You are on page 1of 13

Available online at www.sciencedirect.

com

Cognitive and Behavioral Practice 19 (2012) 606-618


www.elsevier.com/locate/cabp
1
Contains Video

Brief Acceptance and Commitment Therapy and Exposure for Panic Disorder:
A Pilot Study
Alicia E. Meuret, Southern Methodist University
Michael P. Twohig, Utah State University
David Rosenfield, Southern Methodist University
Steven C. Hayes, University of Nevada
Michelle G. Craske, University of California, Los Angeles

Cognitive and biobehavioral coping skills are central to psychosocial therapies and are taught to facilitate and improve exposure
therapy. While traditional coping skills are aimed at controlling maladaptive thoughts or dysregulations in physiology, newer
approaches that explore acceptance, defusion, and values-based direction have been gaining interest. Acceptance and Commitment
Therapy (ACT) involves creating an open, nonjudgmental stance toward whatever thoughts, feelings, and bodily sensations arise in a
given moment, experiencing them for what they are, and moving toward them while inner experiences such as anxiety are present. This
approach can be seen as consistent with exposure therapies and may be utilized to organize and facilitate engagement in exposure
exercises. This study examines the feasibility and efficacy for combining a brief ACT protocol with traditional exposure therapy. Eleven
patients with panic disorder with or without agoraphobia received 4 sessions of ACT followed by 6 sessions of exposure therapy, with
data collected on a weekly basis. Acquisition of ACT skills and their application during exposure was monitored using a novel
“think-aloud” technology. Treatment was associated with clinically significant improvements in panic symptom severity, willingness to
allow inner experiences to occur, and reductions in avoidant behavior. Although preliminary, results suggest that our brief training in
ACT only (as assessed prior to exposure exercises) and in combination with exposure therapy was acceptable to patients and offered
benefits on the order of large effect sizes. Clinical and research implications are discussed.

C and biobehavioral coping skills are central


OGNITIVE
to psychosocial therapies for anxiety disorders and
are taught to facilitate and improve exposure therapy.
more heavily on acceptance and mindfulness processes as
ways of responding to internal experiences (i.e., thoughts,
feelings, bodily sensations) than more traditional CBT
These control-based coping skills typically aim to change protocols. To this end, ACT is more similar to other
catastrophic appraisals or change somatic symptoms. acceptance- and mindfulness-based interventions, such as
While traditional coping skills are aimed at controlling Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy (Segal, Williams, &
maladaptive thoughts or dysregulations in physiology, Teasdale, 2002), that focus on the function of internal
newer approaches that explore acceptance or willingness experiences rather than their form or content.
to experience such states have been gaining interest. One With the focus shifting away from the question of
specific cognitive behavioral modality that is receiving whether “CBT works” to “why it works” (i.e., mediators)
increased attention is Acceptance and Commitment and “for whom” (i.e., moderators), the examination of
Therapy (ACT; Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 2011). ACT is novel approaches seems particularly vital (Kazdin, 2007;
part of the cognitive behavioral tradition, but it focuses McNally, 2007; Meuret, Wolitzky-Taylor, Twohig, &
Craske, 2012). While the combination of therapeutic
1
Video patients/clients are portrayed by actors.
components that make up traditional CBT is generally
effective for patients suffering from an anxiety disorder
(Norton & Price, 2007; Westen & Morrison, 2001), there
Keywords: Acceptance and Commitment Therapy; exposure; behav- is considerable room for improvement. This seems
ioral; panic; efficacy
particularly true for patients suffering from panic
1077-7229/12/606-618$1.00/0 disorder with or without agoraphobia (PD/A). Here,
© 2012 Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies. effect sizes for CBT are the smallest among the anxiety
Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. disorders (Andrews, Cuijpers, Craske, McEvoy, & Titov,
Brief ACT and Exposure 607

2010; Hofmann & Smits, 2008; but not Norton & Price, to reduce emotional arousal but to practice acceptance of
2007), attrition is high (Haby, Donnelly, Corry, & Vos, private experiences and the ability to function in a more
2006), and a large percentage of completers do not reach free, flexible, and values-based way in their presence”
responder status after treatment (T. Brown & Barlow, (Hayes et al., 2011, p. 400; see also Hayes, 1987, p. 365). This
1995). view is consistent with a current view of exposure put forth
ACT involves the creation of an open, allowing attitude by Craske and colleagues (2008) that exposure may work by
and nonjudgmental curiosity toward whatever emotions optimizing learning based on increasing tolerance for fear
arise in a given moment, including panic-related ones, and anxiety, rather than the traditional focus on fear
and the ability to then mindfully turn attention toward reduction. With empirical support for fear reduction
values-based actions (Hayes et al., 2011). It aims to help serving as a predictor of treatment outcome largely lacking
clients notice panic-related inner experiences, see them (Baker, Mystkowksi, Culver, Yi, Mortazavi, & Craske, 2010;
as simply events occurring within the body, allow them to Craske et al., 2008; Culver, Stoyanova & Craske, 2012;
either occur or not, and continue a valued direction with Kircanski et al., 2012; Meuret, Seidel, Rosenfield, Hofmann,
these experiences as aspects of life. While these pro- & Rosenfield, in press), the quest for identifying outcome
cedures are not antithetical to traditional CBT pro- predictors is more pertinent than ever.
cedures, ACT makes no attempts to eliminate the Hence, ACT may offer a potent alternative approach
content of inner experiences. In traditional CBT, clients that enhances the acceptability of and engagement in
are encouraged to learn cognitive skills to control exposure therapy/behavioral change by creation of
negative thoughts or somatic skills to control dysregulated response flexibility and new learning that occurs while
physiology, which are then applied during exposure exposing oneself to the feared stimuli. Preliminary
to feared sensations or situations. In ACT training, evidence for this thesis has been shown in recent
clients learn how to function with or without the presence experimental studies in which brief training in emotional
of panic-related inner experiences, rather than trying acceptance lowered distress and increased tolerance for
to regulate or control them before changing behavior. experimentally induced anxiety symptoms in individuals
As such, there is little concern for the intensity or with PD/A (Campbell-Sills, Barlow, Brown, & Hofmann,
frequency of panic-related thought, feelings, or bodily 2006; Eifert & Heffner, 2003; including carbon-dioxide
sensations. inhalation challenges, Levitt et al., 2004). Similar results
There is support for this conceptualization of PD/A have been shown for pain tolerance (e.g., Hayes, Bissett,
(e.g., Levitt et al., 2004; Levitt & Karekla, 2005; Orsillo, et al., 1999) and exposure to distressing International
Roemer, Block-Lerner, & Tull, 2004) and growing evidence Affective Picture System slides (Arch & Craske, 2006).
supporting the utility of ACT for anxiety disorders (as The current investigation aimed to assess the feasibility
reviewed in Codd, Twohig, Crosby, & Enno, 2011). and efficacy of a behaviorally based ACT treatment that
Correlational studies have shown that psychological in- was systematically divided into a skill training phase and a
flexibility (the target process in ACT) predicts anxiety skills application phase. Patients in this pilot trial suffered
and anxiety sensitivity across a range of patients (Hayes, from a principal DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Associa-
Luoma, Bond, Masuda, & Lillis, 2006). Randomized tion, 1994) diagnosis of PD/A. They received 4 sessions of
controlled trials of ACT for anxiety disorders are emerging, ACT focusing on acceptance, defusion, and values-based
including ACT for test anxiety (L. Brown et al., 2011; Zettle, action, followed by 6 sessions of ACT combined with
2003), social anxiety disorder (Kocovski, Fleming, & Rector, exposure therapy. Panic symptom severity and willingness
2009), generalized anxiety disorder (Roemer, Orsillo, & to accept panic-related inner experiences were examined
Salters-Pedneault, 2008), and obsessive-compulsive disor- after ACT only and after the combination of ACT with
der (Twohig et al., 2010). ACT protocols can be applied exposure.
across a variety of anxiety disorders. This has been Methods
demonstrated in case studies (Codd et al., 2011), effective-
ness studies with samples of participants diagnosed with Patients and Procedure
anxiety disorders (Lappalainen et al., 2007), or studies with Eleven patients (ages 20 to 55, average age=34.3)
clinically severe levels of anxiety and depression, albeit participated in the open series of behaviorally based ACT
undiagnosed (Forman, Herbert, Moitra, Yeomans, & treatment. The study was conducted at two sites: the Stress,
Geller, 2007). Anxiety, and Chronic Disease Research Program at
While some of these studies purposefully exclude Southern Methodist University, Dallas (SMU; N=5), and
exposure exercises for experimental reasons (Codd et al., the Anxiety Disorders Research Center at the University of
2011; Twohig, Hayes, & Masuda, 2006; Twohig et al., 2010), California, Los Angeles (UCLA; N=6). Patients were
ACT is complementary to traditional exposure exercises. recruited by regional postings, newspaper advertisements,
From an ACT perspective, “The purpose of exposure is not and by self-referrals to the anxiety clinics, and selected
608 Meuret et al.

