You are on page 1of 13

MARK R.

FOREHAND and ROHIT DESHPANDÉ*

The authors propose that “ethnic self-awareness”—a temporary state


during which a person is more sensitive to information related to his or her
own ethnicity—moderates consumer response to targeted advertising.
Ethnic self-awareness occurs when a person engages in a process of
self-categorization and uses ethnic criteria as the basis for this catego-
rization. The authors hypothesize that “ethnic primes”—visual or verbal
cues that draw attention to ethnicity—direct self-categorization and in-
crease ethnic self-awareness. To test these hypotheses, the authors con-
duct two experiments. Using 109 Asian and Caucasian participants, Ex-
periment 1 assessed the impact of exposure to an Asian ethnic prime on
ethnic self-awareness and on response to targeted television advertising.
Exposure to an ethnic prime increased the rate at which participants
spontaneously mentioned their ethnicity in self-descriptions (a measure
of ethnic self-awareness) and caused participants to respond more fa-
vorably to same-ethnicity spokespeople and advertising that targeted
their ethnicity. Experiment 2 tested the theory in a print advertising con-
text and extended the design by manipulating the type of ethnic prime
participants saw (Asian or Caucasian) and the market that the focal ad-
vertisement targeted (Asian or Caucasian). Experiment 2 replicated the
findings of Experiment 1 when the focal advertisement targeted
Asians, but not when it targeted Caucasians.

What We See Makes Us Who We Are:


Priming Ethnic Self-Awareness and
Advertising Response

The power of ethnicity as a targeting variable is demon- featuring ethnic actors may no longer do so in today’s multi-
strated implicitly by its frequent use in advertising and ex- cultural environment. At a conscious level, increased ethnic
plicitly by a wealth of prior research that documents positive presence in advertising may reduce the importance con-
consumer response to advertising that features similar-ethnicity sumers place on ethnicity. The prevalence of ethnic actors in
actors or spokespeople (for a review, see Whittler 1989). advertising may also influence unconscious processing in
However, much of the evidence for an ethnicity-based target consumers by simply reducing the salience of ethnicity. For
market effect was collected during an era in which the use of example, although nearly all consumers are aware that
ethnic actors in television advertising was highly unusual Michael Jordan is an African American, his status as cultural
and therefore more salient to the consumer than it otherwise icon often supercedes his status as an African American.
might have been. In comparison, the prevalence of ethnic ac- These developments raise the question whether the use of
tors in current advertising has increased to the point that ethnic actors is perceived by consumers as a targeting tech-
some ethnic groups are overrepresented in advertising rela- nique or, more important, whether such techniques really in-
tive to their composition in the general population (Taylor fluence consumer reactions to advertising to the extent sug-
and Stern 1997; Wilkes and Valencia 1989). As a result, ad- gested by previous research.
vertisements that once drew attention to ethnicity by simply In this project, we propose that the use of ethnic actors as
a targeting technique has little effect on consumer evaluation
of advertising unless the consumer processes the ethnic in-
*Mark Forehand is Assistant Professor of Marketing, University of formation and relates it to his or her self-concept. The like-
Washington Business School (e-mail: forehand@u.washington.edu). Rohit
Deshpandé is Sebastian S. Kresge Professor of Marketing, Harvard Busi-
lihood that a consumer will process ethnic information in
ness School (e-mail: rdeshpande@hbs.edu). advertising is hypothesized to be driven by what we have
termed “ethnic self-awareness”—a temporary state during

Journal of Marketing Research


Vol. XXXVIII (August 2001), 336–348 336
Priming Ethnic Self-Awareness and Advertising Response 337

which a person is more sensitive to information related to the momentary salience of any particular membership is
his or her ethnicity. We propose that ethnic self-awareness highly variable. The probability that a person is aware of a
can be aroused by many factors, including individual differ- particular membership is influenced by the hierarchical or-
ence variables, situational factors, and other contextual or dering of the person’s identities (Burke and Reitzes 1991),
stimulus primes in the environment. how strongly the membership is associated within the per-
Although previous research has analyzed the impact of both son’s self-schema (Markus 1977), and the extent to which
individual difference variables (Deshpandé, Hoyer, and Don- the membership is activated in the social context (Markus
thu 1986) and situational factors (Stayman and Deshpandé and Herzog 1995). Therefore, self-categorization will lead
1989) on consumer response to ethnic information in market- to ethnic self-awareness only when the categorization
ing, little attention has been directed toward the impact of con- process is oriented along ethnic criteria.
textual or stimulus factors (for a notable exception, see Wooten Although rarely mentioned explicitly, the influence of eth-
and Galvin 1993). The lack of research focused on contextual nic self-awareness is apparent in much of the previous research
variables is unfortunate given that they may be of greater prac- on ethnic targeting. For example, recent research has found
tical value to marketers than either situational or individual that both felt similarity to an ethnic spokesperson and the per-
difference variables. First, because consumers are often alone ception of being targeted by an advertisement are important
when exposed to advertisements, nonsocial ethnic primes may mediators of consumer response to ethnic advertising (Aaker,
be a more frequent cause of ethnic self-awareness than their Brumbaugh, and Grier 1999). Both inherently involve some
social counterparts. Second, marketers have much more con- recognition of the match between the endorser’s characteristics
trol over stimulus factors than they do over social or individual and the consumer’s characteristics. As such, factors that in-
difference factors. Compared with controlling a consumer’s crease ethnic self-awareness should increase the likelihood that
social situation, it is relatively easy to manage whether ethnic a consumer will feel similar (or dissimilar) to an ethnic en-
primes are included in a message or whether an advertisement dorser and thereby feel targeted (or not targeted) by an adver-
is presented in a context in which ethnic primes are frequent. tisement that features the ethnic endorser. More direct evidence
To delineate the effect of ethnic self-awareness on response of the power of ethnic self-awareness is provided by research
to ethnic advertising, we divide this article into three sections. that has found that consumer response to ethnic advertising is
In the first section, we explicate the ethnic self-awareness influenced by the context in which the advertising is placed
concept and discuss the individual differences and situational (Wooten and Galvin 1993). Specifically, these authors found
determinants that influence it. In the second section, we pro- that African American participants responded more positively
pose an important source of ethnic self-awareness, advertis- to an advertisement that targeted African Americans if the ad-
ing-based ethnic priming, and present hypotheses related to vertisement was embedded in a mock newspaper article that
it. In the third section, we report two experiments that test the discussed ethnic issues (as opposed to being embedded in an
effect of advertising-based ethnic primes on consumer re- article that did not discuss ethnic issues). Our focus in this ar-
sponse to ethnically targeted advertising. ticle is to better understand the process by which consumers
become ethnically self-aware and to analyze how this aware-
ETHNIC SELF-AWARENESS ness influences their subsequent response to ethnic advertising.
As stated previously, the current research is built from a
concept we call ethnic self-awareness—a temporary state DETERMINANTS OF ETHNIC SELF-AWARENESS
during which a person is more sensitive to his or her ethnic- One class of factors that influences ethnic self-awareness
ity. This is akin to the notion of felt ethnicity that was intro- is individual difference variables. Most notable among these
duced by Stayman and Deshpandé (1989). These authors dis- variables is consumers’ strength of ethnic identification—
tinguished felt ethnicity from objective ethnic membership people’s enduring association with their ethnic background.
by arguing that felt ethnicity is “not just who one is, but how Although people’s strength of ethnic identification is shaped
one feels in and about a situation” (Stayman and Deshpandé by their prior social and cultural experiences, this variable is
1989, p. 361). Similarly, we propose that ethnic self-aware- nonetheless fairly stable and enduring (Stayman and Desh-
ness is a state that is dependent on many factors beyond sim- pandé 1989). Research has found that strength of ethnic
ple ethnic classification. However, the present project departs identification affects the amount of attention consumers give
from previous research by arguing that ethnic self-awareness to ethnic information, the probability that consumers will
can be primed by factors in advertising that are separate and purchase ethnic products, and the response of consumers to
distinct from the consumer’s consumption situation (the fo- advertising featuring ethnic actors (Deshpandé, Hoyer, and
cus of the previous research on felt ethnicity). Donthu 1986; Ellis et al. 1985; Hirschman 1981; Saenz and
In general, we posit here that ethnic self-awareness occurs Aguirre 1991; Williams and Qualls 1989). Moreover, the
when a person is prompted to categorize him- or herself importance of strength of ethnic identification has been
along ethnic criteria and that this ethnically based catego- demonstrated across a variety of ethnicities, including
rization may be elicited by the person’s enduring traits or so- African Americans (Williams and Qualls 1989), Asian
cial situation or by other environmental cues. Self-catego- Americans (Ellis et al. 1985), Hispanics (Deshpandé, Hoyer
rization is a spontaneous and often unconscious process that and Donthu 1986; Saenz and Aguirre 1991), and people of
occurs when people compare themselves with others and as- Jewish heritage (Hirschman 1981).
sess their relative similarity or dissimilarity. For any given Although strength of ethnic identification should be cor-
person, group categorization may occur on the basis of related with consumer ethnic self-awareness, the two con-
many different variables, including ethnicity, gender, age, structs are durationally different. Strength of ethnic identifi-
occupation, and economic status. Although each of these cation is an enduring association between people’s sense of
group memberships is part of the person’s identity or “self,” self and their ethnicity, whereas ethnic self-awareness is the
338 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, AUGUST 2001

