You are on page 1of 20

Psychotherapy Research

ISSN: 1050-3307 (Print) 1468-4381 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/tpsr20

Therapist competence in case conceptualization


and outcome in CBT for depression

Michael H. Easden & Richard B. Fletcher

To cite this article: Michael H. Easden & Richard B. Fletcher (2018): Therapist competence
in case conceptualization and outcome in CBT for depression, Psychotherapy Research, DOI:
10.1080/10503307.2018.1540895

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2018.1540895

Published online: 03 Nov 2018.

Submit your article to this journal

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=tpsr20
Psychotherapy Research, 2018
https://doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2018.1540895

EMPIRICAL PAPER

Therapist competence in case conceptualization and outcome in CBT


for depression

MICHAEL H. EASDEN & RICHARD B. FLETCHER

School of Psychology, Massey University, Albany, New Zealand


(Received 4 December 2017; revised 10 October 2018; accepted 12 October 2018)

Abstract
Objective: This study aimed to investigate the relationship between therapist competence in case conceptualization and
outcome in psychotherapy. Method: Twenty-eight adults received Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT) for depression.
The Conceptualization Rating Scale (CRS) was used to systematically evaluate therapist competence in case
conceptualization using ratings from 225 DVD recordings of live therapy in real time across the first ten sessions of
treatment. Results: Multilevel modelling (MLM) analysis revealed that after controlling for time and other pertinent
therapy influencing variables, therapist competence in case conceptualization explained 40% of within patient variance
and 19% of between patient variance associated with significant and positive change on the BDI-II. Conclusions: This
study provided evidence that increased therapist competence in using case conceptualization in CBT is associated with
greater reductions in depressive symptomology. The implications for supervision, training, and practice are discussed.

Keywords: CBT; case conceptualization; formulation; therapist competence; depression

Clinical or Methodological Significance Summary: Case conceptualization is considered to be an essential component


of psychotherapy training and practice. However, there is a lack of empirical evidence in support of this assertion, particularly
in CBT. This research focuses on therapist competence in case conceptualization in the process of psychotherapy as a means
to investigate therapy outcome-relations and towards establishing empirically based guidelines.

Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT) as espoused


by Aaron T. Beck in the treatment of depression
Case conceptualization is fundamental to the theory
(Beck et al., 1979) is an empirically supported treat-
and practice of psychotherapy (Eells, 2007).
ment for depression as well as a range of other
Researchers and clinicians have referred to case con-
psychological disorders. The central importance of
ceptualization as the “first principle,” an “essential
CBT-specific case conceptualization is widely
component,” the “cornerstone,” or the “heart” of
accepted in practice, although there is a striking gap
psychotherapy (Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979;
in the research to support the validity of CBT case
Bieling & Kuyken, 2003; Chadwick, Williams, &
conceptualization in relation to treatment outcomes
Mackenzie, 2003; Persons, 2001). Case conceptual-
and to better inform evidence-based practice
ization (synonymous with case formulation) is a
guidelines.
“process whereby therapist and patient work colla-
Cognitive–behavioural theory exists as a frame-
boratively first to describe and then to explain the
work for understanding the patient’s presenting pro-
issue a patient presents in therapy. Its primary func-
blems and informs intervention (Beck, 1995;
tion is to guide therapy in order to relieve patient dis-
Needleman, 1999; Persons, 1989, 2008). Interven-
tress and build patient resistance” (Kuyken, Padesky,
tion, in turn, re-informs the evolving case conceptu-
& Dudley, 2009, p. 3).
alization process over the course of therapy, with

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Michael H. Easden, School of Psychology, Massey University, Albany, New
Zealand. Email: michaeleasden@gmail.com

© 2018 Society for Psychotherapy Research


2 M. H. Easden and R. B. Fletcher

the intention of patient mastery and generalisation of developing level of case conceptualization (i.e.,
cognitive–behavioural skills (Dozois, Covin, & descriptive, cross-sectional, longitudinal) colla-
Brinker, 2003). One such intervention is “home- borative empiricism is the catalyst or “heat” that
work” which exists as an exception to the dearth of facilitates understanding and change. This colla-
research into central CBT processes. Homework is borative empiricism is the process whereby thera-
broadly defined as collaborative and planned pist and patient work together to integrate new
between-session activities tailored to meet a patient’s information over time, aligning the lived experi-
therapeutic goals; these tasks are based on an indivi- ence of the patient with CBT-specific theoretical
dualized case conceptualization consistent with the knowledge and expertise of the therapist. Essen-
empirically supported CBT model (Kazantzis, tially, the patient and therapist develop hypotheses
Deane, Ronan, & L’Abate, 2005). As such home- together, transparently, and within a CBT frame,
work is intrinsically linked with case conceptualiz- towards a shared understanding of presenting
ation as during therapy sessions the therapist guides issues, triggers (precipitating factors) and mainten-
the collaborative selection of in-session and ance (perpetuating factors), and protective and
between-session homework tasks or experiments predisposing factors (i.e., in fitting with and build-
based on individualized hypotheses for each patient ing upon what has previously been described as the
for what triggers, maintains, and interrupts their generic 5’Ps; Weerasekera, 1996).
pattern of difficulties (Kuyken, Padesky, & Dudley,
2008). The use of homework in psychotherapy has
increasingly been related to treatment benefits (for
The Empirical Basis of Case
reviews see Kazantzis, Deane, & Ronan, 2000;
Conceptualization in CBT
Kazantzis, Whittington, & Dattilio, 2010; Kazantzis
et al., 2016; Mausbach, Moore, Roesch, Cardenas, CBT is generally considered to be an empirically
& Patterson, 2010). based treatment. The question has been asked in
relation to case conceptualization, “is the emperor
clothed?” to which the conclusion has been “very
sparsely” (Kuyken, 2006). Positive implications of a
A Theoretical Model of Case
well-constructed and integrated case conceptualiz-
Conceptualization in CBT
ation are that they facilitate the linking of theory,
Case conceptualization can be undertaken and applied research and practice, normalise problems and
at many different levels and in many different forms increase empathy, organise large amounts of
(see Easden & Kazantzis, 2018). For instance, case complex information, and enable high quality super-
conceptualization might occur at a descriptive level vision among other benefits. Case conceptualization
or be more in-depth and explanatory; it might take is also suggested to guide treatment intervention by
place at the level of a problem, strength or diagnosis. selecting, focusing and sequencing the order of treat-
Case conceptualization may be cross-sectional (i.e., ment intervention, facilitating a collaborative
in the moment or “mini”-conceptualization) of a par- approach, pre-empting possible therapy interfering
ticular situation, or a longitudinal integration of infor- behaviours, and enabling the simplest most cost
mation. Given the diversity of applications, a number effective interventions.
of commonly used theoretical—often pictorial and While other psychotherapeutic orientations have
diagrammatic—frameworks exist emanating from a made headway into empirical demonstration that
CBT paradigm (e.g., A. Beck’s depression triad of case conceptualization enhances therapeutic out-
self, world/future and others, 1976; J. Beck’s case con- comes within their respective paradigm (for an over-
ceptualization diagram, 1995; Clark’s cognitive model view of structured case conceptualization methods
of panic, 1986; Padesky and Mooney’s 5-part model, from different therapeutic orientations see Eells,
1990; Person’s problem list, 2001). These provide a 2007), the importance of case conceptualization in
bridge between the theoretical and practical appli- CBT remains less clear. The vast majority of case
cation of case conceptualization as an inherent conceptualization research in CBT has focused on
process within CBT. written case conceptualizations based on discrete
The “Case Conceptualization Crucible” portions of therapy. The broad implication is that
(Kuyken et al., 2008, 2009) was proposed as an there is no empirical basis for the clinical utility of
overarching/guiding model of case conceptualiz- case conceptualizations as they apply to a naturalistic
ation in CBT. Three principles guide the approach course of CBT; particularly components pertinent to
(1) collaborative empiricism, (2) levels of concep- cognitive specificity which sets CBT apart from
tualization, and (3) incorporation of patient other therapeutic orientations (Easden & Kazantzis,
strengths. The model asserts that at each 2018).
Psychotherapy Research 3

Re-evaluating the Measurement of Case provides a method of measuring the relationship


Conceptualization: Therapist Competence between therapist use of case conceptualization and
outcomes in CBT. However, surprisingly little
A significant gap in the research concerns how cogni-
research exists bearing on the validity of the case con-
tive therapists conduct case conceptualizations in
ceptualization process in CBT (Bieling & Kuyken,
real-world practice (Kuyken et al., 2009). Prior
2003; Easden & Kazantzis, 2018).
research and measurement tools have tended to
There are a number of measures designed to
focus on the quality of written case conceptualizations
measure therapist competence across broad domains
(Bucci, French, & Berry, 2016). Specifically, the
of therapist skill in CBT (i.e., global competence).
organisation and integration of the information
For example, the Cognitive Therapy Scale (CTS;
from a patient’s presentation and the quality of resul-
Young & Beck, 1980) and the revision CTS-R (Black-
tant written case conceptualizations have traditionally
burn et al., 2001) are commonly used in supervision
been considered to impact how therapists approach a
and training to assess therapist competence in cogni-
patient’s case and implement a treatment plan (Crits-
tive–behavioural oriented therapies; the measure is
Christoph, Cooper, & Luborsky, 1988; Kuyken,
rated based on live or recorded therapy sessions by
Fothergill, Musa, & Chadwick, 2005). However, it
an independent observer or supervisor. Broad judge-
has been noted that written records may not encapsu-
ments are made about the presence and quality of
late a therapist’s full understanding of their patient’s
common features of therapy (e.g., agenda setting,
case conceptualization (Eells, Kendjelic, & Lucas,
case conceptualization, and homework use).
1998). Conceivably, therapists could hold alternative
However, limited research suggests that clinicians are
or divergent hypotheses from what is written in early
unable to reliably agree on the extent to which thera-
case conceptualizations as their therapy sessions pro-
pists demonstrate competence in case conceptualiz-
gress with patients.
ation. In examining the psychometric properties of
However, therapist competence (i.e., the level of skill
the CTS-R Blackburn et al. (2001) found that inter-
in using interventions) has been related to treatment
rater reliability for individual items produced the
benefits in a range of evidence-based psychotherapies
lowest score for the “conceptualization” item (r =
(for a review see Webb, DeRubeis, & Barber, 2010)
0.42, df = 49, p < .01) while the level of competence
and in CBT-specific research (for a review see Zarafo-
in “conceptualization” was one of the few items that
nitis-Müller, Kuhr, & Bechdolf, 2014; see also David-
appeared to improve over the course of training. As a
son et al., 2004; Kuyken & Tsivrikos, 2009; Shaw
global measure of competence, the CTS does not sys-
et al., 1999; Strunk, Adler, & Hollars, 2013; Trepka,
tematically assess different components of therapist
Rees, Shapiro, Hardy, & Barkham, 2004). It is noted
competence in case conceptualization and instead col-
in this research that greater effects were observed for
lapses this judgement into a single broad rating; as
studies targeting depression compared with other
such a high level of inference is required on the part
psychological disorders.
of the rater making the judgements. Kuyken et al.
Barber, Sharpless, Klostermann, and McCarthy
(2016) in a sample of depressed patients (i.e., 2–3
(2007) differentiated and defined two levels of
clients from each of the nine therapists) provided pre-
enquiry into therapist competency, namely; global
liminary data showing a moderate correlation (r = .54,
competence (i.e., as a broad focus on therapist skill
p < .01) between scores on the Collaborative Case
across a range of clinically relevant domains) and
Conceptualization Rating Scale (CCC-RS) and the
limited-domain competence (i.e., a subset of global
CTS-R. Such research acknowledges the importance
competence assessing particular practices for particu-
of therapist competence in case conceptualization in
lar interventions and often within the context of a
relation to broader therapist competencies by addres-
specific psychotherapeutic orientation). Consistent
sing the dearth empirical support for case conceptual-
with previous studies overall therapist competence
ization in CBT. However, present research is yet to
possessed a positive association with therapy out-
provide empirical evidence in regards to therapist
comes. However, the authors recommended further
competence in case conceptualization and therapy
measures of limited-domain competence be devel-
outcome relations (i.e., symptom improvement /
oped to further understand the weaker than expected
recovery).
relationship between therapist competence and treat-
ment outcomes. Consistent with previous research
recommendations and in order to directly address
therapist moment-to-moment use of case conceptu- The Current Study
alization (as opposed to examination of static
written formulations) an operationalized definition The overarching aim of current study was to investi-
of therapist competence in case conceptualization gate the relationship between case conceptualization
4 M. H. Easden and R. B. Fletcher