based on the following inclusion and exclusion criteria: and clinical interviews, patients were enrolled to receive
(a) current principal diagnosis of PD/A (DSM-IV criteria), four individual sessions of ACT followed by six sessions of
(b) 18 years or older, (c) if medicated, on a stable dose of exposure therapy (interoceptive and in-vivo exposure).
psychotropic medication for at least 3 months before study All patients were female and the majority was married
initiation and willingness to maintain on a stable dose (63.6%), well educated (14.4 years), and working
throughout treatment, (d) agreement not to initiate (54.5%). Most were Caucasian (72.7%), with the remainder
additional therapy while participating in the study, and being Hispanic (27.3%). Eighty-one percent had at least
(e) no indication of a history of bipolar disorder, psychotic one additional current DSM-IV Axis I diagnosis (anxiety
disorder, suicidal intention, current substance abuse or disorder [n=9], anxiety and mood disorder [n=2]). Six
dependence, or current organic mental disorder, serious patients were on stable doses of psychotropic medication
medical disease, respiratory illness, or seizures. (antidepressants [n=3], benzodiazepines [n=3]).
Patients who appeared eligible on the basis of an initial The average number of sessions utilized was 8.1
telephone screen were invited for a diagnostic interview (ranging from 1 to 10). Of these, 8 patients completed
using the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM–IV all 10 treatment sessions, 1 patient terminated treatment
(ADIS–IV–L; DiNardo, Brown, & Barlow, 1994) at UCLA or after the 6th session because she no longer felt
the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV, Patient symptomatic, and 2 patients dropped out early in
Edition (SCID; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1995) at treatment (after Session 1 and 3) due to transportation
SMU. Complete phone screens were available from 24 or scheduling difficulties. According to the PDSS (range
individuals. Several more expressed interest but were not 0–4), levels of severity were in a moderate range (PDSS=
phone screened (never returned calls, numbers were not in 2.08, SD=0.76) at pretreatment Table 1.
service) or terminated screen prematurely (largely due to
insufficient reimbursement). Of the 24 phone contacts, 11
were ineligible due to another DSM-IV diagnosis being Intervention
principal (social phobia [1], bipolar disorder [4], specific Our protocol focused heavily on the acceptance
phobia [1], psychosis [1], GAD [1], PTSD [1]) or not element of ACT but also addressed defusion and
endorsing panic attacks [2]). Of the remaining 13 values-based action for the first four sessions, and then
participants, 2 were not eligible because they did not utilized these skills in the context of exposure exercises
meet criteria for a principal diagnosis of PD. The principal for the following six sessions. Patients were trained in ACT
diagnosis of PD/A and other psychiatric diagnoses were as a form of skill training to allow them to more effectively
established by consensus at a weekly staffing meeting. “cope” with panic symptoms. In contrast to traditional
The diagnostic interviews were followed by the clinician- “coping” skills training of CBT, patients learned to accept,
administered Panic Disorder Severity Scale (PDSS; Shear et notice, and be open to inner experiences that related to
al., 1997), which was repeated at posttreatment. All inter- panic without the attempt to eliminate or control them
views were conducted by clinicians trained and certified in (e.g., through corrections of thoughts or regulation of
the respective instruments. Following the initial diagnostic physiology).