momentary salience of people’s ethnic group memberships. other ethnicity-oriented issues and found that exposure to
As such, ethnic self-awareness exists only when people the articles influenced predicted consumption patterns and
process ethnic information and categorize themselves along attitudes toward subsequent targeted advertising. By direct-
ethnic criteria. At any given moment, strong ethnic identi- ing conscious attention to these ethnic issues, the re-
fiers are more likely to be ethnically self-aware than are searchers uncovered effects that had previously been found
weak ethnic identifiers, but possessing strong identification only through manipulations of the social situation or
with an ethnic group in no way necessitates ethnic self- through measurement of preexisting individual differences.
awareness. Instead, ethnic self-awareness often is elicited by The present research argues that these findings constitute a
factors external to the person, and though it may be easier to third method by which ethnic self-awareness can be elicited:
elicit ethnic self-awareness in a strong identifier, a strong contextual or stimulus primes.
identifier is not continuously ethnically self-aware. Moreover, we argue that these contextual or stimulus
A second factor that may influence ethnic self-awareness primes may affect ethnic self-awareness even when no overt
is the ethnic composition of a person’s social environment processing of the ethnic information occurs. It is possible
(McGuire et al. 1978). McGuire and colleagues (1978) pro- that simple cues in the advertising context could prime eth-
pose that particular group memberships are salient to a per- nic self-awareness without the directed attention to ethnic
son to the extent that the person’s membership in that group information found in Stayman and Deshpandé’s (1989) or
is distinctive in the social environment. As a result, minori- Wooten and Galvin’s (1993) work. For example, recent re-
ties will be especially attuned to their ethnicity when they search on multicultural identities has demonstrated that
are surrounded by a dissimilar majority (as compared with identities can be primed by visual images or words (Hong et
situations in which they are surrounded by like minorities), al. 1999). Specifically, the attribution styles of “multicul-
and this will increase the probability that they will sponta- tural” Asian students in Hong Kong became more Western if
neously mention their ethnicity in self-descriptions. the students were first exposed to American cultural icons
Within marketing, Deshpandé and Stayman (1994) have (U.S. flag, Superman) and more Eastern if the students were
applied McGuire and colleagues’ distinctiveness theory to first exposed to Chinese cultural icons (a Chinese Dragon,
an advertising context in a study of spokesperson effective- The Great Wall). These effects of exposure to ethnic icons
ness. The authors recruited participants from the towns of on attributional style were argued to occur because the icons
Austin and San Antonio, Tex., two cities that are similar increase awareness of one ethnicity over others (in a multi-
with the notable exception of their ethnic makeup (the ma- cultural person), presumably by activating nodes in the stu-
jority of Austin residents are Anglo, whereas the majority of dents’ semantic networks related to a specific ethnicity
San Antonio residents are Hispanic). As a result, the experi- (Hong et al. 1999; Wyer and Srull 1986).
menters were provided with participants whose ethnicity In this project, we propose that exposure to what we have
was distinctive (Hispanics in Austin and Anglos in San An- termed ethnic primes—visual or verbal cues that draw at-
tonio) and participants whose ethnicity was not distinctive tention to ethnicity—will influence ethnic self-awareness by
(Anglos in Austin and Hispanics in San Antonio). In addi- encouraging unconscious ethnic processing and categoriza-
tion to replicating the findings of McGuire and colleagues tion. Because of their influence on ethnic self-awareness, we
(1978), Deshpandé and Stayman (1994) found that partici- expect that ethnic primes will influence response to targeted
pants whose ethnicity was distinctive attributed significantly advertising in much the same way that social distinctiveness
more credibility to same-ethnicity spokespersons and that does. Specifically, we expect that ethnic primes will inten-
perceived credibility then mediated subsequent brand atti- sify consumer reaction to same-ethnicity actors. To the ex-
tudes. More recent research has expanded on this effect and tent that the depiction of same-ethnicity actors is positive
argued that distinctiveness is influenced not only by numeric and generally well received, this intensification should lead
prevalence but also by the social status of the group (Grier to more positive reactions to both the spokesperson and the
and Deshpandé 2001). advertising in which the spokesperson is featured.
A consumer’s immediate social situation also affects eth- In summary, the current study differs from previous work
nic self-awareness. Stayman and Deshpandé (1989) and on situational ethnicity and strength of ethnic identification
Wooten (1995) find that ethnic consumers are more likely to in its focus. In previous work on situational ethnicity, the sit-
consume ethnic products when the social context includes uation was defined as “something outside the basic tenden-
other people of the same ethnicity than when the social con- cies and characteristics of the individual and beyond the
text is integrated and includes people of many different eth- characteristics of the stimulus object to be acted upon”
nic groups. Research has also demonstrated that even weak (Belk 1974). Rather than study the social situation, we in-
identifiers may evaluate members of their social groups stead focus on ethnic primes in the advertising context,
more favorably if the social situation induces or enhances which is part of the stimulus itself. The current project is
their group memberships (Cota and Dion 1986; Gerard and also distinguishable from research on strength of ethnic
Hoyt 1974). identification in its focus on a transitory state rather than an
Although previous research has conceptualized ethnic enduring individual association with an ethnic membership.
self-awareness as a by-product of individual differences or As a result, stimulus-based priming completes a triad of fac-
the social environment, there is no reason to expect that eth- tors that influence ethnic self-awareness: the individual, the
nic self-awareness could not also be elicited by any number situation, and the stimulus.
of factors that are neither internal to the individual nor so-
cial. In their research on felt ethnicity, both Stayman and RESEARCH HYPOTHESES
Deshpandé (1989) and Wooten and Galvin (1993) manipu- We propose that ethnic primes in media will elicit ethnic
lated exposure to articles that discussed discrimination or self-awareness in consumers who are exposed to the primes.
Priming Ethnic Self-Awareness and Advertising Response 339

