doing the work in the treatment. She says when clients research to examine how actor and partner effects may change across
are less defensive and genuinely experiencing core emotions (i.e., counseling sessions. For example, theory suggests that the client’s
exhibiting a strong alliance), the therapist feels as though therapy is observing capacity increases across treatment. This suggests that later
moving or is alive (i.e., aroused), whereas defensiveness and lack of in treatment, clients’ session evaluations would be related to both their
authenticity in the client engender boredom or the experience of going own and their therapists’ alliance perceptions. Despite these limita-
nowhere. tions, the findings offer interesting directions for future research.
The present study suggests that we need more research to tease
apart what is happening in these early sessions of treatments and
Conclusion
how these differences may impact therapists’ and clients’ percep-
tions of the alliance and Arousal. This is an important area of The publication of five different meta-analyses is proof that
research because therapists come from a different vantage point researchers are interested in how the therapy alliance relates to
and may feel invigorated by the therapy process, whereas clients treatment progress and outcome (Flückiger, Del Re, Wampold,
may feel more somber, saddened, or unsettled after sessions. Symonds, & Horvath, 2012; Horvath & Bedi, 2002; Horvath et al.,
Clients may feel more introspective versus feelings as if they are 2011; Horvath & Symonds, 1991; Martin, Garske, & Davis, 2000).
“moving” or “energetic.” Watson and Greenberg (1996) even Unfortunately, the majority of our knowledge of the alliance
found that some “change producing interventions are experienced comes from studies that focus only on the client’s perspective. This
as disorganizing and dissatisfying” (p. 272) by clients and that the is one of only a handful of studies that have used the APIM in a
initial reaction is often the result of confronting problems that can therapeutic context, and it is the first study to examine how
make one feel worse before feeling better. In this regard, however, treatment outcome would moderate these actor–partner relation-
it is important to note that reliable change was associated with ships. When using APIM, we found that client’s ratings of the
22 KIVLIGHAN, MARMAROSH, AND HILSENROTH

therapeutic alliance are related to their therapist’s ratings of ses- Gelso, C. J. (2011). The real relationship in psychotherapy: The hidden
sion Depth. We also found that there are significant positive foundation of change. Washington, DC: American Psychological Asso-
relationships between the alliance and Arousal when clients make ciation. doi:10.1037/12349-000
a reliable change in treatment. Our results encourage us to continue Gelso, C. J., Kelley, F. A., Fuertes, J. N., Marmarosh, C., Holmes, S. E.,
to study the alliance and to apply innovative methods, such as Costa, C., & Hancock, G. R. (2005). Measuring the real relationship in
psychotherapy: Initial validation of the therapist form. Journal of Coun-
APIM, to understand and appreciate the complexity and interactive
seling Psychology, 52, 640 – 649. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.52.4.640
nature of the psychotherapy relationship. Gelso, C. J., Kivlighan, D. M., Jr., Wine, B., Jones, A., & Friedman, S. C.
In addition, this was the first study to examine the hypothesis that (1997). Transference, insight, and the course of time-limited therapy.
the therapeutic alliance would be differentially related to aspects of Journal of Counseling Psychology, 44, 209 –217. doi:10.1037/0022-
session outcome. As noted above, we found two significant partner 0167.44.2.209
effects (client alliance and therapist depth, client alliance–RCI inter- Gelso, C. J., Kivlighan, D. M., Jr., Busa-Knepp, J., Spiegel, E. B., Ain, S.,
action and therapist arousal), which both involved working aspects of Hummel, A. M., & Markin, R. D. (2012). The unfolding of the real
session outcome. As argued above, these partner effects are especially relationship and the outcome of brief psychotherapy. Journal of Coun-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

