You are on page 1of 16

Psychotherapy Theory, Research, Practice, Training Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association

2008, Vol. 45, No. 4, 431– 446 0033-3204/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0014330

THE DEVELOPMENT OF THE CLIENT–THERAPIST BOND


THROUGH THE LENS OF ATTACHMENT THEORY

JOSEPH H. OBEGI
University of California, San Diego
The bond between client and therapist, Keywords: attachment behavior, client
a component of the global alliance, is attachment to therapist, therapists, ther-
widely believed to play a crucial role in apeutic alliance, therapeutic bond
supporting the work of therapy. How-
ever, we know little about how the It is a well-accepted idea that the emotional
client–therapist bond becomes estab- bond between client and therapist is the bedrock
lished and have few theoretical tools to of therapeutic alliances. A strong bond facilitates
conceptualize its development. Attach- smooth collaboration, buffers the relationship
from the strain of therapeutic work, and is con-
ment theory, with its focus on the de- sidered a healing element of psychotherapy. De-
velopment and dynamics of intimate spite its central importance, we know very little
relationships, is a lens through which about how the client–therapist bond develops
we can expand our understanding of (Castonguay, Constantino, & Holtforth Grosse,
2006). This poses a problem for experienced cli-
the client–therapist bond. I argue that
nicians and trainees alike: Being unable to easily
the therapeutic bond may be usefully ascertain the maturity of the bond compromises
viewed as an in-progress attachment to the ability to gauge whether rapport building
therapists. Using Bowlby (1969/1982) skills have been effective or to comprehend the
and Ainsworth’s (Ainsworth et al., changes in how clients behave with us over time.
An additional challenge for clinicians is to pre-
1978) ideas about normative attach- dict how personality differences impinge on the
ment development, I present a phase initiation and maintenance of the therapeutic re-
model of attachment to the therapist lationship. Attachment theory, with its focus on
and include behavioral, cognitive, emo- the nature and unfolding of relationships across
tional, and physiological markers of the life span, may be able to redress these issues.
Bowlby and Ainsworth, the originators of at-
each phase. I draw empirical support tachment theory, argued that all people, be they
from the psychotherapy process and infants or elders, seek to establish an affective tie,
alliance literatures and discuss re- or attachment, with a specific other to meet needs
search considerations and clinical im- for physical and psychological security (Ain-
sworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978; Bowlby,
plications of the model. 1988). Attachment and the therapeutic bond are
analogous constructs: both describe social-
emotional connections. However, the character-
istics, motivational basis, and development of
attachment offer several theoretical advantages
Joseph H. Obegi, Department of Psychiatry, University of over existing theories of the therapeutic bond. I
California, San Diego. propose that it is advantageous to view the ther-
Joseph H. Obegi is in private practice in Davis, California.
apeutic bond as an in-progress attachment to the
I thank Barry A. Farber for encouraging me to tackle this
topic and Patricia H. Judd, Brent Mallinckrodt, and Jonathan
therapist. To make this case, I first clarify the
Gale for helpful comments on early versions of this paper. differences between bond and attachment in
Correspondence regarding this article should be ad- terms of their definition and theoretical ideas
dressed to Joseph H. Obegi, 228 B Street, Davis, CA about development and draw attention to the
95616. E-mail: jhobegi@gmail.com strengths of an attachment-based view. Next, I

431
Obegi

propose, using Bowlby and Ainsworth’s devel- Descriptors such as caring, acceptance, confi-
opmental framework, a phase-based model of dence, mutual understanding, and mutual respect
attachment to the therapist. I also consider how a are also used to describe the bond component.
sense of security, or lack thereof, might color the Some alliance models define the bond component
development of attachment to the therapist. Fi- in comparable ways (e.g., Gaston, 1990; Gelso &
nally, I discuss research and clinical implications Carter, 1985) whereas others distinguish between
of viewing the client–therapist bond as an in- aspects of the bond that refer to personal rapport
progress attachment. and those that refer to collaborative effort
(Hatcher & Barends, 2006; Orlinsky, Rønnestad,
A Comparison of the Client–Therapist Bond & Willutzki, 2004). Regardless of these varia-
and Attachment tions in definition, there is a consensus that bonds
play a central role in treatment: Without strong
Bordin’s (1979) pan-theoretical formulation of bonds little collaborative work can be sustained
the alliance is the most widely held view of the and therapeutic progress will be stalled.
therapeutic relationship. In it, the client–therapist
Among attachment-informed clinicians and re-
bond is considered a component of the alliance.
searchers there is a long history of viewing the
According to Bordin, the alliance refers to the
relationship clients have with their therapist as an
“here and now” relationship between client and
attachment. Bowlby (1975, 1988), a psychoana-
therapist (in psychoanalytic terms, the “real” re-
lationship) and consists of goals, tasks, and lyst and researcher, believed that therapists
bonds. Goals referred to the extent to which the should assume the role of temporary attachment
client and therapist agreed on aims of treatment, figure and act as a secure base from which clients
whereas tasks refer to how relevant and potent could confidently explore painful issues. More
the client and therapist perceive the means of forcefully, some, like Ainsworth (1989), have
change to be. However, to enable and maintain argued that clients can and do form an attachment
the work of treatment, a high-quality bond be- to their therapist (e.g., Amini, Lewis, Lannon, &
tween client and therapist is essential. Bond refers Louie, 1996; Farber, Lippert, & Nevas, 1995;
to the socioemotional aspects of the alliance, Mallinckrodt, Gantt, & Coble, 1995) or that alli-
namely, the degree of trust and liking between ances are, in fact, attachments (Holmes, 1996,
client and therapist: 2001).
One appeal of attachment theory is that it ac-
Partner compatibility (bonding) is likely to be expressed and
felt in terms of liking, trusting, respect for each other, and a
counts for behaviors indicative of affectional ties
sense of common commitment and shared understanding in but that are omitted from Bordin’s (1979) view of
the activity. (Bordin, 1994, p. 16) bonds. Anecdotally speaking, clients’ behavior

TABLE 1. Similarities Between Attachment and the Therapeutic Relationship

Attachment Therapeutic relationship

Persists over time and space Clients continue to value the relationship beyond termination and may
turn to representations of the therapist for comfort and support
A tie with specific and noninterchangeable person Clients prefer ongoing consultation with a single therapist and become
reluctant to depend on substitute therapists
Desire to maintain contact with attachment figure
as evidenced by proximity seeking Clients attend sessions regularly, disclose problems, seek advice
Distress felt on separation; grief follows loss Increased anxiety when therapist is out of reach; sense of sadness, loss
upon termination; sense of loss, grief when therapist dies
Joy upon reunion Clients feels pleasure and relief on seeing the therapist either from
week to week or after a short break
Turning to attachment figures for relief from Clients seek out and consult with therapist about distressing problems
distress (safe haven) in hope of relief
Sense of security achieved with contact and Clients feel more confident when therapists are perceived as available,
exploration ensues (secure base) helpful, and empathic; clients use safety of sessions to explore
painful feelings, events, and alternative ways of feeling and acting
Note. Some of the similarities between attachments and the therapeutic relationship that appear in this Table were
drawn from Farber, Lippert, & Nevas (1995).