To measure ethnic self-awareness, we used the spontaneous vidual differences in strength of ethnic identification (Desh-
self-description method that was first developed to study the pandé, Hoyer, and Donthu 1986). Weak identifiers are less
effects of societal distinctiveness by McGuire and col- likely to show a positive bias than strong identifiers and, in
leagues (1978). This measure is particularly sensitive to un- extreme cases, it is possible that weak identifiers may wish
conscious influences on ethnic self-awareness because it al- to disassociate themselves from their group and therefore
lows subjects to describe themselves along any dimensions respond negatively to other in-group members. A second
they believe are relevant and does not explicitly question cause of variability is the subjective construal of whether a
subjects about their ethnic memberships. Although ethnic given ethnic depiction is stereotypical or derogatory. To the
primes should influence the ethnic self-awareness of all con- extent that consumers perceive a depiction to be stereotypi-
sumers who are exposed to the prime, the effect should be cal, they may respond less positively to the targeted adver-
particularly dramatic for consumers whose ethnicity tising than if there was no ethnic presence in the advertising
matches the ethnicity used in the ethnic prime. A great deal at all (Weigl, Loomis, and Soja 1980). Given that increasing
of research has demonstrated that people pay more attention people’s ethnic self-awareness should intensify their reac-
to and demonstrate quicker recall of trait information that is tions to ethnically based advertising, ethnic self-awareness
consistent with their self-schema (Markus and Sentis 1982). should cause consumers to respond even more negatively to
More recently, the tendency to process self-relevant infor- stereotypical advertising than they otherwise would. Al-
mation differentially has been shown for traits as diverse as though these two factors increase the variability of response
gender (Skitka and Maslach 1996), mathematical aptitude within the target market, the domain of analysis in this proj-
(Lips 1995), and a variety of personality variables (Fekken ect is limited to advertising that is not generally perceived to
and Holden 1992). Because of this heightened responsive- be stereotypical or derogatory. As a result, ethnic self-aware-
ness to self-relevant information, consumers with an ethnic- ness should have largely positive outcomes on consumer
ity that matches the ethnicity used in the ethnic prime (eth- evaluation of same-ethnicity spokespeople and advertising.
nic prime congruent) are expected to demonstrate greater Just as ethnic self-awareness may improve reaction to
increases in ethnic self-awareness after prime exposure than same-ethnicity actors and spokespeople, it may have a detri-
are consumers with an ethnicity that does not match the eth- mental effect on the evaluation of different-ethnicity actors
nicity used in the ethnic prime (ethnic prime incongruent). and spokespeople. Consumers who have been ethnically
H1: Exposure to an ethnic prime should increase ethnic self- primed are more likely to perceive actors of a different eth-
awareness among all consumers, and this positive effect nicity to be members of an out-group, which can lead to less
should be more pronounced when the consumer’s ethnicity positive reactions. However, following the tenets of ethno-
is congruent with the ethnicity featured in the ethnic prime centrism, it is likely that the strength of people’s negative re-
than when the two ethnicities are incongruent. actions to an out-group are much weaker than the strength of
Prior research has found that when social context height- their positive reactions to the in-group (LeVine and Camp-
ens ethnic self-awareness, consumers respond more posi- bell 1972; Sumner 1906). In addition, the likelihood of ob-
tively to same-ethnicity spokespersons (Deshpandé and serving negative reactions to an out-group depends greatly
Stayman 1994). A similar effect is expected when ethnic on the evaluator’s prior attitude toward the out-group (Bhat,
primes are used to increase ethnic self-awareness. Ethnic Leigh, and Wardlow 1996; Grier and Brumbaugh 1999).
self-awareness leads to more positive evaluations of same- Given these caveats, it is likely that the negative effects of
ethnicity actors and spokespeople because it highlights the priming ethnic self-awareness on the evaluation of different-
similarity between the consumer and the spokesperson and ethnicity actors will be weaker than the positive effects of
thereby increases the likelihood that the consumer will clas- priming ethnic self-awareness on the evaluation of same-
sify the spokesperson as a member of his or her “in-group” ethnicity actors.
(a classification of similar others). When same-ethnicity ac- H2: After exposure to an ethnic prime, consumers will demon-
tors are classified into the consumer’s in-group, a host of strate more (less) favorable attitudes toward same-ethnicity
positive biases toward the actors should follow. For exam- (different-ethnicity) actors and spokespeople who are fea-
ple, prior research has found that compared with out-group tured in ethnically targeted advertisements.
members, in-group members are perceived to be more inter-
esting and varied (Linville, Fischer, and Salovey 1989) yet Prior research has demonstrated that consumers label
more similar to the evaluator (Taylor et al. 1978). Similar spokespeople as being “like me” or “not like me” depending
others also are usually liked more (Neimeyer and Mitchell on their felt similarity with the spokesperson, and con-
1988) and found more persuasive (Berscheid 1966) than dis- sumers also label products as being “for me” or “not for me”
similar others. Evidence that ethnic primes should magnify depending on whether the products are perceived to be tar-
these positive effects is indirectly provided by research that geted toward them (Aaker, Brumbaugh, and Grier 1999). By
demonstrates that evaluation of in-group members improves drawing attention to ethnicity, ethnic primes should increase
when attention is focused on the evaluator’s group member- the likelihood that consumers will feel targeted by adver-
ships (Abrams 1985). This suggests that consumers should tisements that feature same-ethnicity actors and spokespeo-
respond more favorably to same-ethnicity actors in advertis- ple and decrease the likelihood that consumers will feel tar-
ing after exposure to an ethnic prime. geted by advertisements that feature different ethnicity
It should be noted that though it is predicted that people actors and spokespeople.
will evaluate in-group members more positively than out- H3: After exposure to an ethnic prime, consumers are more
group members, the strength of this positive effect is vari- (less) likely to feel targeted by an advertisement that features
able within the group. One reason for this variability is indi- actors or spokespeople of the same (different) ethnicity.
340 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, AUGUST 2001

The effects of exposure to ethnic primes also should be Stimuli. The videotape stimuli presented a series of televi-
demonstrated on evaluations of targeted advertisements and sion advertisements embedded between two news segments.
the favorableness of cognitive responses to targeted adver- The news segments originally aired on a local news broadcast
tisements. To the extent that the advertising becomes associ- and contained general information about local traffic and
ated with the actors and spokespeople featured in it, it can weather. The series of advertisements presented between the
become an extension of the in-group (or out-group) itself. two news segments contained an ethnically targeted adver-
Recent research on target marketing has found that both the tisement for Nokia cellular telephones and three filler adver-
perception of being targeted by an advertisement and felt tisements (all for consumer nondurables). The Nokia cellular
similarity with models in advertisements mediate consumer telephone advertisement was classified as an Asian-targeted
reactions to the advertisements themselves (Aaker, Brum- advertisement because it featured only Asian actors and ac-
baugh, and Grier 1999). As a result, ethnic primes that in- tresses. The Nokia advertisement was selected because it
fluence the degree to which consumers like ethnic actors originally aired in Singapore (in English) and was never aired
(H2) and the degree to which consumers feel targeted (or not in the local U.S. market, thereby limiting any potential pre-
targeted) by ethnically targeted advertisements (H3) should exposure to the advertisement by the participants.
also influence consumer evaluation of the advertisements In the ethnic prime condition, one additional advertisement
themselves. was included that was expected to prime ethnic awareness.
H4: After exposure to an ethnic prime, members of the target
This advertisement also originally aired in Singapore and fea-
market (nontarget market) will demonstrate (a) more (less) tured Vidal Sassoon hair care products. Although both the
favorable attitudes toward the targeted advertisements, (b) Nokia advertisement and the Vidal Sassoon advertisement
an increase (decrease) in the number of positive cognitive would be considered ethnically targeted advertisements in the
responses to the targeted advertisements, and (c) a decrease United States, the Vidal Sassoon advertisement contained a
(increase) in the number of negative cognitive responses to more direct ethnic prime. This ethnic prime was an express
the targeted advertisements. statement (provided verbally and in text) that Vidal Sassoon
products were “for Asian Hair.” This statement served as an
EXPERIMENT 1
ethnic prime because it primed self-categorization along an
Method ethnic dimension. The direct statement of the product’s de-
Overview and design. The goals of the Experiment 1 were sired audience encourages consumers, perhaps unconsciously,
to assess whether execution elements in advertising can in- to identify the product as being “for me” or “not for me.”
crease ethnic self-awareness in consumers and to measure The final ordering of advertisements in the no ethnic
what impact ethnic self-awareness has on consumer re- prime condition was as follows: Filler Ad 1, Targeted Ad
sponse to an ethnically targeted advertisement. The design (Nokia), Filler Ad 2, Filler Ad 3. In the ethnic prime condi-
of Experiment 1 was a 2 levels of target market membership tion, the ethnic prime advertisement (Vidal Sassoon) was
(target market, Asian, or nontarget market, Caucasian) × 2 placed in the second position, creating the following order-
levels of ethnic prime (presence or absence of prior adver- ing: Filler Ad 1, Ethnic Prime Ad (Vidal Sassoon), Targeted
tisement that primes ethnic self-awareness) between-partic- Ad (Nokia), Filler Ad 2, Filler Ad 3.
ipants factorial. Measures. The first page of the questionnaire asked some
Participants. At a major West Coast university, 109 un- general questions about the newscast and the news anchors,
dergraduate students participated as part of a class require- supporting the television media cover story. This cover story
ment. Of these, 52 students self-reported their ethnicity as was used to direct some attention away from the advertise-
Asian, and 57 students self-reported their ethnicity as Cau- ments to create a more externally valid representation of how
casian. The sample contained slightly more men than consumers typically view advertisements (focus of attention
women: 32 of the 57 Caucasian participants were male, and on the programming rather than the advertisements).
35 of the 52 Asian participants were male. Male and female After completing the cover story measures, participants
subjects were distributed evenly across priming conditions. were asked to “Please tell us about yourself in your own
Procedure. Three weeks before the main experiment, par- words. Please take about a minute to do so” (H1). The prob-
ticipants completed a survey that included several personal- ability that a participant would spontaneously report his or
ity scales and basic demographic items. We used the self- her ethnicity was used as the critical measure of ethnic self-
reported ethnicity measure in this initial survey to classify awareness (McGuire et al. 1978).
participants ethnically. The open-ended response measures were followed by a
During the main experiment, participants were randomly series of questions about the focal advertisement (H4). First,
assigned to either the prime or the no-prime condition. Upon five seven-point semantic differential items were used to as-
entering the lab, participants were informed that the re- sess attitude toward the target advertisement (Aad). These
searchers were “examining attitudes toward television me- items were anchored with “bad”/“good,” “dislike”/“like,”
dia” and that they would be asked to watch an eight-minute “useless”/“useful,” “uninformative”/“informative,” and “un-
segment from a news program and then answer some ques- pleasant”/“pleasant.” An average score across these five
tions about the program in a questionnaire packet. After the measures was calculated to provide an overall rating of atti-
initial instructions, participants were asked to direct their at- tude toward the ad (Cronbach’s α = .91). The attitude toward
tention to the front of the room, where a video monitor the ad measures were followed by a set of cognitive re-
played the videotape containing a series of commercials em- sponse measures that assessed participant reactions to the
bedded between two news segments. When the videotape advertising. Participants were asked to take a minute and
ended, participants were given a questionnaire to complete write down all their reactions to the Nokia advertisement,
that contained all dependent measures. both positive and negative. These cognitive responses were
Priming Ethnic Self-Awareness and Advertising Response 341

