You are on page 1of 17

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/40040925

Development and Validation of a Measure of Interpersonal Strengths: The


Inventory of Interpersonal Strengths

Article  in  Psychological Assessment · December 2009


DOI: 10.1037/a0017269 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS
28 900

2 authors, including:

Robert L. Hatcher
CUNY Graduate Center
39 PUBLICATIONS   2,752 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Robert L. Hatcher on 05 June 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Psychological Assessment © 2009 American Psychological Association
2009, Vol. 21, No. 4, 554 –569 1040-3590/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0017269

Development and Validation of a Measure of Interpersonal Strengths:


The Inventory of Interpersonal Strengths

Robert L. Hatcher Daniel T. Rogers


University of Michigan Kennesaw State University

An Inventory of Interpersonal Strengths (IIS) was developed and validated in a series of large college
student samples. Based on interpersonal theory and associated methods, the IIS was designed to assess
positive characteristics representing the full range of interpersonal domains, including those generally
thought to have negative qualities (e.g., introversion, coldness, submissiveness). The 8 subscales
(octants) of the 64-item IIS demonstrated good circumplex features and reliability. Tests comparing Big
5 interpersonal factors, Inventory of Interpersonal Problems (IIP), Battery of Interpersonal Capabilities
(BIC), and other interpersonal measures demonstrated convergent and discriminant validity and shared
interpersonal structure. The IIS accounted for significant additional variance in life satisfaction and
quality of personal relationships beyond the IIP and the BIC.

Keywords: interpersonal measurement, circumplex, IIS, strengths

Interpersonal theory has developed into a rich and powerful frame- ables were to be equally spaced around the circle. Finally, each
work for understanding the nature of interpersonal relating. The variable was to participate substantially and equally in the overall
introduction of the interpersonal circle (Freedman, Leary, Ossorio, & variable space (communality). Together, these requirements char-
Coffey, 1951; Leary, 1957), illustrated in Figure 1, substantially acterize the mathematical model called the circumplex (Tracey,
impacted the assessment of interpersonal relating (for reviews, see 2000). Robust statistical methods have been developed to test a set
Freedman, 1985; Horowitz, Turan, Wilson, & Zolotsev, 2008; Pincus of variables for their circumplex relationships (Browne, 1992;
& Gurtman, 2006; Wiggins, 1996). The circle is a conceptual tool Tracey, 2000), and several interpersonal measures have demon-
depicting all interpersonal features as blends of two key orthogonal strated these properties. Examples include the IAS-R (Gurtman &
factors, dominance–submission and affiliation– hostility. The specific Pincus, 2000), the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems (IIP; Alden,
blend determines the location of a feature on the circle (e.g., agree- Wiggins, & Pincus, 1990; Horowitz, Rosenberg, Baer, Ureño, &
ableness is a blend of affiliation and submission). Villasenor, 1988), the Battery of Interpersonal Capabilities (BIC;
Wiggins’s Interpersonal Adjective Scales (IAS; Wiggins, 1979; Hofsess & Tracey, 2005; Paulhus & Martin, 1987), and the Circum-
IAS-R; Wiggins, Trapnell, & Phillips, 1988), a measure of inter- plex Scales of Interpersonal Values (CSIV; Locke, 2000).
personal traits, significantly advanced the operationalization of the Early interpersonal theorists and researchers conceptualized
interpersonal circle. The IAS was constructed to meet robust both positive and negative features in all sections of the interper-
requirements. These included the presence of an orderly statistical sonal circle (Freedman et al., 1951; Leary, 1957). For example, for
relationship between the variables, such that those adjacent on the what is now called the “cold– unfriendly” octant, Leary (1957)
circle should share the highest correlations, with diminishing cor- described characteristics ranging from “hard-hearted, frequently
relations as distance on the circle increases between variables, to a angry, often unfriendly,” to “can be frank and honest” (p. 135),
minimum at the opposite side. To represent effectively the blend- with the expectation that more positive features would contribute
ing of affiliation– hostility and dominance–submission, the vari-
to positive, adaptive interpersonal outcomes. Later work on the
interpersonal circle has paid less attention to consistent represen-
tation of positive and negative features, with the exception of the
IIP, to be discussed later. Examination of Figure 1, a modern
Robert L. Hatcher, Psychological Clinic, University of Michigan;
Daniel T. Rogers, Department of Psychology, Kennesaw State University. conceptualization of the circle, demonstrates the issue. Features
Robert L. Hatcher is now at The Wellness Center, The Graduate Center/ that could be described as generally positive appear on the right
The City University of New York. side of the circle, including Dominant–Assured, Extraverted–
This study was supported in part by a generous anonymous donor. An Gregarious, Warm–Friendly, and Agreeable–Trusting. The left
earlier version of this article was presented at the annual meeting of the side of the circle, however, refers to negative behavioral fea-
Society for Interpersonal Theory and Research, Madison, WI, June 2007. tures, including Disagreeable–Mistrusting, Cold–Unfriendly,
We are grateful to Michael Gurtman and Christopher Peterson for consul- Introverted–Inhibited, and Submissive–Unassured. There are con-
tation and to Alex Barends, Maria Slowiaczek, and Nancy Davis for
ditions under which these behaviors and attitudes may be of
contributions in the earlier stages of the study.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Robert L. considerable value (e.g., mistrust of unreasonable returns from
Hatcher, The Wellness Center, The Graduate Center/The City University investments has been proven to be wise). But in general, Leary’s
of New York, 365 Fifth Avenue, Room 6422, New York, NY 10016. efforts to find positive forms of these domains have been largely
E-mail: robert.hatcher@gmail.com lost.
554
INVENTORY OF INTERPERSONAL STRENGTHS 555