432
Client–Therapist Bond and Attachment

closely parallels characteristics of attachment de- ical and psychological safety (Sroufe & Waters,
scribed by Ainsworth (1989; see Table 1 for a 1977; for a recent elaboration of the attachment
summary). First, many clients appear to form an system, see Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). Over
enduring tie to their therapist, that is, they con- the course of many interactions, mental represen-
tinue to value the relationship well after termina- tations, called internal working models, of
tion. In contrast to the long-standing nature of the self and the attachment figure are constructed
attachment, the definition of bond implies that and used to inform encounters with relational
ties last only as long as treatment does. Second, partners.
clients tend to perceive their therapist as unique The implication of the above ideas is that cli-
and noninterchangeable persons (i.e., attachment ents seek therapists because doing so is expected
figures). This figure preference signifies the af- to alleviate distress. Clients rely on the same
finity for and emotional significance of the ther- psychological apparatus to initiate and maintain
apist. Although definitions of bonds make it clear proximity as they do in other attachments (e.g., to
that a sense of liking occurs, figure preference is romantic partners, parents), and they reuse previ-
not specified. Figure preference is evident in the ously constructed mental models as a guide. Bor-
desire to maintain contact with the therapist and din (1980) also believed that clients sought ther-
in the distress, even grief, felt on separation, the apists when in “a state of personal crisis” (p. 63)
third and fourth attributes of attachment. Separa- but had little to say about why clients might do so
tion distress is omitted from definitions of the and what psychological systems mediated in-
alliance’s bond component. Finally, like attached session behavior.
persons, clients purposefully seek comfort and A third potential benefit of applying attach-
security from their preferred figure (therapists) ment theory to the therapeutic bond is that the
that, once gained, reduces stress and engenders theory distinguishes between affectional ties (at-
the confidence to engage in challenging pursuits. tachment) and a sense of safety in them (attach-
Again, bond definitions imply but make no men- ment security). Unlike client–therapist bonds, at-
tion of this attribute. Although there is face va- tachments are not conceived as differing in
lidity to these comparisons, clinical researchers strength (i.e., a strong vs. weak attachment). At-
have only just begun to investigate the possibility tachment theorists have argued that understand-
that clients can become attached to therapists. In ing attachment in a quantitative fashion has little
two questionnaire-based studies that directly ex- use at best and is misleading at worst1 (e.g.,
amined this possibility, some clients did identify Ainsworth, 1972; Sroufe & Waters, 1977).
their therapists as attachment figures (Allen et al., Rather, the quality and patterning of attachment
2001; Parish & Eagle, 2003). behavior is more informative than the presence,
Another appeal of attachment theory is that it intensity, or frequency of any one behavior. Indi-
sheds light on why clients are motivated to seek viduals are held to differ in attachment security, that
out and maintain contact with therapists and de- is, “a sense that one can rely on close relationship
scribes the psychological systems that could sup- partners for protection and support, can safely and
port these efforts. The theory was designed to effectively explore the environment, and can en-
explain the universal disposition of human beings gage effectively with other people” (Mikulincer
to form especially close relationships with a se- & Shaver, 2004, p. 159). In both infancy and
lect few persons (Bowlby, 1975). According to adulthood, researchers have amply demonstrated
Bowlby (1969/1982) and Ainsworth (1972; Ain- that attachment security predicts individual dif-
sworth et al., 1978), an attachment enhances the
chances of survival because it keeps the organism
1
in close proximity to a “stronger and/or wiser” For example, it is an error to infer a “strong” attachment
figure (Bowlby, 1975; p. 292). An organized sys- from the intensity or frequency of any one attachment behav-
tem (the attachment behavioral system) evolved ior. The extent and kind of attachment behavior depends on
the level of threat to felt security. Serious threats will strongly
for the purpose of regulating proximity to a care-
activate the attachment system and, consequently, the most
giver in response to internal (psychological) or potent attachment behaviors will be deployed (e.g., panic-
external (environmental) threats to safety. In stricken cries, desperate clinging). Mild threats will weakly
times of threat, attachment behaviors are de- activate the attachment system and so evoke less potent be-
ployed to increase proximity to a specific figure haviors (e.g., calling, increasing proximity). In either case, the
(an attachment figure), thereby, promoting phys- attachment is unchanged.

433
Obegi

ferences in relationship functioning, affect regu- that clients can form an attachment to therapists.
lation, and psychopathology (Cassidy & Shaver, As well, the perspective of attachment theory
1999). offers several theoretical and practical benefits.
Attachment, once formed, is a stable feature of Explicit in attachment theory are concepts such
a relationship, but the sense of security in that as psychological safety, behavioral systems,
relationship is susceptible to shifts in response to mental representations, and individual differ-
changing interpersonal circumstances or life ences in security that go beyond the definition
stressors (for a review of attachment and change, of the client–therapist bond in alliance
see Davila & Cobb, 2004). Optimally, over the theorizing.
course of repeated interactions with an attach-
ment figure, an inner sense of confidence in the Contrasting Views of Development
protective abilities of the attachment figure is
encoded and enables exploration, a dynamic Despite several decades of research on the
commonly referred to as using the attachment therapeutic relationship, precisely how the client–
figure as a secure base (Ainsworth, 1967). The therapist bond develops remains obscure. (For a
emotional quality and history of interactions with profession predicated on relationships, this is a
the specific figure around whom the system has conspicuous problem.) By bond development I
become organized lends a distinctive quality to mean the processes or stages through which the
attachments and their corresponding mental rep- acquaintance between client and therapist be-
resentations. Attachments that balance closeness comes increasingly complex and mature over
and autonomy and include positive expectations time such that an affective tie (a bond) is estab-
about availability and sensitive responding are lished. Although Bordin (1979) believed that
labeled secure. Attachments in which people “deeper bonds of trust and attachment are re-
chronically lack confidence in the responsiveness quired and developed” (p. 254) in advanced
of attachment figures and constantly pursue stages of therapy, to my knowledge there are no
closeness and reassurance are called anxious, theories that specifically address how bonds un-
whereas attachments characterized by an overem- fold. In addition, because few studies have fo-
phasis on self-reliance and discomfort with emo- cused on the bond component current ideas about
tional or physical closeness are called avoidant. bond development must be inferred from ap-
Anxious and avoidant attachments are called col- proaches to the development of the global alli-
lectively insecure. ance. I briefly discuss some of these approaches
There is an important benefit of viewing the and contrast them with attachment theory’s
emotional connection between clients and thera- model of attachment development.
pists as attachments that vary in attachment se- There are four sources of knowledge regarding
curity rather than as bonds that vary in strength. alliance development. The first is Luborsky’s
First, an attachment-based view of the therapeutic (1976) theoretical distinction between a Type I
relationship posits that attachment, once achieved alliance (experiencing the therapist as supportive)
by clients, is a stable feature, whereas the degree and a Type II alliance (feelings of joint invest-
of attachment security clients feel will fluctuate ment in and responsibility for treatment). Unfor-
because it is a function of their attachment his- tunately, the model has been seldom researched,
tory, the actual behavior of the therapist, and underspecifies how one type evolves into the
current interpersonally relevant events outside of other, and does not clearly integrate Bordin’s
treatment. Therefore, attachment to the therapist (1979) bond component. A second source of data
is durable, even if it is, overall, secure or inse- is investigations into the proposition that alliance
cure, and the level of security felt can fluctuate strength fluctuates according to the phase of treat-
over the course of a single session or multiple ment (e.g., reaching its nadir in the working
ones. In contrast, the client–therapist bond de- phase) or with the working through of inevitable
scribed by Bordin (1979) is unidimensional. For strains and interruptions (e.g., Bordin, 1994; Hor-
example, if trust diminishes after a particularly vath & Luborsky, 1993). The early findings are
difficult exchange or session, the bond is consid- somewhat contradictory (cf., Kivlighan &
ered to have weakened, if not jeopardized (e.g., Shaughnessy, 2000; Stiles et al., 2004), though it
Horvath & Luborsky, 1993). can be safely said that alliance strength rarely
In summary, it seems reasonable to suspect remains unchanged as therapy proceeds (Bache-