coded as positive, negative, or neutral. Two coders who were (A) self-reported at a higher rate than Caucasians (C) (A =
blind to the experimental hypotheses completed all coding. 38%, C = 11%; parameter estimate = 1.85; S.E. = .55, χ2 (1,
The coders demonstrated 88% agreement on their initial N = 109) = 11.38, p < .01). This main effect of ethnicity is
codings. Disagreements between the coders were resolved consistent with research on societal distinctiveness, which
through discussion. A final advertisement evaluation item argues that people in a numerical minority are more likely to
assessed whether the participants felt targeted by the adver- self-report their minority memberships than are people in a
tisement by asking the participants to rate how much they numerical majority (McGuire et al. 1978). Means for all Ex-
believed the advertisement was “intended for me” (H3). periment 1 hypotheses are provided in Table 1.
Attitudes toward the ad spokesperson were assessed using Spokesperson evaluation. H2 proposes that exposure to a
four measures of liking for the spokesperson (H2). The prior ethnic prime will improve the evaluations of the
spokesperson-liking items were seven-point semantic differ- spokesperson in a subsequent ethnically targeted advertise-
ential items anchored by “cold”/“warm,” “unlikable”/“lik- ment if the spokesperson’s ethnicity matched the con-
able,” “insincere”/“sincere,” and “unfriendly”/“friendly” sumer’s ethnicity and lower the evaluations of the
(Whittler and DiMeo 1991). These four items were averaged spokesperson if the spokesperson’s ethnicity did not match
into a single measure of spokesperson liking (Cronbach’s the consumer’s ethnicity. Because the actors featured in the
α = .94). ethnically targeted advertisement were Asian, the Asian par-
The final set of measures included a measure of the par- ticipants were classified as members of the target market,
ticipant’s strength of ethnic identification and measures of and Caucasians were classified as members of the nontarget
other demographic information. The strength of the partici- market. Support for this hypothesis would be found through
pant’s ethnic identification was measured on a nine-point an interaction of ethnic prime with target market member-
scale anchored with “very weakly” and “very strongly” ship on evaluations of the spokesperson. An analysis of vari-
(Deshpandé, Hoyer, and Donthu 1986). The use of a nine- ance (ANOVA) revealed the hypothesized target market ×
point scale for this measure is helpful, because people dis- ethnic prime interaction on evaluations of how likable the
proportionately indicate strong as opposed to weak identifi- spokesperson was (F(1,108) = 5.04, p < .03). Simple effects
cation with their groups. tests revealed that this interaction was driven by a small (dif-
ference of .85 on a seven-point scale) but significant positive
Results effect of ethnic prime on how much target market members
Ethnic self-awareness. H1 proposes that exposure to an liked the spokesperson (NEP = 4.05, EP = 4.90; F(1,51) =
ethnic prime will increase ethnic self-awareness and that the 6.69, p < .01) combined with no significant effect of ethnic
effect of the prime will be moderated by the ethnicity of the prime for nontarget market members (NEP = 4.22, EP =
participant. Consumers with an ethnicity that matched the 4.05; F(1,56) = .29, p > .20). H2 thus received partial sup-
ethnicity used in the ethnic prime (ethnic prime congruent) port, as there was a small, significant, positive effect of an
were predicted to demonstrate greater increases in ethnic ethnic prime on the responses of target market members, but
self-awareness after prime exposure than consumers whose not on nontarget market members.
ethnicity did not match the ethnicity used in the ethnic prime Perception of being targeted. By drawing attention to eth-
(ethnic prime incongruent). Results from a logistic regres- nicity, ethnic primes should increase the likelihood that con-
sion provided partial support for this hypothesis. As pre- sumers will feel targeted by advertisements that feature
dicted, participants who were exposed to the ethnic prime same-ethnicity actors and spokespeople and decrease the
(EP) were more likely to self-report their ethnicity than par- likelihood that consumers will feel targeted by advertise-
ticipants not exposed to the ethnic prime (NEP) (EP = 36%, ments that feature different-ethnicity actors and spokespeo-
NEP = 13%). In the logistic regression, the parameter esti- ple. This hypothesized interaction of prime and ethnicity
mate for this ethnic prime effect was 1.57 (standard error (H3) was found on ratings of how much the advertisement
[S.E.] = .53, χ2 (1, N = 109) = 8.63, p < .01). However, the was “intended for me” (F(1,108) = 15.49, p < .001). The
hypothesized interaction between participant ethnicity and prime significantly reduced Caucasians’ perception of being
prime exposure on ethnic self-awareness was not found (p > targeted (NEP = 5.59, EP = 4.21; F(1,56) = 6.62, p < .01)
.20). There was also an unhypothesized main effect of eth- and significantly increased Asians’ perception of being tar-
nicity on tendency to self-report ethnic information: Asians geted (NEP = 3.85, EP = 5.52; F(1,51) = 8.92, p < .01).

Table 1
EXPERIMENT 1 CELL MEANS (S.E.) AND ANOVA RESULTS

ANOVA Results:
Asian Caucasian Ethnicity × Prime
Participant Ethnicity (Ethnic Prime Congruent) (Ethnic Prime Incongruent) Interaction
Ethnic Prime No Yes No Yes F p
H1: Ethnic self-report percentage 22 56 3 18 — —
H2: Spokesperson liking 4.05 (.28) 4.90 (.18) 4.22 (.21) 4.05 (.23) 5.04 .03
H3: Felt targetedness 3.85 (.38) 5.52 (.42) 5.59 (.39) 4.21 (.36) 15.49 .001
H4a: Attitude toward the ad 4.78 (.24) 5.51 (.19) 4.86 (.18) 4.37 (.25) 7.73 .01
H4b: Positive cognitive responses .52 (.10) 1.16 (.18) .79 (.16) .61 (.20) 6.30 .01
H4c: Negative cognitive responses 1.07 (.21) .44 (.15) .55 (.09) 1.00 (.24) 7.23 .001
342 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, AUGUST 2001