Among these measures, the Battery of Interpersonal Capabilities


(BIC; Paulhus & Martin, 1987) asks respondents to rate the degree
to which they are capable, when the situation calls for it, of
behaving in ways characterized by the 16 IAS segments, such as
warmly, trustingly, or quarrelsomely. The BIC has circumplex
structure (although, because of the positive manifold, the authors
did not recognize this fact; Hofsess & Tracey, 2005). However, the
BIC does not address the issue of positive interpersonal features on
the left side of the interpersonal circle. Instead, it preserves Wig-
gins’s bipolar dimensions as a basis for capabilities, resulting in
questions such as “when the situation requires it, how capable are
you of being quarrelsome (synonyms: uncooperative, hostile)?”
These responses may be useful in some extreme situations, but in
general quarrelsomeness seems unlikely to enhance interpersonal
relating.
The Circumplex Scales of Interpersonal Values (CSIV; Locke,
2000) measure the value an individual places on a range of
interpersonal outcomes, including adaptive and maladaptive be-
Figure 1. The interpersonal circle. haviors. In achieving its circumplex structure, the CSIV scales on
the left side of the interpersonal circle address a degree of adap-
tiveness. For example, the cold– unfriendly octant includes valuing
The loss of focus on positive features was due in part to “appearing cool and detached, being guarded, and concealing their
Wiggins’s (1979) use of “bipolar” dimensions as conceptual an- thoughts and feelings” (Locke, 2000, p. 254). The introverted–
chors for the circle, by which he meant dimensions anchored by inhibited octant assesses the value of “avoiding ridicule and rejec-
opposite characteristics. Wiggins’s IAS contains 16 variables, each tion by avoiding blunders and concealing their positive feelings”
paired with its opposite across the circle (e.g., quarrelsome vs. (Locke, 2000, p. 254). In identifying valued outcomes for the left
agreeable). Factor-analytic investigations of interpersonal adjec- side of the interpersonal circle, the CSIV manages to avoid por-
tives by Big Five researchers yielded factors that were also an- traying these behaviors as completely negative. Yet, the interper-
chored by adjectives of opposite valence, as in extraversion– sonal adaptiveness of being perceived as guarded, detached, and
introversion and agreeableness– disagreeableness (Goldberg, concealing feelings seems unclear.
1992; Hofstee, de Raad, & Goldberg, 1992). It may also be that The Circumplex Scales of Interpersonal Efficacies (CSIE;
early researchers did not recognize that circumplex characteristics Locke & Sadler, 2007) assesses an individual’s belief or confi-
can be embedded in variable sets that are all positively correlated dence in their ability to enact behaviors corresponding to each area
(positive manifold), so that they believed it was important to of the interpersonal circle. The CSIE addresses the left side of the
develop scales anchored in polar opposites (Hofsess & Tracey, circle in ways similar to the IAS, BIC, and CSIV. Many items
2005). reflect a potential to make positive, adaptive contributions to
Contemporary statistical models can remove the effects of the interpersonal relations (e.g., “I can tell them when I am annoyed”
positive manifold, making the underlying circumplex structure and “I can be a follower”). Other items retain the negative focus
evident (Browne, 1992; Tracey, 2000) and opening the way to found in other measures (e.g., “I can be cold and unfriendly when
extend the interpersonal circle’s nomological net to include a fuller I want to,” “I can be cruel when the situation calls for it”). The
range of positive, generally adaptive interpersonal phenomena, positive aspects of the interpersonal circle appear stronger in the
thus enhancing the range of interpersonal theory and measurement. CSIE than most previous interpersonal measures, but the difficulty
The widely used Inventory of Interpersonal Problems (IIP; in finding positive features on the left side persists.
Alden et al., 1990; Horowitz et al., 1988) stimulated our interest in Other measures of social competence, not based on the inter-
positive interpersonal features because it focuses exclusively on personal circle, include the Social Skills Inventory (SSI; Riggio,
negative ones. The IIP’s dimensions are anchored in opposite types 1986) and the Interpersonal Competence Questionnaire (ICQ;
of negative features, preserving the full set of interpersonal dimen- Buhrmester, Furman, Wittenberg, & Reis, 1988). The SSI is a
sions. For example, the pole opposite introversion is intrusiveness, measure of adult social skills. Its seven dimensions are emotional
a negative form of extraversion. expressivity, sensitivity, and control; and social expressivity, sen-
sitivity, control, and manipulation. Gurtman’s (1999) analysis of
the relationship of the SSI to the interpersonal circle using the IIP
Previous Research on Positive Interpersonal Features
indicated that all but two of its 60 items fell in the extraversion,
Although positive interpersonal features have not been a major friendliness, and dominance octants, only partially covering the
focus for interpersonal theorists, there has been broad interest in available range of interpersonal features. Buhrmester et al. (1988)
social competence (Rose-Krasnor, 1997). This diverse conceptual developed the ICQ by taking an interpersonal skills approach to
domain includes social skills and competence, interpersonal com- five key “interpersonal task domains.” These domains are “initia-
petence and capabilities, emotional intelligence, and assertiveness, tion of interactions and relationships,” “self-disclosure of personal
with measures for each category. These measures tend not to information,” “emotional support of others,” “management of in-
engage the full range of positive interpersonal features, however. terpersonal conflicts that arise in close relationships,” and “asser-
556 HATCHER AND ROGERS