434
Client–Therapist Bond and Attachment

lor & Salamé, 2000). The third source of data is treated as a sense of personal rapport and collab-
only peripherally about developmental processes; oration that increases over time, the development
numerous studies have documented factors that of attachment is conceived as progressing
are associated with alliance strength or weakness through overlapping but qualitatively distinct
(e.g., client characteristics, therapist contribu- phases, both in infancy and adulthood (Hazan,
tions; for a review, see Horvath & Bedi, 2002). Gur-Yaish, & Campa, 2004). Bowlby (1969/
The final source of knowledge is the robust find- 1982) and Ainsworth (Ainsworth et al., 1978)
ing that early measurements of alliance strength, stated that attachment unfolds gradually in a se-
say between the third and fifth session, are better ries of phases. In the first phase, or preattachment
predictors of therapeutic outcome than later mea- (0 to 2 months), infants do not discriminate be-
surements (e.g., Martin, Garske, & Davis, 2000). tween one caregiver and another; while their dis-
Some have taken this to mean that the alliance, tress provokes attachment behavior, the behavior
including the bond component, is more-or-less is not focused on any one person and infants
formed in three to five sessions. However, a cor- readily accept comfort from whoever offers it.
relation between alliance strength and outcome is Given the consistent availability of one or a small
insufficient to draw firm conclusions about the number of caregivers, infants are able to move
developmental course of alliances. into the attachment-in-the-making phase (2 to 6
Taken together, these approaches and findings months). Attachment behaviors, primarily signal-
suggest that the client–therapist bond forms early, ing behaviors such as crying, are directed to a
its strength is crucial to positive therapeutic out- preferred figure and this figure is able to soothe
come, and the bond may or may not remain the infant more easily than can other adults. The
uniformly positive or stable over time. However, phase of clear-cut attachment (beginning at 6 to 7
questions remain: How does the first meeting months) marks a new level of behavioral organi-
between client and therapist mature into an inti- zation. The infant takes a more independent role
mate and valued relationship? What behavioral in achieving proximity (via locomotion) and uses
indicators suggest a bond is forming or has the attachment figure as both a haven of safety in
formed? As Horvath (2005) noted: “Progressive times of danger and as a secure base from which
enrichment and complexity is a characteristic of to confidently explore the immediate environ-
all intimate relationships over time, so why ment. Stranger wariness and separation protest
should we assume that this is not the case in become prominent. As well, the infant increas-
therapy?” (p. 5). Theories of bonds and alli- ingly develops a sense of how his attachment
ances are relatively mute on these questions. figure typically responds to his attachment needs.
They also do not deal with the thorny problem In the final phase of the goal-corrected partner-
of explaining how a bond forms in 3 to 5 ship (around 36 months), the child becomes less
nonconsecutive hours of face-to-face contact egocentric and more able to take into account the
or, by one account (Sexton, Littauer, Sexton, & feelings and motives of his attachment figure in
Tommeras, 2005), a single hour. I am hard negotiating his attachment needs. Bowlby sus-
pressed to find evidence of any human relation- pected these same phases described attachment to
ship that develops this way. figures later in life (Ainsworth et al., 1978). In-
The assumptions of bond and attachment are deed, Hazan et al. (2004) successfully applied
different. First, compared to the client–therapist this normative model to adult romantic relation-
bond, which is believed to take root in several ships. In addition, they specified behavioral, cog-
weeks, an attachment forms over months and nitive, physiological, and emotional “attachment
years. Both Bowlby (1969/1982) and Ainsworth markers” for each phase.
(Ainsworth et al., 1978) posited that a clear-cut Thus, a potential advantage of attachment theory
attachment in infancy appears no earlier than 6 is that it can fill a conspicuous gap in theorizing
months of age. Among adults, research findings about the bond; attachment theory proposes a de-
suggest that the majority of romantic relation- velopmental template for understanding how dura-
ships are likely to have all characteristics of at- ble, intimate, and enduring affectional ties come
tachment when relationships endure 2 or more into being and suggests that a set of behavioral
years (Fraley & Davis, 1997; Hazan & Zeifman, signposts mark its developmental progress. As-
1994). suming that clients can form an attachment to
Second, whereas bond development is often therapists and that the attachment unfolds accord-

435
Obegi

ing to Bowlby and Ainsworth’s stages, what Can this person help me lower my distress? Will
might this look like in behavioral terms? In the I be able to work well with this person? Do we
next section, I offer some speculative answers. share similar ideas about what is wrong and what
will help? How available and dependable might
this person be? Searching for answers, clients
The Development of Attachment scrutinize their therapist’s tone of voice, com-
to the Therapist ments, questions, and appearance (Rappoport,
1997). Unless clients collect at least tentatively
Using the framework specified by Bowlby,
affirmative answers, the development of attach-
Ainsworth, and Hazan and colleagues, I outline
ment will be delayed if not aborted. Qualities of
phases of attachment to the therapist. I focus on
the therapist such as warmth, an attitude of non-
normative development, that is, phases and phase
judgment, an air of competence, and a willing-
markers that are relatively independent of indi-
ness to (initially) accommodate clients’ interper-
vidual differences in attachment security.2 Be-
sonal style are critically important.
cause investigations of attachment to the therapist
Physiologically and emotionally, arousal and
is scarce, I draw on the broader literature of
relatively high levels of distress mark the begin-
psychotherapy to support my inferences. Follow-
ing the presentation, I discuss how attachment ning of most therapy sessions in this phase. Inti-
insecurity may alter the model. mate disclosure to a stranger results in some
autonomic arousal (increased heart rate, perspi-
ration), emotions associated with the presenting
Phase 1: Preattachment problems are dominant, and some anxiety about
rejection or shame is present. At this point, the
The agenda of clients in this phase is twofold: presence and ministrations of the therapist have
to seek proximity to a stronger, wiser figure in the only a modest effect on their client’s emotional
hopes of relieving distress caused by current and physiological regulation. Ideally, however,
problems and, unlike in infancy, to assess clients should experience some relief at the end
whether the therapist is a viable attachment fig- of early sessions, an indication to him or her of
ure. Behaviorally, accomplishing this agenda in- the therapist’s viability as an attachment figure.
cludes searching for a therapist, making initial Evidence. Turning toward figures believed to
contact, and surface problem disclosure. Clients be capable of providing support in times of dis-
may ask direct questions about the therapist’s tress, be they family members, friends, or profes-
competence, approach, and personal values in sionals, appears to be a normative phenomenon.
order to judge the therapist’s potential as an at- Although few people who experience psycholog-
tachment figure. With interns or in clinics with ical problems seek help from a mental health
high therapist turnover, clients may inquire about professional (Howard, Cornille, Lyons, & Ves-
the therapist’s longevity. In the course of these sey, 1996), they are most inclined to do so when
early exchanges, mutual agreement on the goals their psychological distress is severe (Oliver,
and methods of treatment occurs. The achieve- Pearson, Coe, & Gunnell, 2005) and alternative
ment of consensus indicates clients’ willingness coping strategies have proven inadequate (Saun-
to further consider the therapist’s potential as an ders, 1993). More important, the first contact with
attachment figure. a therapist does not automatically make him or
Cognitively, clients are primarily relying on
established internal working models to under-
stand and assess the therapist’s behavior; percep- 2
To some readers, the model I describe is more a model of
tions and expectations of the therapist are secure attachment. There are two reasons why I believe “nor-
strongly influenced by experiences with previous mative” is the more appropriate label. First, I am not aware of
attachment figures. Nevertheless, early client– any research, in either infant or adult attachment literatures,
that suggests insecure attachment does not conform to the
therapist interactions are a kind of “groundbreak-
phases Bowlby (1969/1982) and Ainsworth (Ainsworth et al.,
ing” for the construction of a specific internal 1978) proposed. Second, I suspect that insecure attachment to
working model of the therapist, a key process of the therapist does follow the model but that it may exhibit a
which is covert assessments of interpersonal different timing (e.g., some phases may take more or less time
safety. Essentially, clients are “asking”: Is this a to complete) or distorted versions of phase markers. I address
safe person to disclose my private concerns to? this issue later in the paper.

436
Client–Therapist Bond and Attachment

her an attachment figure; clients are no more how safe it is to explore their frailties and failings
likely to engage in treatment when assigned to an by monitoring the extent to which the therapist is
intake therapist than to a subsequently assigned accepting, encouraging, and supportive in re-
therapist (Noel & Howard, 1989). sponse to their disclosures. In other words, clients
Research suggests that clients evaluate, early engage in more overt actions (e.g., more detailed
on, the ability of therapists to act as an attachment disclosures) than in the previous phase in an
figure (i.e., as a person who regulates rather than effort to assess the therapist’s ability to provide
contributes to their distress). In a large sample of comfort, regulate affect, and facilitate explora-
undergraduates, Vogel, Wade, and Haake (2006) tion. Assuming positive assessments of the ther-
found that students do not seek psychotherapy for apist’s behavior, clients give fuller descriptions
fear that therapists might make them feel inade- and divulge details or issues previously felt to be
quate or incompetent (i.e., increase their distress). too shameful or painful to verbalize. Clients also
Clients also enter treatment with various expec- become more responsive to their therapist’s en-
tations, including the expectation that the thera- couragement to explore prompting events, origins
pist will be of assistance and that interactions will of presenting problems, or uncomfortable feel-
be at least somewhat comfortable (Joyce & Piper, ings. Similarly, clients turn to their therapist for
1998). Some express skepticism in the therapy reassurance that their psychological state or situ-
process or inquire about the therapist’s qualifica- ation is manageable and “normal.” They appeal
tions whereas others monitor the therapist’s be- to the therapist’s expertise with direct or indirect
havior for signs of safety (Clarke, Rees, & Hardy, requests for assistance (e.g., advice, information)
2004). The latter has been long observed by psy- or comfort (e.g., empathic understanding, nor-
choanalysts when clients first attempt free asso- malizing, reframing), although they are unlikely
ciation (Greenson, 1967). When clients perceive to readily accept either. Clients in this phase are
therapists as trustworthy and as having expertise
increasingly likely to share accomplishments or
during an initial intake, they are less likely to
experiences of joy with the expectation that the
prematurely terminate treatment (Kokotovic &
therapist will receive them with delight and pro-
Tracey, 1987; Tryon, 1989). Relatedly, Saunders
vide affirmation.
(1999) found that clients, as of the third session,
With each interpersonal cycle of exploration
reported more hope and relief when they per-
ceived their therapists as self-assured and easy to and comfort seeking, clients build a more com-
be close to, whereas they reported more distress plex internal working model of their therapist and
when their therapists appeared emotionally un- the therapeutic relationship. They develop certain
available. In a qualitative review of the engage- beliefs and expectations about this particular
ment literature, Tryon and Winograd (2002) con- therapist’s emotional availability and behavioral
cluded that therapists who are better able to responsiveness. As well, they come to know
respond to clients’ concerns, relay meaningful which of their strategies optimally engages the
diagnostic information, and create a collaborative therapist and how they typically feel during in-
atmosphere were more likely to retain clients teractions with him or her. Their database of
beyond the first session. experience with the therapist allows them to pre-
dict, with increasing accuracy, the therapist’s be-
havior toward them and their resulting feelings.
Phase 2: Attachment-in-the-Making Moreover, the accessibility of the model strength-
At the end of the first phase, clients have ens as treatment continues and begins to have a
identified the therapist as a potential attachment small but reliable impact on clients’ subjective
figure. By deciding to regularly attend sessions well-being and self-worth. For instance, clients
(i.e., maintain proximity), clients have opened the occasionally evoke the model of their therapist
door to developing an attachment to the therapist. outside of session in response to conflict or neg-
Markers of this phase are characterized by tenta- ative emotional states in hopes of reinstituting the
tive but increasingly bold experiments in using comfort previously felt in the therapist’s pres-
the therapist as a safe haven for comfort and ence. The model also begins to compete with the
reassurance and, given positive results, as a se- chronic accessibility of clients’ more solidified
cure base for exploring intimate issues. models of themselves in relation to previous at-
Regarding behavior, clients carefully gauge tachment figures.