Moreover, a simple effects test revealed that when no ethnic nic identification moderates the effects of the ethnic prime
prime was present, Caucasian participants felt more targeted on other dependent variables. To test for this possibility, we
by the Nokia advertisement than Asian participants did (C = performed a median split on strength of ethnic identification
5.59, A = 3.85; F(1,54) = 10.38, p < .01). This finding sup- (the split point was determined separately for Asian and
ports the contention that the presence of ethnic actors, by it- Caucasian participants). We created this new binary variable
self, has little effect on how consumers process advertising. to test for potential interactions between strength of ethnic
Ad response variables. H4 proposes that exposure to an identification and ethnic primes on the various dependent
ethnic prime will improve the evaluations of the advertise- measures. In these analyses, strength of ethnic identification
ment for target market members but lower the evaluations of did not significantly influence any of the dependent vari-
the advertisement for nontarget market members. Support ables and did not interact with ethnic prime.
for this hypothesis would be found through an ethnic
prime × target market membership interaction on each of the Discussion and Limitations
respective measures. This interaction was found on the atti- Experiment 1 demonstrates that exposure to ethnic primes
tude toward the ad measure (F(1,108) = 7.73, p < .01). The influences consumers’ ethnic self-awareness and thereby in-
interaction was driven by a small (difference of .75 points on fluences responses to the spokespeople and advertisements
a seven-point scale) but significant, positive effect of ethnic used in targeted advertising. Exposure to an ethnic prime
prime on attitude toward the advertisement for target market caused members of the target market to respond more favor-
members (NEP = 4.78, EP = 5.51; F(1,51) = 5.31, p < .02) ably to an ethnically targeted advertisement and caused
combined with no effect of ethnic prime for nontarget mar- members of the nontarget market to respond less favorably.
ket members (NEP = 4.86, EP = 4.37; F(1,56) = 2.60, p > The effects of the ethnic prime were significant across all
.10). These findings provide partial support for H4a, as the dependent variables for target market members but not for
prime produced a significant, positive effect for target mar- nontarget market members. Although ethnic prime exposure
ket members but did not produce a significant effect for non- produced consistently less positive reactions from members
target market members. of the nontarget market, these effects did not reach signifi-
H4b and H4c predicted an ethnic prime × target market cance on any of the dependent measures.
membership interaction on the number of positive and neg- One possible explanation of these asymmetric priming ef-
ative cognitive responses consumers generated to the tar- fects is that target market members are more sensitive to the
geted advertisement. Partial support was found for H4b, as content of targeted advertising than are nontarget market
there was an interaction of prime and ethnicity on the num- members because of heightened self-relevance. Moreover,
ber of positive cognitive responses generated (F(1,108) = the activation of group awareness may directly influence
6.30, p < .01). Prior exposure to the ethnic prime increased consumer reactions to group-relevant information but only
the number of positive cognitive responses to the advertise- indirectly influence reactions to information regarding other
ment for target market members (NEP = .52, EP = 1.16; groups. This suggests that ethnic priming would not cause
F(1,51) = 7.23, p < .001) and had no effect on the number of Asian participants to respond negatively to advertisements
positive cognitive responses for nontarget market members that target other minorities, but it might cause Anglo partic-
(NEP = .79, EP = .61; F(1,56) = .67, p > .20). ipants to respond more positively to advertising that targets
H4c also received partial support, as there was a signifi- their ethnic group.
cant ethnic prime × target market membership interaction on A second explanation of the nonsymmetric target market
the number of negative cognitive responses generated and nontarget market effects is that ethnic primes merely
(F(1,108) = 8.97, p < .001). Prior exposure to an ethnic magnify a preexisting tendency toward biased evaluation of
prime significantly reduced the number of negative cogni- the in-group. Deshpandé and Stayman (1994) find that eth-
tive responses to the advertisement for target market mem- nic consumers respond favorably to targeted advertising to
bers (NEP = 1.07, EP = .44; F(1,51) = 5.88, p < .02) and di- the extent that their ethnicity is a numerical minority in the
rectionally increased the number of negative cognitive general population, and research on ethnocentrism has
responses to the advertisement for nontarget market mem- found that members of an ethnic minority have a greater ten-
bers (NEP = .55, EP = 1.00; F(1,56) = 3.23, p < .10). dency toward biased evaluation of their in-group than mem-
Strength of ethnic identification. We also proposed that bers of an ethnic majority have toward biased evaluation of
ethnic self-awareness is durationally distinct from strength an out-group (LeVine and Campbell 1972). If these argu-
of ethnic identification, because strength of ethnic identifi- ments are applied to Experiment 1, it is possible that the eth-
cation is an enduring state in a person whereas ethnic self- nic prime activated the biases of both minority and majority
awareness is momentary consciousness of group member- consumers, but the magnitude of the potential bias was
ships. If this is indeed the case, the ethnic prime should have much larger among minority consumers than among major-
no effect on the participant’s reported strength of ethnic ity consumers.
identification. As expected, the ethnic prime did not influ- Although the first experiment provides support for the
ence participants’ strength of ethnic identification general tenets of elicited self-awareness, a methodological
(F(1,108) = .59, p > .20). Similar to the tendency to self-re- limitation of the first experiment warrants attention. Specif-
port ethnic information, strength of ethnic identification var- ically, the use of a distinct advertisement as the ethnic prime
ied by ethnicity: Asian participants reported a higher level of opens up the results to alternative explanations. Participants
ethnic identification than did Caucasian participants (A = who viewed the ethnic prime (the explicit Asian comments
7.59, C = 6.76; F(1,108) = 4.99, p < .03). in the Vidal Sassoon advertisement) were also viewing an
Although the ethnic prime had no effect on strength of advertisement and product category that participants in the
ethnic identification, it is still possible that strength of eth- no ethnic prime condition did not view. As a result, the ob-
Priming Ethnic Self-Awareness and Advertising Response 343

served effects could be due to other elements in the Vidal the next advertisement. After the participants finished re-
Sassoon advertisement or the exposure of participants in the viewing all five advertisements, the advertisement booklets
prime condition to two advertisements that feature Asian were collected and questionnaires that contained all depend-
models as opposed to just one for participants in the no ent measures were distributed.
prime condition. Moreover, this design makes it difficult to Stimuli. Print advertisements were used as stimuli. The
conclude whether the differential effects of the prime on the ethnic prime was a modified Northwest Airlines advertise-
target and nontarget market are due to the ethnic differences ment that had originally been created to promote Business
between these two groups or to differences in their targeting First, a program for business travelers. The original adver-
status. tisement featured a small photo in the center of an otherwise
To help resolve these issues, we conducted a second experi- white page with large text above the photo announcing the
ment that kept the amount of information presented constant program, small text alongside the photo describing the pro-
across the prime and no prime conditions. We created this equiv- gram, fine print at the bottom of the page detailing restric-
alency by using two ethnic primes, one that featured an Asian tions, and the Northwest Airlines logo in the lower right-
ethnic prime and one that featured a Caucasian ethnic prime. hand corner. To make the product category more relevant to
These primes were contained in an advertisement that preceded the participants, the product promoted by the advertisement
the targeted advertisement, but the advertisement they were in- was changed from a Business First program to a Students
cluded in was not itself a targeted advertisement. The placement First program. Although this change required alterations to
of these primes in a nontargeted advertisement removes the al- the content of the large text at the top of the page and the
ternative explanation that the elicited self-awareness results were small text alongside the photo, the text position, font, and
the product of exposure to two sequential targeted advertise- size were kept as they were in the original advertisement.
ments rather than the product of the prime itself. The targeted ad- To create both an Asian and a Caucasian ethnic prime
vertisement that participants evaluated in Experiment 2 was also from this one advertisement, we manipulated two elements
manipulated so that some participants viewed a Caucasian-tar- of the advertisement. First, the large text at the top of the
geted advertisement after prime exposure and some participants page was altered to say either “Travel overseas to Asia” (an
viewed an Asian-targeted advertisement after prime exposure. Asian prime) or “Travel overseas to Europe” (a Caucasian
prime). Compared with the rest of the large text, the words
EXPERIMENT 2
“Asia” and “Europe” were written in a significantly larger
Method font and were in bold. All of the small and fine text was
Overview and design. The goal of Experiment 2 was to repli- identical between the two prime versions, with the exception
cate the elicited self-awareness findings observed in Experiment of four spots where the advertisements either said “Asia” or
1 in a context in which the amount of information presented was “Europe.” The second element of the advertisement that we
equivalent across the prime and no prime conditions. To gain manipulated to create an Asian or Caucasian ethnic prime
greater control over the information presented in both the prime was the photograph placed at the center of the advertise-
and the targeted advertisement, we used print stimuli instead of ment. Following the findings of Hong and colleagues
video stimuli. The design of Experiment 2 was a 2 levels of eth- (1999), we used photographs of cultural icons as ethnic
nicity (Asian or Caucasian) × 2 levels of ethnic prime (Asian primes. In the Asian prime version, a photograph of the
ethnic prime or Caucasian ethnic prime) × 2 levels of advertis- Great Wall of China was included. In the Caucasian prime
ing target audience (focal advertisement targeted to either Asians advertisement, a photograph of Big Ben, the clock tower/
or Caucasians) between-participants factorial. This design al- bell on the British Parliament, was included.
lows a more complete analysis of the effects of ethnic primes on The focal advertisements that were evaluated by the par-
targeted advertising across both Asians and Caucasians. ticipants were two versions of an IBM advertisement that
Participants. At a major West Coast university, 175 under- was originally used to advertise IBM Global Services. Both
graduate students participated as part of a class requirement. advertisements featured a photograph of an approximately
Of these, 61 students self-reported their ethnicity as Asian, 35-year-old man’s face in front of a hazy background. One
and 114 students self-reported their ethnicity as Caucasian. advertisement featured the face of an Asian man, the second
Procedure. Three weeks before the main experiment, par- the face of a Caucasian man. Both advertisements also fea-
ticipants completed a survey that included several personal- tured a small box filled with text that was placed alongside
ity scales and basic demographic items. The self-reported the photograph of the man. The text of the original adver-
ethnicity measure in this initial survey was used to classify tisements was changed in several ways for the purposes of
participants ethnically. the experiment. First, the product category was changed
During the main experiment, participants were randomly from IBM Global Services to the IBM ThinkPad laptop
assigned to one of the four prime/target advertisement con- computer to make the product category more relevant to the
ditions. Upon entering the lab, participants were informed student population. The rest of the text in the box was edited
that the researchers were “examining attitudes toward print so that the Asian and Caucasian versions of the advertise-
media” and that participants would review several print ad- ment contained identical information. The text provided in-
vertisements and then answer some questions about them in formation about the man’s name and occupation, a quotation
a questionnaire packet. Each participant was given an ad- from the man, the ThinkPad Web site URL, and a slogan
vertisement booklet to review. The experimenter read the in- statement for the ThinkPad. John Lee was selected as the
structions to the participants and then controlled the pace at man’s name, because Lee is a surname that both Asians and
which participants read the advertisements. The experi- Caucasians may have.
menter allowed the participants to review each advertise- In a pretest, 48 subjects who did not participate in the
ment for 20 seconds before instructing them to proceed to main experiment evaluated the two IBM models to ensure
344 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, AUGUST 2001

