You are on page 1of 4

BRIEF REPORT

Relationship Between Visual-Motor Integration


and Handwriting Skills of Children in Kindergarten:
A Modified Replication Study
Christopher J. Daly, Gail T. Kelley, Andrea Krauss

KEY WORDS OBJECTIVE. The purpose of this study was to examine the relationship of performance on the
• development Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration (VMI; Beery, 1997) to handwriting legibility in children
• fine motor skills attending kindergarten. The relationship of using lined versus unlined paper on letter legibility, based on a
• visual-motor integration modified version of the Scale of Children’s Readiness in PrinTing (Modified SCRIPT; Weil & Cunningham
Amundson, 1994) was also investigated.
METHOD. Fifty-four typically developing kindergarten students were administered the VMI; 30 students
completed the Modified SCRIPT with unlined paper, 24 students completed the Modified SCRIPT with lined
paper. Students were assessed in the first quarter of the kindergarten school year and scores were analyzed
using correlational and nonparametric statistical measures.
RESULTS. Strong positive relationships were found between VMI assessment scores and student’s ability
to legibly copy letterforms. Students who could copy the first nine forms on the VMI performed significantly
better than students who could not correctly copy the first nine VMI forms on both versions of the Modified
SCRIPT.
CONCLUSION. Visual-motor integration skills were shown to be related to the ability to copy letters legi-
bly. These findings support the research of Weil and Cunningham Amundson. Findings from this study also
support the conclusion that there is no significant difference in letter writing legibility between students who
use paper with or without lines.

Daly, C. J., Kelley, G. T., & Krauss, A. (2003). Brief report—Relationship between visual-motor integration and handwrit-
ing skills of children in kindergarten: A modified replication study. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 57,
459–462.

Christopher J. Daly, MS, OTR/L, is Senior Occupational


Therapist, New York City Department of Education,
New York, New York. Mailing address: 96 Schermerhorn
H andwriting skill development is an
increasingly relevant topic in occupa-
tional therapy. Handwriting skills and the
among children in the first years of ele-
mentary school. These differences result in
writing readiness that spans the age range
Street, Apt. 4C, Brooklyn, New York 11201;
cjdaly7@ix.netcom.com. underlying performance components are from 4 to 6 years, depending on the devel-
particularly germane topics in pediatric opment of each individual child.
Gail T. Kelley, MS, OTR/L, is Occupational Therapist, and school-based occupational therapy due Developmentally, a kindergarten age child
New York City Department of Education, New York, to the high frequency of referrals for chil- is sharpening fine motor skills and visual
New York.
dren with handwriting legibility problems perceptual skills that together enable them
Andrea Krauss, DSW, BCP, OTR/L, at the time of the study
(Oliver, 1990; Reisman, 1991). The rise in to perform activities requiring visual-motor
was Associate Professor, Touro College, School of Health occupational therapy referrals coupled with integration, specifically handwriting
Sciences, Bay Shore, New York. Dr. Krauss maintains a the increase in the demands placed on (Beery, 1997). Weil and Cunningham
pediatric private practice in Great Neck, Long Island, school-aged children has led to an even Amundson place an emphasis on visual-
New York. stronger emphasis on handwriting skill motor integration as an integral compo-
research and effective intervention nent in handwriting skill development,
(Benbow, Hanft, & Marsh, 1992; Lamme, while Benbow et al. (1992) cite visual-
1979; McHale & Cermak, 1992). motor skills and fine motor coordination as
Lamme (1979), Laszlo and Bairstow foundational component skills of hand-
(1984), and Weil and Cunningham writing.
Amundson (1994) recognize the wide Further controversy exists regarding
range of skills and maturational differences the use of lined versus unlined paper for