437
Obegi

Physiologically, the presence of the therapist Phase 3: Clear-Cut Attachment


evokes negative arousal less often than in the
previous phase. Instead, arousal is triggered pri- Near the end of Phase 2, clients have a good
marily by struggles with ongoing life problems. sense of the therapist’s responsiveness, availabil-
Contact with the therapist often leads to soothing ity, and sensitivity during times of distress. They
and is regularly pleasurable; the therapist fre- perceive benefits to seeking support and have a
quently has a positive effect on the clients’ emo- subjective perception of being supported. More and
tional regulation. As this phase draws to a close, more they view the therapist as their unique and
irreplaceable consultant. In infancy, the markers of
clients are inclined to feel trusting, accepted, un-
clear-cut attachment include figure preference, sep-
derstood, and comfortable rely on the therapist.
aration protest, stranger wariness, and attachment
Evidence. What clients say and do not say in
behavior that are organized around a preferred
treatment supports the current characterization of
figure (safe haven, secure base behavior). Though
a client’s behavior in the attachment-in-the-
less obvious, clients may exhibit similar markers.
making phase. Using qualitatively coded inter-
Behaviorally, clients in this phase are more
views conducted well after the beginning of treat-
likely to have perceptible reactions to separations
ment, Hill, Thompson, Cogar, and Denman
initiated by their therapist (e.g., vacations, thera-
(1993) found that clients in long-term therapy pist illness). They may protest (e.g., express an-
continue to monitor when it is sufficiently safe or ger or dread), disclose that the separation was
comfortable to share certain types of information stressful, or express relief when sessions resume.
(e.g., personal secrets, negative reactions to Moreover, they may report considerable reluc-
interventions). However, over time, clients ap- tance relying on the “on-call” therapist during the
pear to disclose more as they feel more confi- intervening separation. In general, clients’ attach-
dent in the security of the therapeutic relation- ment behavior becomes organized around the
ship. For example, Farber and Hall (2002; Hall person of the therapist and clients’ are more re-
& Farber, 2001) found that the duration of sponsive to the therapist’s interventions. Clients
therapy and alliance strength both positively explicitly turn to the therapist in between sessions
predicted disclosures. during times of crisis, report a desire for consul-
Several studies suggested that clients gradually tation between sessions, actively participate in
build and rely on an internal working model of making the session agenda (e.g., bringing ques-
their therapist. Early in treatment clients appeared tions), or recall previous conversations they
to have limited access to the model, invoke them found particularly influential (“I remember you
less often, and report only modest levels of com- said . . .”). Other less certain indications of figure
fort after doing so (Knox, Goldberg, Woodhouse, preference are inquires about the therapist’s per-
& Hill, 1999; Rosenzweig, Farber, & Geller, sonal interests and well-being or expressions of
1996). Nevertheless, early models can exert some gratitude either verbally or through gift giving.
effect on clients’ self-perceptions. Shortly after By this phase, clients have a reasonably com-
treatment begins (approximately eight or nine plete internal working model of the therapist that
sessions) clients become more self-accepting and they turn to during times of emotional or inter-
self-loving when they have internalized their personal strain. By “complete” I mean that the
therapists as understanding and nurturing (e.g., model has the all components of internal working
Quintana & Meara, 1990). models, namely, a rich network of memories of
Research also suggests that the emotional cli- experiences with the therapist, beliefs and expec-
mate of the therapeutic relationship becomes tations about the self and the therapist, and strat-
more positive overall as treatment progresses. egies for fulfilling attachment needs (e.g., for
Clients report more trust, reliance, and “joyful comfort, security; Collins, Guichard, Ford, &
connection” as treatment progresses (Quintana & Feeney, 2004). Evoking this model during times
Meara, 1990) as well as feelings of safety, com- of need is a cognitive form of proximity seeking
fort, and acceptance (Rosenzweig et al., 1996; that may manifest itself in various ways (e.g.,
Stiles et al., 1994). Based on these findings, it is recollection of in-session conversations or sensa-
tempting to speculate that anxiety-based physio- tions, evoking the memories or psychological
logical arousal caused by the presence of the presence of the therapist) and, more often than
therapist diminishes toward the end of this phase. not, provides either some measure of relief (i.e.,

438
Client–Therapist Bond and Attachment

security) or facilitates independent exploration of Not only were levels of these components posi-
and coping with the issue at hand. Given the tively correlated with both the duration and fre-
therapist’s status as an attachment figure, a severe quency of therapy, they were generally compara-
therapeutic rupture during this phase is likely to ble to the levels clients reported regarding their
be perceived by clients as an “attachment injury” primary attachment figure.
(Johnson, Makinen, & Millikin, 2001), that is, a With continued treatment, clients appear to
sense of abandonment or betrayal that occurs have greater access to the internal working model
when an attachment figure is unavailable during a of their therapist and this access impacts both
time of intense need or responds in a grossly their behavior under stressful circumstances and
invalidating manner. This injury can radically the extent to which self-representations are re-
undermine confidence in the availability and sen- vised. Clients deliberately invoke the model of
sitivity of the attachment figure (i.e., substantially their therapist during challenging situations or
“rewrites” the mental model of the other in neg- experiences of painful affect (e.g., Geller & Far-
ative terms) and, left unaddressed, may cause ber, 1993; Knox et al., 1999). As treatment
irreparable damage to the attachment and result lengthens, clients’ self-representation becomes
in premature termination. more accepting and less critical, apparently re-
The primary physiological and emotional flecting the internalization of a positive therapeu-
markers in this phase are characterized by regu- tic relationship (Arnold, Farber, & Geller, 2000).
lation that is a result of contact with the therapist. Thus, the cognitive impact of the therapeutic
The presence of the therapist, rather than provok- relationship is consistent with predictions of
ing arousal as in the first phase of attachment, attachment theory: Persons seek proximity
regularly leads to soothing. Clients find contact in when threatened in order to achieve a sense of
this phase pleasurable and feel trusting, accepted, security and progressively internalize the qual-
and understood. Comfort with the intimacy of the
ity of relationships.
sessions is higher than in previous phases and
Several findings suggest that the emotional im-
uninhibited, genuine displays of affect, like cry-
pact and intensity of the bond between client and
ing, warm greetings, or mutual laughter, are more
therapist deepens over time. For some clients,
the norm.
internalization of interactions with their therapist
Evidence. Although there is a dearth of em-
pirical research on therapist-initiated separations, appears to promote symptom reduction and im-
considerable anecdotal evidence documents that provement in overall functioning (Harrist, Quin-
clients’ reactions to breaks in treatment are sim- tana, Strupp, & Henry, 1994). Invoking memo-
ilar to, though perhaps less intense than, the re- ries of therapeutic interactions is also linked to
actions of children to separations. For example, improvements in clients’ subjective rating of
in a small survey conducted by Webb (1983) progress and gaining a sense of relief or comfort
clinicians reported that clients reacted most com- (Geller & Farber, 1993; Rosenzweig et al., 1996).
monly with anger and anxiety, feelings often Clients are more likely to report missing their
associated with fears of abandonment, and some therapist between sessions during the second year
reported that clients looked forward to resuming of treatment (Rosenzweig et al., 1996) and not
sessions (i.e., reunion). In residential settings, uncommonly desire to continue the therapeutic
patients may respond to separations with violence dialogue beyond the consulting room (Geller &
(Adshead, 1998) or their symptoms may destabi- Farber, 1993). These findings also support the
lize (Persaud & Meux, 1994). When a therapist view that the therapeutic relationship functions in
dies unexpectedly, clients in long-term therapy the service of emotional regulation. Accordingly,
may experience loss and mourning (Rendely, a similar degree of physiological regulation
1999; Schwartz & Silver, 1990) not unlike that of would also be expected but I am not aware of
bereaved spouses. More generally, clients report data that bear on this issue. Finally, Woodhouse,
that the relationship with their therapists contains Schlosser, Crook, Ligiero and Gelso (2003)
many components of attachment (Parish & Eagle, found a positive association between clients’
2003), including proximity seeking, feeling com- sense of security with their therapist and treat-
forted and supported, perceiving the attachment ment duration. They also found that security and
figure as wiser, viewing the therapist as irreplace- negative transference were positively correlated
able, and having strong feelings for the therapist. leading them to speculate that clients delved into