that the advertisements were equivalent except for the this effect is consistent with McGuire’s distinctiveness the-
model’s ethnicity. Participants were randomly assigned to ory (McGuire et al. 1978).
evaluate either the Caucasian or Asian version of the adver- Spokesperson evaluation. H2 proposes that exposure to a
tisement and were instructed to evaluate the model’s friend- congruent ethnic prime will improve the evaluations of the
liness, happiness, intelligence, trustworthiness, and attrac- spokesperson in a subsequent ethnically targeted advertise-
tiveness. All responses were collected on seven-point ment if the spokesperson’s ethnicity matches the consumer’s
semantic differential items. No significant differences were ethnicity and will lower the evaluations of the spokesperson
found in participants’ evaluation of the models on any of if the spokesperson’s ethnicity does not match the con-
these items (p > .20). Separate analyses were also run for sumer’s ethnicity. Because Experiment 2 used two different
Caucasian participants and Asian participants, and again no targeted advertisements as the object of evaluation, Asian
significant differences were found on evaluation of the mod- participants were not always members of the target market,
els (p > .20). and Caucasians were not always members of the nontarget
Three other advertisements were selected as filler adver- market. As a result, H2 is most easily tested through a series
tisements in the experiment. The three advertisements were of a priori contrasts. This allows an analysis of the effects of
selected because they did not feature human models, which the two types of ethnic primes on Asian and Caucasian sub-
thus helped prevent any unintended ethnic primes. These jects’ responses to an Asian-targeted advertisement (a repli-
advertisements were for FTD flower bouquets, American cation of Experiment 1) and an analysis of the effects of two
Express credit cards, and Refresh eye drops. All advertise- types of ethnic primes on Asian and Caucasian subjects’ re-
ments (including the prime and target advertisements) were sponses to a Caucasian-targeted advertisement (an extension
presented in black and white. The final ordering of adver- of Experiment 1). Means and overall ANOVA results for H2,
tisements was as follows: Filler Ad 1, Ethnic Prime Ad H3, and H4 are reported in Table 2.
(Northwest), Targeted Ad (IBM), Filler Ad 2, Filler Ad 3. The first planned contrast used to test H2 is a comparison
Measures. The same set of dependent measures was used of the reactions of Asian participants to an Asian model af-
in Experiment 2 as in Experiment 1, with one exception. The ter being exposed to either an Asian or a Caucasian prime.
measure of perception of being targeted used in Experiment Support for H2 was found, as Asians responded more favor-
1 was replaced with the more complete, three-item targeted- ably to the model in the Asian-targeted advertisement after
ness scale developed by Aaker, Brumbaugh, and Grier exposure to an Asian ethnic prime (AEP) than after exposure
(1999). to a Caucasian ethnic prime (CEP) (AEP = 5.22, CEP =
4.21; F(1,34) = 7.45, p < .01). However, Caucasian response
Analysis and Results to the Asian model was not influenced by the type of ethnic
Ethnic self-awareness. H1 proposes that exposure to an prime (AEP = 4.05, CEP = 4.33; F(1,55) = .94, p > .20).
ethnic prime will increase ethnic self-awareness and that this These results provide support for the in-group predictions of
effect will be more pronounced when the ethnic prime is H2, but not its out-group predictions.
congruent with the participant’s ethnicity than when it is in- Corresponding planned contrasts were also conducted on
congruent with the participant’s ethnicity. Results from a lo- participant evaluation of the Caucasian model. Type of eth-
gistic regression provided support for this hypothesis. As nic prime did not affect the evaluations of the Caucasian
predicted, participants who were exposed to a congruent model for either Asian (AEP = 4.30, CEP = 4.39; F(1,34) =
ethnic prime (CP) were more likely to self-report their eth- .04, p > .20) or Caucasian (AEP = 4.70, CEP = 4.46;
nicity than participants exposed to an incongruent ethnic F(1,55) = .63, p > .20) participants. These results fail to sup-
prime (IP) (CP = 34%, IP = 21%). In the logistic regression, port H2 when the focus of evaluation is a Caucasian model.
the parameter estimate for the effect of a congruent ethnic Perception of being targeted. By drawing attention to eth-
prime was .71 (S.E. = .39, χ2 (1, N = 175) = 3.39, p < .03, nicity, congruent ethnic primes should increase the likelihood
one-tailed test). The differential effect of a congruent ethnic that consumers will feel targeted by advertisements that fea-
prime over an incongruent ethnic prime on self-awareness ture same-ethnicity actors and spokespeople and decrease the
was also influenced by participant ethnicity. Asian partici- likelihood that consumers will feel targeted by advertisements
pants’ ethnic self-report rate increased from 28% with an in- that feature different-ethnicity actors and spokespeople. This
congruent prime to 56% with a congruent ethnic prime, prediction was supported for Asian participants, who felt
whereas the rate for Caucasian participants increased only more targeted by the Asian-targeted advertisement after ex-
from 12% to 15%. To test the significance of these prime ef- posure to an Asian ethnic prime than after exposure to a Cau-
fects by ethnicity, we conducted another logistic regression casian ethnic prime (AEP = 4.93, CEP = 3.69; F(1,34) = 5.90,
that allowed for differential effects of the prime on Asians p < .02). However, ethnic prime type did not influence how
and Caucasians. In this regression, the effect of the prime targeted Caucasians felt by the Asian-targeted advertisement
was significant for Asians (parameter estimate = 1.22; S.E. = (AEP = 3.12, CEP = 3.40; F(1,55) = .47, p > .20). For the ad-
.55, χ2 (1, N = 175) = 4.99, p < .01, one-tailed test) but in- vertisement featuring an Asian model, there was a significant
significant for Caucasians (p > .20). Despite this difference main effect of ethnicity on the perception of being targeted, as
in significance, the interaction between participant ethnicity Asians felt more targeted than did Caucasians (A = 4.31, C =
and prime congruence was not significant (p > .10). Finally, 3.26; F(1,90) = 10.34, p < .01). Again, these results support
as in Experiment 1, there was an unhypothesized main effect the in-group predictions of H3 but not the out-group predic-
of ethnicity on tendency to self-report ethnic information: tions. When the focus of evaluation was a Caucasian-targeted
Asians (A) self-reported at a higher rate than Caucasians (C) advertisement and the target market/nontarget market roles
(A = 42%, C = 14%; parameter estimate = 1.57; S.E. = .38, are therefore reversed, none of the planned contrasts testing
χ2 (1, N = 175) = 17.42, p < .01). Although unhypothesized, H3 reached significance (p > .20).
Table 2
EXPERIMENT 2 CELL MEANS (S.E.) AND ANOVA RESULTS

ANOVA Results:
Ethnicity of Model in Ad Asian Caucasian Three-Way Interaction
of Ethnicity of Model,
Participant Ethnicity Asian Caucasian Asian Caucasian Participant, and Prime
Priming Ethnic Self-Awareness and Advertising Response

Ethnic Prime Asian Caucasian Asian Caucasian Asian Caucasian Asian Caucasian F p
H2: Spokesperson liking 5.22 (.26)a 4.21 (.27)a 4.05 (.20) 4.34 (.22) 4.30 (.29) 4.39 (.33) 4.70 (.23) 4.46 (.19) 5.17 .02
H3: Felt targetedness 4.93 (.36)b 3.69 (.37)b 3.12 (.28) 3.40 (.28) 3.33 (.38) 3.64 (.44) 3.61 (.30) 3.48 (.25) 4.32 .04
H4a: Attitude toward the ad 4.39 (.30) 4.07 (.29) 4.13 (.22) 4.26 (.24) 4.34 (.22) 4.05 (.20) 4.21 (.27) 5.22 (.26) .47 n.s.
H4b: Positive cognitive responses 1.67 (.19)c .82 (.20)c .40 (.15) .31 (.16) .20 (.21) .36 (.25) .71 (.17) .79 (.14) 2.63 .10
H4c: Negative cognitive responses .61 (.25)d 1.18 (.26)d 1.73 (.19) 1.38 (.21) 1.40 (.25) 1.82 (.29) .71 (.20) .68 (.17) .55 n.s.
Notes: Superscript letters denote significant planned contrasts (p < .05).
345
346 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, AUGUST 2001

