You are on page 1of 17

A Pilot Study of a Abstract

Kindergarten Summer This pilot study examined an implementation of


a kindergarten summer school reading program
School Reading in 4 high-poverty urban schools. The program
targeted both basic reading skills and oral lan-
guage development. Students were randomly
Program in High- assigned to a treatment group (n ⫽ 25) or a
typical practice comparison group (n ⫽ 28)
Poverty Urban Schools within each school; however, randomization
was compromised due to school circumstances,
resulting in a quasi-experimental design. In-
struction was delivered by the schools’ regular
teachers during 20 full-day summer school ses-
Carolyn A. Denton sions. Each day treatment group students re-
Emily J. Solari ceived large-group listening comprehension
and vocabulary lessons anchored in storybook
Dennis J. Ciancio reading, along with small-group lessons focused
Steven A. Hecht on basic reading skills and listening comprehen-
sion. The intervention was associated with im-
Paul R. Swank proved outcomes for treatment group students
University of Texas Health Science Center at in word reading and listening comprehension
Houston with mixed results for phonemic awareness and
no significant between-group differences in
reading fluency or vocabulary. Such an ap-
proach is potentially efficacious, suggesting the
need for further research.

Although recent educational initiatives


have recognized the importance of early
reading instruction, there remains a dispar-
ity in reading achievement between stu-
dents from high- and low-income families.
According to results of the 2007 National
Assessment of Educational Progress report
(Lee, Grigg, & Donahue, 2007), only half of
U.S. fourth-grade students who are eligible
for the free or reduced-price lunch program
are able to read at or above a basic level and
only 17% of the students within this low
The Elementary School Journal socioeconomic group read proficiently. In
Volume 110, Number 4
© 2010 by The University of Chicago. All rights reserved.
contrast, 79% of students ineligible for the
0013-5984/2010/11004-0001$10.00 free or reduced-price lunch program read

This content downloaded from 207.162.240.147 on July 24, 2016 14:02:16 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
424 THE ELEMENTARY SCHOOL JOURNAL

at or above the basic level and 44% of the effective when words are taught both di-
students within this higher socioeconomic rectly and indirectly through exposure in the
group are proficient readers. In the absence context of reading and listening, when stu-
of quality reading intervention, it is highly dents receive multiple exposures to vocabu-
likely that primary-grade students with lary words, and when students are actively
reading difficulties will experience contin- engaged in vocabulary instruction. Regard-
ued reading problems in the upper elemen- ing reading comprehension, the NRP recom-
tary and secondary grades (Francis, Shay- mended that instruction should guide stu-
witz, Stuebing, Shaywitz, & Fletcher, 1996; dents toward an awareness of their own
Juel, 1988; Torgesen & Burgess, 1998). Thus cognitive processes while reading (i.e., meta-
the failure to provide effective instruction cognitive processing). In order for many stu-
virtually guarantees continuing gaps in dents to learn to comprehend text, it is nec-
reading performance between students essary that teachers model and directly teach
with and without economic disadvantage. the practice of applying cognitive and meta-
cognitive comprehension strategies. In a ran-
domized experimental study, Solari and Ger-
Early Reading Instruction ber (2008) found that kindergarten students
The problem is daunting but not insur- who received direct instruction in vocabulary
mountable. There is converging research ev- words as well as strategies for comprehen-
idence that most young students at risk for sion of text that was read to them signifi-
reading difficulties can make adequate cantly outperformed their peers in listening
progress when provided with quality class- comprehension. Additionally, effects of the
room reading instruction along with short- vocabulary and listening comprehension in-
term supplemental intervention if needed tervention in kindergarten were detected in
(Mathes & Denton, 2002; Torgesen, 2000). first grade when treatment students outper-
This quality reading instruction in the pri- formed comparison students on a simple
mary grades addresses phonemic awareness, reading-comprehension task.
phonics, spelling, reading fluency, vocabu-
lary, and reading comprehension (Foorman
& Torgesen, 2001; National Reading Panel, Summer Learning
2000; Rayner, Foorman, Perfetti, Pesetsky, & Research has suggested that students
Seidenberg, 2001; Snow, Burns, & Griffin, who come from low-income families tend
1998). It is particularly important that young to lose ground during the summer months
students at risk for reading difficulties re- while those from middle- and high-income
ceive explicit instruction in phonological families are more likely to maintain the
awareness and phonics with ample opportu- knowledge and skills learned during the
nities to apply skills in connected text (Na- previous school year (Alexander, Entwisle,
tional Reading Panel [NRP], 2000; Snow et & Olson, 2001, 2007; Borman, Benson, &
al., 1998). In their synthesis of research, the Overman, 2005; Cooper, Nye, Charlton,
NRP found an effect size for phonics-based Lindsay, & Greathouse, 1996). For example,
interventions of 0.98 for struggling readers in Cooper et al. (1996) found that, on average,
kindergarten through second grade (Ehri, children from low-income backgrounds
Nunes, Stahl, & Willows, 2001). showed significant academic losses, ending
In addition to phonological processing, the summer about 3 months behind their
oral language proficiency has been found middle-class peers.
to be an important predictor of later read- Alexander and colleagues (2001, 2007)
ing ability (Bishop & Adams, 1990; Scarbor- had a different, but no less important find-
ough, 1989). The NRP (2000) findings indi- ing when they analyzed the progress made
cate that vocabulary instruction is most during the school year and summer break

JUNE 2010

This content downloaded from 207.162.240.147 on July 24, 2016 14:02:16 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
SUMMER SCHOOL READING PILOT 425

by high-, middle-, and low-SES children, us- peers. Other authors have suggested that
ing longitudinal data collected from a ran- the quality of summer school programs
dom representative sample of students in tends to be higher in schools that serve
Baltimore. The researchers analyzed scores higher-income populations (e.g., Roderick,
on the reading-comprehension subtest of the Bryk, Jacob, Easton, & Allensworth, 1999).
California Achievement Test administered in Lauer et al. (2006) conducted a compre-
the fall and spring of grades 1–5, and again at hensive meta-analysis of the effects of read-
the end of grade 9, reporting a discrepancy ing and mathematics intervention provided
between scores of high- and low-SES chil- outside of the regular school day, both after
dren of 26.48 scale score points at the begin- school and during summer school. Only
ning of first grade. Between the fall and studies that included comparison groups
spring test administrations in grades 1–5 were included, but few had true experi-
(during the school year), this score difference mental designs. The majority were con-
was virtually unchanged, indicating that ducted with students who were either low-
schooling was effective in at least maintain- performing or from low-income families.
ing the reading levels of low-income students The researchers identified 30 studies with
relative to high-SES students. However, the reading outcomes published between 1986
gap widened to 48.48 points by the end of and 2003 that met their inclusionary crite-
grade 5 due to changes between the spring ria. Based on their analyses, Lauer and col-
and fall test administrations (i.e., over the leagues reported a mean effect size for out-
summer breaks). Interestingly, the results in- of-school reading interventions (combining
dicated that the high-SES group made signif- summer school and after school programs)
icant gains over the summer while the low- of .13, with somewhat larger effects (.22) for
SES group declined only slightly or remained studies involving students in the lower
unchanged. By the end of grade 9 there was elementary grades. The meta-analysis in-
a difference of 73.16 points between the cluded five studies that specifically exam-
scores of the high- and low-SES groups. ined the effects of summer school reading
intervention for students in kindergarten
through first grade, with widely varying
Effects of Summer School Programs effect sizes (ranging from ⫺.17 to .73).
Although common sense suggests that In an experimental study, Schacter and
summer school programs should be effec- Jo (2005) demonstrated the efficacy of a
tive in reducing summer learning losses or summer school reading intervention for
facilitating reading gains over the summer first-grade students at risk for reading dif-
break, existing research on the topic has ficulties. Students who were randomly as-
produced mixed results. There have been signed to an experimental condition partic-
few experimental or quasi-experimental ipated in a 2 hour reading program 5 days
studies in this area, leaving many unan- per week for 7 weeks. Instruction in both
swered questions regarding summer school word- and text-level reading skills was pro-
program effectiveness. Cooper, Charlton, vided in both whole-group and small-
Valentine, and Muhlenbruck (2000) per- group formats. The program consisted of
formed a meta-analysis of research on 93 40 minutes of small-group reading instruc-
summer school programs (with only a tion, 15 minutes of whole-group instruc-
small number of studies that had compari- tion, 15 minutes of independent phonics
son groups), finding average gains of one- practice, 10 minutes of paired reading us-
fifth of a standard deviation for participat- ing decodable text, 10 minutes of teacher
ing students. This review suggested that read-aloud, and 30 minutes of writing ac-
middle-class students benefited from sum- tivities. Results indicated significant differ-
mer school more than their lower-SES ences between the experimental and control