Table 1
Session-by-session EM Means (SD)
ACQ (0-56) ASI (0-64) FFMQ (15 - 75) PDSS (0-4)
Pre/Session 1 23.27 (3.72) 37.81 (4.45) 42.55 (2.23) 2.08 (0.20)
Session 2 18.68 (3.75) 33.39 (4.51) 41.23 (2.29)
Session 3 16.39 (3.77) 31.33 (4.54) 43.73 (2.31)
Session 4 17.48 (3.85) 31.42 (4.69) 46.21 (2.44)
Mid/Session 5 12.78 (3.85) 25.34 (4.66) 47.40 (2.41) 1.31 (0.21)
Session 6 14.70 (3.86) 22.23 (4.68) 47.34 (2.42)
Session 7 14.55 (3.91) 22.76 (4.77) 46.55 (2.50)
Session 8 9.58 (3.98) 19.32 (4.88) 50.54 (2.60)
Session 9 8.91 (3.95) 19.10 (4.81) 51.13 (2.54)
Session 10 8.54 (3.95) 17.02 (4.82) 52.39 (2.55)
Post treatment 7.66 (3.96) 15.03 (4.82) 53.40 (2.55) 0.54 (0.23)
Note. ACQ = Agoraphobia Cognition Questionnaire; ASI = Anxiety Sensitivity Index; FFMQ = Composite Non-Judging and Non-Reactivity
Scales of the Five Factor Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ); PDSS = Panic Disorder Severity Scale.
Brief ACT and Exposure 609

Sessions 1–4: ACT Skill Acquisition Training panic attack that occurred. After that was brought to mind,
the patient was asked to mindfully notice all inner
Session 1. Session 1 began with basic descriptions of experiences that were occurring. This involved noticing
the intervention and what is to be expected in all bodily experiences, such as the feeling of sitting in a
treatment (Video 1). ACT was presented as a means chair, the tightness of one's jaw, and other sensations
to back away from and not get entangled with panic- related to panic attacks. It was suggested to the patient,
related inner experiences. This can be confusing for “When you feel yourself getting sucked into the emotions—
many patients because it is usually assumed that like we sometimes do with very intriguing television
treatment focuses on reducing or regulating panic- shows—pull yourself back and continue to watch it.”
related inner experiences. The logic of the ACT The patient was asked to shift attention to any emotions that
approach was introduced by leading the patient were experienced. Specifically, the patient was asked to
through an exercise to demonstrate that the more notice all aspects of that emotion with curiosity instead of
effort at regulating panic-related inner experiences, judgment or evaluation. This was done more formally in a
the poorer the actual control over them. This was mindfulness exercise that involved watching thoughts pass
ultimately highlighted with the “man in the hole” met- by without grabbing onto or pushing any thoughts away.
aphor (Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999, pp. 101-104) Finally, it was suggested that when inner experiences are
in which the patient is in a hole (the panic-related noticed from a mindful and nonjudgmental stance, they
inner experiences) and digging (attempting to control are easier to experience. It was suggested that the patient
or regulate these internal events) does not get the take this position to other inner experiences in life.
individual out of the hole, but enlarges it (Video 2).
Instead of trying to regulate the panic-related inner ACT-based between-session exercises. At the end of the first
experiences, it is suggested that the patient focus on training session, patients received detailed instructions on
finding ways to step back from these events and the rationale and practice of two, 15-minute, daily
mindfully watch them occur. This is explained through between-session exercises. The first was called “Practicing
the “two scales” metaphor (Video 3; Hayes, Strosahl, Awareness of Your Experience.” Homework in our ACT
et al., 1999, pp. 133-134): the patient is asked to take protocol was structured and closely monitored with the
the focus off the first scale (the level or intensity of aid of modern technology. The specific exercise was the
panic-related inner experiences) and to shift that 15-minute audio-script previously described. The second
focus and energy over to the second scale (the degree was a self-guided exercise in which patients recorded
of willingness to experience these events as they are in themselves practicing awareness of breathing, panic-
the moment). related symptoms, and any other internal stimuli. At
As part of our abbreviated ACT protocol, patients also specific intervals during the exercise (minute: 3, 7, 11),
participated in a 15-minute audio exercise that involved patients were prompted to verbalize their thinking using a
accepting, stepping back from thoughts, and mindfully digital voice recorder (see Measures section). At each
watching them. It began by remembering the most recent weekly session during the ACT skills acquisition training,

Video 2. Illustrating ill-fated attempts to control anxiety using the


Video 1. Treatment description and expectations (Session 1). “Man in the Hole” metaphor (Session 1).
610 Meuret et al.

inner experience and controlling things in our outside


environments). The counterproductive result of attempt-
ing to control inner experiences was compared to attempt-
ing to not get anxious when connected to a polygraph
machine. Allowing the inner experiences to occur was
compared to a Chinese finger trap, a child's toy in which
trying to get out paradoxically traps the child (Hayes,
Strosahl, et al., 1999, pp. 104-105). Generally, the focus of
the session was to increase the patient's willingness to
experience panic-related inner experiences and cease
attempts to regulate them.

Session 3. Largely focused on defusion from the literal


meaning of panic-related cognitions, this session began
with a relatively simple exercise that helped the patient to
Video 3. Stepping back from controlling anxiety using the “Two view having verbal abilities as a blessing and a curse. The
Scales” metaphor (Session 1).
patient was asked to look around the room for anything
she could not name. Similarly, she was asked to see if she
therapists reviewed reports of between-session exercises to could find anything about which she had no opinion—
better assist in identifying examples of successful accep- that she could not evaluate. Most patients can name
tance and cognitive defusion and support further everything and can evaluate just about everything. It was
improving of skills. The aim of this close monitoring, noted that this process is very useful, but it can become
otherwise atypical for ACT protocols, was two-fold: first, to problematic when applied to our own inner experiences.
assess the feasibility of structured exercises using audio- For example, when anxiety occurs it is usually named and
scripts, and second, to better assess the degree and quality evaluated negatively. The same applies to many other
of skill training in a home-based setting. panic-related inner experiences. It was also noted that this
process cannot be stopped; everyone with cognitive
Session 2. Session 2 focused on the counterproductive abilities does this (Video 5). Next, a series of defusion
effects of attempting to control or regulate panic-related exercises were covered that aimed to give patients the
inner experiences (Video 4). The process of attempting ability to experience inner events as just events and less
to control events that are negatively evaluated was tied to their cognitive meanings and evaluations. Exer-
normalized, but it was highlighted that this process is cises included “Milk, Milk, Milk” (Hayes, Strosahl, et al.,
much more successful when applied to events outside of 1999, pp. 154-156) and “Passengers on the Bus” (Video 6;
our bodies (e.g., the difference between controlling an Hayes, Strosahl, et al., pp. 157-158). Both exercises focus
on seeing cognitive activity as an ongoing process that one

Video 4. Demonstrating the counterproductive effects of controlling/


regulating panic-related inner experiences (Session 2). Video 5. Experiencing “language” for what it is (Session 3).
Brief ACT and Exposure 611

Video 6. Exploring the “Passenger on the Bus” metaphor as an Video 8. Introduction to exposure therapy in the context of ACT
example for recognizing thinking as an ongoing process (Session 3). (Session 5).