and therapy outcome. In order to achieve this aim the female (64%) patients of average age 44.75 years
Conceptualization Rating Scale (CRS) was devel- (SD = 11.51; range from 20 to 62 years). The partici-
oped and utilized as a new measure of limited- pants identified their ethnicity as predominantly
domain competence and we present preliminary European / Caucasian (n = 24; 86%).
data on the psychometric properties. The CRS was
designed to be an operationalization of in-session Therapists. Seven female therapists of average age
(i.e., observer rated based on live or video recorded 36.86 years (SD = 11.60; range from 23 to 50 years)
therapy sessions) case conceptualization competen- provided the course of CBT; all were intern psychol-
cies in CBT (Beck et al., 1979). The purpose of the ogists in their second to last or final year of training
CRS was both as, (1) a supervision tool to provide towards a doctoral qualification fulfilling the require-
feedback to therapists regarding their competence ments for registration as a clinical psychologist. Each
in case conceptualization in CBT, and as (2) a therapist had undertaken two advanced courses in the
research tool hoped to provide data that more theory and practice of CBT for depression. Each
closely captures real-world practice towards provid- course was a 5-day intensive postgraduate applied
ing empirical support for processes and outcomes training course involving role-plays, supervision in
related to case conceptualization in CBT. techniques, and other active learning activities.
Although research into limited-domain compe-
tence in CBT is sparse, there is a relatively broad
Raters. Raters were five females and one male of
base of research into homework and the importance
average age 31 years old (SD = 12.80; range from
of between-session tasks. Headway into the measure-
22 to 50 years). The raters were all postgraduate psy-
ment of therapist competence in homework use using
chology students with a minimum of one to two years
the Homework Adherence and Competence Scale
postgraduate experience. Raters were trained at two
(HAACS) provided an opportunity to investigate
sites at Massey University in Auckland, New
the relationship between two areas of limited-
Zealand and La Trobe University in Melbourne,
domain competence considered to be important for
Australia. Raters at both sites received up to 13 h of
positive therapy outcomes.
training broken up into three days. Training was
It follows that our primary hypothesis was that: (1)
delivered by the primary author and a senior clinical
greater therapist competence in case conceptualization
psychologist with expert knowledge in the field.
would predict improvement in depressive symptoms as
Raters were given an overview of case conceptualiz-
measured on the BDI-II (hypothesis 1), and (2) the
ation in CBT and orientation to the CRS use and
impact of therapist competence in homework use
development. Short clips of therapy sessions were
(which is theorised to be heavily informed be individua-
then used to represent more competent and less com-
lised case conceptualization) would also be associated
petent use of different components of CBT case con-
with CBT outcomes (hypothesis 2). Finally, we con-
ceptualization (e.g., therapist and patient linking
sidered that (3) pertinent patient attributes (i.e.,
thoughts, emotion and behaviour in a collaborative
beliefs about homework, symptom severity and com-
or non-collaborative manner; therapist incorporating
plexity) would moderate the relationship between
patient strengths). Lastly raters viewed three full
therapist competence and outcome (hypothesis 3).
DVD recorded therapy sessions independently then
discussed and calibrated scores against each other
and benchmark criterion scores assigned by the
trainers.
Method
Participants
Measures
Patients. For inclusion in the study patients were
required to be experiencing a major depressive Conceptualization Rating Scale (CRS). The
episode (DSM-IV-TR) for the first time. Patients CRS is an observer rated measure of therapist skills
were required to be between 18 and 65 years old, pro- in eliciting information used to develop comprehen-
ficient in reading, writing, and conversing in English. sive case conceptualizations based on live therapy
Patients were not eligible if they were taking central recordings. There are four domains for each item
nervous system (CNS) acting drugs, or if they met that include the (1) integration, (2) importance, (3)
criteria for substance abuse, psychosis, or borderline therapist competence, and (4) fit / match of the in-
personality disorder. In total 28 patients met the session use of case conceptualization with the
DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000) criteria for Major written case conceptualization (i.e., to assist in deter-
Depressive Disorder (MDD) and received treatment. mining whether content relevant to important aspects
The sample consisted of 10 male (36%) and 18 of case conceptualization discussed during a therapy
Psychotherapy Research 5

session are then captured in written case conceptual- theoretical foundations on which the CCD was
izations produced by therapists). Each of the four created. Although other written formats for case con-
domains are applied to 12 items considered most rel- ceptualizations in CBT exist, the continued popular-
evant to discussion and development of case concep- ity and common usage of the CCD in clinical practice
tualizations, namely, (1) identification of a situation, after over 20 years of existence provided a sound
(2) automatic thoughts, (3) meaning of automatic rationale for investigating assumptions regarding the
thoughts, (4) emotion, (5) behaviour, (6) relevant empirical utility of the CCD. As such each item on
background data, (7) core beliefs, (8) conditional the CRS pertained to components of the CCD
assumptions, (9) compensatory strategies, (10) a dis- (e.g., relevant situation, automatic thoughts,
order-specific model, (11) client resilience/strengths, emotion, behaviour, core beliefs, etc). In additional
and (12) sharing the overall case conceptualization. to items mirroring the different components of the
For the purposes of this study therapist compe- CCD the items “disorder specific model,” “resili-
tence across these items was the focus as the ence/strength,” and the “sharing of the overall con-
primary predictor variable. The integration and ceptualization” were included in the CRS.
importance domains are simple dichotomous scales In constructing item scales, the theoretical prop-
included primarily to cue the rater to the presence osition and “metacompetencies” identified by
of relevant case conceptualization components Kuyken et al. (2008, 2009) were adopted which sup-
during the course of a session, in order to then gener- ported a focus on three interrelated areas: (1) colla-
ate ratings of therapist competence in the use of these borative empiricism (i.e., working together to
components. Therapist competence in case concep- develop mutual understanding, consistently sharing
tualization was measured on a 7-point sub-scale (0 aspects of the case conceptualization and seeking
= less competent to 6 = more competent) with descriptive patient feedback on the case conceptualization as it
anchors. While the stand alone tick-box for inte- is develops and evolves in session, etc), (2) depth of
gration and importance draws the rater’s attention discussion (i.e., level of cognitive / behavioural
the presence or absence of particular conceptualiz- exploration, hypothesising and testing links between
ation components (and might be used to readily different situations and components of the case con-
provide feedback to therapists neglecting to discuss, ceptualization), and (3) patient strength/resilience
for example emotion or client strengths, or to focus (i.e., explicit focus on existing coping or adaptive
on less important or relevant components of the con- qualities consistent with holistic and comprehensive
ceptualization) the therapist competence domain case conceptualization in modern CBT practice).
incorporates whether or not a component of case
conceptualization was judged to be integrated skil-
Expert feedback and face validity. Feedback
fully (or competently omitted) and thus considered
on draft versions of the CRS measure was sought
important / relevant. For instance, to obtain a more
by clinicians and researchers considered to be
competent score therapists were required to identify
experts, including a panel of clinicians (n = 8) with
and integrate one or more important components of
peer-reviewed publications in the field of psychother-
the case conceptualization into the session (e.g.,
apy case conceptualization. Some notable and con-
select a specific and relevant situation, explicitly ask
sistent feedback that resulted in changes to early
about thoughts, meanings and/or conditional
drafts included ensuring the CRS had the flexibility
assumptions attributed, emotion and behaviour).
to assess therapists as competent when particular
This was required to be done with appropriate
areas of the conceptualization were not integrated
depth of discussion also fostering links between
or deeply explored (e.g., the therapist appropriately
different components, and eliciting patient feedback
judges that it is inadvisable to activate a core belief
intermittently (i.e., about relevance, acceptance and
as in the case of a fragile patient, weak therapeutic
their understanding of session content).
alliance, early in treatment, or when activation of a
core belief might significantly increase depression or
anger). A second round of feedback from experts
Conceptual and theoretical basis. It was
reflected good content validity with a general consen-
intended that the different items and domains on
sus that the CRS was a comprehensive case concep-
the CRS would reflect cognitive behavioural theory
tualization measure appropriate for use in CBT.
(Beck, 1976) and be acceptable within the framework
of competent day-to-day practice of CBT. In turn,
the J. Beck Case Conceptualization Diagram Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II; Beck
(CCD; Beck, 1995, 2011) was used to provide a fra- et al., 1979; Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996). The
mework for items on the CRS. This decision was pri- BDI-II is a 21-item patient self-report measure
marily made due to the cognitive behavioural widely used to assess depression severity. Each item
6 M. H. Easden and R. B. Fletcher