tion of personal rights and displeasure with others” (p. 992). yielded a measure that assessed the general construct of interper-
Gurtman’s study demonstrated that the domains of the ICQ, like sonal strengths.
those of the SSI, all fell in the extraversion, friendliness, and We expected IIS items and octant scales to demonstrate cohe-
dominance octants of the interpersonal circle. siveness through large and equal participation in their common
interpersonal variable space, as shown by equal and large commu-
nalities in a circumplex model. We expected IIS octants to be
Obstacles to Developing a Full Positive/Adaptive/Normal equally spaced around the circumplex, as the model requires. We
Interpersonal Circle Measure expected a structural analysis of the relationship between the IIS
and the IIP to show the orderly, sinusoidal correlational patterns
The lack of adjectives describing relatively positive, adaptive
predicted by interpersonal theory. For example, the IIS coopera-
features on the left side of the interpersonal circle poses a chal-
tiveness variable should have its peak correlation with the IIP’s
lenge to conceptualizing and assessing these domains. If the IAS is
overly submissive octant, and should show an orderly shift around
any indication (e.g., “warmthless”), even established negative ad-
the circle toward a low point with the opposite octant, overly
jectives are hard to find, although Hofstee et al. (1992) populated
domineering. We expected the interpersonal factors of the Big Five
a fairly complete circle with common words such as bossy, sly,
to show the same orderly relationship with the IIS. These steps
abrupt, harsh, insincere, detached, cold, inhibited, glum, unsocia-
would validate the measure as a cohesive representative of the
ble, silent, timid, and naı̈ve. It is clear that a full adjective-based
interpersonal circle.
positive interpersonal circle measure is not possible.
If the IIS reflected positive, adaptive interpersonal strengths, we
Another obstacle is the overlap between introverted character-
expected positive correlations between each of its octant scales and
istics and various forms of pathology, including depression, anx-
measures of life satisfaction, relationship quality, and interpersonal
iety, narcissistic sensitivity, and the social withdrawal associated
competence (ICQ), and negative correlations with IIP octants and
with shame and grief (Gurtman, 1992; Horowitz, 2004). This is
with Big Five Neuroticism. We expected IIS octants on the left
true as well for the interpersonal domains that deal with submis-
side of the interpersonal circle to correlate negatively with neurot-
siveness, coldness, and disagreeableness. Pathologies related to
icism and the IIP. We expected respondents’ overall elevation
extraverted behaviors, such as hypomania, are less common. Fur-
(Gurtman & Balakrishnan, 1998), the mean IIS score, to account
ther, behaviors on the left side of the circumplex that may con-
for variance in life satisfaction above and beyond their elevation
tribute positively to interpersonal relationships when used appro-
on the IIP—showing that these features have explanatory power
priately are, when used excessively or exclusively, more socially
beyond the mere absence of interpersonal pathology (Peterson,
disruptive or difficult than those on the right side. For example,
2006b). If access to a wide range of positive interpersonal features
when used to excess, agreeableness is generally more acceptable
contributes to successful interpersonal relationships (Leary, 1957;