important because they are a statistical way of identifying collabora- seling Psychology, 59, 495–506. doi:10.1037/a0029838
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tion and influence. Therefore, these partner effects provide some Greenberg, L., & Safran, J. (1987). Emotion in psychotherapy: Affect,
evidence to support differential outcome effects. cognition and the process of change. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Hatcher, R. L. (1999). Therapists’ view of treatment alliance and collab-
The concepts of work-related and relationship-related outcomes
oration in therapy. Psychotherapy Research, 9, 405– 423.
comes from Gelso’s (2011) distinction that the working alliance is
Hatcher, R. L., & Barends, A. W. (1996). Patient’s view of the alliance in
related to the work of counseling, whereas the real relationship is psychotherapy: Exploratory factor analysis of three alliance measures.
related to the personal relationship aspect of counseling. Future re- Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 6, 1326 –1336.
searchers could explore this conceptualization in more depth by doi:10.1037/0022-006X.64.6.1326
simultaneously measuring the therapeutic alliance and the real rela- Hatcher, R. L., & Barends, A. W. (2006). How a return to theory could help
tionship and relating them both to the different aspects of session alliance research. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training,
outcome. In addition, future research may want to examine other 43, 292–299. doi:10.1037/0033-3204.43.3.292
aspects of session outcome like session impact, which more explicitly Hatcher, R. L., Barends, A., Hansell, J., & Gutfreund, M. (1995). Patients’
operationalizes work and relationship impacts (Stiles et al., 1994). and therapists’ shared and unique views of the therapeutic alliance: An
investigation using confirmatory factor analysis in a nested design.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 636 – 643. doi:
References 10.1037/0022-006X.63.4.636
Hill, C. E., & Lambert, M. J. (2004). Methodological issues in studying
Ackerman, S. J., Hilsenroth, M. J., Baity, M. R., & Blagys, M. D. (2000).
psychotherapy process and outcomes. In M. J. Lambert (Ed.), Bergin
Interaction of therapeutic process and alliance during psychological
and Garfield: Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (5th ed.,
assessment. Journal of Personality Assessment, 75, 82–109. doi:
10.1207/S15327752JPA7501_7 pp. 84 –135). New York, NY: Wiley.
Blagys, M., & Hilsenroth, M. (2000). Distinctive features of short-term Hilsenroth, M. (2007). A programmatic study of short-term psychody-
psychodynamic-interpersonal psychotherapy: A review of the compara- namic psychotherapy: Assessment, process, outcome, and training. Psy-
tive psychotherapy process literature. Clinical Psychology: Science and chotherapy Research, 17, 31– 45. doi:10.1080/10503300600953504
Practice, 7, 167–188. doi:10.1093/clipsy.7.2.167 Horvath, A. O., & Bedi, R. P. (2002). The alliance. In J. C. Norcross (Ed.),
Boswell, J. F., Castonguay, L. G., & Wasserman, R. H. (2010). Effects of Psychotherapy relationships that work: Therapist contributions and
psychotherapy training and intervention use on session outcome. Jour- responsiveness to patient needs (pp. 37– 69). New York, NY: Oxford
nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78, 717–723. doi:10.1037/ University Press.
a0020088 Horvath, A. O., Del Re, A. C., Flückiger, C., & Symonds, D. (2011).
Clemence, A., Hilsenroth, M., Ackerman, S., Strassle, C., & Handler, L. Alliance in individual psychotherapy. Psychotherapy, 48, 9 –16. doi:
(2005). Facets of the therapeutic alliance and perceived progress in 10.1037/a0022186
psychotherapy: Relationship between patient and therapist perspectives. Horvath, A. O., & Greenberg, L. S. (1989). Development and validation of
Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 12, 443– 454. doi:10.1002/cpp the Working Alliance Inventory. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 36,
.467 223–233. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.36.2.223
Derogatis, L. (1993). Brief Symptom Inventory. Minneapolis, MN: National Horvath, A. O., & Symonds, B. D. (1991). Relation between the working
Computer Systems. alliance and outcome in psychotherapy: A meta-analysis. Journal of
Dunlop, W. L., & Beauchamp, M. R. (2011). Does similarity make a Counseling Psychology, 38, 139 –149. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.38.2.139
difference? Predicting cohesion and attendance behaviors within exer- Jacobson, N. S., Roberts, L. J., Berns, S. B., & McGlinchy, J. B. (1999).
cise group settings. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, Methods for defining and determining the clinical significance of treat-
15, 258 –266. doi:10.1037/a0023642 ment effects: Description, application and alternatives. Journal of Con-
Eugster, S. L., & Wampold, B. E. (1996). Systematic effects of participant sulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 300 –307. doi:10.1037/0022-006X
role on evaluation of the psychotherapy session. Journal of Consulting .67.3.300
and Clinical Psychology, 64, 1020 –1028. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.64.5 Jacobson, N. S., & Truax, P. (1991). Clinical significance: A statistical
.1020 approach to defining meaningful change in psychotherapy research.
Flückiger, C., Del Re, A. C., Wampold, B. E., Symonds, D., & Horvath, Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 12–19. doi:10.1037/
A. O. (2012). How central is the alliance in psychotherapy? A multilevel 0022-006X.59.1.12
longitudinal meta-analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 59, 10 – Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., & Bolger, N. (1998). Data analysis in social
17. doi:10.1037/a0025749 psychology. In D. Gilbert, S. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of
Fosha, D. (2000). The transforming power of affect: A model of accelerated social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 233–265). Boston, MA: McGraw-
change. New York, NY: Basic Books. Hill.
CLIENT AND THERAPIST ALLIANCE 23