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 459


Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 12/29/2019 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms
handwriting instruction (Lamme, 1979; ty of kindergarten students have the devel- of typically developing children from the
Pasternicki, 1987; Weil & Cunningham opmental advantage to succeed in learning first four classrooms to volunteer was used.
Amundson, 1994). A pilot study conduct- correct letter formation (Weil & Fifty-four participants were used in the
ed by Weil and Cunningham Amundson Cunningham Amundson). study, 29 were male and 25 were female. At
determined that using unlined paper pro- The purpose of this study was to par- the time of the assessments (October to
moted handwriting legibility in kinder- tially replicate the Weil and Cunningham November 1999), the participants’ ages
garten age children. Lamme likewise sup- Amundson (1994) research, regarding the ranged from 59 months (4.91 years) to 71
ported the use of paper without lines for relationship between visual-motor and months (5.91 years), with a mean age of 65
this age group, proposing that unlined handwriting skills in kindergarten-age stu- months (5.41 years), SD = 3.40. Fifty par-
paper enhances letter legibility by decreas- dents. This study also sought to contribute ticipants were right-handed and four partic-
ing the demands placed upon the child. to the body of literature that recognizes the ipants were left-handed.
However, Pasternicki opposed this view VMI as a useful screening tool, as well as
concluding that children demonstrated acknowledge visual-motor integration as an Instrumentation and Data Collection
improved letter legibility using lined paper important prewriting skill. Opposing views The Modified SCRIPT and the VMI Short
for writing activities. regarding the type of paper media that best Form (ages 3 to 8 years) were used for this
Weil and Cunningham Amundson promote letter legibility also warranted study. Thirty students completed the
(1994) examined the relationship between investigation in this study. The study Modified SCRIPT without lines and 24
visual-motor integration skills and the abil- explored three research questions: students completed the Modified SCRIPT
ity to copy letters legibly in kindergarten 1. Is there a relationship between kinder- with lines. The classrooms were randomly
students. The Weil and Cunningham garten student scores on the VMI and assigned assessments with and without
Amundson study had a sample of 60 typi- their ability to copy letters legibly? lines. The Modified SCRIPT handwriting
cally developing students, ages 64 to 75 2. Will students who correctly copy the first assessment differed from the original
months. The study employed the Scale of nine forms on the VMI (score of 12) SCRIPT (Weil & Cunningham Amund-
Children’s Readiness in PrinTing (SCRIPT; demonstrate more legible handwriting as son, 1994) with regard to page layout and
Weil & Cunningham Amundson), a hand- indicated by higher scores on the use of lined paper. The original SCRIPT
writing assessment using unlined paper that Modified SCRIPT? has a horizontal page layout with two rows
was developed by the first author, as well as 3. Will students using Modified SCRIPT containing three boxes of equal size. The
the Developmental Test of Visual Motor assessments with unlined paper produce boxes in the first row contain one letterform
Integration (VMI; Beery, 1997). Included more legible letters versus those using each whereas the box below is blank for let-
in their study were 30 female and 30 male assessments with lined paper? ter reproduction. Weil and Cunningham
students from six different kindergarten Amundson included all lowercase letters of
classes from a single school district in Method the alphabet, followed by eight uppercase
Seattle, Washington. Weil and letters (A, K, M, N, V, W, Y, Z).
Cunningham Amundson found a moderate Study Design The original SCRIPT (Weil &
correlation (r = .47, p < .001) between stu- Fifty-four typically developing kindergarten Cunningham Amundson, 1994) was mod-
dent’s visual-motor skills (score on VMI) students were administered the VMI; 30 ified for comparison of handwriting legibil-
and their ability to copy letters legibly students completed the Modified SCRIPT ity using lined versus unlined paper. The
(score on SCRIPT). The authors found with unlined paper, 24 students completed Modified SCRIPT also used a horizontal
that as students’ scores on the VMI the Modified SCRIPT with lined paper. A page layout but with two vertical rows.
increased, so did scores on the SCRIPT. two-member research team evaluated and Letter forms were positioned in columns
Students who were able to copy the first scored the participant assessments. The with space to the right for copying. On the
nine forms on the VMI were found to per- evaluators were occupational therapy grad- unlined version of the Modified SCRIPT
form better on the SCRIPT, using Mann- uate students trained in evaluation scoring the model letters were presented without
Whitney U tests (2-tailed, p = .02). Results and analysis as a part of degree require- lines and the reproduction space was also
also showed that students who were able to ments. This experiment was conducted void of lines. The lined version of the
copy the first nine forms on the VMI were during the 1999–2000 school year. Modified SCRIPT contained model letters
better able to copy letter forms legibly, presented within lines and lined reproduc-
thereby placing them at an optimal level at Participants tion spaces to the right. The Modified
which to begin formal handwriting instruc- The participants in this study were selected SCRIPT used the same order and number
tion (Beery; Benbow et al., 1992). The from four kindergarten classrooms from an of lowercase and uppercase letterforms as
study further recommended that the appro- elementary school in Suffolk County, New the original SCRIPT. The Modified
priate time to initiate formal handwriting York. The school principal recruited class- SCRIPT scoring procedure was the same as
instruction was the second half of the room teachers to recruit kindergarten stu- on the SCRIPT (Weil & Cunningham
kindergarten school year, when the majori- dents for this study. A convenience sample Amundson), where subjects received one