This content downloaded from 207.162.240.147 on July 24, 2016 14:02:16 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
426 THE ELEMENTARY SCHOOL JOURNAL

groups at posttest on measures of decoding district in the Southwest. The schools were
and comprehension. Furthermore, these all rated academically acceptable by the
gains were maintained at 3 months and at the state education agency. The average ethnic
end of the following academic school year. composition of the schools was 50% Afri-
This study was unique in that the interven- can American, 48% Hispanic, 1% white,
tion addressed both word-level and text-level and 1% Asian and other ethnicities. On av-
skills. Extant research suggests that summer erage, 95% of the students who attended
reading programs for at-risk populations the schools were classified as economically
tend to concentrate instructional time on disadvantaged.
word-level reading skills with less attention Schools 1 and 2 served students in pre-
to vocabulary and comprehension (Roderick kindergarten through fifth grade. Each of
et al., 1999). these schools had two kindergarten summer
school classrooms. In each school the teacher
in one classroom taught the experimental
Purpose of the Study reading program while the other teacher im-
Given the limited and contradictory re- plemented typical summer school instruc-
search base relative to summer school read- tion. School 3 served only early childhood
ing programs, as well as the continued through kindergarten and was paired with
prevalence of reading difficulties among School 4 which served only grades 1–5, be-
students from economically disadvantaged cause most of the students in School 3 would
backgrounds, there is a need for further attend School 4 when they entered first
research examining the efficacy of summer grade. In this study School 3 provided the
school reading intervention provided in the comparison classroom and School 4 pro-
early grades. The purpose of this pilot vided the experimental classroom. School 4
study was to examine the effects of an im- did not typically offer kindergarten summer
plementation of a kindergarten summer school but was piloting a model in which
school reading program that focused on kindergarten students who would be enter-
both word-level (i.e., phonemic awareness, ing the school as first graders would attend
phonics) and text-level (i.e., vocabulary, summer school at School 4 in order to be-
comprehension) skills in four high-poverty come acclimated to their new school. A fifth
urban schools. We addressed the following school that agreed to participate had to be
research question: Is participation in the dropped from the study because its kinder-
experimental summer school reading pro- garten summer school attendance was signif-
gram associated with better outcomes in icantly lower than expected and did not al-
word reading, phonemic awareness, vocab- low for the provision of both an experimental
ulary, listening comprehension, and oral and comparison classroom within the school.
reading fluency than participation in typi- Students. All students who attended
cal summer school instruction? We hypoth- English-only summer school classes in par-
esized that children who received the ex- ticipating schools were eligible to partici-
perimental reading program would have pate in the study, including those with
higher outcomes in each domain than those identified mild to moderate disabilities.
who received typical summer school in- Students were selected by their schools us-
struction. ing the schools’ normal procedures for de-
termining which students attend summer
school. Parents and guardians of these stu-
Method dents were informed of this decision by
Schools and Participants school personnel. In May, before the end of
This study was conducted in four ele- the regular school year, we randomly as-
mentary schools in a large urban school signed these students to the treatment or

JUNE 2010

This content downloaded from 207.162.240.147 on July 24, 2016 14:02:16 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
SUMMER SCHOOL READING PILOT 427

comparison summer school classrooms and TABLE 1. Student Demographic Information by


Group
administered standardized pretests. Sum-
mer school instruction began during the Treatment Comparison
first week of June. (n ⫽ 25) (n ⫽ 28)
Summer school was also open to any Characteristics n % n %
student whose parents wanted them to at-
Ethnicity:
tend. Eight additional students who had African-American 21 84 19 68
not been selected by their schools enrolled Hispanic 2 8 8 28
in summer school after the term began. White 0 0 1 4
Asian and other 2 8 0 0
Since school personnel needed to immedi- Gender:
ately assign these children to a classroom, Male 16 64 7 25
they were asked to alternate the assignment Female 9 36 21 75
Economically
between the treatment and comparison disadvantaged:
classes within their schools and thus these Yes 23 92 25 89
students were not randomly assigned to No 1 4 2 7
Data unavailable 1 4 1 4
condition. We have reason to believe that
the schools did not strictly alternate be-
tween treatment and comparison class-
rooms when placing these students, but day of summer school at participating
tended to place students with lower read- schools and had some data (44 treatment,
ing performance in treatment classrooms. 48 comparison). Twenty-six of these stu-
For example, the mean pretest standard dents withdrew from summer school be-
scores on the Woodcock-Johnson Tests fore the end of the term, eight others had
of Achievement–III (WJ III; Woodcock, incomplete standardized test data for vari-
McGrew, & Mather, 2001) letter-word iden- ous reasons, and five were excluded be-
tification subtest for the treatment and cause the schools violated their randomiza-
comparison students who were placed by tion to condition. This left a final sample of
their schools were 105.25 (SD ⫽ 11.79) and 53 students (25 treatment, 28 comparison).
120.75 (SD ⫽ 8.58), respectively. Therefore, The mean age of treatment-group students
we characterize this pilot study as quasi- at posttest was 74.68 months (SD ⫽ 3.82),
experimental rather than experimental. In and the mean age of comparison-group stu-
addition, 10 students who had been ran- dents was 75.14 months (SD ⫽ 3.61). Table
domized to the comparison condition were 1 displays other demographic characteris-
moved by school personnel to the treat- tics, revealing gender differences between
ment classrooms without the researchers’ the groups (the treatment group was pre-
knowledge. We did not include the data dominantly male, while the comparison
from these students in our analyses. group was predominantly female).
In total, some assessment data were col- Teachers. Three teachers delivered the
lected for 147 students. For nearly all of experimental curriculum while three other
these students we collected standardized teachers delivered their typical summer
pretest data before the end of the regular school instruction. All of the participating
school year. Of the 147 students, 44 did not teachers were regular school district em-
attend summer school at all, leaving a sam- ployees selected by their schools to teach
ple of 103. Of these, 11 were dropped from summer school. During the previous school
the study because they attended the school year all of the teachers had taught kinder-
in which there were too few kindergarten garten except for one, who had taught first
summer school students to form treatment grade. Three noncertified paraprofessionals
and comparison classrooms, leaving a sam- also delivered small-group instruction in
ple of 92 students who attended at least one reading comprehension in the treatment