Kenny, D. A., Mannetti, L., Pierro, A., Livi, S., & Kashy, D. A. (2002). Pesale, F. P., Hilsenroth, M. J., & Owen, J. J. (2012). Patient early session
The statistical analysis of data from small groups. Journal of Personality experience and treatment outcome. Psychotherapy Research, 22, 417–
and Social Psychology, 83, 126 –137. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.1.126 425. doi:10.1080/10503307.2012.662607
Kim, B. K., Ng, G. F., & Ann, A. J. (2009). Client adherence to Asian Raudenbush, S. W., Bryk, A. S., Cheong, Y. F., & Congdon, R. (2005).
cultural values, common factors in counseling, and session outcome with HLM6: Hierarchical linear and nonlinear modeling [Computer software
Asian American clients at a university counseling center. Journal of manual]. Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International.
Counseling and Development, 87, 131–142. doi:10.1002/j.1556-6678 Richards, C. (2011). Alliance ruptures: Etiology and resolution. Counsel-
.2009.tb00560.x ling Psychology Review, 26, 56 – 62.
Kivlighan, D. M., Jr. (2007). Where is the relationship in research on the Stiles, W. B., Reynolds, S., Hardy, G. E., Rees, A., Barkham, M., &
alliance? Two methods for analyzing dyadic data. Journal of Counseling Shapiro, D. A. (1994). Evaluation and description of psychotherapy
Psychology, 54, 423– 433. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.54.4.423 sessions by clients using the Session Evaluation Questionnaire and the
Ledermann, T., & Kenny, D. A. (2012). The common fate model for dyadic Session Impacts Scale. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 41, 175–185.
data: Variations of a theoretically important but underutilized model. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.41.2.175
Journal of Family Psychology, 26, 140 –148. doi:10.1037/a0026624 Stiles, W. B., Shapiro, D. A., & Firth-Cozens, J. A. (1988). Verbal
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Lent, R. W., Hoffman, M., Hill, C. E., Treistman, D., Mount, M., & response mode use in contrasting psychotherapies: A within-subjects
comparison. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 727–
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Singley, D. (2006). Client-specific therapist self-efficacy in novice ther-


apists: Relation to perceptions of session quality. Journal of Counseling 733. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.56.5.727
Psychology, 53, 453– 463. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.53.4.453 Stiles, W. B., & Snow, J. S. (1984). Counseling session impact as viewed
Mallinckrodt, B. (1993). Session impact, working alliance, and treatment by novice counselors and their clients. Journal of Counseling Psychol-
outcome of brief counseling. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 40, ogy, 31, 3–12. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.31.1.3
25–32. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.40.1.25 Sullivan, H. (1954). The psychiatric interview. Oxford, England: W. W.
Martin, D. J., Garske, J. P., & Davis, K. M. (2000). Relation of the Norton & Co.
therapeutic alliance with outcome and other variables: A meta-analytic Tryon, G., & Kane, A. S. (1995). Client involvement, working alliance, and
review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 438 – 450. type of therapy termination. Psychotherapy Research, 5, 189 –198. doi:
doi:10.1037/0022-006X.68.3.438 10.1080/10503309512331331306
Owen, J., Quirk, K., Hilsenroth, M. J., & Rodolfa, E. (2012). Working Watson, J. C., & Greenberg, L. S. (1996). Pathways to change in the
through: In-session processes that promote between-session thoughts psychotherapy of depression: Relating process to session change and
and activities. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 59, 161–167. doi: outcome. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 33,
10.1037/a0023616 262–274. doi:10.1037/0033-3204.33.2.262
Pesale, F. P., & Hilsenroth, M. J. (2009). Patient and therapist perspectives
on session depth in relation to technique during psychodynamic psycho- Received February 15, 2013
therapy. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 46, 390 – Revision received September 26, 2013
396. doi:10.1037/a0016999 Accepted September 26, 2013 䡲

View publication stats

You might also like