439
Obegi

more difficult issues when they felt a sense of ment and had therapies that were relatively long
security. in duration (over 70 sessions).
The durability inherent in the partnership
phase is most akin to the state of the alliance in
Phase 4: Goal-Corrected Partnership the working phase of therapy as described by
Greenson (1967). Greenson’s definition of the
Once a base of security is firmly established, alliance highlights the ability of the client to do
clients and therapists are free to begin a more egal- the work of therapy, that is, to move easily be-
itarian partnership. In this phase, there is consider- tween regression in service of the ego and jointly
ably less focus on issues of security between client analyzing the content that arises. The prerequisite
and therapist and less overt proximity seeking by for this flexibility is a therapeutic relationship
the client. Instead, attention is directed almost that is sufficiently reliable to encourage the cli-
exclusively toward presenting problems. Clients ent’s exploration of problematic experiences and
are able to imagine that outside influences and to buffer the client from the emotional strain of
events may realistically impinge on their thera- analytic work. Psychoanalytic writers agree that a
pist’s availability, and they either make the nec- firmly established working alliance is necessary
essary accommodations in their behavior or ne- to support the repetitive working through of re-
gotiate these obstacles with the therapist to sistances and transference issues (e.g., Fialkow &
achieve mutually agreeable solutions. Muslin, 1987).
The behavior of clients in this phase is char-
acterized by genuine consultation. Although the
The Role of Individual Differences in
therapist is still seen as someone with special
Attachment Security
expertise, clients confidently contribute their per-
spective on current problems as well as ideas Many clients will not conform closely to the
about treatment strategies. There is mutual curi- normative model I have proposed. As Bowlby
osity about present struggles and equal invest- (1984) noted (and as Bordin, 1979, recognized),
ment in addressing them. Both client and thera- people differ in their ability to “collaborate with
pist flexibly adapt to changes in schedules or that person [who provides a secure base] in such
interruptions of treatment. Not only is there less a way that a mutually rewarding relationship is
proximity seeking (other than attending sessions), initiated and maintained” (p. 104). Some clients,
but therapy-interfering transferences are rare. due to psychopathology or insecurity, may never
The internal working model of the therapist reach the final phase of attachment, may move
becomes further elaborated during this phase and through the phases at different speeds, or may
continues to influence clients’ habitual ways of exhibit distorted versions of attachment markers
thinking, feeling, and behaving both toward in each phase. A benefit of applying attachment
themselves and toward other important individu- theory to the therapeutic relationship is that, un-
als on whom they rely. The person of the thera- like Bordin’s formulation, the theory integrates
pist is a source of soothing and grounding rather normative processes and personality differences
than arousal, and interactions between client and (i.e., attachment security); it gives a frame of
therapist take on a “business as usual” flavor. reference to judge deviations from “normal” or
Clients’ attachment to their therapist still sup- “optimal” developmental processes. Because cli-
ports emotional regulation but this pattern is in- ents enter treatment with polished attachment
creasingly internalized. strategies that are the outcome of their own in-
Evidence. The empirical literature is terpersonal history with attachment figures, inse-
strangely silent on the characteristics of the curity should manifest itself in differences in
client–therapist relationship in long-term thera- in-session behavior and in the growth of attach-
pies. Characteristics of Luborsky’s (1976) Type ment to the therapist. I discuss this implication in
II alliance, discussed earlier, overlap with those terms of two broad types of insecurity: avoidant
in this phase, namely, a sense of teamwork and and anxious attachment.
the client’s ability to contribute his or her own The deactivating strategies of avoidant at-
ideas and reflections. Luborsky found that clients tached clients may retard the development of
who developed Type II alliances were more attachment. Because of their intense discomfort
likely to have improved over the course of treat- with dependence and intimacy, avoidant clients

440
Client–Therapist Bond and Attachment

will be prone to distancing and distrust in early dence to cope independently, they amplify their
therapeutic encounters and their typical defensive distress, engage in self-deprecation, and con-
maneuvers (e.g., suppressing thoughts and feel- vince themselves that they are closer or more
ings related to intimacy, self-inflation, devalua- similar to others than is true, all in an effort to
tion; Mikulincer, Shaver, Cassidy, & Berant, elicit compassion, support, and avoid the threat
2008) will give therapists the impression that of isolation (Mikulincer et al., 2008). Thus,
attachment development is absent. Thus, anxiously attached clients are motivated to ac-
avoidant clients may take longer to move through celerate attachment development, display am-
each phase of attachment and the expression of plified versions of attachment markers, and
attachment markers may be subtle or subdued possibly exhibit markers of later phases prema-
(e.g., they may appear less distressed than others turely. Several research findings point to these
in Phase 1, make fewer, less overt, or abortive possibilities. Using the AAI, Dozier and Lee
bids for reassurance, or their disclosures may (1995) found that attachment-related anxiety
involve less feeling in Phase 2). Research find- was associated with more self-reported symp-
ings point in this direction. In a study of under- tomatology. Dozier (1990) found that highly
graduates, avoidance seemed to impede the de- anxious patients sought more help, were more
velopment of attachment to peers (Fraley & disclosing, and utilized treatment more than
Davis, 1997). Using the Adult Attachment Inter- highly avoidant patients. Yet, more anxious
view (AAI, George, Kaplan, & Main, 1996), Do- clients, as assessed by self-report scales, have
zier (1990) found that, among patients who had difficulty forming strong alliances (Mallinck-
been in short-term residential treatment for psy- rodt, Coble, & Gantt, 1995; Satterfield & Ly-
chiatric problems, more avoidant patients tended ddon, 1995), the strength of their alliances vac-
to reject or avoid help, be less disclosing, and illate dramatically across treatment (Kanninen
underutilize treatment. In a psychiatric case man- et al., 2000), and they experience more thera-
agement program, more avoidant patients tended peutic ruptures (Eames & Roth, 2000) that are
to underreport symptoms (Dozier & Lee, 1995). likely due to unrealistic expectations of care.
Avoidant mothers receiving a home visiting in- When attachment security is assessed with the
tervention were less likely to be invested in treat- CATS, the subscale indicative of attachment-
ment and were unlikely to use crisis intervention related anxiety (Preoccupied-Merger) is posi-
or supportive therapy services (Korfmacher, tively associated with negative transference
Adam, Ogawa, & Egeland, 1997). In studies us- (Woodhouse et al., 2003).
ing self-report measures of adult attachment,
more avoidant clients were unlikely to view their Discussion
therapists as attachment figures (Parish & Eagle,
2003) and had difficulty forming strong alliances I have asserted that the emotional connection
(e.g., Kivlighan, Patton, & Foote, 1998; Satter- between client and therapist can be fruitfully
field & Lyddon, 1995, 1998). Being more viewed through the lens of attachment theory. In
avoidant was associated with a precipitous de- doing so, I have proposed the following: (a) the
cline in alliance strength at termination (Kanni- client–therapist bond is better conceived as an
nen, Salo, & Punamaki, 2000) and prematurely in-progress attachment to a therapist; (b) attach-
dropping out of a treatment for eating disorders ment to the therapist evolves in a series of se-
(Tasca et al., 2006). A similar picture appears quential but overlapping phases; (c) each phase
when attachment security is measured with re- contains unique markers that fall across psycho-
spect to the therapist using the Client Attachment logical domains; (d) over time, therapists pro-
to Therapist Scale (CATS; Mallinckrodt, Gantt, gressively meet the criteria of an attachment fig-
& Coble, 1995). Mallinckrodt and colleagues ure; (e) clients’ development of attachment to the
found that avoidance was associated with weaker therapist is influenced by attachment insecurity;
alliances (Mallinckrodt, Coble, & Gantt, 1995) (f) clients build an internal working model of
and less in-session exploration (Mallinckrodt, their therapist; and, finally, (g) the ability of cli-
Porter, & Kivlighan, 2005). ents to evoke this model during times of stress is
Anxiously attached clients will exhibit a dif- related to therapeutic change. Below, I discuss
ferent developmental pattern. Because they are research issues involved in investigating attach-
inclined to feel helpless and lack the confi- ment to therapists and clinical implications.