Ad response variables. H4 proposes that exposure to a specific travel destination is differentially attractive to dif-
congruent ethnic prime will improve the evaluations of the ferent ethnic groups, the advertisement that contained the
advertisement for target market members but lower the eval- primes does not explicitly target either ethnicity, because
uations of the advertisement for nontarget market members. anyone, regardless of ethnicity, could be interested in travel-
Although planned contrasts revealed no such effects on the ing to a given location. As a result, both experiments used
attitude toward the ad measure (p > .20), the type of ethnic primes that should prompt ethnic classification, but those
prime influenced the number of positive and negative cogni- used in Experiment 2 are much less direct. The only major
tive responses Asian participants had to the Asian-targeted finding from Experiment 1 that was not replicated in Exper-
advertisement. Specifically, Asian participants reported iment 2 was the positive effect of ethnic priming on Asian
more positive responses to the Asian-targeted advertisement evaluations of an Asian-targeted advertisement. Again, this
after exposure to the Asian ethnic prime (AEP = 1.67, CEP = null effect may be attributed to the subtlety of the primes
.82; F(1,34) = 9.45, p < .01) and fewer negative responses used.
(AEP = .61, CEP = 1.18; F(1,34) = 4.73, p < .05). However, Another interesting finding across both experiments is the
the type of ethnic prime influenced neither how Caucasian general lack of ethnic priming effects on Caucasians. When
participants evaluated the Asian-targeted advertisement (p > the focal advertisement was targeted to Asians (Experiment
.20) nor their cognitive responses to it (p > .20). As for H2 1 and the replication half of Experiment 2), the only effects
and H3, these findings provide support for the positive in- of ethnic primes on Caucasians were limited increases in
group effects predicted by H4 but not the negative out-group ethnic self-awareness and decreases in how targeted the
effects. Caucasians felt by the Asian-targeted advertisements (both
No support for H4 was found in participant responses to findings in Experiment 1). This finding suggests that ethnic
the Caucasian-targeted advertisement. The only findings of primes that focus attention on exclusion from a group can
note for responses to the Caucasian-targeted advertisement increase ethnic self-awareness in the same way that attention
were main effects of ethnicity on the number of positive and to inclusion in a group does. However, this increase in eth-
negative cognitive responses generated. Compared with nic self-awareness did not lead to the development of nega-
Asians, Caucasians generated significantly more positive tive attitudes toward the out-group or toward advertisements
cognitive responses to the Caucasian-targeted advertisement that target an out-group. As stated previously, these null
(A = .28, C = .75; F(1,83) = 5.77, p < .02) and significantly findings are not surprising given the relative difficulty of ob-
fewer negative cognitive responses (A = 1.61, C = .69; serving negative out-group effects compared with positive
F(1,83) = 19.08, p < .01). in-group effects (Aaker, Brumbaugh, and Grier 1999;
Strength of ethnic identification. As in Experiment 1, we LeVine and Campbell 1972). In addition to not demonstrat-
found that though a congruent ethnic prime increased ethnic ing negative out-group attitudes, Caucasian participants in
self-awareness, it did not increase strength of ethnic identi- Experiment 2 also failed to demonstrate any form of positive
fication (F(1,173) = .86, p > .20). This again supports the in-group bias toward same-ethnicity models or advertise-
contention that ethnic self-awareness is durationally distinct ments that feature same-ethnicity models.
from strength of ethnic identification and that strength of There are two primary explanations for the absence of
ethnic identification is an enduring state that is not as sub- ethnic priming effects on Caucasians. First and foremost is
ject to momentary change. The only significant predictor of the fact that “Caucasian” is technically a broad racial con-
strength of ethnic identification was again ethnicity, as struct rather than a more narrowly defined ethnic construct.
Asian participants reported higher levels of ethnic identifi- As a result, Caucasians may be more likely to resonate to
cation than did Caucasian participants (A = 7.07, C = 6.10; ethnic primes that relate to a specific heritage (e.g., Swedish,
F(1,173) = 9.65, p < .01). Moreover, a binary strength of eth- French, Irish) rather than to a generalized racial category
nic identification variable created through a median split did (e.g., Caucasian). For example, although participants of
not significantly influence any of the dependent variables English heritage might respond to the photos of Big Ben
and did not interact with ethnic prime. used in Experiment 2, participants of other Caucasian her-
itages may not. It should be noted that though Asian her-
DISCUSSION itages can be delineated into just as many subclassifications
Across all dependent measures, Experiment 2 success- as can Caucasian heritages, Asian Americans may still be
fully replicated the findings of Experiment 1 when the focus more likely to respond to general Asian primes because of
of evaluation was an Asian-targeted advertisement. As in their current minority status.
Experiment 1, Asians responded more positively to Asian- A second possible cause of the differential effects of eth-
targeted advertising and spokespeople if they were first ex- nic primes on Asians and Caucasians is the differential dis-
posed to an Asian ethnic prime. Exposure to an Asian ethnic tinctiveness of the two classifications within the general pop-
prime also increased the ethnic self-awareness of Asians and ulation. Because the majority of the population from which
made them feel more targeted by subsequent targeted adver- participants were drawn is Caucasian, Asian participants are
tising. These significant priming effects are particularly more likely to feel socially distinctive than are Caucasian
noteworthy given the relatively subtle prime manipulation participants (McGuire et al. 1978). Because of this low so-
used in Experiment 2. In Experiment 1, the ethnic prime ex- cial distinctiveness, Caucasian participants are less likely to
plicitly stated that a product was “for Asian hair” and respond to contextual primes such as those under study here.
thereby forced participants to evaluate whether the product
was “for me” or “not for me.” In contrast, the second exper- Implications and Further Research
iment used photographs of cultural icons and potential travel One critical implication of this research is that positive
destinations as ethnic primes. Although it is possible that a target market effects may become more difficult to create as
Priming Ethnic Self-Awareness and Advertising Response 347