is rated on a “0” to “3” point scale with possible completed measure of homework compliance
scores ranging from 0 to 63. The BDI-II has been (Munro, 2006).
extensively researched and is a commonly used
measure in the treatment of depression (Dozois, Personality Beliefs Questionnaire (PBQ;
Dobson, & Ahnberg, 1998; Schotte, Maes, Cluydts, Beck et al., 2001; Beck & Beck, 1991; Butler,
De Doncker, & Cosyns, 1997; Steer, Ball, Ranieri, Brown, Beck, & Grisham, 2002). The PBQ is a
& Beck, 1999). It has demonstrated excellent internal 126-item self-report measure rated on a 5-point
consistency (α = .92) among depressed outpatients Likert scale from “0” to “4” (0 = “I don’t believe it
(Beck et al., 1996) and good validity across a range at all” to 4 = “I believe it totally”). Patient’s endorse-
of domains (i.e., content, concurrent, factorial com- ments of dysfunctional personality belief statements
position). In the current study internal consistency generate nine categories including Avoidant, Depen-
for the BDI-II was excellent with values all equal to dent, Obsessive Compulsive, Histrionic, Passive-
or greater than α = .90. Aggressive, Narcissistic, Paranoid, Schizoid, and
Antisocial. The PBQ categories provide an indication
of the presence of personality disorders where a link
Homework Adherence and Competence Scale between these categories and associated personality
(HAACS; Kazantzis, Wedge, & Dobson, 2005). disorder diagnoses has been demonstrated (Beck
The HAACS is an observer-rated 19-item measure et al., 2001; Butler et al., 2002). Cronbach’s alpha
rated on a 7-point Likert scale with descriptive (α) coefficients for individual scales on the PBQ
anchors, designed to measure adherence and compe- ranged from .79 to .94 across two studies (Beck
tence in the review, design and assigning of homework et al., 2001; Butler et al., 2002). Beck et al. (2001)
tasks (in the current study scored by raters based on reported the median inter-correlations between
DVD recordings of therapy sessions). Total adherence scales were moderate (r = .51) and Pearson test-
percentage agreement has been demonstrated to range retest correlations for the individual PBQ categories
from 74% to 85% and competence scales have pro- were adequate ranging from r = .57 to r = .93.
duced ICCs ranging from .79 to .83 (Wedge, 2005).
Internal consistency has been demonstrated to be
Composite Diagnostic Interview (CIDI)
excellent (α = .80) for the competence domain
version 2.1 (WHO, 1997). The CIDI is a structured
(Kazantzis et al., 2006). In a further study (Munro,
computerised assessment tool designed to assist clin-
2006) Cronbach’s alpha (α) were .77 for adherence
icians in providing diagnoses for a range of mental
and .81 for competence. Internal consistency scores
disorders consistent with the DSM-IV. Of particular
for each section review (α = .70), assign (α = .82),
relevance to the current study the CIDI was used to
and design (α = .80) were produced in a similar
generate a potential and preliminary diagnosis and
range. Concurrent validity was also demonstrated
severity of first episode Major Depressive Disorder
with 20% of shared variance (r = .45) with a thera-
(MDD). The CIDI Version 2.1 has been demon-
pist-completed measure of homework adherence and
strated to have adequate validity (Andrews &
competence. Internal consistency for the therapist
Peters, 1998). Kessler et al. (2003) in a large
competence scale in the current study ranged from
sample (N = 9090) door to door survey found a con-
α = .76 to α = .89. Intraclass correlation coefficients
cordance (Cohen’s k = .40) between the CIDI and
reflected high rates of agreement (ICC = .91).
the structured clinical interview for DSM-IV
(SCID) for those diagnosed with experiencing a
major depressive episode over the last 12 months.
Homework Rating Scale (HRS-II; Kazantzis
et al., 2005). The HRS-II client-version is a 12-
item questionnaire rated on a 5-point Likert scale Procedure
(0 = “not at all” to 4 = “extremely”) used to rate
patient beliefs about between-session activities (i.e., Screening. Clients either self-referred or were
homework). Chronbach alpha (α) coefficients have referred by a third party (e.g., clinical agency,
been reported to range from .71 to .91 across sessions family, GP). Of the 251 individuals who made
(Sachsenweger, Fletcher, & Clarke, 2015) for the initial phone contact to the study, 186 were not
client version. A three factor model was produced found to meet inclusion criteria after an initial 30-
(Bjornholdt, 2006) consisting of items pertaining to minute structured telephone interview by study
benefits and completion (Factor 1), costs and com- coordinators. The 65 remaining met preliminary
pletion (Factor 2), and client beliefs (Factor 3). The criteria for MDD and attended appointments for
HRS-II has demonstrated concurrent validity with further screening on the CIDI and an initial inter-
37% of shared variance (r = .61) with a patient- view with a therapist. Of these 37 did not meet
Psychotherapy Research 7

inclusion criteria. Each of the 28 patients accepted and sharing of the case conceptualization. The
were allocated to a new therapist at random and HRS-II, CIDI and PBQ were used primarily as con-
offered up to 20 CBT sessions (average of 17.79 trols to reduce confounds associated with patient who
sessions) with an additional two booster sessions might hold strongly negative views about homework
offered at two months and six months following (potentially influencing the impact of therapist com-
therapy. petence in homework use) and where the symptom
severity and complexity of a patient might account
Rating procedure for process measures. All for change that would otherwise be attributed to
therapy sessions were video recorded on DVD. For therapist effects. Thus where possible, the study
the 28 patients receiving therapy, DVD recordings adopted a process-oriented design, rather than pre–
of 225 individual CBT sessions were observed in post, to facilitate more in-depth analysis.
their entirety and rated using the CRS and the
HAACS. The HAACS adherence and competence Treatment fidelity. Regular fidelity checks and
scale ratings were based solely on observations of weekly supervision sessions with therapists were
the DVD recorded sessions. The CRS competence carried out over the course of therapy by a senior
scale was the primary predictor variable of interest registered clinical CBT psychologist. Ratings were
in the current study. The CRS integration, impor- made based on DVD recorded therapy sessions for
tance, and competence scales were also based on each therapist using the Cognitive Therapy Scale
DVD recorded sessions. Of note, the CRS fit / (CTS; Young & Beck, 1980) to provide an indication
match scale ratings were made by raters comparing of the level of therapist global competence in the car-
the case conceptualization content observed to be rying out the treatment protocol as intended. Thera-
discussed in-session with available written case con- pists were required to achieve a score of at least 40 in
ceptualization data; this was done to examine if thera- three CTS assessments or were required to demon-
pists were able to capture the content of in-session strate increased competence in further CTS assess-
case conceptualizations in their static / written case ments until this level of competence was attained.
conceptualizations subsequently generated. To facili- CTS scoring was carried out and reviewed by the
tate this aspect of the rating procedure therapists therapist’s clinical supervisor, an experienced regis-
completed J. Beck CCDs (Beck, 1995) systematically tered clinical psychologist; her role was independent
at five sampling points (Sessions 1, 3, 5, 8, and 10). to other aspects of the current study.
Information included in the J. Beck CCDs was gener-
ated collaboratively with the patient during each
session and the J. Beck CCDs were formally written Data Analytic Approach and Procedure
up by therapists between sessions. Inter-rater agree- All analyses were undertaken using SPSS version
ment was based on ratings by pairs of independent 17.0; SPSS MIXED procedure was used for multile-
raters. DVD-recorded sessions were randomly allo- vel modelling analysis (MLM; Raudenbush & Bryk,
cated to pairs of raters; randomisation and allocation 2002; Singer & Willett, 2003). We used the Full
began before all therapy sessions were complete, Maximum Likelihood (FML) which provides a
some DVD-recordings were rated when available. description of fit of the model for both fixed and
All ratings were double coded and completed for random parameters and allows for comparison of
over 80% of the total available sessions. Ratings goodness of fit across models. Intercepts and slopes
were made for session one through to session ten as were modelled as random effects in the analyses as
the focus of this study. we would expect variation in scores. We used an
unstructured covariance matrix which allowed us to
Client-rated outcomes. The BDI-II was the estimate a correlation between intercepts and
primary dependent (outcome) variable and was com- slopes. Data were centred consistent with recommen-
pleted at every session. The HRS-II (client-rated dations for MLM analyses (Peugh & Enders, 2005;
version) was used to assess client beliefs about home- Singer & Willett, 2003). Akaike Information Cri-
work and was also completed after every session. The terion (AIC) was used as an indication of model fit
CIDI was used as a measure of symptom severity and given the number of parameters included. Pseudo
completed at intake. The PBQ was used as a measure R 2 was used as a measure of effect size to determine
of patient complexity and was completed once at within patient and between patient variance
session eight, where changes in personality beliefs explained consistent with recommendations by
have been suggested to be more measurable from Singer and Willett (2003).
session eight onwards (Beck & Freeman, 1990), The data were structured as person-period data that
and was intended to further inform development examines both between person (i.e., comparison of
8 M. H. Easden and R. B. Fletcher