441
Obegi

Research Considerations A major challenge is how to empirically iden-


tify when a phase of attachment has been
Verifying a normative model of attachment reached. One approach is to measure components
development in therapy dyads necessarily in- of attachment and the time frame in which each
volves four challenges: documenting the exis- component is transferred to a potential attach-
tence, sequencing, and patterning of phases (i.e., ment figure. The approach has proven successful
to what extent phases overlap); empirically iden- in the area of romantic relationships using the
tifying phase markers; determining the time WHOTO scale (Hazan & Zeifman, 1994). An
frame for each phase; and reliably measuring analogous measure, the Components of Attach-
developmental progress. Regarding the task of ment Questionnaire (CAQ; Parish & Eagle,
documenting the phases, the literature on the 2003), shows promise as a scale for rating the
therapeutic relationship is only partially helpful extent to which a therapist fulfills the functions of
because studies in this area frequently target an attachment figure. Care must be taken in using
phases of treatment, which may or may not cor- current attachment assessments (e.g., the AAI or
respond to the developmental status of the ther- self-report scales) to measure attachment devel-
apeutic relationship, and no consensus exists on opment. These tools specifically assess the degree
either the phases themselves or how they should of attachment security and not the extent to which
be identified. In addition, attachment markers, one person is attached to another. They are best
especially the emotional and physiological ones, used to track changes in the security of clients
have not been studied in all time frames. Cer- and client–therapist dyads over time or to exam-
tainly, qualitative and longitudinal studies of at- ine how insecurity impedes attachment to the
tachment development will be essential. Once the therapist.
normative attachment process is better described,
variations that insecurity may impose should be
aggressively researched since most clients pre- Clinical Implications
senting for psychotherapy exhibit insecurity. For clinicians, there are several advantages of
Regarding the issue of time, variability in treat- recasting the bond component of the alliance in
ments, in addition to differences in attachment se- attachment terms. The first is the addition of a
curity, prevent generalizable estimates for each motivational framework to alliance theorizing.
phase. Treatments vary in their session frequency, Attachment theory provides a rationale for why
session duration, and length. Because interpersonal clients pursue treatment, why the person of the
contact is necessary for an attachment to form, therapist is such an integral part of successful
therapeutic dyads that have lengthier or more fre- treatment, why the quality of the relationship is
quent contact are expected to steadily advance important, and how therapeutic relationships
through the phases, whereas shorter therapies may might evolve normatively as well as idiosyncrat-
cap the growth of attachment. An open question is ically. Second, the proposed framework offers a
whether brief therapies that mobilize strong af- heuristic guide to clinicians. It includes a number
fects accelerate attachment development. of observable behavioral markers that can be
There are good reasons to suspect that attach- used to monitor the unfolding of the therapeutic
ment to therapists may not take as long to de- relationship. Deviations from the phases can be
velop as romantic attachments. First, the client is useful adjuncts to the assessment of attachment
usually in intense distress, which mobilizes the insecurity beyond self-report or interview mea-
attachment system. Second, the weekly format of sures. Third, identifying the current phase of at-
most psychotherapies guarantees regular and sus- tachment might also inform judgments about the
tained contact; absent is the haphazard contact timing and choice of therapeutic interventions,
characteristic of the early phase of an adult ro- for example, abstaining from interventions in
mantic relationship. Third, the social expectation Phases 1 or 2 that require the emotional durability
of disclosure of private and emotionally sensitive of a clear-cut attachment.
issues, a fertile test bed of trust and intimacy, Although I have not discussed the therapist’s
exists from the first session. Finally, because role in the client’s attachment process due to
therapists are trained to alleviate distress and space limitations, Bowlby (1988) certainly be-
establish relationships, they are near tailor-made lieved that the behavior of the therapist was a
attachment figures. critical ingredient to the therapeutic relationship:

442
Client–Therapist Bond and Attachment

Even so, a patient’s way of construing his relationship with becoming attached and that this may contribute to
his therapist is not determined solely by the patient’s history: therapeutic change. Of course, this idea is not
it is determined no less by the way the therapist treats him.
Thus the therapist must strive always to be aware of the nature original—it is present in many conceptualizations
of his own contribution to the relationship which, among of change, including Bowlby’s (1988). If attach-
other influences, is likely to reflect in one way or another what ment theory is to be applied more fully to adult
he experienced himself during his own childhood. (p. 141) psychotherapy, the process of therapeutic change
Indeed, there is ample evidence that therapists’ will need more attention from theorists. The ideas
behavior is an important variable in alliance of Mikulincer and Shaver (2004), among others
strength (e.g., Ackerman & Hilsenroth, 2003). (Fonagy, Gergely, Jurist, & Target, 2002; Wallin,
Behaviors that positively correlate with higher 2007), offer fertile ground for this work.
alliance scores (e.g., warmth, trustworthiness) are In summary, I have proposed a framework of
consistent with characteristics of a security- attachment to the therapist that offers an alterna-
enhancing attachment figure. Research also sug- tive and expanded view of the emotional connec-
gests that the therapist’s own security impacts the tion or bond between clients and therapists. I
therapeutic process, as does the match in security argued that it is advantageous and possibly more
between therapist and client (for a review, see accurate to view the therapeutic bond as an un-
Berant & Obegi, 2008). Thus, it seems likely that folding attachment, that the development of at-
the length of phases will turn on, in part, the tachment to the therapist conforms to Bowlby
ability of therapists to provide a security- and Ainsworth’s phases of attachment, and that
enhancing climate that is adapted to individual each phase should evince certain attachment
differences in attachment security. markers across a range of behavioral domains.
Although I reviewed evidence for the framework,
Attachment, Alliance, and Therapeutic Change considerable work will be necessary to establish
its validity and to document the variations that
Because I have focused on the bond compo- individual differences in attachment security may
nent of the alliance, I have not addressed how impose. Nevertheless, an attachment perspective
attachment theory can also be applied to the other on the therapeutic bond shows promise as a rich
aspects of alliance, namely, collaboration on source of testable hypotheses.
tasks and goals. In brief, an attachment-based
view suggests that collaboration is a function of
the client’s sense of security in the therapeutic References
relationship or, if a clear-cut attachment to the
ACKERMAN, S. J., & HILSENROTH, M. J. (2003). A review
therapist has formed, how securely attached the of therapist characteristics and techniques positively
client is. Security should enable clients to explore impacting the therapeutic alliance. Clinical Psychology
difficult issues, recruit necessary resources, and Review, 23, 1–33.
problem-solve effectively. In the nonclinical lit- ADSHEAD, G. (1998). Psychiatric staff as attachment fig-
ures: Understanding management problems in psychi-
erature, several studies have found that security is atric services in the light of attachment theory. British
related to effective conflict management with Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 64 – 69.
partners (for a review see Mikulincer & Shaver, AINSWORTH, M. D. S. (1967). Infancy in Uganda: Infant
2007). Thus, in attachment terms the alliance, care and the growth of love. Oxford, England: Johns
that is, the degree of collaboration on tasks and Hopkins Press.
AINSWORTH, M. D. S. (1972). Attachment and depen-
goals in the context of a mutual bond, is simply dency: A comparison. In J. L. Gewirtz (Ed.), Attach-
the degree of attachment security a client feels ment and dependency. Oxford, England: Winston.
within an in-progress attachment. Future theoriz- AINSWORTH, M. D. S. (1989). Attachments beyond in-
ing should elaborate this distinction. fancy. American Psychologist, 44, 709 –716.
AINSWORTH, M. S., BLEHAR, M. C., WATERS, E., &
Although strong alliances are related to thera- WALL, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychologi-
peutic change, alliance theory has little to say cal study of the strange situation. Oxford, England: Erl-
about how and why this occurs (Castonguay et baum.
al., 2006). By necessity, I have given limited ALLEN, J. G., HUNTOON, J., FULTZ, J., STEIN, H., FONAGY,
attention to how an attachment-based view con- P., & EVANS, R. B. (2001). A model for brief assess-
ment of attachment and its application to women in
ceptualizes therapeutic change. I suggested that inpatient treatment for trauma-related psychiatric dis-
opportunities to revise long-standing internal orders. Journal of Personality Assessment, 76, 421– 447.
working models are embedded in the process of AMINI, F., LEWIS, T., LANNON, R., & LOUIE, A. (1996).