This content downloaded from 207.162.240.147 on July 24, 2016 14:02:16 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
428 THE ELEMENTARY SCHOOL JOURNAL

classrooms. The research team provided and supplies missing words. Split-half reli-
the treatment-group teachers and parapro- ability is .82–.85 in the age range of interest.
fessionals with 2 full days of professional For WJ III analyses we used W scores, a
development, periodic coaching support, continuously scaled standard score.
instructional materials, and fully devel- The CTOPP blending-words and sound-
oped lesson plans for implementing the matching subtests assess phonological
curriculum. awareness. The reliability of the CTOPP was
investigated using estimates of content sam-
pling, time sampling, and scorer differences.
Measures Most of the average internal consistency and
At pretest and posttest we administered alternate form reliability coefficients exceed
standardized assessments measuring pho- .80. The test/retest coefficients range from .70
nological awareness, word reading, text to .92. In the blending-words subtest the sub-
reading fluency, listening comprehension, ject is orally presented with isolated parts of
and vocabulary. In addition, we adminis- a word and is asked to put these parts to-
tered researcher-developed, curriculum- gether to make the whole word (e.g., t-r-a-p:
based measures (CBMs). All assessments trap). In the sound-matching subtest students
were individually administered to each select from pictures the one that represents a
participating child by a trained adult exam- word beginning or ending with the same
iner. Pretests were administered to most sound as a target word (e.g., Which word
students in May during the last 2 weeks of starts with the same sound as pan: pig, hat, or
the regular school year; students who came cone?). For CTOPP subtests we analyzed raw
to summer school after it had already be- scores because some students could not be
gun without having been previously iden- assigned standard scores at pretest due to
tified by their schools were pretested dur- very low raw scores.
ing the first week of summer school. Post- The TPRI oral reading fluency task is an
testing occurred for all children during the individually administered test of fluent and
last 3 days of the summer school session. accurate reading of connected text. Stu-
Standardized measures. The standard- dents’ fluency is evaluated with a timed
ized measures were (a) the WJ III (Woodcock 1-minute oral reading sample. The score is
et al., 2001) letter-word identification and oral the number of correctly read words per
comprehension subtests; (b) the Comp- minute. Researchers have consistently doc-
rehensive Test of Phonological Processing umented a moderate to strong positive re-
(CTOPP; Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, lationship between oral reading fluency
1999) blending-words and sound-matching and reading comprehension for students in
subtests; (c) the Texas Primary Reading In- grades 1–5 (Fuchs, Fuchs, Hosp, & Jenkins,
ventory (TPRI; Foorman, Fletcher, & Francis, 2001). In the current study we administered
2004) oral reading fluency task; and (d) the the same TPRI story, which is approxi-
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test– 4 (PPVT-4; mately on a beginning-of-first-grade level,
Dunn & Dunn, 2007). at both pretest and posttest. Analyses uti-
The WJ III is a nationally standardized, lized raw scores.
individually administered achievement bat- The PPVT-4 is a well-established stan-
tery widely used in reading intervention dardized receptive vocabulary assessment
research. Letter-word identification as- in which students select from pictures the
sesses the ability to identify letters and read one that matches a word pronounced by
words presented in a list format. Split-half the tester. Split-half reliability is .93–.97 in
reliability is .98 –.99 in the age of interest. In the age range of interest. The PPVT-4 man-
the oral comprehension subtest the partici- ual recommends using growth scale value
pant listens to sentences or short passages (GSV) scores for measuring change in a

JUNE 2010

This content downloaded from 207.162.240.147 on July 24, 2016 14:02:16 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
SUMMER SCHOOL READING PILOT 429

student’s vocabulary over time, stating that read-aloud session included planned interac-
the GSV measures “an examinee’s vocabu- tive comprehension instruction, while the af-
lary with respect to an absolute scale of ternoon session incorporated planned inter-
knowledge. As an examinee’s vocabulary active vocabulary instruction. Each of these
grows, the GSV will increase” (Dunn & sessions was followed by a set of small-group
Dunn, 2007, p. 21). rotations. During each rotation (in the morn-
Curriculum-based measures. The CBMs ing and again in the afternoon) small groups
were designed to assess mastery of con- of children rotated between two teaching ta-
cepts and skills taught in the treatment bles and two learning centers. Each group
classrooms. Pretest results on the CBMs spent 20 minutes at each of these four sta-
were used by teachers to guide their in- tions; every 20 minutes the teacher cued the
structional planning. The reading CBMs as- students to move to their next station. At one
sessed (a) identification of letter-sound teaching table, the teacher delivered basic
correspondences (30 items consisting of reading instruction during both the morning
upper- and lower-case letters; 15 letters that and afternoon rotations, while at the second
were taught in the research curriculum and teaching table the paraprofessional taught
15 untaught letters), (b) decodable word mathematics in the morning and reading
reading (25 three-letter words composed of comprehension in the afternoon. Each small-
letter-sounds taught in the research curric- group comprehension lesson focused on the
ulum), (c) high-frequency word reading (30 book that had been read aloud during the
items; 15 words students were taught in the days’ large-group sessions. Thus, every day
experimental curriculum and 15 untaught each child received a total of four 20-minute
words), and (d) identification of pictures small-group lessons (two basic reading, one
representing the vocabulary words that mathematics, and one reading comprehen-
were taught in the program (54 items). sion) at the teaching tables and worked at a
Each measure was untimed and individu- total of four learning centers. At one learning
ally administered. Raw scores consisted of center students independently completed a
the number of correct items. journal writing activity designed to reinforce
vocabulary that had recently been taught.
Other learning centers addressed reading
Procedures and math objectives and were planned by the
The summer school session lasted for 20 individual teachers. Figure 1 illustrates a
days in June and early July. Classes were sample daily schedule. Small-group sizes
held daily from 8:00 a.m. to 2:30 p.m. ranged from two to five students; most teach-
Treatment-group students attended an av- ers taught their most at-risk students in
erage of 18.24 days of summer school smaller groups and higher-performing stu-
(SD ⫽ 1.83); comparison-group students at- dents in larger groups.
tended an average of 18.54 days (SD ⫽ Contents of the reading program. The
1.60). Class sizes at the beginning of the experimental reading program was devel-
summer school session ranged from 11 to oped by the authors specifically for this study
16 in the treatment classrooms and 12 to 16 and is unpublished. Treatment-group stu-
in the comparison classrooms. dents received explicit, systematic instruction
The experimental reading intervention in phonemic awareness, phonics, recognition
was incorporated across the school day and of high-frequency words, and sentence read-
followed a consistent daily schedule. Each ing in the two basic reading lessons every
day teachers read the same picture book day. Teachers provided direct instruction in
aloud to the children during two 45-minute new phonics and phonemic awareness skills,
whole-group sessions: once in the morning and students practiced these skills through a
and again in the afternoon. The morning variety of hands-on activities with teacher