than clear boundary-setting. It is a challenge to find a path through
Paulhus & Martin, 1988), respondents who show more uniform
this terrain, creating items that effectively express these positive
levels of endorsement around the circle should have greater life
features but buffering them from more pathological/problematic
satisfaction and relationship quality.
interpretations. For example, although some persons may be un-
assured and submissive, others, sharing the same general disposi- Development of the IIS
tion, may be able to be cooperative, work well as an assistant, and
reach compromises with others. Some persons may tend to be This section reports the three phases of the development of the
disagreeable and mistrusting, yet others sharing this general dis- Inventory of Interpersonal Strengths (IIS). The first phase involved
position may be able to argue effectively with others, without initial item development, examination, and revision, with piloting
being arrogant, manipulative, or calculating. in a primarily undergraduate sample. The second phase used two
additional undergraduate samples to test the new item set and
refine a 64-item version. The third phase confirmed the structure of
Research Goals the 64-item set as a standalone measure in a new undergraduate
sample. Each of these phases is described as a separate study in
The goal of the present research was to develop and validate a this section of the report.
measure of positive interpersonal features for all domains of the The second section of the report details a study of the conver-
interpersonal circle, with robust circumplex properties, that would gent and discriminant validity of the final 64-item version. The IIS
assess dispositions toward others that are likely to be adaptive, was tested in relation to several established interpersonal mea-
positive, and routine. We conceptualized these features within the sures, and its ability to account for variance in life satisfaction and
broad construct of interpersonal strengths, which we viewed as relationship satisfaction was examined.
encompassing interpersonal features that contribute to interper-
sonal competence, emotional stability, fewer interpersonal prob- Study 1
lems, and better relationship outcomes, among other positive in- In this study, item development and initial testing for the IIS
dicators. As a result, the structure of some items would overlap to were conducted.
a degree with existing, single construct scales (e.g., traits, attitudes,
capabilities, efficacies, preferences). We saw the strengths con- Method
struct as similar to the inclusive interpersonal problems construct
that informs the IIP, with items that also overlap with single Participants
construct scales to some degree. This combination of single con- An undergraduate sample of 325 completed, for course credit, a
structs was deemed appropriate insofar as item development brief demographic questionnaire and an initial 149-item version of
INVENTORY OF INTERPERSONAL STRENGTHS 557

the IIS. The total sample, from a large Midwestern university, rotate the circumplex using an established criterion item (in this
included 134 men (41%) and 160 (49%) women, with 31 not case, “I can be very persuasive” at 90°). The items were divided
indicating gender (10%). The mean age was 20.0 years (range 18 into eight octant sets on the basis of their location within sequential
to 60). Participants were 5% African American, 11% Asian Amer- 45° ranges on the circumplex.
ican, 2.5% Latina or Latino, 76% Caucasian, and 5.5% other
(primarily biracial).
Results