443
Obegi

Affect, attachment, memory: Contributions toward Effects of dismissing attachment strategies. Develop-
psychobiologic integration. Psychiatry: Interpersonal ment and Psychopathology, 7, 217–226.
and Biological Processes, 59, 213–239. EAMES, V., & ROTH, A. (2000). Patient attachment ori-
ARNOLD, E. G., FARBER, B. A., & GELLER, J. D. (2000). entation and the early working alliance: A study of
Changes in patients’ self-representation over the course patient and therapist reports of alliance quality and
of psychotherapy. Journal of the American Academy of ruptures. Psychotherapy Research, 10, 421– 434.
Psychoanalysis, 28, 449 – 466. FARBER, B. A., & HALL, D. (2002). Disclosure to thera-
BACHELOR, A., & SALAMÉ, R. (2000). Participants’ per- pists: What is and is not discussed in psychotherapy.
ceptions of dimensions of the therapeutic alliance over Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58, 359 –370.
the course of therapy. Journal of Psychotherapy Prac- FARBER, B. A., LIPPERT, R. A., & NEVAS, D. B. (1995).
tice & Research, 9, 39 –53. The therapist as attachment figure. Psychotherapy: The-
BERANT, E., & OBEGI, J. (2008). Attachment-informed ory, Research, Practice, Training, 32, 204 –212.
psychotherapy research with adults. In J. H. Obegi & FIALKOW, N. J., & MUSLIN, H. L. (1987). Working
S. E. Berant (Eds.), Attachment theory and research in through: A cornerstone of psychotherapy. American
clinical work with adults. New York: Guilford Press. Journal of Psychotherapy, 41, 443– 452.
BORDIN, E. S. (1979). The generalizability of the psycho- FONAGY, P., GERGELY, G., JURIST, E., & TARGET, M.
analytic concept of the working alliance. Psychother- (2002). Affect regulation, mentalization, and the devel-
apy: Theory, Research & Practice, 16, 252–260. opment of self. New York: Other Press.
BORDIN, E. S. (1980). A psychodynamic view of counsel- FRALEY, R. C., & DAVIS, K. E. (1997). Attachment for-
ing psychology. Counseling Psychologist, 9, 62–70. mation and transfer in young adults’ close friendships
BORDIN, E. S. (1994). Theory and research on the ther- and romantic relationships. Personal Relationships, 4,
apeutic working alliance: New directions. In A. O. Hor- 131–144.
vath, & L. S. Greenberg (Eds.), The working alliance: GASTON, L. (1990). The concept of the alliance and its
Theory, research, and practice (pp. 13–37). Oxford, En- role in psychotherapy: Theoretical and empirical con-
gland: Wiley. siderations. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice,
BOWLBY, J. (1975). Attachment theory, separation anxi- Training, 27, 143–153.
ety, and mourning. In D. A. Hamburg & K. H. Brodie GELLER, J. D., & FARBER, B. A. (1993). Factors influ-
(Eds.), New psychiatric frontiers (2nd ed., pp. 292–309). encing the process of internalization in psychotherapy.
Psychotherapy Research, 3, 166 –180.
New York: Basic Books.
GELSO, C. J., & CARTER, J. A. (1985). The relationship in
BOWLBY, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attach-
counseling and psychotherapy: Components, conse-
ment (2nd ed.). New York: Basic Books. (Original work
quences, and theoretical antecedents. Counseling Psy-
published 1969)
chologist, 13, 155–243.
BOWLBY, J. (1984). The making and breaking of affec-
GEORGE, C., KAPLAN, N., & MAIN, M. (1996). Adult
tional bonds. London: Tavistock.
Attachment Interview (2nd ed.). Unpublished manu-
BOWLBY, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-child attach-
script, Department of Psychology, University of Cali-
ment and healthy human development. New York: Basic fornia, Berkeley.
Books. GREENSON, R. R. (1967). The technique and practice of
CASSIDY, J., & SHAVER, P. R. (Eds.), (1999). Handbook psychoanalysis. New York: International Universities
of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applica- Press.
tions. New York: Guilford. HALL, D. A., & FARBER, B. A. (2001). Patterns of patient
CASTONGUAY, L., G., CONSTANTINO, M., J., & HOLT- disclosure in psychotherapy. Journal of the American
FORTH G ROSSE , M. (2006). The working alliance:
Academy of Psychoanalysis and Dynamic Psychiatry,
Where are we and where should we go? Psychotherapy: 29, 213–230.
Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 43, 271–279. HARRIST, R. S., QUINTANA, S. M., STRUPP, H. H., &
CLARKE, H., REES, A., & HARDY, G. E. (2004). The big HENRY, W. P. (1994). Internalization of interpersonal
idea: Clients’ perspectives of change processes in cog- process in time-limited dynamic psychotherapy. Psy-
nitive therapy. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, chotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 31,
Research and Practice, 77, 67– 89. 49 –57.
COLLINS, N. L., GUICHARD, A. C., FORD, M. B., & HATCHER, R., L., & BARENDS, A., W. (2006). How a
FEENEY, B. C. (2004). Working models of attachment. return to theory could help alliance research. Psycho-
In W. S. Rholes & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), Adult attach- therapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 43, 292–
ment: Theory, research, and clinical implications (pp. 299.
196 –239). New York: Guilford. HAZAN, C., GUR-YAISH, N., & CAMPA, M. (2004). What
DAVILA, J., & COBB, R. J. (2004). Predictors of change in does it mean to be attached? In W. S. Rholes & J. A.
attachment security during adulthood. In W. S. Rholes Simpson (Eds.), Adult attachment: Theory, research,
& J. A. Simpson (Eds.), Adult attachment: Theory, re- and clinical implications (pp. 55– 85). New York: Guil-
search, and clinical implications (pp. 133–156). New ford.
York: Guilford. HAZAN, C., & ZEIFMAN, D. (1994). Sex and the psycho-
DOZIER, M. (1990). Attachment organization and treat- logical tether. In K. Bartholomew & D. Perlman (Eds.),
ment use for adults with serious psychopathological Advances in personal relationships: Vol. 5. Attachment
disorders. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 47– 60. processes in adulthood (pp. 151–178). Philadelphia:
DOZIER, M., & LEE, S. W. (1995). Discrepancies between Kingsley.
self- and other-report of psychiatric symptomatology: HILL, C. E., THOMPSON, B. J., COGAR, M. C., & DENMAN,