chooses to respond to; it is highlighted that cognitions are (Video 7; Hayes, Strosahl, et al., 1999, pp. 247-248).
not real and do not cause behavior. Again, it was noted Acceptance and defusion, and how they can support
that if the patient could view panic-related inner pursuing ones values, was also briefly covered in this
experiences as just bodily sensations, thoughts, and session.
emotions, that they would be easier to experience.
Sessions 5–10: Exposure-Based Skill Application Training
Session 4. The last ACT session focused on setting up
Following the ACT sessions, patients received 6
the exposure therapy sessions. Session 4 began with a brief
individual sessions of exposure therapy. These exposure
values-based discussion, where larger life goals were tied
sessions were very similar to what occurs in CBT exposure
into the therapy. It was highlighted that treatment is not
therapy for panic disorder, except that the rationale and
just about reducing panic attacks, but finding ways to
focus of all exposures were to facilitate the ACT processes
bring important, meaningful aspects of life back into
that patients learned during the first phase of treatment.
focus. The pursuit of one's values was compared to
Thus, instead of performing the exposures under contexts
moving through a muddy swamp where the things that
of fear reduction, challenging of cognitive distortions, or
are important are at the other end of the swamp—moving
reappraising the danger of sensations, the focus was to
through the swamp is in the service of the patient's values
provide opportunities to practice and become more skilled
at applying ACT skills in the presence of panic-related inner
experiences (Video 8). The exposure phase included:
(a) educating patients about the mechanisms and benefits
of exposure, (b) creating a fear and avoidance hierarchy,
(c) therapist-guided preparation for in-session exposure,
(d) in-session interoceptive and in-vivo exposures, initially
therapist-guided, (e) therapist debriefing following in-
session exposure, and (f) instructing patients in between-
session exercises (i.e., interoceptive or in-vivo exposures),
albeit unmonitored. Patients were instructed to apply their
ACT skills throughout the entire exposure session.
In general, the exposures were an opportunity to
behave with one's panic-related thoughts, feelings, and
bodily sensations. This supported ACT and response
flexibility skills with the intent to ultimately realize that
one can drive for and reach life goals, even in the
presence of unpleasant inner experiences. To that end, it
Video 7. Examining life goals by using the “Moving Through a Muddy was explicitly stated that the level of anxiety or fear was
Swamp” metaphor (Session 4). not the determining factor. Rather, it was explained that
612 Meuret et al.

“Willingness can do surprising things to one's inner (0–4) is used to indicate how often a specific thought
experiences. If one is willing to experience anxiety it may occurred. Test-retest reliability is .86 and Cronbach's
or may not show up. Thus, we are not going to judge the alpha is .80 (Chambless et al., 1984).
success of these exposures on how high the anxiety gets, but
instead in how open you are to what might show up.” To Five Factor Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ; Baer, Smith,
effectively plan for interoceptive (i.e., eliciting panic Hopkins, Krietemeyer, & Toney, 2006)
sensations such as a racing heart or shortness of breath) The original 39-item FFMQ, which is based on a factor
and in-vivo exposures (i.e., seeking places and situations that analysis of five mindfulness scales, assesses five compo-
were previously avoided because of the fear of panic nents of mindfulness, including observing, describing,
sensations), a hierarchy of least to most anxiety-provoking acting with awareness, accepting without judgment, and
items was created. Movement up the hierarchy was not based nonreactivity to inner experience. Items are rated on a
on reductions in anxiety at the preceding step but high 5-point scale, ranging from 1 (never or very rarely true) to 5
willingness to experience panic-related inner experiences. (very often or always true). Because this investigation
focused on promoting nonjudging/reactivity and less on
the other aspects of mindfulness, only the accepting
Supervision without judgment and nonreactivity scales were utilized.
Weekly group supervision meetings were held. The The composite score of the two scales are referred to as
group consisted of the respective site investigators (AEM at mindfulness for the purposes of this study because they
SMU, MGC at UCLA), ACT expert supervisors (MT, SH), reflect key aspects of mindfulness from an ACT perspec-
one Ph.D. and four-senior level Ph.D. graduate students tive (Fletcher & Hayes, 2005). The FFMQ has good
who conducted the therapy (AEM, ARM, DCS, BD, KW). psychometric properties (Baer et al., 2006; K. Brown &
The supervision served to ensure the treatment feasibility Ryan, 2003) and scores have been shown to increase
and integrity of the delivery following ACT and exposure following mindfulness-based training (K. Brown & Ryan).
principles. Additionally, both the ACT and exposure phases The alpha coefficients for the nonjudging and nonreac-
followed treatment protocols developed for this pilot study. tivity subscales were .87 and .75, respectively (Baer et al.).

Credibility/Expectancy Questionnaire (CEQ; Devilly & Borkovec,


Measures 2000)
All questionnaires were assessed prior to each treat- The CEQ contains a total of 6 items; 4 measure
ment session (Weeks 0–9) and posttreatment (Week 10), expectancy and 2 measure credibility of treatment.
with the exception of the PDSS, which was assessed at pre-, Patients are asked to rate items on a scale of 1 to 9 (1=
mid-, and posttreatment (Weeks 0, 4, 10). not at all logical, 5=somewhat logical, and 9=very logical). The
scale has shown good internal consistency and test-retest
Panic Disorder Severity Scale (PDSS; Shear et al., 1997) reliability (Devilly & Borkovec).
The PDSS is a semistructured 7-item interview rating
scale to assess overall PD severity on a 5-point Likert scale Recording and Evaluation of On-Line Thought Processes:
ranging from 0 (none) to 4 (extreme). It includes ratings for Think-Aloud Paradigm
panic severity and intensity, anticipatory anxiety, avoid- A modified and extended version of the think-aloud
ance of sensations and situations, and impairment in work paradigm (Davison, Vogel, & Coffman, 1997; Davison,
and social functioning. The instrument has excellent Williams, Nezami, Bice, & DeQuattro, 1991; Williams,
interrater reliability (Shear et al.). Kinney, Harap, & Liebmann, 1997) was used to assess
concurrent, situation-specific and patient representative
Anxiety Sensitivity Index (ASI; Reiss, Peterson, Gursky, &
thoughts. This on-line method of recording verbalizations
McNally, 1986)
provides a sample of thinking that differs from the
The ASI assesses degree of concern about the possible
constraints of forced-choice formats and retrospective
negative consequences of anxiety symptoms. Respondents
judgments (e.g., questionnaires), and appears to be less
rate each of the 16 items, ranging from 0 (very little) to 4
susceptible to responder biases inherent in retrospective
(very much). The ASI has high levels of internal
judgments. Such think-aloud methodologies have been
consistency and good test-retest reliability and is sensitive
used to evaluate thoughts during exposure to feared
to treatment for PD/A (Reiss et al., 1986).
situations (Williams et al., 1997) and are considered valid
Agoraphobia Cognition Questionnaire (ACQ; Chambless, Caputo, indicators of covert verbalizations when derived from
Bright, & Gallagher, 1984) instructions to “say in a continuous stream whatever one is
The ACQ measures the occurrence of 14 frightening saying to oneself at that moment” (vs. report upon what
or maladaptive thoughts about catastrophic conse- one thinks one has been saying over a past interval) and
quences of anxiety and panic. A 5-point Likert-type scale limited to brief snippets of time (30 seconds; Hayes,
Brief ACT and Exposure 613