This content downloaded from 207.162.240.147 on July 24, 2016 14:02:16 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
430 THE ELEMENTARY SCHOOL JOURNAL

FIG. 1.—Sample daily schedule for summer school research curriculum

feedback. Emphasis was placed on teaching Vocabulary instruction occurred in a


students to blend sounds together to sound whole-group setting and through indepen-
out words and to map sounds to print to dent practice in the form of journal writing.
spell words phonetically (i.e., recognize The whole-group instruction consisted of a
sounds in spoken words and record these storybook read-aloud with integrated explicit
sound with letters). Students applied reading vocabulary instruction. Three new vocabu-
skills and strategies in brief decodable text. lary words were taught daily (words were
Listening comprehension instruction took preselected from the storybook text). Vocab-
place in both whole-group and small-group ulary instruction consisted of introducing the
formats. Whole-group comprehension in- words in the context of the storybook, pro-
struction consisted of a whole-book read- viding students with a short and easy-to-
aloud with discussion focused through guid- remember definition, teaching the meaning
ing questions written specifically for each of the words in new contexts or usages, and
book. Small-group instruction included ex- practicing word use orally and through writ-
plicit, direct instruction in three comprehen- ing and drawing pictures associated with the
sion skills: finding the main idea, summariz- words (at the journal-writing center).
ing, and sequencing. The length of material Fidelity of implementation. We col-
to be comprehended increased as students lected observational data to measure the
progressed within each of these skills. For fidelity with which teachers and parapro-
example, during the first few intervention fessionals implemented the experimental
sessions, concentrating on direct recall of program. Treatment-group teachers and
facts from text, students were presented with paraprofessionals were observed during in-
short sentences and asked to answer ques- struction using an observation instrument
tions related to facts in the sentences. Once that allowed for ratings of program adher-
students mastered this skill at the sentence ence, student on-task behavior, and instruc-
level, they moved onto multisentence pas- tional quality. Instructional quality for all
sages and finally to full paragraphs. intervention components was evaluated

JUNE 2010

This content downloaded from 207.162.240.147 on July 24, 2016 14:02:16 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
SUMMER SCHOOL READING PILOT 431

using a three-point scale rating the teach- engaging students in using the words, and
ers’ provision of appropriate positive and (d) using the words in novel contexts and in
corrective feedback, lesson pacing, redirec- varied grammatical forms. These were in-
tion of off-task behavior, use of instruc- corporated into the program materials pro-
tional time, and warmth and enthusiasm vided to the treatment teachers and they
toward students (with a rating of 3 mean- achieved an average rating of 100% for
ing the behavior was observed for “most of these indicators. Their mean rating for stu-
the lesson,” 2 meaning “some of the les- dent on-task behavior was 98%, and the
son,” and 1 meaning “rarely”). We report mean quality rating was 96%.
fidelity as percentages, comparing each For the large-group comprehension com-
teacher’s ratings for each observation with ponent, treatment fidelity indicators included
a “perfect” rating on the instrument. (a) introducing the book to students in an
Each teacher was observed three times understandable way, (b) providing students
delivering basic reading instruction and with a problem/guiding question, (c) asking
two times delivering comprehension and appropriate questions to guide students to
vocabulary instruction. Each paraprofes- answer the problem/guiding question, (d)
sional was observed two times delivering engaging students in discussion of the key
small-group comprehension instruction. elements of the book, and (e) allowing stu-
Observations of basic reading were con- dents multiple opportunities to respond.
ducted by the first author, who was the Treatment-group teachers were provided
lead developer of that component. Obser- curriculum materials that included these ele-
vations of comprehension instruction were ments, and, on average, they achieved a rat-
conducted by the second author, the pri- ing of 99% for these indicators. The mean
mary developer of that component. Obser- rating for on-task behavior was 97% and the
vations of vocabulary instruction were con- mean quality rating was 95%. The nature of
ducted by a research assistant who had a the small-group comprehension instruction
primary role in the development of that was different in that it was more focused on
component. Since each component was ob- explicit instruction in comprehension skills,
served by only one person, interobserver and therefore fidelity of implementation was
reliability was not established. On some measured according to a different set of cri-
occasions, observations of basic reading teria: (a) presenting the lesson in a clear way,
skills, comprehension, and vocabulary may (b) modeling expected student behaviors
have been completed on the same day, but when needed, (c) providing students multi-
sometimes these were conducted on differ- ple opportunities to respond, (d) scaffolding
ent days. responses when necessary, and (e) providing
For the basic reading (decoding and appropriate corrective feedback. Paraprofes-
text reading) instructional component, pro- sionals in the treatment group achieved an
gram adherence was defined as implement- average rating of 96% for these behaviors
ing the lessons as they were described in during small-group instruction. The mean
the teacher’s manual, which was loosely on-task behavior rating was 98% and the
scripted. Treatment-group teachers had av- mean quality rating was 94%.
erage scores of 95% on program adherence, Comparison classroom instruction. Each
student on-task behavior, instructional comparison classroom teacher was observed
quality, and average overall fidelity. during one full reading lesson to verify the
For the vocabulary component, pro- nature of typical school practice. Following
gram adherence was defined as (a) provid- the observation, each teacher was asked
ing an understandable word definition or whether the observed lesson was typical of
explanation for each target word, (b) pro- their daily instruction, and all stated that this
viding explicit vocabulary instruction, (c) was the case. Observations indicated that in-

This content downloaded from 207.162.240.147 on July 24, 2016 14:02:16 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
432 THE ELEMENTARY SCHOOL JOURNAL

TABLE 2. Pretest and Posttest Observed Score Means and Standard Deviations; Cohen’s d Effect Sizes
Comparing Groups on Pretest-Posttest Difference Scores

Treatment (n ⫽ 25) Comparison (n ⫽ 28)


Pretest Posttest Pretest Posttest
Measure M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) d
a
Letter-word ID 105.08 (11.62) 109.64 (11.86)* 106.89 (17.87) 106.46 (17.89) .91
Oral comprehensiona,d 98.08 (9.67) 100.75 (7.74)* 99.00 (11.67) 99.08 (10.46) .45
b
Blending words 11.56 (1.73) 13.16 (2.19)* 11.89 (2.66) 12.25 (2.73) .76
b
Sound matching 9.28 (2.23) 9.84 (2.34) 9.50 (1.99) 9.71 (2.67) .27
Oral reading fluencyc,e 27.10 (26.91) 32.10 (29.02) 35.88 (29.68) 43.73 (33.95) ⫺.28
Picture vocabularya,d 86.42 (8.91) 89.75 (7.62) 89.61 (10.67) 92.00 (7.84) .31
Letter-sounds CBMc,e 23.82 (4.65) 25.23 (2.93) 22.81 (6.84) 24.00 (6.79) .10
High-frequency CBMc,e 17.05 (10.52) 23.36 (7.74)* 19.12 (10.88) 21.73 (10.62) .81
Decodable-words CBMc,e 10.73 (8.09) 19.55 (6.71) 13.77 (9.87) 17.77 (8.75) .76
Vocabulary CBMc,e 36.82 (4.93) 42.00 (5.89) 37.50 (5.23) 41.69 (6.11) .30