460 July/August 2003, Volume 57/Number 4


Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 12/29/2019 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms
point for each correct letter following spec- children’s performance on the VMI and the Discussion
ified criteria (available upon request). A ability to copy 34 letterforms (Weil & A strong positive relationship was found
possible high score of 34 and low score of 0 Cunningham Amundson, 1994), was sup- between kindergarten student’s perfor-
may have been achieved. ported with a Pearson product-moment mance on the VMI and their ability to copy
The VMI Short Form (ages 3 to 8 correlation coefficient (Pearson r) of r = .64, letter forms legibly (r = .64, p = .001). This
years) contains 15 geometric shapes that p < .01. For data analysis the authors estab- finding is consistent with the research of a
become progressively more complex and lished that an r value of at least r = .30 number of other authors in this area regard-
challenging to copy. The student is asked to would signify an acceptable effect size as ing the relationship between visual-motor
copy the shape in the space provided below, well as a moderate relationship between and handwriting skills (Maeland, 1992;
with three shapes presented on each page. variables and r = .50 and above represented Tseng & Cermak, 1993; Tseng & Murray,
The VMI is used primarily as a screening a strong relationship (Cohen, 1988). Thus, 1994; Weil & Cunningham Amundson,
tool to identify proficiency in visual-motor a strong relationship was demonstrated 1994). Our findings support the conclusion
integration (Beery, 1997). The VMI and between visual-motor skills and the ability that the VMI was a useful screening tool for
the Modified SCRIPT were administered to copy letters legibly. handwriting abilities, thereby noting visual-
to participating classrooms in group format The second research question, which motor integration as a requisite skill for
as instructed by the VMI manual (Beery). asked if students who could correctly copy handwriting legibility.
The VMI was administered to students 1 the first nine forms on the VMI (score of Students who were able to correctly
week prior to the Modified SCRIPT by the 12) would demonstrate superior handwrit- copy (n = 30) the first nine forms on the
same examiner; all participants used stan- ing legibility as indicated by higher scores VMI (score of 12) performed better on the
dard #2 pencils. The VMI was scored by on the Modified SCRIPT, was supported Modified SCRIPT in comparison to those
specific criteria in accordance with instruc- by Kruskal-Wallis Test (2-tailed, p < 0.001). students who could not correctly copy (n =
tion manual (Beery). Students received one On VMI assessments, 24 students were 24) the first nine forms. This finding lends
point for each correctly copied shape until unable to copy the first nine forms correct- support to the body of research that sug-
three errors in a row were recorded. A pos- ly while 30 students were able to correctly gests that students are ready to engage in
sible high score of 18 and low score of 0 copy the forms. The 30 students who could formal handwriting instruction once they
may have been achieved. correctly copy the first nine forms on the have mastered the ability to copy the first
Data Analysis VMI demonstrated higher scores on Mod- nine forms on the VMI (Beery, 1997;
ified SCRIPT assessments (see Table 1). Benbow et al., 1992; Weil & Cunningham
The first two authors administered and Amundson, 1994). The large standard devi-
The third research question asked if
scored all assessment measures using the ation for Modified SCRIPT assessments
there was a difference in kindergarten chil-
standardized procedures. Examiners did not supported statements regarding the wide
dren’s writing legibility for Modified
score the tests personally administered. variation of letter copying abilities in this
SCRIPT assessments on unlined paper (n =
Prior to scoring, raters reviewed and agreed age group of children (Benbow et al.;
30) versus lined paper (n = 24). The analy-
upon scoring criteria and jointly rated Lamme, 1979; Weil & Cunningham
sis revealed no relationship between student
assessments when the criteria for correct Amundson). Recognition of the matura-
groups using Kruskal-Wallis Test (2-tailed,
answers was in question. Descriptive statis- tional variations among kindergartners
p = .60). The total mean score for students
tics were used to calculate the means and should be considered when implementing a
using lined assessments (n = 24) was 20.7,
standard deviations for all assessment handwriting curriculum.
SD = 9.3. The total mean score for students
scores. Because the data on the Modified In the current study, students were
using unlined assessments (n = 30) was
SCRIPT and VMI total scores had approx- assessed in the first quarter of the kinder-
19.5, SD = 7.8. Thus, no significant differ-
imately normal distribution, a Pearson
ence was found in the writing performance garten school year; 24 of 54 typically devel-
product-moment correlation coefficient
of kindergarten students using unlined or oping students were unable to correctly
was used to test the first research question.
lined assessments. copy the first nine forms on the VMI. Weil
The Kruskal-Wallis test was used to analyze
research questions two and three because
scores were not normally distributed in Table 1. Modified SCRIPT Total Mean Scores for Groups of Children Scoring Below 12 or 12
each group and due to the small group size. and Above on the VMI (Kruskal-Wallis Test)
The data were analyzed using SPSS statisti- M. SCRIPT 2-tailed
cal package (Norusis, 1991). Group n M SD Low/High Scores p value
VMI Scores
< 12 24 14.2 7.4 4/31 < 0.001
Results VMI Scores
≥ 12 30 24.9 5.9 11/32 < 0.001
The first research question, which asked if Note. Modified SCRIPT = Modified Scale of Children’s Readiness in PrinTing, VMI = Developmental Test of
there is a relationship between kindergarten Visual-Motor Integration.