consumers adjust to the more frequent use of ethnic cues in spond negatively to advertisements that are targeted to them.
advertising. The experimental findings suggest that mar- If consumer response to the advertisements had been nega-
keters cannot automatically expect to produce target market tive, the elicitation of ethnic self-awareness might have had
effects simply by including an ethnic spokesperson in an ad- the opposite effect on consumer response to the advertise-
vertisement. To achieve a positive response from the target ments. Instead of accentuating or bringing out a positive re-
market, marketers instead must either elicit ethnic self- sponse from the target market, greater ethnic self-awareness
awareness among their consumers through advertisement may have caused target market members to respond even
execution variables or expose their consumers to targeted more negatively to the targeted advertisement. Further re-
advertisements at times when the consumers are likely to search should address this issue by assessing the relative im-
feel distinctive already. The easiest way for marketers to do pact of ethnic self-awareness on advertising that is generally
this may be to place their advertising in situations where the perceived positively and advertising that is generally per-
surrounding content is likely to contain ethnic primes. This ceived negatively.
conclusion is a marked departure from a great deal of early Other potential avenues for further research include
research, which observed target market effects simply by al- analyses of the potential interactions between ethnic primes
tering actor characteristics and suggests that consumers’ ex- and individual difference variables and between ethnic
pectations about advertising have changed dramatically over primes and social variables. The differential pattern of re-
time. Only by tracking the evolution of these expectations sults for Asians and Caucasians suggests that these interac-
will marketers be able to predict what factors will elicit dis- tions may be a large part of the story, and a better under-
tinctiveness in the future. standing of how they interact may be essential to successful
A second important implication of this research is the targeted advertising.
lack of effect of ethnic primes on nontarget market members
in general and Caucasian nontarget market members in par- REFERENCES
ticular. One risk that arises when marketers aggressively tar- Aaker, Jennifer, Anne Brumbaugh, and Sonya Grier (2000), “Non-
get a minority group is that this practice could alienate the target Markets and Viewer Distinctiveness: The Impact of Target
nontarget market. To the extent that the nontarget market Marketing on Advertising Attitudes,” Journal of Consumer Psy-
also consumes the marketer’s product or service, this alien- chology, 9 (3), 127–40.
Abrams, Dominic (1985), “Focus of Attention in Minimal Inter-
ation could be very costly, especially when the nontarget group Discrimination,” British Journal of Social Psychology, 24
market is much larger than the target market. Because most (1), 65–74.
targeted campaigns in the United States focus on some sub- Belk, Russell W. (1974), “An Exploratory Assessment of Situa-
segment of the population, Caucasians are frequently mem- tional Effects in Buyer Behavior,” Journal of Marketing Re-
bers of this larger nontarget market. This project’s findings search, 1 (May), 156–63.
suggest that the risk of a nontarget market backlash may not Berscheid, Ellen (1966), “Opinion Change and Communicator–
be as great as previously thought. First, Caucasians demon- Communicatee Similarity and Dissimilarity,” Journal of Person-
strated no backlash toward targeted advertising in either of ality and Social Psychology, 4 (6), 670–80.
the studies. Second, although Asians were generally sensi- Bhat, Subodh, Thomas W. Leigh, and Daniel L. Wardlow (1996),
tive to ethnic primes, the ethnic primes did not affect how “The Effect of Homosexual Imagery in Advertising on Attitude
Toward the Ad,” Journal of Homosexuality, 31 (1/2), 161–76.
Asians responded to advertising that targeted non-Asian Burke, Peter J. and Donald C. Reitzes (1991), “An Identity Theory
populations. Although the absence of significant nontarget Approach to Commitment,” Social Psychology Quarterly, 54
market effects in these studies cannot be generalized to all (September), 239–51
situations, it suggests that negative nontarget market reac- Cota, Albert A. and Kenneth L. Dion (1986), “Salience of Gender
tions are far from automatic. and Sex Composition of Ad Hoc Groups: An Experimental Test
One limitation of this research is that only two ethnic of Distinctiveness Theory,” Journal of Personality and Social
groups, Asians and Caucasians, were used as participants Psychology, 50 (4), 770–76.
and as targets for the experimental advertising. It is possible Deshpandé, Rohit, Wayne D. Hoyer, and Naveen Donthu (1986),
that the observed effects could change if different popula- “The Intensity of Ethnic Affiliation: A Study of the Sociology of
tions were used as participants. For example, the generally Hispanic Consumption,” Journal of Consumer Research, 13 (2),
214–20.
elusive nontarget market effects in this study may have oc- ——— and Douglas M. Stayman (1994), “A Tale of Two Cities:
curred if the target minority group was instead African Distinctiveness Theory and Advertising Effectiveness,” Journal
Americans. To the extent that African Americans are more of Marketing Research, 31 (1), 57–64.
frequent victims of stereotypes or negative preconceptions, Ellis, Seth, James McCullough, Melanie Wallendorf, and Chin
advertising targeting them may elicit more negative nontar- Tiong Tan (1985), “Cultural Values and Behavior: Chineseness
get market reactions. Conversely, advertising targeted to Within Geographic Boundaries,” in Advances in Consumer Re-
African Americans might produce even more pronounced search, Vol. 12, Elizabeth C. Hirschman and Morris B. Holbrook,
positive target market responses. Although this project was eds. Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 126–28.
focused on the theoretical process by which ethnic self- Fekken, G. Cynthia and Ronald R. Holden (1992), “Response La-
awareness influences advertising response, the potential for tency Evidence for Viewing Personality Traits as Schema Indi-
cators,” Journal of Research in Personality, 26 (2), 103–20.
different effects based on the ethnic group targeted is of Gerard, Harold B. and Michael F. Hoyt (1974), “Distinctiveness of
great practical importance and warrants further research. Social Categorization and Attitude Toward In-Group Members,”
A second limitation of the present experiment is that it Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29 (6), 836–42.
used targeted appeals that were generally positive in their Grier, Sonya A. and Anne M. Brumbaugh (1999), “Noticing Cul-
presentation of ethnic actors and generally well liked by the tural Differences: Ad Meanings Created by Target and Non-tar-
target audience. However, it is possible for consumers to re- get Markets,” Journal of Advertising, 28 (1), 79–93.
348 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, AUGUST 2001

——— and Rohit Deshpandé (2001), “Social Dimensions of Con- Saenz, Rogelio and Benigno E. Aguirre (1991), “The Dynamics of
sumer Distinctiveness: The Influence of Group Social Status and Mexican Ethnic Identity,” Ethnic Groups, 9, 17–32.
Identity on Advertising Persuasion,” Journal of Marketing Re- Skitka, Linda J. and Christina Maslach (1996), “Gender as
search, 38 (May), 00–00. Schematic Category: A Role Construct Approach,” Social Be-
Hirschman, Elizabeth C. (1981), “American Jewish Ethnicity: Its havior and Personality, 24 (1), 53–73.
Relationship to Some Selected Aspects of Consumer Behavior,” Stayman, Douglas M. and Rohit Deshpandé (1989), “Situational
Journal of Marketing, 45 (Summer), 102–10. Ethnicity and Consumer Behavior,” Journal of Consumer Re-
Hong, Ying-yi, Michael W. Morris, Chi-yue Chiu, and Verónica search, 16 (December), 361–71.
Benet-Martínez (2000), “Multicultural Minds: A Dynamic Con- Sumner, W.G. (1906), Folkways. New York: Ginn.
structivist Approach to Culture and Cognition,” American Psy- Taylor, Charles R. and Barbara B. Stern (1997), “Asian-Americans:
chologist, 55 (7), 709–20. Television Advertising and the ‘Model Minority’ Stereotype,”
LeVine, Robert A. and Donald T. Campbell (1972), “Sumner and Journal of Advertising, 26 (2), 47–61.
the Universal Syndrome of Ethnocentrism,” in Ethnocentrism: Taylor, Shelley E., Susan T. Fiske, Nancy L. Etcoff, and Audrey J.
Theories of Conflict, Ethnic Attitudes, and Group Behavior. Ruderman (1978), “Categorical and Contextual Bases of Person
New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1–21. Memory and Stereotyping,” Journal of Personality and Social
Linville, Patricia W., Gregory W. Fischer, and Peter Salovey Psychology, 36 (7), 778–93.
(1989), “Perceived Distributions of the Characteristics of In- Weigel, Russell H., James W. Loomis, and Matthew J. Soja (1980),
Group and Out-Group Members: Empirical Evidence and a “Race Relations on Prime-Time Television,” Journal of Person-
Computer Simulation,” Journal of Personality and Social Psy- ality and Social Psychology, 39 (5), 884–93.
chology, 57 (2), 165–88. Whittler, Tommy E. (1989), “Viewer’s Processing of Source and
Lips, Hilary M. (1995), “Gender as Schematic Category: A Role Message Cues in Advertising Stimuli,” Psychology and Market-
Construct Approach,” Social Behavior and Personality, 24 (1), ing, 6 (Winter), 287–309.
53–73. ——— and Joan DiMeo (1991), “Viewer’s Reactions to Racial
Markus, Hazel (1977), “Self-Schemata and Processing Information Cues in Advertising Stimuli,” Journal of Advertising Research,
About the Self,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31 (6), 37–46.
35 (2), 63–78. Wilkes, Robert E. and Humberto Valencia (1989), “Hispanics and
——— and A. Regula Herzog (1995), “The Sociocultural Self- Blacks in Television Commercials,” Journal of Advertising, 18
Concept,” in Trends and Issues in Theoretical Psychology, Ian (1), 19–25.
Lubek and Rene van Hezewijk, eds. New York: Springer Pub- Williams, Jerome D. and William J. Qualls (1989), “Middle-Class
lishing Co., 39–45. Black Consumers and Intensity of Ethnic Identification,” Psy-
——— and Keith P. Sentis (1982), “The Self in Social Information chology and Marketing, 6 (Winter), 263–86.
Processing,” in Psychological Perspectives on the Self, Vol. 1, J. Wooten, David B. (1995), “One-of-a-Kind in a Full House: Some
Suls, ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 41–70. Consequences of Ethnic and Gender Distinctiveness,” Journal of
McGuire, William J., Claire V. McGuire, Pamela Child, and Terry Consumer Psychology, 4 (3), 205–24.
Fujioka (1978), “Salience of Ethnicity in the Spontaneous Self- ——— and Tiffany Galvin (1993), “A Preliminary Examination of
Concept as a Function of One’s Ethnic Distinctiveness in the So- the Effect of Context-Induced Felt Ethnicity on Advertising Ef-
cial Environment,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- fectiveness,” in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 20, Leigh
ogy, 36 (5), 511–20. McAlister and Michael Rothschild, eds. Provo, UT: Association
Neimeyer, Robert A. and Kelly A. Mitchell (1988), “Similarity and for Consumer Research, 253–56.
Attraction: A Longitudinal Study,” Journal of Social and Per- Wyer, Robert S. and Thomas K. Srull (1986), “Human Cognition
sonal Relationships, 5 (2), 131–48. in Its Social Context,” Psychological Review, 93 (3), 322–59.

You might also like