NOTE.—Letter-word ID ⫽ Letter-word identification; CBM ⫽ curriculum-based measure.


a
Standard scores reported.
b
Scale scores reported.
c
Raw scores reported.
d
Treatment group n ⫽ 25; comparison group n ⫽ 26.
e
Treatment group n ⫽ 22; comparison group n ⫽ 26.
*p ⬍ .05, one tailed.

struction in comparison classrooms differed aloud to the students. Little direct instruc-
from that in the treatment classrooms on im- tion was observed in this classroom.
portant dimensions. All observed lessons, ex- We observed no formal vocabulary in-
cept for a few minutes in one classroom struction in comparison-group classrooms.
when students worked in learning centers, On very few occasions teachers paused
were implemented in whole-class formats. while reading aloud to students to provide
The comparison-group teachers were a quick explanation of a word, but this ac-
observed implementing a variety of activi- tivity did not meet the criteria described
ties. In the first classroom, the teacher im- above for the treatment condition. Simi-
plemented a published summer school cur- larly, we did not observe purposeful in-
riculum, including phonological awareness struction focused on comprehension. Refer-
and worksheet activities as well as individ- ences to comprehension took place during
ual text reading. Students in this classroom whole-group lessons when the teacher read
were mostly on task during instruction, a book to or with the students and asked
which included explicit modeling of the them for predictions related to the book.
use of analogies to known words to help However, we did not observe a book read-
spell unknown words. No direct instruc- aloud session for one of the three compar-
tion was observed in the second classroom; ison teachers, and it is possible that she
students recited poems and sang songs re- may have provided contextualized vocab-
lated to alphabetic knowledge, after which ulary or comprehension instruction during
the teacher read a book aloud to the class. such a read-aloud format.
In the third classroom students participated
in chants and songs related to alphabetic
knowledge for a portion of the class, fol- Results
lowed by an activity related to word read- Descriptive statistics for all pretest and
ing and spelling and another activity in posttest measures by treatment condition
which volunteers read sentences. The les- can be found in Table 2. For the WJ III
son concluded with the teacher reading subtests, analyses were performed using W

JUNE 2010

This content downloaded from 207.162.240.147 on July 24, 2016 14:02:16 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
SUMMER SCHOOL READING PILOT 433

TABLE 3. Posttest Effects for Pretest, Intervention Group, and Teacher on Standardized Measures

WJ III Letter-Word WJ III Oral


Identification Comprehension CTOPP Blending Words
Source df F p df F p df F p

Pretest 1, 46 283.98 ⬍.0001 1, 44 16.63 .0001 1, 46 41.70 ⬍.0001


Group 1, 46 5.71 .011 1, 44 2.83 .049 1, 46 3.54 .033
Teacher 4, 46 3.04 .013 4, 44 .83 .257 4, 46 2.27 .038

Peabody Picture
CTOPP Sound Matching TPRI Oral Reading Fluency Vocabulary
df F p df F p df F p

Pretest 1, 46 33.84 ⬍.0001 1, 40 289.63 ⬍.0001 1, 44 90.88 ⬍.0001


Group 1, 46 .015 .350 1, 40 .29 .295 1, 44 .38 .269
Teacher 4, 46 .099 .210 4, 40 .59 .338 4, 44 .26 .451

NOTE.—P-values reflect one-tailed tests of significance.

scores, but we report pre-post standard the standardized measures are presented in
scores in Table 2 to enable comparison to Table 3.
other studies. Similarly, the PPVT analyses Preliminary data investigations in-
were performed using GSV scores and the cluded analyses of pretest equivalence and
CTOPP analyses were conducted on raw examination of outliers, potential depar-
scores, but we report pre-post standard tures from normality, and homogeneity of
score descriptive statistics for ease of inter- variance. Simple ANOVA indicated no sig-
pretation. Oral Reading Fluency analyses nificant between-group differences on pre-
were performed and results reported using test scores on any variable; however, we
raw scores reflecting the number of words included pretest in our analyses because of
read correctly per minute. For this pilot its significant contribution to outcomes on
study, we were interested in estimating the all variables. We also included teacher as a
effects of the intervention on students’ fixed effect in our models to account for the
growth over the 4-week period; we calcu- effects of individual teachers beyond those
lated Cohen’s d effect sizes comparing the of treatment-group assignment. As our hy-
treatment and comparison groups on their pothesis was unidirectional and the inter-
pre-post difference scores for each variable. vention was not expected to result in wors-
Effect sizes ranged from small (on growth
ening performance, we conducted one-
in sound matching, PPVT, and the letter-
tailed tests of significance. Moreover, in a
sound CBM) to large (on growth in blend-
pilot test, for which the goal is not evaluat-
ing words and all measures of word read-
ing the efficacy of an intervention but de-
ing) favoring the treatment group. The
termining whether a randomized trial is
effect on growth in oral comprehension
warranted, we judged that type II error was
was moderate. Only one effect size favored
more serious than type I error.
the comparison group, a small effect on
oral reading fluency. Several of the distributions were found
to violate the assumption of homogeneity
of variance and some were also found to
Standardized Measures depart significantly from the normal distri-
First we report the results for standard- bution. The problem of heterogeneous vari-
ized measures, followed by the results for ances in analysis of variance (ANOVA) can
the researcher-created CBMs. Results for be handled in one of several ways. One way

This content downloaded from 207.162.240.147 on July 24, 2016 14:02:16 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
434 THE ELEMENTARY SCHOOL JOURNAL

is to use a nonparametric ANOVA such as Von Mises, p ⫽ .2289; Anderson-Darling, p ⬎