average change trajectories comparing one patient to For observational interrater data (i.e., the CRS and
another) and within person variation (i.e., over time HAACS) two raters generated independent scores for
across individual patients). In the current study each each observation. The average score for each pair of
successive measurement of depression severity ratings was selected to represent the patients’ score.
(Level 1) was nested within each patient (Level 2). This was done in consideration of high interrater
Rather than treating patients as nested within thera- reliability scores obtained. All remaining data were
pists (i.e., clustered data structure, often but not treated as continuous time series data within the
always measured at one time point), in order to data set to allow for the possibility of differences in
examine change across sessions we examined how the level of variables over time (i.e., Level 1). The
change in depressive symptoms differ as a function exception to this was the treatment of symptom sever-
of pertinent patient characteristics. Initially at the ity (i.e., CIDI) and personality beliefs (i.e., PBQ)
within person level (Level 1) we introduce therapist which were treated as constant over time and
competence (in both case conceptualization and divided into categories of severity (i.e., Level 2).
homework use) as predictor variables which might Specifically, the CIDI generates four possibilities for
be associated with observed depressive symptom indications of symptom severity associated with
change. We then introduce pertinent patient charac- depression: “no diagnosis” and “mild” (coded as
teristics that might moderate the strength of this “0”), “moderate” (coded as “1”) and “severe”
association at the between person level (Level 2). (coded as “2”). The PBQ generates values associated
At present when using MLM analysis there is no with lesser or greater degrees of disordered personal-
consensus in regards to appropriate sample size ity beliefs and patient complexity. Total scale person-
required to produce stable and precise estimates. ality beliefs scores were divided evenly between the
Rules of thumb are inconsistent across studies lower and higher 50th percentile (coded as “0” and
although generally recommend a minimum of 10, “1” respectively) splitting patients into one of two
20 or 30 observations/units at Level 2 while larger categories (i.e., high or low personality beliefs).
samples are always advisable (McNeish & Stapleton, This splitting of the total scale PBQ score also
2016; Snijders & Bosker, 2012). These studies do not coincided with recommendations for measuring
take into account the number of observations per observable differences in personality beliefs in
variable (i.e., at successive time-points) which can patients with features of personality disordered
be used to guide sample size decisions in clustered beliefs (Beck et al., 2001).
data. Kwok et al. (2008) noted that a large number
of waves/repeated measures data units at Level 1
were often not applicable in psychological research, Results
although reliable estimates for the individual growth
Preliminary Analyses
models can be obtained with 8 or more measurement
waves. Wynants et al. (2015) recommended at least Patients presented with a range of severity of depressive
10 observation points for predefined models. In the symptoms and generally improved over the course of
current study the use of 10 observation points (i.e., the study. Mean baseline BDI-II score was 31.03
10 sessions yielding n = 225 data units) and 28 (SD = 10.87; range from 13 to 53). Mean post treat-
patients (despite only 7 therapists providing ment BDI-II score was 16.80 (SD = 11.12; range
therapy) would suggest that this present analysis from 2 to 45). The results of OLS regression estimates
should lead to unbiased estimates able to produce (see Table I) showed that the average patient had an
meaningful results. initial change in depressive symptoms of 8.47 points
For coding data we determined which variables will which increased by .16 points each session. On
be modelled as “random” effects or as “fixed” effects. average, for example, by session 10 BDI-II scores
Essentially “random” effects were obtained when the
coefficients for variables were allowed to vary over the Table I. Descriptive statistics for the individual growth parameters
course of therapy (i.e., time-variant, continuous data) obtained by fitting separate within-person OLS regression models
sessions within patients and “fixed” effects were for BDI Change as a function of curvilinear time.
obtained when coefficients for variables were con- Initial status Rate of change
stant (i.e., time-invariant, categorical data) for indi- (intercept) (slope)
vidual patients (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002; Snijders
& Bosker, 2012). Therapist competence in both Mean 8.47∗∗∗ 0.16∗∗∗
case conceptualization and homework in its raw Standard error 1.06 0.04
Bivariate 0.34∗
form was continuous data that are variable over the correlation
course of therapy for individual patients and was
therefore modelled as a random effect at Level 1. Note. ∗ p < .05, ∗∗ p < .01, ∗∗∗ p < .001.
Psychotherapy Research 9

improved by 10.07 (8.47 + (10 × .16)). These results Table II. Mean ratings on conceptualization rating scale (CRS)
showed that improvement (i.e., change trajectory) in Sub-scales.
depressive symptom change was maintained as CRS sub-scales Score
increases over the course of therapy for the average
patient. The correlation coefficient of r = .34 revealed Integration (0–12) 2.39 + 2.36 (0 − 12)
a positive relationship between time (session) and Importance (0–12) 9.00 + 1.79 (1.5 − 13.5)
Competence (0–72) 25.14 + 9.78 (0 to 55)
depressive symptom change.
Fit / Match (0–72) 29.58 + 11.36 (8.5 − 61.5)
Initially, we were able to visually observe raw
levels of therapist competence in case conceptualiz- Note. Values are given as means ± SD, with ranges in parentheses.
ation for each session for individual patients (see
Figure 1). Table II provides descriptive character-
istics of domains on the CRS. Competence scores on the CRS to provide an indication of the full
on the CRS ranged from 0 to 55 (M = 25.14, SD scale reliability of the overall measure. Cronbach’s
= 9.78). Not surprisingly bivariate correlation alpha coefficients for integration, importance, com-
revealed a negative association between fitted petence and fit / match were .72, .66, .79 and .87
initial status and fitted slope (rate of change) of respectively. High Cronbach’s alpha scores
CRS competence (r = −.82, p < .01), where for suggested that total scale scores for each domain
example those therapists with higher initial compe- were a good indicator of individual items in each
tence had a slower rate of improvement in compe- domain.
tence scores, where those with lower competence
scores had greater room for more rapid improve-
ment over time. Interrater Reliability
Intercorrelations were calculated (see Table III) in
To establish interrater reliability Cohen’s Kappa (k)
order to provide an indication of the direction (i.e.,
coefficients were used for dichotomous data
positive or negative association) of relationships
domains (i.e., Integration and Importance) and Intra-
hypothesised between variables in the present study,
class Correlation Coefficients (ICCs) were used for
which were generally as expected.
continuous data domains (i.e., Competence and Fit/
Match). Total score reliability estimates (see
Table IV) for Integration (k = .83) and Importance (k
Internal Consistency
= .65) were excellent and adequate respectively.
Cronbach’s alpha (α) coefficients were computed to Total score reliability estimates for Competence
determine the internal consistency of each domain (ICC = .93) and Fit/Match (ICC = .86) were both

Figure 1. Therapist competence scores in case conceptualization for each patient on the CRS by session (over time).
10 M. H. Easden and R. B. Fletcher
Table III. Intercorrelations among predictor, control and dependent variables.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

1. CRS integration –
2. CRS importance .769∗∗ –
3. CRS competence .807∗∗ .699∗∗ –
4. CRS fit / match .271∗∗ .272∗∗ .455∗∗ –
5. HAACS adherence .041 −.032 .106 .131 –
6. HAACS competence .084 −.002 .186∗∗ .193∗∗ .803∗∗ –
7. PBQ (complexity) −.061 −.064 −.025 .078 −.049 −.062 –
8. HRS-II (patient beliefs) −.043 .030 −.058 .000 −.057 −.031 −.255∗∗ –
9. CIDI (symptom severity) −.049 −.015 −.059 −.043 −.008 .041 .176∗∗ .162∗ –
10. BDI raw −.149∗ −.142∗ −.165∗ −.166∗ −.044 −.166 .335∗∗ .036 .240∗∗ –
11. BDI change .138∗ .173∗∗ .142∗ .067 .062 .075 −.061 −.080 .440∗∗ −.440∗∗ –

Note. ∗ p < .05, ∗∗ p < .01.

Table IV. Individual item and total scale reliability estimates for the CRS.

Integration Importance Competence Fit / Match

Situation 0.79 0.32 0.91 0.87


Automatic thoughts 0.82 0.45 0.89 0.91
Meaning of ATs 0.66 0.46 0.81 0.77
Emotion 0.80 0.17 0.88 0.69
Behaviour 0.73 0.29 0.82 0.76
Relevant childhood data 0.80 0.59 0.90 0.85
Core beliefs 0.71 0.60 0.91 0.85
Conditional assumptions 0.77 0.66 0.90 0.77
Compensatory strategies 0.73 0.42 0.76 0.69
Disorder-specific model 0.79 0.15 0.94 0.68
Resilience / strengths 0.66 0.12 0.88 0.89
Sharing of overall CC 0.87 0.40 0.99 0.96
Total estimate 0.83 0.65 0.93 (0.87) 0.86 (0.75)

Note. Integration and Importance are reported as Cohen’s Kappa, Competence and Fit / match reported as ICCs. Individual item estimates
are reported as average measure ICC (2, k), total ICCs are reported as average measure ICC (2, k) and single measure ICC (2, 1) estimates.

excellent. As with total scale scores, individual item Multilevel Model Analysis
reliability estimates for Competence were the most
robust (ICC = .76–.99). Interrater reliability was Prior to conducting the MLM analysis data were
demonstrated to be fairly consistent over time (i.e., screened for each variable and assumptions sur-
at each individual time-point for sessions one to rounding normality and homoscedacity were met.
ten) for Integration (ICC range .78 to .85), Importance To ensure there was no biases to due to any systema-
(ICC range .60 to .71), Competence (ICC range .91 to tic pattern of missing data, Little’s Missing Comple-
.96) and Fit/Match (ICC range .77 to .90). One tely At Random (MCAR) analysis was performed.
exception to this was Importance at session 10 (ICC The non-significant result of the MCAR analysis
= .36) which was significantly lower than other indi- (χ 2 = 27.095, df = 28, p = .51) revealed that there
vidual session estimates. This was likely as a result was no indication of non-random missingness.
of session 10 being a unique session in that the proto- MLM analysis was then conducted to test if thera-
col intended for therapists to formally share verbal pist competence in case conceptualization and/or
and/or written case conceptualizations with the homework use was related to more positive outcomes
patient, likely causing disagreement between raters. in consideration of other salient therapy variables (see
Lastly, Competence at session 8 was adequate but Table V). Analysis of fixed effects in Model 1 (i.e.,
lower than other estimates (ICC = .64), also likely unconditional means model or “null” baseline)
due to a divergence in the protocol, whereby thera- found that patients had a non-zero positive BDI-II
pists were asked to complete the PBQ with clients score at intake (γ00 = 10.78, p < .01; SE = 1.33) and
systematically at this session which interfered with the introduction of fixed effects of time in Model 2
therapists routine therapy agenda. (i.e., unconditional growth model incorporating
Psychotherapy Research 11
Table V. Uncontrolled and controlled effects of therapist competence in case conceptualization with therapist competence in homework,
patient beliefs about homework, symptoms severity and personality complexity on depressive symptom change.