White, & Bissett, 1998). The think-aloud samples, For the outcomes that were measured each week (ASI,
collected both during the daily ACT between-session ACQ, and mindfulness), we modeled the growth curve of
exercises as well as during the exposures, served as symptoms over time as discontinuous (Singer & Willett,
measures of the extent to which ACT skills were acquired 2003), allowing the slope of improvement to change
and applied during exposure. between the training and exposure phases of the study
During the between-session acceptance-skill training, (see Figure 1). Time was coded as weeks and ranged from
patients recorded their internal dialogue at prompted 0–10 (0=baseline, 4=midtreatment, 10=posttreatment;
(minute: 3, 7, 11), 30-second intervals during the 0–4=ACT skills training, 4–9=exposure). Because the
15-minute exercise period using a digital voice recorder repeated measures over time were correlated, we mod-
for recording. Recordings, with a date and time stamp, eled the error covariance matrix of the repeated measures
were downloaded via a USB port to a PC. as auto-regressive. The effect size of each slope during
each phase (ES) was estimated as the percent of the Level
1 error variance (σ 2) which was accounted for by that
Clinical Outcome and Data Illustration
slope (Singer & Willett).
Treatment Fidelity, Competence, Credibility, and Compliance Results indicated that the slopes of improvement were
All sessions were audio- or videotaped and discussed in significant during both phases of the study for all three
weekly supervision meetings by expert clinicians to ensure outcomes. In particular, ASI decreased significantly during
that therapists adhered to the treatment protocol. A the ACT phase and the exposure phases of the study, b=-3.11,
random sample of 10% of all recorded treatment sessions t(59)=-3.49, p=.001, ES=26.4% and b=-1.61, t(46)=-2.41,
was evaluated blindly for protocol adherence by an pb.05, ES=15.4%. Similarly, ACQ decreased significantly
independent, experienced master's-level clinicians who during both phases, b=-2.09, t(47)=-4.76, pb.001, ES=46.0% ,
scored videos on previous ACT trials (Twohig et al., 2010). and b=-1.01, t(39)=-3.24, pb.005, ES=27.9%. Also, mindful-
Therapist adherence and competence were rated against ness increased significantly during each phase of the study,
an adherence/competence checklist that was created to b=1.13, t(54)=2.38, pb.05, ES=38.3% and b=1.16, t(43)=
assess adherence in the study by Twohig et al. It contains 3.39, pb.005, ES=22.7%, respectively. For all outcomes, the
items pertaining to how well the therapist covered slopes were not significantly different between the two phases
elements central to ACT and therapist competence of the study (ps=.27, .11, and .95 for ASI, ACQ, and
rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1=not at all and 5= mindfulness, respectively).
extremely). Adherence and competence were rated high PDSS was measured at baseline, after ACT skills
(adherence: M=4, SD=0 competence: M=4.75, SD=0.5). training, and after exposure sessions. Thus, the growth
Treatment credibility ratings were completed after the curve was modeled as a single linear slope over time
educational session (Session 1). Patients’ ratings for (5 data points are needed for a discontinuous slope),
expectancy and credibility were high. Mean (SD) ratings and time was coded 0, 1, 2 for the three assessments of
for expectancy 7.0 (SD: 1.44) and 6.09 (SD: 1.55) for PDSS (0 = pretreatment, 1 = midtreatment, 2 = posttreat-
credibility. Homework compliance (out of 13 ACT ment). Results indicated that PDSS decreased signifi-
exercises per week [assessed via electronic time/date cantly over time, b = -.73, t(24) = 8.26, p b.001, ES = 73.3%
stamp] also was high: Week 1: 6.4 (SD: 5.7); Week 2: 8.7 (see Figure 1).
(SD: 3.3); Week 3: 6.8 (SD: 4.7); Week 4: 7.0 (SD: 4.9). All treatment completers (n=8 of the 11 participants)
were “treatment responders” (i.e., PDSS score reduction
Reduction in panic symptom severity and increased mind- of at least 30% as defined by Shear, Clark, & Feske, 1998)
fulness. We analyzed the data using mixed-effects (range=67–100% reduction).
regression models (MRM; also referred to as multilevel
models, random-coefficient models, etc.). MRM allows Illustration of think-aloud snippets. We obtained data
inclusion of all participants, regardless of missing data, from a total of 550 vocalizations. To illustrate feasibility,
and can be used in samples as small as 10 participants adherence to homework instructions, and treatment
(Maas & Hox, 2005; Snijders & Bosker, 1993) by using progress, we present the complete transcripts of first
regression coefficients (slopes) rather than Level 2 and last week of ACT training on the example of two
variances. Further, MRM focuses on slopes of improve- participants (total of 165 snippets; 30% of the total
ment over time, which take into account all the data for sample; Figure 2). Overall, patients were very receptive to
each participant, thus yielding results that are more and compliant with the recording devices for measuring
reliable than analyses focusing on single end-point data their verbalizations.
(e.g., ANCOVA). As a result, MRM is the method of As described earlier, patients were prompted to say in a
choice for analyzing longitudinal psychiatric data (Hamer continuous stream whatever came to their mind at the
& Simpson, 2009). very moment of prompting. These repeated 30-second
614 Meuret et al.