the Kruskal-Wallis test, but this limits one to .15) were not. However, two outliers were
simple analyses. Since in this case we needed evident in the comparison group (precisely
to control for pretest levels on the measures the same as evident in the PPVT distribu-
and teacher effects, the nonparametric ap- tion). From pretest to posttest, these two stu-
proach was not appropriate. An alternative dents lost 29 and 36 standard score points,
method involves using a mixed-models anal- indicating likely testing error. If these stu-
ysis to model the variance separately. Using dents’ data are eliminated, the results do not
this model results in a log-likelihood statistic change appreciably, except that there is no
that can then be compared to the log likeli- longer any hint of nonnormality; there are
hood for a model assuming equal variances. still differences in variances and the group
Twice the difference in the log likelihoods is effect is significant (F(1, 44) ⫽ 2.83, p ⫽ .0498,
approximately distributed as chi-square and one-tailed). The teacher effect was not signif-
can be used to test the homogeneity assump- icant for this variable, but the pretest effect
tion as well. This method allows us to include was. After the outliers were dropped, the ef-
other terms in the model, such as pretests fect size for the difference score on this vari-
and teacher effects, and thereby provides a able was in the medium range (d ⫽ .45).
reasonable solution to the case of heteroge- For CTOPP analyses, we utilized raw
neous variance across groups in our data. scores since some students scored too low at
Thus, for each outcome we ran an initial pretest to be assigned standard scores. For
model that included the pretest assessment, CTOPP sound matching, there was no indi-
treatment-group assignment, and teacher cation of heterogeneous variances or of non-
(nested within group) as predictors of the normality in the data. The model, however,
posttest allowing for the variances to be dif- did not show any group or teacher effects,
ferent across groups. A second model was only that of the pretest. For CTOPP blending
then run that kept all the predictors in the words, there was no indication of heteroge-
model but restricted the variance to be held neous variances nor of nonnormality in the
constant across groups. The log-likelihood residuals, but the group effect was significant
differences were then tested to see which (F(1, 46) ⫽ 3.54, p ⫽ .033, one-tailed) over and
model was more appropriate for the data. above the effect of the pretest and that of
The residuals for each model were also ob- teacher nested within group. The effect size
tained and tested for normality, as this is the for the difference score in blending words
actual assumption for such models. was in the large range (d ⫽ .76).
For WJ III letter-word identification there For TPRI oral reading fluency, the vari-
was no indication of heterogeneous variances ances again appeared to be different
or nonnormality. Controlling for the pretest (␹2(1) ⫽ 6.90, p ⬍ .01) and the residual dis-
and teachers nested within group, the result tribution was determined to be nonnormal
for treatment group was significant (F(1, on all tests (Shapiro-Wilks, p ⫽ .0094). The
46) ⫽ 5.71, p ⫽ .0106, one-tailed). The effect data did not fit the pattern that might have
size for letter-word identification difference allowed a generalized model, so we ap-
scores was large (d ⫽ .91). plied a square-root transformation, which
For WJ III oral comprehension, the data removed the significant nonnormality.
indicated that the group variances were dif- However, this model did not demonstrate
ferent (␹2(1) ⫽ 18.1, p ⬍ .01). Regardless, the any significant group effects.
group differences were also significant (F(1, For the PPVT-4 GSV score, there was a
46) ⫽ 3.25, p ⫽ .039, one tailed). Normality significant difference in the models assum-
was borderline as one test (Shapiro-Wilks) ing equal versus unequal variances (␹2(1) ⫽
was significant ( p ⫽ .043), but the other three 18.1, p ⬍ .01). In addition, the residuals for
(i.e., Kolmogorov-Smirnov, p ⬎ .15; Cramer- this model showed a marked skewness

JUNE 2010

This content downloaded from 207.162.240.147 on July 24, 2016 14:02:16 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
SUMMER SCHOOL READING PILOT 435

TABLE 4. Posttest Effects for Pretest Errors, Intervention Group, and Teacher on Curriculum-Based Measures

High-Frequency- Decodable-Words
Letter-Sound CBM Words CBM CBM Vocabulary CBM
Source df F p df F p df F p df F p

Pretest 1, 41 82.11 ⬍.0001 1, 41 149.18 ⬍.0001 1, 41 50.08 ⬍.0001 1, 41 102.49 ⬍.0001


Group 1, 41 1.74 .097 1, 41 2.98 .046 1, 41 1.85 .090 1, 41 .44 .256
Teacher 4, 41 2.36 .035 4, 41 1.46 .116 4, 41 1.83 .071 4, 41 .38 .412

NOTE.—P-values reflect one-tailed tests of significance.

which was reflected in a significant lack of words (F(1, 41) ⫽ 2.98, p ⫽ .046, one-tailed).
normality detected by the Shapiro-Wilks The effects of pretest were significant for all
test ( p ⬍ .0001). However, the results were variables, and effects for teacher were sig-
primarily due to two cases, both in the nificant for letter-sounds. No effects be-
comparison group, who were substantially yond those of pretest were evident for de-
different at posttest compared to pre, but in codable words or vocabulary.
the opposite from the expected direction.
These were the same two cases with outli- Discussion
ers in the WJ III oral comprehension distri-
The purpose of this pilot study was to exam-
bution. One showed a PPVT standard score
ine the potential efficacy of a kindergarten
decline of 57 points over 4 weeks, while the
summer school reading intervention for
other lost 21 standard score points. Drop-
students attending high-poverty schools in
ping these two cases from the analysis re-
order to determine whether further ex-
moved the significant nonnormality from
perimental research of the intervention
the residual distribution; however, this did
approach was warranted. Through a quasi-
not affect the results, which showed that
experimental design, outcomes in phonemic
the pretest GSV score was related to post-
awareness, word reading, text reading flu-
test but neither teacher effects nor group
ency, listening comprehension, and vocabu-
effects were statistically significant.
lary were investigated to determine if a brief,
intensive summer school reading program
Curriculum-Based Measures could be associated with positive effects on
Since the CBMs were untimed and con- student outcomes. Results indicated that stu-
sisted of items normally taught in kinder- dents who attended the intensive summer
garten and first grade, they had the poten- reading program made significantly better
tial for ceiling effects, which were evident gains on measures of word reading and lis-
in the distributions. Therefore, using the tening comprehension when compared to
GLIMMIX procedure in SAS, we analyzed students who received typical summer
probabilities of the error frequencies on school instruction, although word reading
each measure based on a Poisson distribu- outcomes could also be attributed in part to
tion. Again, we accounted for pretest and differences between teachers. Results related
teacher effects in evaluating between- to phonological awareness were mixed, and
group effects. Table 4 presents the results. there were no significant between-group dif-
Although the treatment group made ferences detected in oral reading fluency or
larger mean gains than the comparison vocabulary. These findings are encouraging,
group on all CBMs, significant between- especially considering that past research sug-
group differences were detected in only gests that the gap in academic performance
one of the four measures after accounting between students who come from low-
for teacher and pretest: high-frequency income and higher-income backgrounds

This content downloaded from 207.162.240.147 on July 24, 2016 14:02:16 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
436 THE ELEMENTARY SCHOOL JOURNAL