Fixed effects Parameter Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6

Initial status
π0i Intercept γ00 10.78∗∗∗ 8.47∗∗∗ 5.73∗∗∗ 9.65∗∗∗ 7.40∗∗∗ 6.16
1.33 1.06 1.42 3.05 2.48 4.60
CRS (competence) γ01 0.11∗ 0.12∗ 0.13∗∗
0.05 0.06 0.05
HAACS (competence) γ02 −0.03 −0.04 −0.04
0.04 0.04 0.03
HRS (patient beliefs) γ03 −0.01
0.07
CIDI (symptom severity) γ04 2.49∼
1.22
PBQ (complexity) γ05 −2.32
1.94
Variance components Parameter Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6

Level 1 Within person s2e 37.91∗∗∗ 28.60∗∗∗ 16.77∗∗∗ 28.41∗∗∗ 16.66∗∗∗ 17.09∗∗∗
3.78 3.02 1.81 3.03 1.81 1.90
Level 2 In initial status s20 42.65∗∗∗ 23.39∗∗∗ 23.52∗∗∗ 23.84∗∗∗ 24.20∗∗∗ 18.99∗∗
12.89 8.35 7.71 8.51 7.93 6.93
In rate of change s21 0.61∗∗∗ 0.51∗∗ 0.58∗∗∗ 0.48∗∗ 0.36∗∗
0.20 0.20 0.21 0.20 0.14
Covariance σe 0.00∗ 0.02∗ 0.02∼ 0.02∼ 0.01
0.01 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.01
Psuedo R 2 statistics and goodness of fit
R2e 0.25 0.41 0.00 0.42 0.40
R20 0.45 0.00 −0.02 −0.03 0.19
R21 0.16 0.11 0.11 0.20

Deviance 1537.64 1483.91 1349.76 1465.32 1330.55 1132.13


AIC 1543.64 1495.98 1365.76 1481.32 1350.55 1157.13
BIC 1553.93 1516.56 1392.99 1508.65 1384.44 1208.66
Δ AIC 47.02 123.57 7.91 132.12 181.81

Note. ∼p < .10, ∗ p < .05, ∗∗ p < .01, ∗∗∗ p < .001.

time) showed that BDI-II scores continued to have a change by 0.11 units (p < .01; SE = 0.05). Analysis
non-zero positive change over time (γ02 = 8.47, of random effects revealed that inclusion of therapist
p < .01; SE = 1.06). The analysis of random effects competence in case conceptualization accounted for
showed that a quarter of within patient variance in a 41% reduction in within patient variance. The
depressive symptoms is explained by time (R2e = within-patient variance (i.e., continuous variable,
25%, p < .001) and almost half of between patient random effects) refers to how much patients tended
variance (R20 = 45%, p < .001). The significant to change in depression severity over time, referring
results of variance components suggest that further to the mean of the change for the average individual
variance is able to be explained by the addition of in our sample. Thus, without giving due consider-
further predictor variables to the model at both ation to pertinent patient variables considered to
Level 1 and Level 2. Models 1 and 2 effectively estab- influence the therapy outcome (i.e., symptom sever-
lished a baseline of positive treatment outcomes over ity at intake, dysfunctional personality) therapist
time (i.e., reduction in depressive symptoms) and competence in case conceptualization showed a posi-
demonstrated that there was sufficient variation tive association with treatment outcomes (i.e.,
between individual patients to justify further com- reduction in depressive symptoms over time) for the
parison using additional substantive predictors. average patient.
Hypothesis 1. Model 3 (i.e., the conditional Hypothesis 2. Model 4 introduced therapist com-
growth model) introduced therapist competence in petence in homework use (HAACS Competence) into
case conceptualization (CRS Competence) into the the model independently of therapist competence in
model. Analysis of fixed effects revealed that for case conceptualization (CRS Competence). Analysis
every one unit change on the BDI-II, therapist com- of fixed effects revealed no direct significant associ-
petence in case conceptualization is estimated to ation (γ02 = −.03, NS; SE = .04) between therapist
12 M. H. Easden and R. B. Fletcher

competence in homework use and positive change in depressive symptoms (BDI Change). Thus, analysis
depressive symptoms and no reduction in within of fixed effects of the final model (Model 6) demon-
patient or between patient variance was observed. strated that taken together, after controlling for
Although therapist competence in homework did salient variables that impact the ability of a therapist
not appear to be a strong predictor of positive to display competence in case conceptualization and
change in depressive symptoms, the reduction in homework use during therapy, that for every one
the Akaike Information Criterion (Δ AIC = 7.91) unit change on the BDI-II, therapist competence in
from Model 3 to Model 4 suggested that therapist case conceptualization is estimated to change by
competence in homework use provided a better 0.13 units (p < .01; SE = 0.05). Model 6 explained
overall fit for in the prediction of positive change in the greatest amount of variance at Level 1 (R2e =
depressive symptoms and should be retained in the 40%, p < .01) and Level 2 (R20 = 19%, p < .001).
subsequent models. As such therapist competence Thus, the introduction of Level 2 predictors (i.e., cat-
in homework did not reveal a strong association egorical variables, fixed effects) begins to account for
with treatment outcomes independently, although between-patient variance; that is, that the individual
may provide some greater impact when we simul- patient would tend to show more or less change in
taneously considering the therapist’s competence in depressive symptoms associated with either severe,
case conceptualization (e.g., if a therapist has linked moderate or mild initial symptom severity, and
aspects of a shared case conceptualization with whether there were lesser or greater complexity of
between session tasks or experiments hypothesised personality features respectively. The variance
to ultimately lead to patient skill acquisition and explained by Level 1 predictors (i.e., continuous vari-
self-awareness towards depressive symptom ables, random effects), however, remained consistent
reduction). and suggests stability of the model. The reduction in
Hypotheses 1 and 2. Model 5 represented the the Akaike Information Criterion (Δ AIC = 181.81)
combination of therapist competence in case concep- from the “baseline” or unconditional growth model
tualization and in homework use investigated indivi- (Model 3) to Model 6 represent a “very strong”
dually in Models 3 and 4. Analysis of fixed effects overall fit of the final model.
for therapist competence in homework use in
Model 5 showed that patients had a non-zero positive
BDI-II score at intake which remained significant
Discussion
(γ00 = 7.40, p < .01; SE = 2.48) suggesting that the
inclusion of additional predictors may be useful in This is the first study to investigate therapist compe-
further explaining variance in the final model. Analy- tence in case conceptualization as it relates to out-
sis of fixed effects revealed that the inclusion of thera- comes in CBT for depressed patients (i.e.,
pist competence in homework use in the model symptom change). In order to examine the hypoth-
marginally increased the magnitude of therapist com- esised relationship between case conceptualization
petence in case conceptualization (γ01 = 0.12, p < .01; and symptom change, the CRS was designed and
SE = 0.06). Analysis of random effects explained developed. The CRS was found to be a reliable
more variance at Level 1 than previous models measure with acceptable face and content validity.
(R2e = 41%, p < .01). Overall, the reduction in the The therapist competence scale which formed the
Akaike Information Criterion (Δ AIC = 132.12) in basis of the present investigation was found to have
Model 5 produced the largest reduction in compari- the most robust reliability overall compared with
son to previous models suggesting that a combined other sub-domains on the CRS. Internal consistency
model of therapist competence in case conceptualiz- was at least adequate across domains, and good for
ation and homework use should be retained for therapist competence (α = .79). Consistent with con-
further models. ventional guidelines (Cicchetti, 1994) interrater
Hypotheses 3. Having entered each variable sys- reliability was good across domains and excellent
tematically towards a “final model” therapist compe- for the therapist competence scale (ICC = .93). Inter-
tence in both case conceptualization (CRS rater reliability for therapist competence was excel-
Competence) and homework use (HAACS Compe- lent at the individual item level (ICC = .76–.99) and
tence) were retained as predictor variables. After con- at the level of the individual therapy session across
sideration of variance explained both individually and ten therapy sessions (ICC = .91–.96); this was with
in combination, homework beliefs (HRS Beliefs), per- the exception of lower, although still good interrater
sonality beliefs (PBQ Complexity) and symptom reliability at session eight (ICC = .64).
severity (CIDI Symptom Severity) each were retained, High interrater reliability for the integration sub-
all of which provided significant reductions in var- domain (k = .83) indicated that raters were able to
iance associated with a relationship with change in agree as to whether or not therapists made explicitly
Psychotherapy Research 13

discussed different components of case conceptualiz- outcome (γ01 = .11, p < .01; SE = 0.05) which pro-
ation in session irrespective of the level of compe- vided support for this hypothesis. The results
tence in doing so (i.e., did the therapist select a showed that across models between 40% to 42% of
situation and enquire about automatic thoughts, within patient variance in depression scores (i.e., a
emotions, behaviour, attempt to share aspects of the measure of how much on average the individual
case conceptualization, etc). Similarly high interrater patient tended to change or vary over time) was
reliability for the fit/match sub-domain (ICC = .93) attributed to therapist competence in case conceptu-
suggested raters were able to agree when information alization, and that after controlling for time and other
relevant to the case conceptualization was captured in pertinent patient characteristics this result remained
written conceptualizations (i.e., J. Beck Case Con- stable at 40% in the final model. These results
ceptualization Diagram or other relevant written suggest that the CRS would be a useful measure of
format selected by therapists). However, interrater therapist competence in case conceptualization in
reliability for the importance sub-domain (k = .65) CBT to assist in enhancing resolution of depressive
was relatively low; this suggests that raters were less symptomology.
able to agree on what aspects of the case conceptual- Hypothesis 2. Hypothesis two was not strongly
ization should be prioritised for discussion, and con- supported. Therapist competence in homework use
versely when aspects of the case conceptualization as measured by the HAACS was not associated
were less relevant to the particular session (e.g., expli- with depressive symptom change in our sample
cit discussion and identification of negative core (γ02 = −.03, NS; SE = .04). However, therapist com-
beliefs would be generally be inadvisable in the first petence in homework and in case conceptualization
session of therapy). Of note, raters had the poorest as measured on the HAACS and CRS respectively
agreement (k = .12) for the importance of integrating has a small positive correlation (r = .186; p < .01).
client resilience / strengths as an explicit focus of dis- As such, the association between therapist compe-
cussion in session. This might reflect a need for tence in homework use and therapist competence in
further development of the importance item and case conceptualization as this related to outcomes
rater training to correctly identify the relative impor- was less clear and would warrant further investi-
tance and timing of different aspects of case concep- gation. One meta-analysis suggested that using
tualization during the course of therapy. This might ratings incorporating both patient and therapist
further reflect a lack of existing guidelines in experiences, consideration of timing (i.e., retrospec-
regards as to how a therapist can determine the tive or anticipatory) and types of homework tasks
importance of different aspects of the developing (e.g., thought records, specific behaviour exper-
case conceptualization over time. Few studies have iments) are more likely to more accurately account
directly investigated clinicians’ perceptions as to for the impact of homework on treatment outcomes
what is important in case conceptualization. Existing (Mausbach et al., 2010). Consistent with this
research suggests a lack of complete consensus finding, individually, patient beliefs about homework
among clinicians and a need for improved clinical (i.e., measured on the HRS-II) were found to explain
training to address this need (Flitcroft, James, Free- a greater amount of variance in depression scores
ston, & Wood-Mitchell, 2007; Huisman & Kangas, than therapist competence in homework use. Tenta-
2018). tively, the results might suggest that supervisors and
Internal consistency and total scale interrater clinicians should consider which measures are most
reliability of the CRS were comparable to levels likely to capture patient and therapist experiences
established for well-trained raters using the CTS and the idiosyncrasies of unique homework tasks as
(Vallis et al., 1986) and the CTS-R (Blackburn these link to individualised between session home-
et al., 2001), while individual item and individual work tasks.
session level estimates were more robust with less Hypothesis 3. The introduction of pertinent
variability observed on the CRS. Having demon- patient attributes in the final combined model
strated that the CRS was a fairly reliable measure in accounted for 19% of between patient variance,
the current sample in relation to accepted conven- where consideration of personality complexity,
tions and established measures of therapist compe- symptom severity and factors related to homework
tence we proceeded to test our primary hypotheses. (i.e., therapist competence and patient beliefs) are
Hypothesis 1. We examined the validity of the likely to further moderate the extent to which thera-
CRS competence scale as the primary predictor to pists are able to demonstrate competence in case con-
determine therapy outcome (i.e., depressive symp- ceptualization and how this might influence symptom
toms change on the BDI-II). After controlling for change between patients. However, interpreting the
time, there was a positive association between thera- relative contribution of covariates (i.e., PBQ, CIDI,
pist competence in case conceptualization and and HRS-II) was less readily interpretable. The
14 M. H. Easden and R. B. Fletcher