444
Client–Therapist Bond and Attachment

D. W. (1993). Beneath the surface of long-term ther- tionship: Development of the Client Attachment to
apy: Therapist and client report of their own and each Therapist scale. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 42,
other’s covert processes. Journal of Counseling Psy- 307–317.
chology, 40, 278 –287. MALLINCKRODT, B., PORTER, M. J., & KIVLIGHAN,
HOLMES, J. (1996). Attachment, intimacy, autonomy: Us- D. M. J. (2005). Client attachment to therapist, depth of
ing Attachment theory in adult psychotherapy. Lanham, in-session exploration, and object relations in brief psy-
MD: Aronson. chotherapy. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice,
HOLMES, J. (2001). The search for the secure base: Attach- Training, 42, 85–100.
ment theory and psychotherapy. London: Brunner- MARTIN, D. J., GARSKE, J. P., & DAVIS, M. K. (2000).
Routledge. Relation of the therapeutic alliance with outcome and
HORVATH, A. O. (2005). The therapeutic relationship: other variables: A meta-analytic review. Journal of
Research and theory: An introduction to the special Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 438 – 450.
issue. Psychotherapy Research, 15, 3–7. MIKULINCER, M., & SHAVER, P. R. (2004). Security-based
HORVATH, A. O., & BEDI, R. P. (2002). The alliance. In self-representations in adulthood: Contents and pro-
J. C. Norcross (Ed.), Psychotherapy relationships that cesses. In W. S. Rholes & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), Adult
work: Therapist contributions and responsiveness to pa- attachment: Theory, research, and clinical implications
tients (pp. 37– 69). New York: Oxford University Press. (pp. 159 –195). New York: Guilford.
HORVATH, A. O., & LUBORSKY, L. (1993). The role of the MIKULINCER, M., & SHAVER, P. R. (2007). Attachment in
therapeutic alliance in psychotherapy. Journal of Con- adulthood: Structure, dynamics, and change. New York:
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 561–573. Guilford.
HOWARD, K. I., CORNILLE, T. A., LYONS, J. S., & VESSEY, MIKULINCER, M., SHAVER, P. R., CASSIDY, J., & BERANT,
J. T. (1996). Patterns of mental health service utiliza- E. (2008). Attachment-related defensive processes. In
tion. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 696 –703. J. H. Obegi & E. Berant (Eds.), Attachment theory and
JOHNSON, S. M., MAKINEN, J. A., & MILLIKIN, J. W. research in clinical work with adults. New York: Guil-
(2001). Attachment injuries in couple relationships: A ford.
new perspective on impasses in couples therapy. Jour- NOEL, S. B., & HOWARD, K. I. (1989). Initial contact and
nal of Marital & Family Therapy, 27, 145–155. engagement in psychotherapy. Journal of Clinical Psy-
JOYCE, A. S., & PIPER, W. E. (1998). Expectancy, the chology, 45, 798 – 805.
therapeutic alliance, and treatment outcome in short-
OLIVER, M. I., PEARSON, N., COE, N., & GUNNELL, D.
term individual psychotherapy. Journal of Psychother-
(2005). Help-seeking behaviour in men and women
apy Practice & Research, 7, 236 –248.
with common mental health problems: Cross-sectional
KANNINEN, K., SALO, J., & PUNAMAKI, R. L. (2000).
study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 186, 297–301.
Attachment patterns and working alliance in trauma
ORLINSKY, D. E., RØNNESTAD, M. H., & WILLUTZKI, U.
therapy for victims of political violence. Psychotherapy
(2004). Fifty years of psychotherapy process-outcome
Research, 10, 435– 449.
research: Continuity and change. In M. J. Lambert
KIVLIGHAN, D. M. J., PATTON, M. J., & FOOTE, D. (1998).
Moderating effects of client attachment on the coun- (Ed.), Bergin and Garfield’s handbook of psychother-
selor experience-working alliance relationship. Journal apy and behavior change (pp. 307– 89). New York:
of Counseling Psychology, 45, 274 –278. Wiley.
KIVLIGHAN, D. M. J., & SHAUGHNESSY, P. (2000). Pat- PARISH, M., & EAGLE, M. N. (2003). Attachment to the
terns of working alliance development: A typology of therapist. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 20, 271–286.
client’s working alliance ratings. Journal of Counseling PERSAUD, R. D., & MEUX, C. J. (1994). The psychopa-
Psychology, 47, 362–371. thology of authority and its loss: The effect on a ward of
KNOX, S., GOLDBERG, J. L., WOODHOUSE, S. S., & HILL, losing a consultant psychiatrist. British Journal of Med-
C. E. (1999). Clients’ internal representations of their ical Psychology, 67, 1–11.
therapists. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 46, 244 – QUINTANA, S. M., & MEARA, N. M. (1990). Internaliza-
256. tion of therapeutic relationships in short-term psycho-
KOKOTOVIC, A. M., & TRACEY, T. J. (1987). Premature therapy. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 37, 123–
termination at a university counseling center. Journal 130.
of Counseling Psychology, 34, 80 – 82. RAPPOPORT, A. (1997). The patient’s search for safety:
KORFMACHER, J., ADAM, E., OGAWA, J., & EGELAND, B. The organizing principle in psychotherapy. Psychother-
(1997). Adult attachment: Implications for the thera- apy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 34, 250 –261.
peutic process in a home visitation intervention. Ap- RENDELY, J. (1999). The death of an analyst: The loss of
plied Developmental Science, 1, 43–52. a real relationship. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 35,
LUBORSKY, L. (1976). Helping alliances in psychother- 131–152.
apy. In J. L. Claghorn (Ed.), Successful psychotherapy ROSENZWEIG, D. L., FARBER, B. A., & GELLER, J. D.
(pp. 368 –377). New York: American Psychological As- (1996). Clients’ representations of their therapists over
sociation. the course of psychotherapy. Journal of Clinical Psy-
MALLINCKRODT, B., COBLE, H. M., & GANTT, D. L. chology, 52, 197–207.
(1995). Working alliance, attachment memories, and SATTERFIELD, W. A., & LYDDON, W. J. (1995). Client
social competencies of women in brief therapy. Journal attachment and perceptions of the working alliance
of Counseling Psychology, 42, 79 – 84. with counselor trainees. Journal of Counseling Psychol-
MALLINCKRODT, B., GANTT, D. L., & COBLE, H. M. ogy, 42, 187–189.
(1995). Attachment patterns in the psychotherapy rela- SATTERFIELD, W. A., & LYDDON, W. J. (1998). Client

445
Obegi

attachment and the working alliance. Counselling Psy- scales predict outcome in a randomized controlled trial
chology Quarterly, 11, 407– 415. of two group therapies for binge eating disorder: An
SAUNDERS, S. M. (1993). Applicants’ experience of the aptitude by treatment interaction. Psychotherapy Re-
process of seeking therapy. Psychotherapy: Theory, Re- search, 16, 106 –121.
search, Practice, Training, 30, 554 –564. TRYON, G. S. (1989). A study of engagement and prema-
SAUNDERS, S. M. (1999). Clients’ assessment of the affec- ture termination in a university counseling center.
tive environment of the psychotherapy session: Rela- Counselling Psychology Quarterly, 2, 419 – 429.
tionship to session quality and treatment effectiveness. TRYON, G. S., & WINOGRAD, G. (2002). Goal consensus
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55, 597– 605. and collaboration. In J. C. Norcross (Ed.), Psychother-
SCHWARTZ, H. J., & SILVER, A. S. (Eds.). (1990). Illness apy relationships that work: Therapist contributions and
in the analyst: Implications for the treatment relation- responsiveness to patients (pp. 109 –125). New York:
ship. Madison, CT: International Universities Press.
Oxford University Press.
SEXTON, H., LITTAUER, H., SEXTON, A., & TOMMERAS, E.
VOGEL, D., L, WADE, N., G, & HAAKE, S. (2006). Mea-
(2005). Building an alliance: Early therapy process and
suring the self-stigma associated with seeking psycho-
the client-therapist connection. Psychotherapy Re-
search, 15, 103–116. logical help. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 325–
SROUFE, A., & WATERS, E. (1977). Attachment as an 337.
organizational construct. Child Development, 48, 1184 – WALLIN, D. J. (2007). Attachment in psychotherapy. New
1199. York: Guilford.
STILES, W., B, GLICK, M., J, OSATUKE, K., HARDY, WEBB, N. B. (1983). Vacation-separations: Therapeutic
G. E., SHAPIRO, D., A, AGNEW-DAVIES, R., ET AL. implications and clinical management. Clinical Social
(2004). Patterns of alliance development and the Work Journal, 11, 126 –138.
rupture-repair hypothesis: Are productive relation- WOODHOUSE, S. S., SCHLOSSER, L. Z., CROOK, R. E.,
ships U-shaped or V-shaped? Journal of Counseling LIGIERO, D. P., & GELSO, C. J. (2003). Client attach-
Psychology, 51, 81–92. ment to therapist: Relations to transference and client
TASCA, G. A., RITCHIE, K., CONRAD, G., BALFOUR, L., recollections of parental caregiving. Journal of Coun-
GAYTON, J., LYBANON, V., et al. (2006). Attachment seling Psychology, 50, 395– 408.

Acknowledgment of Ad Hoc Reviewers


Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training extends its thanks to the
following individuals who contributed their time and expertise to the journal by
serving as reviewers for articles published in 2008:
Dan Brossart William H. Gottdiener Alissa G. Putman
Grace I. L. Caskie Chad V. Johnson Juan R. Riker
Rochelle L. Coffey Frances Kelley Lewis Z. Schlosser
Changming Duan Georgios K. Michele A.
Lisa M. Edwards Lampropoulos Schottenbauer
F. Barton Evans David M. Lawson Jonathan Schwartz
Eric A. Fertuck Rayna D. Markin Barbara J. Thompson
Suzanne Friedman Shelley F. McMain Jessica Walker
Jean S. Gearon Catherine Monk Susan S. Woodhouse
Bernard S. Gorman

446

You might also like