4 64

40
PDSS (0-4)

ASI (0-64)
2

20
1

0 0
0 2 4 6 8 10 0 2 4 6 8 10

56 75

FFMQ Non-Judge plus Non-react (15-75)


60
40
ACQ (0-56)

45

20

30

0 15
0 2 4 6 8 10 0 2 4 6 8 10
Weeks Weeks

Figure 1. Individual slopes for weekly measures of ASI, ACQ, and Mindfulness during Phase 1 (pretreatment [Week 0], through end of AC
phase [Week 4]) and for Phase 2 (Weeks 4 through end of exposure [Week 9]), and posttreatment [Week 10]). PDSS scores were collected
only at pre-, mid-, and posttreatment and thus raw scores are graphed. The 5 missing PDSS scores (out of 33) were interpolated from
individual slopes taken from the overall HLM analysis. Patterns of dashes for each participant are the same in the four sections of the figure.

snippets were recorded on digital recorders with a time, examine the degree to which skill acquisition and
duration, and date stamp. Data of this type offer much subsequent application (e.g., during exposure) mediate
needed insight into the acquisition and application of treatment outcome.
coping skills. We selected extracts to indicate the variety
that we observed in terms of the verbalizations. Patient Discussion and Clinical Implications
1004 offered some dialogue of increased willingness to The results of our pilot study indicate that a brief ACT
experience panic sensations and interest in practicing protocol, focusing heavily on acceptance as well as
acceptance while having a panic attack. Patient 1002 defusion and values-based action, alone and in combina-
showed a greater grasp of acceptance, thus exemplifying tion with exposure therapy, is a feasible and likely
that the skills learned in the initial four sessions can effective intervention. Large reductions in panic symptom
transfer to practices outside of therapy. It should be noted severity were observed during ACT skills training, and
that the transcripts from our selected patients may not be continued improvements were observed during the
representative of all patients included in the study. exposure phase of treatment. The observed reduction in
Further studies will be needed to construct a coding panic symptom severity compared well to traditional CBT
scheme like the one by Williams and colleagues (1997) to trials. For instance, in the study by Barlow and colleagues
Brief ACT and Exposure 615

(2000), intent-to-treat PDSS decreased from 1.82 (0.55) at to the other side, to things that she really wanted to do,
pretreatment to 1.14 (0.74) at posttreatment for patients but has not done due to panic and anxiety. Such “on-line”
receiving 13 sessions of CBT only. Comparably, PDSS information provides invaluable insight into patients’
levels in our study were 2.08 (0.20) at pretreatment, 1.31 progress toward acquiring skills outside the therapist's
(0.21) after ACT training, and 0.54 (0.23) at posttreat- office. Recordings can be easily played back during the
ment. Significant reductions were also seen on measures session with the patient, providing a unique opportunity
of anxiety sensitivity and cognitive misappraisals, whereas to refine skills and discuss obstacles.
increases occurred on measures of mindfulness. Further- While this is not the first investigation into the effects
more, the think-aloud recordings demonstrated the of ACT for anxiety disorders (see L. Brown et al., 2011;
feasibility, and partial success, of measuring the acquisi- Dalrymple & Herbert, 2007; Kocovski et al., 2009; Roemer
tion of ACT skills. Transcripts of the verbalizations, as et al., 2008; Twohig et al., 2010; Wetherell et al., 2011;
depicted in Figure 2, support patients’ ability to accept, Zettle, 2003), it is the first to use ACT as a model for
step back from, and mindfully watch their anxious exposure to feared stimuli. Prior studies have either
thoughts and sensations. For instance, Patient 1002 excluded exposure from ACT methods for analytic
verbalized her desire to move through the swamp to get reasons (Codd et al., 2011; Twohig et al., 2006; Twohig

Figure 2. Complete transcripts from the “Think-Aloud” recordings for Patient 0102 and Patient 0104 for their first and last week of
acceptance skill training.
616 Meuret et al.

Figure 2 (continued ).

et al., 2010), or included exposure exercises within the tested. While these findings are preliminary, they suggest
general ACT protocol as a natural extension of the that ACT offers a viable option for working with anxious
underlying model (e.g., Dalrymple & Herbert). In the clients. ACT may be particularly suitable when traditional
current study, we systematically examined the effects of CBT approaches fail, especially for reasons such as poor
ACT training without exposure, followed by ACT skills motivation to participate or difficulties tolerating panic-
combined with exposure. A brief and limited ACT related inner experiences. For instance, emerging re-
protocol led to significant reductions in panic symptoms search on moderators indicates that individuals with
and improvements in mindfulness, and significant addi- higher levels of catastrophic cognitions fail to respond
tional benefits were observed during the phase in which well to cognitive restructuring techniques and CBT
ACT skills were implemented in exposure. However, due (Meuret, Hofmann, & Rosenfield, 2010; Wolitzky-Taylor,
to the lack of an exposure-only control condition, the Arch, Rosenfield, & Craske, under review). Conceivably,
degree to which ACT augments exposure remains to be theoretical arguments against beliefs of catastrophic
Brief ACT and Exposure 617