PBQ was used as a proxy for patient complexity (i.e., relations (see a meta-analytical review by Webb
due to the presence of features of dysfunctional et al., 2010 and accompanying supplementary
beliefs) in conjunction with the CIDI which was data). Furthermore strict inclusion criteria meant
used as a proxy of symptom severity. The HRS-II patients in this study would not be representative of
was used to consider if beliefs about homework the range depressed patients in a naturalistic out-
(e.g., that monitoring thoughts / beliefs, emotion patient setting likely to have multiple comorbid diag-
and behaviour or engaging in behavioural exper- noses. However, even within this fairly homogeneous
iments in problem situations, etc) separate to the sample patient complexity (e.g., personality beliefs,
competence of the therapist conceptualizing and symptom severity) contributed to the ability to
planning between session activities. However, none explain between patient differences in our model.
of these measures independently contributed signifi- Similarly, the study was unique in that clients
cantly to the final model. Consistent with these find- taking CNS acting medication were excluded.
ings Kuyken and Tsivrikos (2009) found that more Although in practice clients are routinely prescribed
competent therapists achieved improved outcomes antidepressant medication concurrently while under-
regardless of patient comorbidity and/ presumed going CBT for depression, the outcomes and results
complexity. Strunk, Brotman, DeRubeies, and of the current study cannot be considered attribu-
Hollon (2010) found that competence was more table to the effect of medication, while nor can they
highly related to outcomes in patient with higher be considered superior. Further investigation of
symptom severity, but not associated with whether therapist competence in case conceptualization in a
clients met criteria for personality disorders pre- range of clients groups (e.g., personality disorders,
sumed to be higher in complexity. In practice the psychotic spectrum disorders), different settings
implication is that matching the most competent or (e.g., inpatient, forensic, naturalistic) and with
experienced therapists with patients deemed to be larger sample sizes of patients and particularly thera-
most difficult may be inconsequential or less impor- pists is recommended.
tant that might be presumed during a triage or refer- In the present study the Cognitive Therapy Scale
ral process. (CTS; Young and Beck, 1980) was not able to be
included in analyses due to insufficient data, nor
were there any other available measures of therapist
competence in case conceptualization at the incep-
Limitations and Future Research
tion of the study to provide further examination of
The results of the current study support the over- concurrent validity. Future research would ideally
arching hypothesis that therapist competence in compare global domain and limited domain
case conceptualization is associated with positive measures of therapist competence as these become
outcomes in CBT in the context of depression. available.
However, it must be noted that this study does Once sufficient reliability is established, among
not directly assess therapist competence as a com- measures of global and limited domain competence,
plete construct (Barber et al., 2007). More specifi- therapy outcome-relations can begin to be eluci-
cally, the results support the systematic approach dated. One relatively large study of 43 therapists pro-
taken to demonstrate therapist competence in vided CBT to 1247 patients with anxiety and/or
case conceptualization as defined and operationa- depression. In general therapist competence on the
lised in the CRS measure as a whole. Future CTS-R was not associated with improved patient
research could provide a closer analysis of relative outcomes. However, there was some association
contribution of therapist competence on different between the most competent (top 10%) and least
components of case conceptualization as measured competent (bottom 10%) therapists with patient
by the individual items within the CRS (i.e., behav- improvement and patient deterioration respectively
ioural, emotional, the importance of systematic (Branson, Shafran, & Myles, 2015). While the
sharing of the case conceptualization, focus on authors suggested that measures of global compe-
client strengths, etc). tence such as the CTS-R may be limited in their
The present study was limited by the modest ability to detect specific therapist effects, the research
number of patients (N = 28) included in the sample. also speaks to the complex relationship between
In contrast, the number of individual session ratings different aspects of therapist competence and
(N = 225) generated using therapist competence therapy outcome. Future research might adopt such
measures was relatively large. Overall, the sample an approach alongside comparison of measures of
was comparable or larger than previous studies con- therapist competence that seek to directly measure
ducted using independently rated measures of thera- or tap into case conceptualization as a process and
pist competence to evaluate treatment-outcome central focus of psychotherapy.
Psychotherapy Research 15

Implications for Practice challenging or upsetting cognitions. Furthermore,


discussing the most important or clinically relevant
This study presents several implications relevant to
content in depth, as agreed upon by both the therapist
the practice of CBT. Firstly, the present research
and patient, and routinely helping to patient to notice
has identified particular areas of therapist compe-
how different aspects of their case conceptualization
tence in case conceptualization hypothesised to
are linked to both individual weaknesses and
account for change in CBT for depression. Data
strengths is likely to improve treatment outcomes.
from this study support therapists’ systematic inte-
In terms of implications for training, the present
gration of metacompetencies outlined in the CRS
study involved postgraduate-level independent
considered pertinent to comprehensive cognitive be-
observers and clinicians. Research has demon-
havioural case conceptualization. Table VI summar-
strated that expert therapists are generally more
ises the key features of case conceptualization
skilled in constructing case conceptualizations and
included in the CRS that have demonstrated clinical
perhaps better able to make judgements regarding
utility as a whole (while future studies with larger
what makes a quality case conceptualization than
samples might investigate the relative contribution
experienced or novice therapists (Eells, Lombart,
of each feature). Notwithstanding limitations of the
Kendjelic, Turner, & Lucas, 2005). Regardless,
study, systematic attention to these key areas by the
the use of well-trained raters and a structured and
therapist is suggested to be of benefit to the course
systematic approach to case conceptualization (as
of treatment for depressed patients. The features
we adopted in the current study) has been advocated
are intended to strengthen empirically, and provide
as a potential means to evaluate case conceptualiz-
guidelines for the process of developing and integrat-
ation in CBT (Bieling & Kuyken, 2003; Kuyken
ing case conceptualization as intended in standard
et al., 2005). Our findings suggest that through suf-
CBT for depression (Beck, 1976; Beck et al.,
ficient training (i.e., thirteen hours or the equivalent
1979). For example, attention has been drawn to
of two days of training covering an introduction to
the importance of developing an individualized case
case conceptualization theory and practice in CBT
conceptualization in CBT (Beck, 1995; Needleman,
and the use of the CRS) independent raters con-
1999; Persons, 2001). The guidelines in Table VI
sidered to possess “novice” level experience are
provide some structure towards achieving an indivi-
able to reach high levels of agreement using the
dualized and clinically valid case conceptualization.
CRS.
The results of the present study begin to support
A third implication applies not just to the develop-
the consistent and systematic integration of different
ment of competence in delivering evidence-based
components of case conceptualization as outlined on
interventions consistent with training provided, but
the J. Beck Case Conceptualization Diagram (CCD
also to the maintenance of therapist competence
1995, 2011) written format. Put another way, the
overtime, and as this applies to use and implemen-
research supports the need to discuss each part of
tation of standardised treatment manuals (Waller
the case conceptualization with the patient systemati-
& Turner, 2016). Varying levels of therapist compe-
cally in therapy sessions, but also to do this in a timely
tence in case conceptualization have been observed
manner as the patient is able to cope with increasingly
between therapists and across time, supporting the

Table VI. Common features of in-session therapist competence in CBT case conceptualization.

1. The therapist should consistently discuss in-depth all aspects of a CBT case conceptualization (e.g., automatic thoughts, core
beliefs) apart from when clinically inadvisable (i.e., would cause undue distress without future benefit)
2. The therapist should also make judgements about the timing of discussions of “deeper-level” more inferential (e.g., or potentially
traumatic or difficult) aspects of the conceptualization and gradually increase the depth of discussion
3. The therapist should foster discussions to link different areas of case conceptualization
4. The therapist should seek regular patient feedback (i.e., collaborative empiricism) at each level of conceptualization including the
tentative overall conceptualization, mini-conceptualizations, and relevant disorder-specific models during the session in regards to
the patient’s:
a) Acceptance of the conceptualization
b) Understanding of the conceptualization
c) Assessment of clinical relevance (i.e., importance) of the conceptualization to the resolution of presenting problems and
building of patient strengths
5. The therapist should conceptualize patient strengths as well as weakness as routine practice (NB: The same attribute may severe to
function and both strength and weakness in different situations, and provides opportunities to validate a patient’s efforts to cope)