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 461


Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 12/29/2019 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms
and Cunningham Amundson (1994) Implications for Practice Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the
demonstrated that only 7 of 59 typically behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ:
Results of this pilot study suggest that visu- Erlbaum.
developing kindergarten students were
al-motor integration, as measured by the Lamme, L. L. (1979). Handwriting in an early
unable to correctly copy the first nine forms
VMI, is strongly related to kindergarten childhood curriculum. Young Children,
on the VMI when tested in the second half
students’ ability to copy letterforms. These 35(1), 20–27.
of the kindergarten school year. The limit-
data support the use of the VMI as a Laszlo, J. I., & Bairstow, P. J. (1984).
ed data from this study may lend support to
screening tool for examining the visual- Handwriting: Difficulties and possible
Weil and Cunningham Amundson, who
motor integration skills of typically devel- solutions. School Psychology International,
concluded that the latter half of the kinder-
oping kindergarten students. Based on 5, 207–213.
garten school year was a developmentally Maeland, A. F. (1992). Handwriting and per-
Modified SCRIPT assessment scores, a
appropriate time to initiate formal hand- ceptual motor skills in clumsy, dysgraphic,
wide range of letter copying abilities was
writing instruction for most students. and normal children. Perceptual and Motor
demonstrated by this age group of typical-
No relationship was found between Skills, 13, 1207–1217.
ly developing children in the first quarter
the use of lined or unlined writing paper McHale, K., & Cermak, S. A. (1992). Fine
of the kindergarten school year.
and the handwriting legibility of kinder- motor activities in elementary school:
Replication of this study including admin-
garten students. Weil and Cunningham Preliminary findings and provisional impli-
istration of the VMI and the Modified cations for children with fine motor prob-
Amundson (1994) found that using paper
SCRIPT to kindergarten students at differ- lems. American Journal of Occupational
without writing guidelines appeared to
ent intervals during the school year would Therapy, 46, 898–903.
enhance the writing legibility of kinder-
allow for better examination of visual- Norusis, M. J. (1991). SPSS guide to data anal-
garten students. Although data from Weil
motor integration as a predictor of hand- ysis for SPSS /PC+ (2nd ed.). Chicago:
and Cunningham Amundson and Lamme
writing readiness and legibility.▲ SPSS.
(1979) supported the use of unlined paper
Oliver, C. E. (1990). A sensorimotor program
to enhance letter legibility, findings were
for improving writing readiness skills in
not supported by this replication study. Acknowledgments elementary-age children. American Journal
Thus, the results of the current study sug- of Occupational Therapy, 44, 111–116.
We thank the students, parents, and teach-
gest that kindergarten age children be Pasternicki, J. (1987). Paper for writing:
ers in the school district of this research
allowed to experiment with various types of Research and recommendations. In J.
who were involved and participated in the
writing paper media when initially learning Alston & J. Taylor (Eds.), Handwriting:
study. Sincere thanks are extended to
proper letter formation, and to explore Theory, research, and practice (pp. 68–80).
Joseph Indelicato, PhD, for his guidance
options that may enhance the quality of London: Croom-Helm.
with data analysis. This research project
written output. Reisman, J. E. (1991). Poor handwriting: Who
met partial fulfillment of the requirements
is referred? American Journal of
for the first author’s master of science Occupational Therapy, 45, 849–852.
Limitations degree, Touro College, School of Health Tseng, M. H., & Cermak, S. A. (1993). The
Sciences. influence of ergonomic factors and percep-
Limitations to the current study regard
sample and scoring procedures. Since a tual-motor abilities on handwriting perfor-
mance. American Journal of Occupational
convenience sample from only one school References Therapy, 47, 919–926.
district was used, the participants did not
Beery, K. E. (1997). The Beery-Buktenica devel- Tseng, M. H., & Murray, E. A. (1994).
effectively represent a heterogeneous popu-
opmental test of visual-motor integration Differences in perceptual motor measures
lation of kindergarten students, as a ran- in children with good and poor handwrit-
(Rev. 4th ed.). Parsippany, NJ: Modern
dom sample would have. Additionally, only ing. Occupational Therapy Journal of
Curriculum Press.
typically developing students were assessed ; Benbow, M., Hanft, B., & Marsh, D. (1992). Research, 14(1), 19–36.
thus, the research has no implications for Handwriting in the classroom: Improving Weil, M. J., & Cunningham Amundson, S. J.
learning disabled students. Finally, inter- written communication. In C. B. Royeen (1994). Relationship between visuomotor
rater reliability and test–retest reliability (Ed.). AOTA self study series: Classroom and handwriting skills of children in
were not statistically established for the applications for school-based practice. kindergarten. American Journal of
Modified SCRIPT. Rockville, MD: AOTA. Occupational Therapy, 48, 982–988.

462 July/August 2003, Volume 57/Number 4


Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 12/29/2019 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms

You might also like