outcome may be overturned by deeply entrenched, Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1251–1263. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.001.
enduring negative attributions, thus leading to minimal Chambless, D. L., Caputo, G. C., Bright, P., & Gallagher, R. (1984).
improvement. Techniques that focus on acceptance of Assessment of fear of fear in agoraphobics: The body sensations
thoughts and sensations, as opposed to their correction, questionnaire and the agoraphobic cognitions questionnaire.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52, 1090–1097.
may be more beneficial for individuals with highly Codd, R. T., Twohig, M. P., Crosby, J. M., & Enno, A. M. (2011).
elevated catastrophic misappraisals. Treatment of three anxiety cases with acceptance and commit-
Even though the findings from this study possess a high ment therapy in a private practice. Journal of Cognitive Psychother-
apy, 25, 203–217. http://dx.doi.org/10.1891/0889-8391.25.3.203.
degree of internal validity (e.g., use of standardized Craske, M. G., Kircanski, K., Zelikowsky, M., Mystkowski, J., Chowdhury, N.,
assessments throughout, use of a structured treatment & Baker, A. (2008). Optimizing inhibitory learning during exposure
protocol, supervision of the independent variable by therapy. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 46, 5–27. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.brat.2007.10.003.
experts), without a control condition it is impossible to Culver, N., Stoyanova, M. S., & Craske, M. G. (2012). Emotional variability
know to what extent the findings were the result of and sustained arousal during exposure. Journal of Behavior Therapy
nonspecific treatment factors. Furthermore, the durabil- and Experimental Psychiatry, 43, 787–793. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.jbtep. 2011.10.009.
ity of the results remain unknown due to the absence of Dalrymple, K. L., & Herbert, J. D. (2007). Acceptance and commit-
long-term follow-up data, as well as posttreatment ment therapy for generalized social anxiety disorder: A pilot study.
diagnostic assessments. Nonetheless, the results of the Behavior Modification, 31, 543–568. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
0145445507302037.
pilot data suggest that training in ACT skills and their Davison, G. C., Vogel, R. S., & Coffman, S. G. (1997). Think-aloud
incorporation into exposure is a powerful novel interven- approaches to cognitive assessment and the articulated thoughts
tion that is worthy of further investigation. in simulated situations paradigm. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 65, 950–958. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.
Appendix A. Supplementary data 65.6.950.
Davison, G. C., Williams, M. E., Nezami, E., Bice, T. L., & DeQuattro, V. L.
Supplementary data to this article can be found online (1991). Relaxation, reduction in angry articulated thoughts, and
at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpra.2012.05.004. improvements in borderline hypertension and heart rate. Journal of
Behavioral Medicine, 14, 453–468.
References Devilly, G., & Borkovec, T. (2000). Psychometric properties of the
credibility/expectancy questionnaire. Journal of Behavior Therapy
Andrews, G., Cuijpers, P., Craske, M. G., McEvoy, P., & Titov, N. (2010). and Experimental Psychiatry, 31, 73–86.
Computer therapy for anxiety and depressive disorders is DiNardo, S. C., Brown, T. A., & Barlow, D. H. (1994). Anxiety Disorders
effective, acceptable and practice health care: A meta-analysis. Interview Schedule for DSM-IV: Lifetime Version (ADIS-IV-L). San Antonio,
PLoS One, 5, e13196. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone. TX: Psychological Corporation/Graywind.
0013196. Eifert, G. H., & Heffner, M. (2003). The effects of acceptance versus control
Arch, J. J., & Craske, M. G. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness: contexts on avoidance of panic-related symptoms. Journal of Behavior
Emotion regulation following a focused breathing induction. Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 34, 293–312. http://dx.doi.org/
Behavior Research and Therapy, 44, 849–858. http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.jbtep. 2003.11.001.
10.1016/j.brat.2005.12.007. First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1995).
Baer, R., Smith, G., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Structured clinical interview for DSM–IV (SCID–I/P Version 2.0). New
Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets York, NY: Biometrics Research Department, New York State
of mindfulness. Assessment, 13, 27–45. http://dx.doi.org/ Psychiatric Institute.
10.1177/1073191105283504. Fletcher, L., & Hayes, S. C. (2005). Relational Frame Theory,
Baker, A., Mystkowksi, J., Culver, N., Yi, R., Mortazavi, A., & Craske, M. G. Acceptance and Commitment Therapy, and a functional analytic
(2010). Does habituation matter? Emotional processing theory definition of mindfulness. Journal of Rational Emotive and Cognitive
and exposure therapy for acrophobia. Behaviour Research and Behavioral Therapy, 23, 315–336. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
Therapy, 48, 1139–1143. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2010. s10942-005-0017-7.
07.009. Forman, E. M., Herbert, J. D., Moitra, E., Yeomans, P. D., & Geller, P. A.
Barlow, D. H., Gorman, J. M., Shear, M. K., & Woods, S. W. (2000). (2007). A randomized controlled effectiveness trial of acceptance
Cognitive-behavioral therapy, imipramine, or their combination and commitment therapy and cognitive therapy for anxiety and
for panic disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the depression. Behavior Modification, 31, 772–799. http://dx.doi.org/
American Medical Association, 283, 2529–2536. 10.1177/0145445507302202.
Brown, K., & Ryan, R. (2003). The benefits of being present: Haby, M. M., Donnelly, M., Corry, J., & Vos, T. (2006). Cognitive
Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of behavioural therapy for depression, panic disorder and general-
Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 822–848. http://dx.doi.org/ ized anxiety disorder: a meta-regression of factors that may predict
10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822. outcome. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 40, 9–19.
Brown, L. A., Forman, E. M., Herbert, J. D., Hoffman, K. L., Yuen, E. K., http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/j.1440-1614.2006.01736.
& Goetter, E. M. (2011). A randomized controlled trial Hamer, R. M., & Simpson, P. M. (2009). Last observation carried
of acceptance-based behavior therapy and cognitive therapy forward versus mixed models in the analysis of psychiatric clinical
for test anxiety: A pilot study. Behavior Modification, 35, 31–53. trials. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 639–641. http://dx.doi.org/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0145445510390930. 10.1176/appi.ajp. 2009.09040458.
Brown, T. A., & Barlow, D. H. (1995). Long-term outcome in Hayes, S. C. (1987). A contextual approach to therapeutic change. In
cognitive-behavioral treatment of panic disorder: Clinical N. Jacobson (Ed.), Psychotherapists in clinical practice: Cognitive and
predictors and alternative strategies for assessment. Journal of behavioral perspectives (pp. 327–387). New York, NY: Guilford.
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 754–765. http://dx.doi.org/ Hayes, S. C., Bissett, R., Korn, Z., Zettle, R. D., Rosenfarb, I., Cooper, L.,
10.1037/0022-006X.63.5.754. & Grundt, A. (1999). The impact of acceptance versus control
Campbell-Sills, L., Barlow, D. H., Brown, T. A., & Hofmann, S. G. rationales on pain tolerance. The Psychological Record, 49, 33–47.
(2006). Effects of suppression and acceptance on emotional Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J. B., Bond, F., Masuda, A., & Lillis, J. (2006).
responses of individuals with anxiety and mood disorders. Acceptance and commitment therapy: Model, processes and
618 Meuret et al.