Note. Summarised from key features of the therapist competence scale of the Conceptualization Rating Scale (CRS). The CRS is available by
request from the primary author.
16 M. H. Easden and R. B. Fletcher

notion that regardless of the validity of the treatment Bostrom, & Bertagnolli, 1999; Persons, Roberts,
manual that the ability of the therapist to demon- Zalecki, & Brechwald, 2006). The broad impli-
strate competence in using that manual will directly cation is that measures and methods suitable for
impact the resultant treatment utility of the manual use in routine psychotherapy practice must be
for an individual patient. It has been asserted that developed, validated, and shown to be effective
“the interventionist does not always equal the inter- before definitive conclusions can be drawn in
vention” (Nezu & Nezu, 2005). As such integrity regards to empirically-based guidelines for
checks and systematic feedback are ideally offered routine clinical practice (Castonguay, Barkham,
to therapists regarding their levels of competence Lutz, & McAleavey, 2013).
in different components of treatment based on live
observation or ratings of therapy sessions (e.g.,
using observational measures such as the CTS or
Conclusion
the CRS in the context of CBT). It has been
suggested that systematic “spot checks” and This research is the first to investigate how thera-
regular supervisory feedback be provided to thera- pist competence relates to depressive symptom
pists in consideration of the cost of detailed and change using time-series data, with a focus on
time consuming adherence and competence case conceptualization as a measurable and valid
ratings. In our sample therapists demonstrating area of central therapist competencies in CBT.
high levels of competence in early sessions were Case conceptualization research in CBT has
likely to remain competent. One meta-analysis con- tended to focus on examination of inter-clinician
cluded that the impact of therapist competence on reliability in constructing case conceptualizations,
psychotherapy outcomes was heavily moderated by utilizing expert benchmarks of “gold standard”,
the therapeutic relationship (Webb et al., 2010). “good enough” or idealised prototypes of case con-
As such, to some extent the increase in competence ceptualization content (Easden & Kazantzis,
might be explained by the level of collaboration 2018). The present study adopted a novel
intrinsic to high ratings of therapist competence in approach and more directly considered how case
case conceptualization on the CRS (as per guide- conceptualization is used in practice when thera-
lines outlined in Table VI). Based on our findings pists are provided minimally sufficient training in
taking successive measures of therapist competence CBT and case conceptualization. By observing
in case conceptualization early in the therapy therapist use of case conceptualization in-vivo the
process, until a therapist has demonstrated a results suggest that with sufficient training thera-
minimum level of competence, would be sufficient pists can agree on what is important and relevant
to begin to infer future performance with an individ- in a case conceptualization. They can recognise
ual patient. However the current research has yet to different components and can agree on how well
elucidate more fully under what conditions therapist or how skilfully a therapist has integrated these
competence is likely to change over time. For (or chosen not to integrate these) into a therapy
example, one study reported levels of therapist com- session. By utilizing the CRS, we provide prelimi-
petence trending towards decreasing over time; this nary evidence to support the assertion that cogni-
was despite showing an association between tive case conceptualization is associated with
increased therapist competence and improved improved treatment outcomes (i.e., treatment
overall outcomes of a composite score based on utility) in CBT. Although further validation is
measures of anxiety, depression and functioning required, the CRS (1) provides a valid and reliable
(Brown et al., 2013). structure for assessing case conceptualization com-
Alternatively, clinicians have suggested methods petencies in CBT during therapist sessions, (2) is
of systematic and ongoing self-monitoring of an suitable for use as a training instrument in the
individual therapist’s own performance (for practice of case conceptualization in CBT, (3)
reviews of established methods see Duncan & can be used to inform future attempts to operatio-
Reese, 2015; Lambert & Shimokawa, 2011; nalize and isolate aspects of case conceptualization,
Miller, Duncan, Sorrell, & Brown, 2005; and (4) is based on cognitive behavioural theory.
Østergård, Randa, Hilde & Hougaard, 2018), Finally, the present research has added to the
self-reflective practice (Bennett-Levy, Thwaites, basis of empirical support for both cognitive behav-
Haarhoff, & Perry, 2015; Haarhoff, Gibson, & ioural theory and case conceptualization as a
Flett, 2011) or suggested the examination of indi- central therapeutic process in psychotherapy and
vidual therapist’s levels of performance at the encourages extension of this research in future
level of the clinical practice (Okiishi, Lambert, process-oriented clinical trials and naturalistic
Nielsen, & Ogles, 2003; Persons, 2006; Persons, research.
Psychotherapy Research 17