Inventory of Interpersonal Strengths (IIS), Preliminary A total of 107 items met the criteria for retention. All IIS octants
Version had at least 10 items, and some had as many as 14. These initial
results, including octant item lists, were presented for review at the
An initial item set was developed by reviewing items on estab- Ohio Society for Psychotherapy Research meeting on April 8,
lished measures related to interpersonal functioning, including the 2006. The major critique was that the “warm–friendly” octant
Inventory of Interpersonal Problems (IIP; Alden et al., 1990; contained items with an active, assertive quality (e.g., “I pursue
Horowitz et al., 1988), the Interpersonal Adjective Scales (IAS; new relationships with others”), and other item content was sug-
Wiggins, 1995; Wiggins et al., 1988), the circumplex Big Five gested. An additional 42 items were developed to fill out the circle,
(Hofstee et al., 1992), and the Values in Action scale (Peterson, which, when added to the 107 retained items, brought the total
2006b). Of these, the IIP, with its sampling of a range of interper- number of items for testing in the next study back to 149.
sonal actions, attitudes, and values, had the greatest influence on
item development. In addition, the research team, composed of the
Study 2
authors and three other clinical psychologists, created items based
on a shared understanding of the interpersonal circle derived from This study aimed to identify a 64-item version of the IIS using
the above work and from Kiesler (1996), Hofstee et al. (1992), two separate samples.
Carson (1969), Leary (1957), and the work of Wiggins (e.g.,
Wiggins & Broughton, 1985) and Gurtman (e.g., Gurtman &
Balakrishnan, 1998). The research team chose to pursue a sentence Method
(vs. single word) format that did not use a fixed item stem (e.g.,
Participants
“I’m good at ______.,” “It’s easy for me to ______.”) so that the
IIS items varied in their basic structure. These decisions were Sample 1. An undergraduate sample of 563 completed, for
based on the goals of describing positive features on the left side course credit, a brief demographic questionnaire and the new 149
of the interpersonal circumplex and capturing the broad construct IIS item set produced by Study 1, along with other measures. Of
of interpersonal strengths. Based on the Big Five and the IAS, the the total sample, 231 participants were from a large Midwestern
IIS’s response scale reflects the respondent’s evaluation of how university, including 103 men (45%) and 127 (55%) women, with
characteristic these interpersonal features were of themselves. All 1 not indicating gender. The mean age was 18.6 years (range 18 to
items were required to clearly reference an aspect of interpersonal 26). Participants were 5% African American, 13% Asian Ameri-
relating, for example, “I’m warm with other people” versus “I am can, 2.6% Latina or Latino, 0.4% Native American, 73% Cauca-
a warm person.” In this way an initial list of 339 items was sian, and 5.6% other (primarily biracial), with two participants not
developed. The list was then sorted into groups of similar items, indicating race. The remainder of the sample consisted of 332
and redundant or unclear items were eliminated or rewritten. The participants from a medium-sized Southern university, including
items were piloted with a group of 20 staff and student trainees in 52 men (16%) and 279 (84%) women, with 1 not indicating
a university-based psychological clinic, with comments and dis- gender. Participants’ ages were not recorded, but in later samples
cussion in a group format. This additional feedback contributed to drawn from this same population, the mean age was 25.8 years.
the revision and selection of a set of 149 items for initial testing. Participants were 14% African American, 2% Asian American,
4% Latina or Latino, 1% Native American, 71% Caucasian, and
Procedure 8% other (primarily international students), with one participant
not indicating race.
Participants were directed to an institutional review board- Sample 2. An undergraduate sample of 326 completed the
approved website study containing a consent form, a demographic same protocol as Sample 1. Of the total sample, 153 participants
survey, and the preliminary 149-item version of the IIS. IIS items were from a large Midwestern university, including 62 men
were rated on a 6-point scale, ranging from very little like me to (40.5%) and 91 (59.5%) women. The mean age was 19.0 years
almost always like me. Three unrotated factors were retained from (range 18 to 37). Participants were 1% African American, 14%
principal axis factoring, one a general factor or positive manifold Asian American, 2.6% Latina or Latino, 2% Native American,
(Alden et al., 1990; Tracey, Rounds, & Gurtman, 1996), and two 74% Caucasian, and 6.6% other (primarily biracial), with 1 not
forming preliminary axes for circumplex analysis. Item loadings indicating race. The remainder of the sample consisted of 173
on these two factors were used to locate the items in circular space participants from a medium-sized Southern university, including
and to determine the length of the vector in the circular space 25 men (14%) and 148 (86%) women. As in the first sample, the
defined by the factors (Dryer & Horowitz, 1997; Locke, 2000). ages of these participants’ were not recorded. Participants were 9%
Items with vector length less than .15 were discarded (Alden et al., African American, 3% Asian American, 3% Latina or Latino, 1%
1990) and the remaining items were refactored. The location of Native American, 80% Caucasian, and 4% other (primarily bira-
axes in factor analysis is arbitrary, and common practice is to cial).
558 HATCHER AND ROGERS