outcomes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1–25. http://dx.doi.org/ Reiss, S., Peterson, R., Gursky, D., & McNally, R. (1986). Anxiety
10.1016/j.brat.2005.06.006. sensitivity, anxiety frequency, and the prediction of fearfulness.
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Acceptance Behaviour Research and Therapy, 24, 1–8.
and commitment therapy: An experiential approach to behavior change. Roemer, L., Orsillo, S. M., & Salters-Pedneault, K. (2008). Efficacy of an
New York, NY: Guilford. acceptance-based behavior therapy for generalized anxiety
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K., & Wilson, K. G. (2011). Acceptance and disorder: Evaluation in a randomized controlled trial. Journal of
Commitment Therapy: The process and practice of mindful change, 2nd Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 1083–1089. http://dx.doi.org/
ed. New York, NY: Guilford Press. 10.1037/a0012720.
Hayes, S. C., White, D., & Bissett, R. T. (1998). Protocol analysis and the Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M., & Teasdale, J. D. (2002). Mindfulness-based
“silent dog” method of analyzing the impact of self-generated cognitive therapy for depression. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
rules. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 15, 57–63. Shear, M. K., Brown, T. A., Barlow, D. H., Money, R., Sholomskas, D. E.,
Hofmann, S. G., & Smits, J. A. (2008). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for Woods, S. W., Gorman, J. M., & Laszlo, A. (1997). Multicenter
adult anxiety disorders: A meta-analysis of randomized placebo- collaborative panic disorder severity scale. American Journal of
controlled trials. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69, 621–632. Psychiatry, 154, 1571–1575.
Kazdin, A. E. (2007). Mediators and mechanisms of change in Shear, M. K., Clark, D., & Feske, U. (1998). The road of recovery
psychotherapy research. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 3, 1–27. in panic disorder: Response, remission, and relapse. Journal of
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091432. Clinical Psychiatry (Suppl. 8), 59, 4–8.
Kircanski, K., Mortazavi, A., Castriotta, N., Baker, A., Mystkowski, J., Yi, Singer, J. D., & Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis.
R., & Craske, M. G. (2012). Challenges to the traditional exposure New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
paradigm: variability in exposure therapy for contamination fears. Snijders, T. A., & Bosker, R. J. (1993). Standard errors and sample sizes
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 43, 745–751. for two-level research. Journal of Statistics Education, 18, 237–259.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep. 2011.10.010. Twohig, M. P., Hayes, S. C., & Masuda, A. (2006). Increasing willingness
Kocovski, N. L., Fleming, J. E., & Rector, N. E. (2009). Mindfulness and to experience obsessions: Acceptance and commitment therapy as
acceptance-based group therapy for social anxiety disorder: An a treatment for obsessive compulsive disorder. Behavior Therapy,
open trial. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 16, 276–289. 37, 3–13.
Lappalainen, R., Lehtonen, T., Skarp, E., Taubert, E., Ojanen, M., & Twohig, M. P., Hayes, S. C., Plumb, J. C., Pruitt, L. D., Collins, A. B.,
Hayes, S. C. (2007). The impact of CBT and ACT models using Hazlett-Stevens, H., & Woidneck, M. R. (2010). A randomized
psychology trainee therapists: A preliminary controlled effectiveness clinical trial of acceptance and commitment therapy vs. progressive
trial. Behavior Modification, 31, 488–511. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ relaxation training for obsessive compulsive disorder. Journal of
0145445506298436. Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78, 705–716. http://dx.doi.org/
Levitt, J. T., Brown, T. A., Orsillo, S. M., & Barlow, D. H. (2004). The 10.1037/a0020508.
effects of acceptance versus suppression of emotion on subjective Westen, D., & Morrison, K. (2001). A multidimensional meta-analysis
and patient psychophysiological response to carbon dioxide of treatments for depression, panic, and generalized anxiety
challenge in patients with panic disorder. Behavior Therapy, 35, disorder: an empirical examination of the status of empirically
747–766. supported therapies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
Levitt, J. T., & Karekla, M. (2005). Integrating acceptance and 69, 875–899. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.69.6.875.
mindfulness with cognitive behavioral treatment for panic Wetherell, J. L., Liu, L., Patterson, T. L., Afari, N., Ayers, C. R., Thorp,
disorder. In S. M. Orsillo, & L. Roemer (Eds.), Acceptance and S. R., . . ., & Petkus, A. J. (2011). Acceptance and Commitment
mindfulness-based approaches to anxiety (pp. 165–188). New York: Therapy for generalized anxiety disorder in older adults: A
Springer. preliminary report. Behavior Therapy, 42, 127–134.
Maas, C. J. M., & Hox, J. J. (2005). Sufficient sample sizes for multilevel Williams, S. L., Kinney, P. J., Harap, S. T., & Liebmann, M. (1997).
modeling. Methodology, 1, 85–91. Thoughts of agoraphobic people during scary tasks. Journal of
McNally, R. J. (2007). Mechanisms of exposure therapy: How neurosci- Abnormal Psychology, 106, 511–520. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
ence can improve psychological treatments for anxiety disorders. 0021-843X.106.4.511.
Clinical Psychological Review, 27, 750–759. http://dx.doi.org/ Wolitzky-Taylor, K.B., Arch, J.J, Rosenfield, D. & Craske, M.G. (under
10.1016/j.cpr.2007.01.003. review). Prognostic and prescriptive moderators of treatment
Meuret, A. E., Hofmann, S. G., & Rosenfield, D. (2010). Catastrophic outcome for anxiety disorders: A comparison of cognitive
appraisal and perceived control as moderators of treatment behavioral therapy to acceptance and commitment therapy.
response in panic disorder. International Journal of Cognitive Zettle, R. D. (2003). Acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT)
Therapy, 3, 262–277. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/ijct.2010.3.3.262. versus systematic desensitization in treatment of mathematics
Meuret, A. E., Seidel, A., Rosenfield, B., Hofmann, S.G., & Rosenfield, D. anxiety. The Psychological Record, 53, 197–215.
(in press). Does fear reactivity during exposure predict panic
symptom reduction? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology.
The pilot study was funded in part by the generous support of the
Meuret, A. E., Wolitzky-Taylor, K. B., Twohig, M. P., & Craske, M. G.
Beth and Russell Siegelman Foundation (Meuret). We wish to thank
(2012). Coping skills and exposure therapy in panic disorder and
agoraphobia: Latest advances and future directions. Behavior the study therapists, Anne R. Miller, Deborah Corbitt-Schindler,
Therapy, 43, 271–284. Barbara Depreeuw, Kathryn Williams, and our therapy adherence
Norton, P. J., & Price, E. C. (2007). A meta-analytic review of adult rater, Kate Morrison.
cognitive-behavioral treatment outcome across the anxiety disorders. Address correspondence to Alicia E. Meuret, Ph.D., Department of
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 195, 521–531. http://dx.doi.org/ Psychology, Southern Methodist University, Dallas, TX, 72505 (e-mail:
10.1097/01.nmd.0000253843.70149.9a.
ameuret@smu.edu) or Michael P. Twohig, Ph.D., Department of Psychol-
Orsillo, S. M., Roemer, L., Block-Lerner, J., & Tull, M. T. (2004).
ogy, Utah State University, Logan, UT (e-mail: michael.twohig@usu.edu).
Acceptance, mindfulness, and cognitive-behavioral therapy:
Comparisons, contrasts, and application to anxiety. In S. C. Hayes,
M. M. Linehan, & V. M. Follette (Eds.), Mindfulness, acceptance, and Received: October 12, 2011
relationships: Expanding the cognitive-behavioral tradition (pp. 66–95). Accepted: May 8, 2012
New York, NY: Guilford Press. Available online 30 June 2012

You might also like