Acknowledgement Bennett-Levy, J., Thwaites, R., Haarhoff, B., & Perry, H. (2015).
Experiencing CBT from the inside out: A self-practice/self-reflection
The authors thank the experts who kindly provided workbook for therapists. New York: The Guildford Press.
feedback for revision and development of drafts of Bieling, P., & Kuyken, W. (2003). Is cognitive case formulation
the Conceptualization Rating Scale (CRS) particu- science or science fiction? Clinical Psychology: Science and
Practice, 10(1), 52–69. doi:10.1093/clipsy.10.1.52
larly Judith Beck, Willem Kuyken, Keith Dobson, Bjornholdt, A. (2006). A preliminary psychometric investigation of the
and Beverly Haarhoff. The authors also acknowl- homework rating scale-II (Unpublished master’s thesis). Albany:
edge Nikolaos Kazantzis for his contribution as Massey University.
the primary investigator in the “CBT Depression Blackburn, I., James, I. A., Milne, D., Baker, C., Standart, S.,
Study” along with Paul Merrick and Janet Garland, A., & Reichelt, F. (2001). The revised cognitive
therapy scale (CTS-R): Psychometric properties. Behavioural
Leathem. and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 29, 431–446. doi:10.1017/
S1352465801004040
Branson, A., Shafran, R., & Myles, P. (2015). Investigating the
relationship between competence and patient outcome with
Funding CBT. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 68, 19–26. doi:10.
1016/j.brat.2015.03.002
This research was supported by the Massey Univer-
Brown, L., Craske, M., Glenn, D., Stein, M., Sullivan, G.,
sity School of Psychology, and partially funded by Sherbourne, C., … Rose, R. (2013). CBT competence in
Albany Strategic Research and Lottery Health novice therapists improves anxiety outcomes. Depression and
Research Council grants. We wish to thank Margo Anxiety, 30(2), 97–115. doi:10.1002/da.22027
Munro for her assistance with the research design Bucci, S., French, L., & Berry, K. (2016). Measures assessing the
quality of case conceptualization: A systematic review. Journal of
and grant applications.
Clinical Psychology, 72(6), 517–533. doi:10.1002/jclp.22280
Butler, A. C., Brown, G. K., Beck, A. T., & Grisham, J. R. (2002).
Assessment of dysfunctional beliefs in borderline personality
disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 1231–1240.
ORCID
doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00031-1
Michael H. Easden http://orcid.org/0000-0002- Castonguay, L., Barkham, M., Lutz, W., & McAleavey, A. (2013).
Practice-oriented research: Approaches and applications. In M.
2577-2352
J. Lambert (Ed.), Bergin and Garfield’s handbook of psychotherapy
and behaviour change (6th ed., pp. 85–133). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Chadwick, P., Williams, C., & Mackenzie, J. (2003). Impact of
case formulation in cognitive behaviour therapy for psychosis.
References Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 671–680. doi:10.1016/
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical S0005-7967(02)00033-5
manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev). Washington, DC: Cicchetti, D. V. (1994). Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb for
Author. evaluating normed and standardized assessment instruments in
Andrews, G., & Peters, L. (1998). The psychometric properties of psychology. Psychological Assessment, 6(4), 284–290. doi:10.
the composite international diagnostic interview. Social 1037/1040-3590.6.4.284
Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 33, 80–88. doi:10. Clark, D. M. (1986). A cognitive approach to panic. Behaviour
1007/s001270050026 Research and Therapy, 24, 461–470. doi:10.1016/0005-7967
Barber, J. P., Sharpless, B. A., Klostermann, S., & McCarthy, K. S. (86)90011-2
(2007). Assessing intervention competence and its relation to Crits-Christoph, P., Cooper, A., & Luborsky, L. (1988). The accu-
therapy outcome: A selected review derived from the outcome racy of therapists’ interpretations and the outcome of dynamic
literature. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 38(5), psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
493–500. doi:10.1037/0735-7028.38.5.493 56, 490–495. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.56.4.490
Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. Davidson, K., Scott, J., Schmidt, U., Tata, P., Thornton, S., &
New York: International Universities Press. Tyrer, P. (2004). Therapist competence and clinical outcome
Beck, J. (1995). Cognitive therapy: Basics and beyond. New York: in the prevention of parasuicide by manual assisted cognitive be-
Guilford Press. haviour therapy trial: The POPMACT study. Psychological
Beck, J. (2011). Cognitive therapy: Basics and beyond (2nd ed.). Medicine, 34, 855–863. doi:10.1017/S0033291703001855
New York: Guilford Press. Dozois, D. J. A., Covin, R., & Brinker, J. (2003). Normative data
Beck, A. T., & Beck, J. S. (1991). The personality belief questionnaire on cognitive measures of depression. Journal of Consulting and
(Unpublished manuscript). University of Pennsylvania. Clinical Psychology, 71(1), 71–80. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.71.
Beck, A. T., Butler, A. T., Brown, G. K., Dahlsgaard, K. K., 1.71
Newman, C. F., & Beck, J. S. (2001). Dysfunctional beliefs dis- Dozois, D. J. A., Dobson, K. S., & Ahnberg, J. L. (1998). A psy-
criminate personality disorders. Behaviour Research and chometric evaluation of the Beck depression inventory-II.
Therapy, 39, 1213–1225. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00099-1 Psychological Assessment, 10, 83–89. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.
Beck, A. T., & Freeman, A. (1990). Cognitive therapy of personality 10.2.83
disorders. New York: Guilford. Duncan, B. L., & Reese, R. J. (2015). The partners for change
Beck, A. T., Rush, A., Shaw, B., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive outcome management system (PCOMS) revisiting the client’s
therapy of depression. New York: The Guilford Press. frame of reference. Psychotherapy, 52(4), 391–401. doi:10.
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck depression 1037/pst0000026
inventory manual (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Easden, M. H., & Kazantzis, N. (2018). Case conceptualization
Corporation. research in cognitive behavior therapy: A state of the science
18 M. H. Easden and R. B. Fletcher
review. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 74(3), 356–384. doi:10. Kuyken, W., Fothergill, C., Musa, M., & Chadwick, P. (2005).
1002/jclp.22516 The reliability and quality of cognitive case formulation.
Eells, T. (2007). Handbook of psychotherapy case formulation (2nd Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1187–1201. doi:10.1016/
ed.). New York: Guilford Press. j.brat.2004.08.007
Eells, T., Kendjelic, E., & Lucas, C. (1998). What’s in a case for- Kuyken, W., Padesky, C., & Dudley, R. (2008). The science and
mulation? Development and use of a content coding manual. practice of case conceptualization. Behavioural and Cognitive
Journal of Psychotherapy Practice and Research, 7(2), 144–153. Psychotherapy, 36, 757–768. doi:10.1017/S1352465808004815
Eells, T., Lombart, K. G., Kendjelic, E. M., Turner, L., & Lucas, Kuyken, W., Padesky, C., & Dudley, R. (2009). Collaborative case
C. P. (2005). The quality of psychotherapy case formulations: A conceptualization: Working effectively with clients in cognitive-be-
comparison of expert, experienced, and novice cognitive-behav- havior therapy. New York: The Guilford Press.
ioral and psychodynamic therapists. Journal of Consulting and Kuyken, W., & Tsivrikos, D. (2009). Therapist competence, co-
Clinical Psychology, 73(4), 579–589. doi:10.1037/0022-006X. morbidity and cognitive-behavioral therapy for depression.
73.4.579 Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 78, 42–48. doi:10.1159/
Flitcroft, A., James, I. A., Freeston, M., & Wood-Mitchell, A. 000172619
(2007). Determining what is important in a good formulation. Kwok, O., Underhill, A., Berry, J., Luo, W., Elliot, T., & Yoon, M.
Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 35, 325–333. doi:10. (2008). Analyzing longitudinal data with multilevel models: An
1017/S135246580600350X example with individuals living with lower extremity intra-
Haarhoff, B., Gibson, K., & Flett, R. (2011). Improving the quality articular fractures. Rehabilitation Psychology, 53(3), 370–386.
of cognitive behaviour therapy case conceptualization: The role doi:10.1037/a0012765
of self-practice/self-reflection. Behavioural and Cognitive Lambert, M. J., & Shimokawa, K. (2011). Collecting client feed-
Psychotherapy, 39, 323–339. doi:10.1017/S1352465810000871 back. Psychotherapy, 48, 72–79. doi:10.1037/a0022238
Huisman, P., & Kangas, M. (2018). Evidence-based practices in Mausbach, B. T., Moore, R., Roesch, S., Cardenas, V., &
cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT) case formulation: What do Patterson, T. L. (2010). The relationship between homework
practitioners believe is important, and what do they do? compliance and therapy outcomes: An updated meta-analysis.
Behaviour Change, 35(1), 1–21. doi:10.1017/bec.2018.5 Cognitive Therapy and Research, 34(5), 429–438. doi:10.1007/
Kazantzis, N., Bjornholdt, A., Munro, M., Dobson, K., Merrick, s10608-010-9297-z
P., Fetcher, R., & Jones, D. (2006). Development of the homework McNeish, D., & Stapleton, L. (2016). The effect of small sample
rating scale: A measure of patients’ beliefs about homework in cogni- size on two-level model estimates: A review and illustration.
tive therapy. Poster presented at the 40th annual convention of Educational Psychology Review, 28(2), 295–314. doi:10.1007/
the association for behavioural and cognitive therapies s10648-014-9287-x
(ABCT), Chicago. Miller, S. D., Duncan, B. L., Sorrell, R., & Brown, G. S. (2005).
Kazantzis, N., Deane, F. P., & Ronan, K. R. (2000). Homework The partners for change outcome management system. Journal
assignments in cognitive and behavioral therapy: A meta-analy- of Clinical Psychology, 61(2), 199–208. doi:10.1002/jclp.20111
sis. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 7, 189–202. doi:10. Munro, M. D. (2006). Client and therapist variability with psy-
1093/clipsy.7.2.189 chotherapy homework: A preliminary psychometric evaluation of
Kazantzis, N., Deane, F. P., Ronan, K. R., & L’Abate, L. (Eds.). two scales (Unpublished master’s thesis). Albany: Massey
(2005). Using homework assignments in cognitive behavior University.
therapy. New York: Routledge. Needleman, L. (1999). Cognitive case conceptualization: A guidebook
Kazantzis, N., Wedge, P., & Dobson, K. (2005). Homework adher- for practitioners. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
ence and competence scale (HAACS). Nezu, A. M., & Nezu, C. M. (2005). Comments on “evidence-
Kazantzis, N., Whittington, C., & Dattilio, F. (2010). Meta-analysis based behavioral medicine: What is it and how do we achieve
of homework effects in cognitive and behavioral therapy: A repli- it?”: The interventionist does not always equal the interven-
cation and extension. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 17 tion—the role of therapist competence. Annals of Behavioral
(2), 144–156. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2850.2010.01204.x Medicine, 29(1), 80–80. doi:10.1207/s15324796abm2901_11
Kazantzis, N, Whittington, C, Zelencich, L, Kyrios, M, Norton, P, Okiishi, J., Lambert, M. J., Nielsen, S. L., & Ogles, B. M. (2003).
& Hofmann, S, G. (2016). Quantity and quality to homework Waiting for supershrink: An empirical analysis of therapist
compliance: A meta-analysis of relations with outcome in effects. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 10, 361–373.
Cognitive Behavior Therapy. Behavior Therapy, 47(5), 755– doi:10.1002/cpp.383
772. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2016.05.002 Østergård, O. K, Randa, H., & Hougaard, E. (2018). The effect of
Kessler, R., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Koretz, D., using the partners for change outcome management system as
Merikangas, K., … Wang, P. (2003). The epidemiology of feedback tool in psychotherapy: A systematic review and
major depressive disorder: Results from the national comorbid- meta-analysis. Psychotherapy Research. doi:10.1080/10503307.
ity survey replication (NCS-R). Journal of the American Medical 2018.1517949
Association, 289(23), 3095–3105. doi:10.1001/jama.289.23. Padesky, C. A., & Mooney, K. A. (1990). Presenting the cognitive
3095 model to clients. International Cognitive Therapy Newsletter, 6,
Kuyken, W. (2006). Evidence-based case formulation: Is the 13–14.
emperor clothed? In N. Tarrier (Ed.), Case formulation in cogni- Persons, J. (1989). Cognitive therapy in practice: A case formulation
tive behaviour therapy: The treatment of challenging and complex approach. New York: Norton.
clinical cases (pp. 12–35). London: Brunner-Routledge. Persons, J. (2001). Individualized formulation and treatment plan-
Kuyken, W, Beshai, S, Dudley, R, Abel, A, Görg, N, Gower, P … ning. In J. Persons, J. Davidson, & M. Tompkins (Eds.),
McManus, F. (2016). Assessing competence in collaborative Essential components of cognitive-behavior therapy for depression
case conceptualization: Development and preliminary (pp. 22–55). Washington, DC: American Psychological
psychometric properties of the Collaborative Case Association.
Conceptualization Rating Scale (CCC-RS). Behavioural and Persons, J. (2006). Case formulation-driven psychotherapy.
Cognitive Psychotherapy, 44(2), 179–192. doi:10.1017/ Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 13(2), 167–170.
S1352465814000691 doi:10.1111/j.1468-2850.2006.00019.x
Psychotherapy Research 19
Persons, J. (2008). A case formulation approach to cognitive-behavior Strunk, D. R., Brotman, M. A., DeRubeis, R. J., & Hollon, S. D.
therapy. New York: Guilford. (2010). Therapist competence in cognitive therapy for
Persons, J., Bostrom, A., & Bertagnolli, A. (1999). Results of ran- depression: Predicting subsequent symptom change. Journal of
domized controlled trials of cognitive therapy for depression Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78(3), 429–437. doi:10.
generalize to private practice. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 1016/j.brat.2010.03.011
23(5), 535–548. doi:10.1023/A:1018724505659 Trepka, C., Rees, A., Shapiro, D., Hardy, G., & Barkham, M.
Persons, J., Roberts, N., Zalecki, C., & Brechwald, W. (2006). (2004). Therapist competence and outcome of cognitive
Naturalistic outcome of case formulation-driven cognitive-be- therapy for depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 28(2),
havior therapy for anxious depressed outpatients. Behaviour 143–157. doi:10.1023/B:COTR.0000021536.39173.66
Research and Therapy, 44(7), 1041–1051. doi:10.1016/j.brat. Vallis, T. M, Shaw, B. F, & Dobson, K. S. (1986). The cognitive
2005.08.005 therapy scale: Psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting
Peugh, J. L., & Enders, C. K. (2005). Using the SPSS mixed pro- and Clinical Psychology, 54(3), 381–385. doi:10.1037/0022-
cedure to fit cross-sectional and longitudinal multilevel models. 006X.54.3.381
Educational and Psychological Measurement, 65(5), 717–741. Waller, G., & Turner, H. (2016). Therapist drift redux: Why well-
Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear meaning clinicians fail to deliver evidence-based therapy, and
models: Applications and data analysis methods (2nd ed.). how to get back on track. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 77,
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 129–137. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2015.12.005
Sachsenweger, M. A., Fletcher, R. B., & Clarke, D. (2015). Webb, C. A., DeRubeis, R. J., & Barber, J. P. (2010). Therapist
Pessimism and homework in CBT for depression. Journal of adherence/competence and treatment outcome: A meta-ana-
Clinical Psychology, 71(12), 1153–1172. doi:10.1002/jclp.22227 lytic review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78
Schotte, C. K. W., Maes, M., Cluydts, R., De Doncker, D., & (2), 200–211. doi:10.1037/a0018912
Cosyns, P. (1997). Construct validity of the Beck depression Wedge, P. (2005). The development of the homework adherence and
inventory in a depressive population. Journal of Affective competence scale (HAACS): A measure for assessing therapist adher-
Disorders, 46(2), 115–125. doi:10.1016/S0165-0327(97)00094-3 ence and competence in administering homework assignments within
Shaw, B., Olmsted, M., Dobson, K., Sotsky, S., Elkin, I., cognitive behaviour therapy (Unpublished master’s thesis). Albany:
Yamaguchi, J., … Imber, S. (1999). Therapist competence Massey University.
ratings in relation to clinical outcome in cognitive therapy of Weerasekera, P. (1996). Multiperspective case formulation: A step
depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67(6), towards treatment integration. Melabar, FL: Krieger Publishing.
837–846. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.67.6.837 World Health Organization. (1997). Composite international diag-
Singer, J. D., & Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analy- nostic interview (CIDI-auto), version 2.1. Geneva: Author.
sis. New York: Oxford University Press. Wynants, L., Boumeester, W., Moons, K. G., Moerbeek, M.,
Snijders, T., & Bosker, R. (2012). Multilevel analysis: An introduc- Timmerman, D., Van Huffel, S., … Vergouwe, Y. (2015). A
tion to basic and advanced multilevel modelling (2nd ed.). simulation study of sample size demonstrated the importance
London: Sage. of the number of events per variable to develop prediction
Steer, R. A., Ball, R., Ranieri, W. F., & Beck, A. T. (1999). models in clustered data. Journal of Epidemiology, 68(12),
Dimensions of the beck depression inventory-II in clinically 1406–1414. doi:10.1016/j.jclinepi.2015.02.002
depressed outpatients. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55(1), Young, J, & Beck, A. T. (1980). Cognitive therapy scale rating
117–128. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-4679(199901)55:1<117:: manual. Retrieved from www.beckinstitute.org.
AID-JCLP12>3.0.CO;2-A Zarafonitis-Müller, S., Kuhr, K., & Bechdolf, A. (2014). The
Strunk, D. R., Adler, A. D., & Hollars, S. N. (2013). Cognitive relationship between therapist’s competence and adherence to
therapy skills predict cognitive reactivity to sad mood following outcome in cognitive-behavioural therapy: Results of a metaa-
cognitive therapy for depression. Cognitive Therapy and nalysis. Fortschritte der Neurologie-Psychiatrie, 82(9), 502–510.
Research, 37(6), 1214–1219. doi:10.1007/s10608-013-9570-z doi:10.1055/s-0034-1366798

You might also like