Results 95% CI [168°, 186°]; Restrain ⫽ 233°, 95% CI [223°, 242°];


Cooperate ⫽ 268°, 95% CI [259°, 277°]; Consider ⫽ 307°, 95%
Item locations were generated in the first sample using the CI [300°, 314°]. The 95% CIs for each of these estimates included
method described in Study 1. Because the estimated circumplex the expected angular location for the octant except for the Consider
location of some items can vary considerably across samples, we octant, expected at 315°.
selected items separately (using the criteria of Study 1) for each of A second analytic method, Tracey’s (1997) RANDALL, was
the two samples, assigned them to octants, and compared the also used to assess the circumplex structure of the IIS. This method
results. Items that were close in location within their respective is based on the fact that the octant correlation matrix of a fully
octants in both samples (less than 22.5° apart) were retained, conforming circumplex has a mathematically determined pattern
yielding 10 to 16 items per octant. The eight items with the largest of ordered relations. The extent to which a given correlation matrix
vector lengths for each octant were selected for initial testing as demonstrates this ordering can be evaluated with an exact proba-
candidates for the 64-item version. bility, accompanied by a correspondence index. For the current
The resulting octant set was tested for circumplex characteristics sample p ⬍ .001, with a correspondence index of .96, indicating an
using CIRCUM (Browne, 1992), which provides a stringent con- excellent fit.
firmatory test for circumplex structure. Some trial and error was
required to create satisfactory item groups for octants. The result- Study 3
ing item set, the IIS-64, was designed to optimize fit under
CIRCUM analysis in both samples. Circumplex structure was This study was a confirmatory test for the IIS-64 structure as a
tested as a fully constrained model, with m ⫽ 3 and vector length standalone instrument.
and octant separation set as equal for all octants. The fit for the first
sample (N ⫽ 563) was good, with maximum likelihood estimated Method
root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) ⫽ .050, 90% Participants
CI [0.033, 0.066], with probability of “close” fit (Browne, 1992) of
.491, where any p ⬎ .05 is acceptable. The RMSEA is the An undergraduate sample of 684 completed, for course credit, a
preferred index for fit for CIRCUM (Browne, 1992); RMSEA is a brief demographic questionnaire and the 64-item version of the IIS
parsimony-weighted index of model fit, which takes into account developed in Study 2, along with other measures described in the
model complexity (Acton & Revelle, 2002). Browne and Cudeck second section of the report. Of the total sample, 265 participants
(1992) suggested that RMSEA values less than .08 indicate a were from a large Midwestern university, including 93 men (35%)
“reasonable” fit. The CIRCUM model fit for the second sample and 172 (65%) women. The mean age was 19.6 years (range 18 to
(N ⫽ 326) was also good, with RMSEA ⫽ .056 (90% CI [0.033, 56). Participants were 6% African American, 11% Asian Ameri-
0.079], close fit probability 0.311. The total combined sample can, 3% Latina or Latino, 1% Native American, 69% Caucasian,
(N ⫽ 889) also showed good fit, with RMSEA ⫽ 0.051 (90% CI and 9% other (primarily biracial), with 1 not indicating race. The
[0.039, 0.063]), close fit probability .439. This test confirms suc- remainder of the sample consisted of 419 participants from a
cess in selecting items that fit circumplex criteria in this sample, medium-sized Southern university, including 117 men (28%) and
and thus may be sample-specific. Independent confirmation is 302 (72%) women, whose mean age was 25.8 years (range 18 to
required. 56). Participants were 10% African American, 3% Asian Ameri-
The octants were named in a group meeting of 20 clinician- can, 4% Latina or Latino, 0.5% Native American, 77% Caucasian,
researchers, who developed and rank-ordered a list of possible and 15.5% other (primarily biracial). Participants were 10% Afri-
names (see Appendix for octant names and items). The actual can American, 3% Asian American, 4% Latina or Latino, 0.5%
angle location and vector length for each octant was determined Native American, 77% Caucasian, and 5.5% other (primarily bi-
using the combined samples (N ⫽ 889) by means of a CIRCUM racial).
analysis with unconstrained octant angular location and vector
length. With the Connect octant set at 0°, the estimates were as
Results
follows: Engage ⫽ 43°, 95% CI [37°, 50°]; Lead ⫽ 86°, 95% CI The IIS octant correlations for this confirmatory sample (N ⫽
[77°, 95°]; Direct ⫽ 127°, 95% CI [117°, 136°]; Balance ⫽ 177°, 684) are presented in Table 1, along with octant means, standard