tends to widen during the summer months word reading and listening comprehension
(Borman et al., 2005; Cooper et al., 1996). development are aligned with those of
The most robust effect size was associ- Schacter and Jo (2005), who found positive
ated with growth in word reading as mea- effects for a summer school program de-
sured by the WJ III. Treatment-group stu- livered to children in kindergarten and first
dents also performed significantly better grade when instruction addressed both
than comparison-group students in high- word- and text-level reading skills. For
frequency word recognition. Although stu- young students at risk for reading difficulties,
dents in the treatment classrooms outper- particularly those from low-income back-
formed comparison students in reading grounds who possibly have more limited op-
decodable words, this difference was not portunities to build wide background knowl-
statistically significant after accounting for edge, it may be particularly important to
teacher-level effects. These results are en- provide instruction in listening comprehen-
couraging, as learning to read words accu- sion in addition to word-level reading in-
rately and fluently is an essential goal of struction. More research directly examining
early reading instruction. Although some this question is warranted.
children in the treatment classrooms re- In the area of phonemic awareness, the
mained at risk for reading failure at the end treatment group had significantly better
of summer school, the instruction they re- outcomes in phoneme blending, but not in
ceived in phonics and word recognition sound matching. Nonetheless, the positive
may have created a foundation to support outcome for phoneme blending may be im-
their progress in decoding in first grade. portant. The ability to segment words into
Students’ growth in listening comprehen- smaller parts (i.e., onsets and rimes, pho-
sion in this study is also noteworthy. The nemes) and blend word parts together to
relationship between listening and reading form words has been shown to be strongly
comprehension is strong; students’ ability to related to early reading development (e.g.,
comprehend written text is similar to their Stahl & Murray, 1994), and deficits in this
comprehension of the text when it is spoken domain have been reliably associated with
(Bell & Perfetti, 1994; Gernsbacher, Varner, reading difficulties and disabilities (e.g.,
& Faust, 1990). This relationship has been Fletcher, Lyon, Fuchs, & Barnes, 2007; Vel-
confirmed by studies that demonstrate a sig- lutino, Fletcher, Snowling, & Scanlon,
nificant predictive relationship between lis- 2004). Although the treatment group had
tening comprehension and reading compre- more standard score growth than the com-
hension (Aarnoutse, van den Bous, & Brand- parison group on sound matching, the dif-
Gruwel, 1998; Garner & Bochna, 2004; Nation ference was not statistically significant. Ob-
& Snowling, 2004). Moreover, research indi- servations in comparison classrooms
cates that listening comprehension may be a indicated that these students practiced re-
precursor target skill that could be useful citing and singing rhyming poems and
for identifying students at risk for reading- songs, and direct instruction in phonologi-
comprehension failure (Carlisle & Felbinger, cal awareness was observed in one class-
1991; Catts & Hogan, 2003; Duke, Pressley, & room. This instruction may have been suf-
Hilden, 2004). The approach to comprehen- ficient to promote growth in identifying
sion instruction implemented in the current initial sounds in words.
study was also found to be efficacious in a As beginning readers learn to recognize
previous study of listening comprehension many words orally at sight and quickly de-
instruction delivered to English language code unknown words, their reading becomes
learners in kindergarten (Solari & Gerber, more fluent (Ehri, 2005). Although between-
2008) and appears to merit further study. group differences in oral reading fluency
The findings of this study in terms of were not statistically significant, comparison-

JUNE 2010

This content downloaded from 207.162.240.147 on July 24, 2016 14:02:16 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
SUMMER SCHOOL READING PILOT 437

group students made greater gains than tion in each condition limited our ability to
treatment-group students on this variable. distinguish between the effects of the inter-
Future implementation of the intervention vention and the effects of assignment to
may include more practice reading con- particular teachers. Also related to the
nected text to develop the automaticity asso- small sample size is our high attrition rate;
ciated with fluent reading (Chard, Vaughn, & many of the students we had pretested and
Tyler, 2002). assigned to the treatment or comparison
There is evidence that vocabulary devel- conditions at the end of the regular school
opment can be promoted in young children year did not attend summer school and
when teachers use an interactional read- were subsequently unavailable for assess-
aloud style and extended book discussion to ment, while several others left before the
decontextualize language (e.g., Dickinson & end of the summer school term and thus
Smith, 1993), and other research has demon- lacked posttest data. Equally important, the
strated significant effects for approaches to randomization of students to classrooms
vocabulary instruction similar to the one im- was compromised by the schools. These
plemented in the current study (e.g., Elley, factors may have biased our results, al-
1989; Wasik & Bond, 2001). However, stu- though schools’ apparently nonrandom
dents who attended the treatment classrooms placement of newly arriving students into
did not differ significantly from comparison- classrooms probably resulted in a more
group students on the PPVT-4 or on the challenging test of the intervention since
vocabulary CBM. This may be due to the students placed in treatment classrooms
short duration of summer school; however, were more impaired than those placed in
even with a short-term intervention, one comparison classrooms. However, the vio-
would expect some differential movement of lation of randomization limits the general-
treatment-group students on the CBM of vo- izability of our results. Other limitations
cabulary words that were specifically taught related to study implementation are (a) the
in the program. The relatively high perfor- experimental condition incorporated small-
mance of both groups at pretest on the group instruction provided by paraprofes-
vocabulary CBM suggests that the lack of sionals, while paraprofessionals were not
differences could be due to the fact that available to the comparison-group teach-
many children in both groups already ers, and (b) interrater reliability for class-
knew the meanings of the instructed room observations was not established
words. An alternative explanation is that since only one person observed each inter-
the research curriculum introduced words vention component (i.e., one person ob-
at too quick a pace (three new words per served basic reading instruction, another
day) and that students were unable to use observed comprehension, and a third ob-
the words to the extent necessary to de- served vocabulary) and only one person
velop an understanding of them and re- observed in the comparison classrooms.
member their meanings. Both possibilities
should be considered prior to beginning an
efficacy study of this intervention. Implications for Research and Practice
In spite of these limitations, this pilot
study demonstrates that larger-scale exper-
Limitations of the Pilot Study imental research of similar interventions is
The results of this pilot study are not warranted. Summer school provides edu-
necessarily generalizable to other student cators with an important opportunity to
populations. The study was limited by its provide instruction that may have positive
small sample size. In particular, the fact effects on reading outcomes for students
that only three teachers provided instruc- from low-income families. As it is currently

This content downloaded from 207.162.240.147 on July 24, 2016 14:02:16 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
438 THE ELEMENTARY SCHOOL JOURNAL

implemented, summer school instruction Catts, H. W., & Hogan, T. P. (2003). Language
offered to primary-grade students often basis of reading disabilities and implications
for early identification and remediation.
consists of low-intensity activities that are
Reading Psychology, 24, 223–246.
not guided by clear instructional objectives. Chard, D. J., Vaughn, S., & Tyler, B. (2002). A
Often, summer school programs fail to uti- synthesis of research on effective interven-
lize research-based curricular programs tions for building reading fluency with ele-
(Heyns, 1987; Roderick et al., 1999). Requir- mentary students with learning disabilities.
ing kindergarten and first-grade students Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 386 – 406.
Cooper, H., Charlton, K., Valentine, J. C., & Mu-
who are at risk for reading difficulties to hlenbruck, L. (2000). Making the most of
attend summer school and using that time summer school: A meta-analytic and narra-
to provide instruction similar to that pro- tive review. Monographs of the Society for Re-
vided in this study may help narrow the search in Child Development, 65, 1–130.
persistent gap in reading outcomes be- Cooper, H., Nye, B., Charlton, K., Lindsay, J., &
Greathouse, S. (1996). The effects of summer
tween children from low- and higher- vacation on achievement scores: A narrative
income backgrounds. This question merits and meta-analytic review. Review of Educa-
further empirical study. tional Research, 66, 227–268.
Dickinson, D. K., & Smith, M. W. (1993). Long-
Note term effects of preschool teachers’ book
readings on low-income children’s vocabu-
lary and story comprehension. Reading Re-
Correspondence concerning this article search Quarterly, 29, 104 –123.
should be addressed to Carolyn Denton, Chil- Duke, N. K., Pressley, M., & Hilden, K. (2004).
dren’s Learning Institute, 7000 Fannin St., UCT Difficulties with reading comprehension. In
2443, Houston, TX 77030. E-mail: Carolyn. C. A. Stone, R. Stillman, B. J. Ehren, & K.
A.Denton@uth.tmc.edu. Apel (Eds.), Handbook of language and literacy
(pp. 501–520). New York: Guildford.
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). Peabody
References
Picture Vocabulary Test (4th ed.). Minneapo-
lis: Pearson Assessments.
Aarnoutse, C. A. J., van den Bos, K. P., & Brand- Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to read words: The-
Gruwel, S. (1998). Effects of listening com- ory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of
prehension training on listening and read- Reading, 9, 167–188.
ing. Journal of Special Education, 32, 115–126. Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Stahl, S. A., & Willows,
Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Olson, L. S. D. M. (2001). Systematic phonics instruction
(2001). Schools, achievement, and inequal- helps students learn to read: Evidence from
ity: A seasonal perspective. Educational Eval- the National Reading Panel’s meta-analysis.
uation and Policy Analysis, 23(2), 171–191. Review of Educational Research, 71(3), 393– 447.
Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Olson, L. S. Elley, W. B. (1989). Vocabulary acquisition from
(2007). Lasting consequences of the summer listening to stories. Reading Research Quar-
learning gap. American Sociological Review, terly, 24, 174 –187.
72, 167–180. Fletcher, J. M., Lyon, G. R., Fuchs, L. S., & Barnes,
Bell, L. C., & Perfetti, C. A. (1994). Reading skill: M. A. (2007). Learning disabilities: From identifi-
Some adult comparisons. Journal of Educa- cation to intervention. New York: Guildford.
tional Psychology, 86, 244 –255. Foorman, B. R., Fletcher, J. M., & Francis, D. J.
Bishop, D. V., & Adams, C. (1990). A prospective (2004). Early reading assessment. In W. Ev-
study of the relationship between specific ers & H. J. Walberg (Eds.). Testing student
language impairment, phonological disor- learning, evaluating teaching effectiveness (pp.
der and reading retardation. Journal of Child 81–125). Stanford, CA: Hoover Press.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 31(7), 1027–1050. Foorman, B. R., & Torgesen, J. K. (2001). Critical
Borman, G. D., Benson, J., & Overman, L. T. elements of classroom and small-group in-
(2005). Families, schools, and summer learn- struction promote reading success in all chil-
ing. Elementary School Journal, 106, 131–150. dren. Learning Disabilities Research and Prac-
Carlisle, J. F., & Felbinger, L. (1991). Profiles in tice, 16(4), 202–211.
listening and reading comprehension. Jour- Francis, D. J., Shaywitz, S. E., Stuebing, K. K.,
nal of Educational Research, 84, 345–354. Shaywitz, B. A., & Fletcher, J. M. (1996). De-

JUNE 2010

This content downloaded from 207.162.240.147 on July 24, 2016 14:02:16 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).
SUMMER SCHOOL READING PILOT 439

velopmental lag versus deficit models of & Allensworth, E. (1999). Ending social promo-
reading disability: A longitudinal, individ- tion: Results from the first two years. Chicago:
ual growth curves analysis. Journal of Educa- Consortium on Chicago Public Schools.
tional Psychology, 88, 3–17. Scarborough, H. S. (1989). Prediction of reading
Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Hosp, M. K., & Jenkins, disability from familial and individual dif-
J. R. (2001). Oral reading fluency as an indi- ferences. Journal of Educational Psychology,
cator of reading competence: A theoretical, 81(1), 101–108.
empirical, and historical analysis. Scientific Schacter, J., & Jo, B. (2005). Learning when school
Studies of Reading, 5, 239 –256. is not in session: A reading summer day-camp
Garner, J. K., & Bochna, C. R. (2004). Transfer of intervention to improve the achievement of
a listening comprehension strategy to inde- exiting first-grade students who are economi-
pendent reading in first-grade students. cally disadvantaged. Journal of Research in
Early Childhood Education Journal, 32, 69 –74. Reading, 28(2), 158 –169.
Gernsbacher, M. A., Varner, K. R., & Faust, M. E. Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.).
(1990). Investigating differences in general (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young
comprehension skill. Journal of Experimental children. Washington, DC: National Acad-
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, emy Press.
16, 430 – 445. Solari, E. J., & Gerber, M. M. (2008). Early com-
Heyns, B. (1987). Schooling and cognitive devel- prehension instruction for Spanish-speaking
opment: Is there a season for learning? Child English language learners: Teaching text-
Development, 58, 151–160. level reading skills while maintaining effects
Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A on word-level skills. Learning Disabilities Re-
longitudinal study of 54 children from first search and Practice, 23(4), 155–168.
through fourth grades. Journal of Educational Stahl, S. A., & Murray, B. A. (1994). Defining
Psychology, 80, 437– 447. phonological awareness and its relationship
Lauer, P. A., Akiba, M., Wilkerson, S. B., Apthorp, to early reading. Journal of Educational Psy-
H. S., Snow, D., & Martin-Glenn, M. L. (2006). chology, 86, 221–234.
Out-of-school-time programs: A meta-analysis Torgesen, J. K. (2000). Individual differences in
of effects for at-risk students. Review of Educa- response to early interventions in reading:
tional Research, 76, 275–313. The lingering problem of treatment resisters.
Lee, J., Grigg, W., & Donahue, P. (2007). The Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 15,
nation’s report card: Reading 2007 (NCES 55– 64.
2007-496). Washington, DC: National Center Torgesen, J. K., & Burgess, S. R. (1998). Consis-
for Education Statistics, Institute of Educa- tency of reading-related phonological pro-
tion Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. cesses throughout early childhood: Evidence
Retrieved March 10, 2009, from http:// from longitudinal-correlational and instruc-
nationsreportcard.gov tional studies. In J. Metsala & L. Ehri (Eds.),
Mathes, P. G., & Denton, C. A. (2002). The preven- Word recognition in beginning reading (pp.
tion and identification of reading disability. 161–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Seminars in Pediatric Neurology, 9(3), 185–191. Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J.,
Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Beyond & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading
phonological skills: Broader language skills disability (dyslexia): What have we learned
contribute to the development of reading. in the past four decades? Journal of Child
Journal of Reading, 27, 342–356. Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 2– 40.
National Reading Panel (2000). Teaching children Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A.
to read: An evidence-based assessment of the sci- (1999). Comprehensive test of phonological pro-
entific research literature on reading and its im- cessing. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
plications for reading instruction. Washington, Wasik, B. A., & Bond, M. A. (2001). Beyond the
DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. pages of a book: Interactive book reading
Rayner, K., Foorman, B. R., Perfetti, C. A., Pe- and language development in preschool
setsky, D., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2001). How classrooms. Journal of Educational Psychology,
psychological science informs the teaching 93, 243–250.
of reading. Psychological Science in the Public Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N.
Interest, 2, 31–74. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson III tests of achieve-
Roderick, M., Bryk, A. S., Jacob, B. A., Easton, J. Q., ment. Itasca, IL: Riverside.

This content downloaded from 207.162.240.147 on July 24, 2016 14:02:16 PM


All use subject to University of Chicago Press Terms and Conditions (http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/t-and-c).

You might also like