Table 1
Intercorrelations, Means, Standard Deviations, Alpha Reliabilities, and Vector Lengths of IIS Subscales

IIS subscale Connect Engage Lead Direct Balance Restrain Cooperate Consider M SD ␣ Vector

Connect — 4.61 1.24 .93 .93


Engage .79 — 4.16 1.36 .84 .91
Lead .62 .72 — 4.20 1.32 .85 .92
Direct .48 .55 .78 — 4.10 1.29 .84 .92
Balance .45 .49 .65 .78 — 4.10 1.34 .76 .91
Restrain .53 .48 .57 .64 .74 — 4.23 1.24 .79 .91
Cooperate .66 .51 .50 .50 .62 .77 — 4.59 1.17 .84 .93
Consider .77 .64 .52 .43 .52 .66 .81 — 4.66 1.14 .90 .93

Note. N ⫽ 686. All correlations are significant at p ⬍ .01. IIS ⫽ Inventory of Interpersonal Strengths.
INVENTORY OF INTERPERSONAL STRENGTHS 559

deviations, alphas, and vector lengths. Octant means, which were gage octant, closer on the interpersonal circle to the dominant axis,
based on item endorsement, were in the higher range of the 6-point conveys energetic but caring engagement with others. The Lead
scale. Although the current study provided no data to suggest why octant focuses on competitiveness, taking charge, and assertive-
this might be, we hypothesize that on average the participants felt ness. The Direct octant, as in frank and forthright (Leary, 1957),
positively about their interpersonal strengths. Similar patterns have reflects the ability to pursue important purposes, including per-
been observed for endorsing positive interpersonal features on the sonal needs, without being derailed by the needs of others. As
BIC (Paulhus & Martin, 1987) and denying negative interpersonal such, it does not necessarily convey the qualities of disagreeable-
features on the IIP (Horowitz et al., 1988). ness, vindictiveness, or lack of concern for others that characterize
Octant scores were tested for circumplex characteristics using the related octant items found in the IAS and the IIP. In this and
CIRCUM in a fully constrained model, with m ⫽ 3. The fit was other octants on the left side of the circle, it was important to
good, with maximum likelihood estimated RMSEA ⫽ .055, 90% include more conditional descriptors (e.g., “I am comfortable
CI [0.041, 0.070], with probability of “close” fit of .252. Actual disagreeing with others”) so as to shift emphasis from more
octant angle location and vector length were estimated with extreme forms of this characteristic (e.g., “I disagree with others”).
CIRCUM analysis, with octant angular location and vector length The Balance octant conveys the ability or characteristic of taking
unconstrained. With the Connect octant set at 0°, the estimates for some distance from social involvement and pressures, while re-
the remaining octants were: Engage ⫽ 39°, 95% CI [32°, 46°]; taining a level of connection, care, and concern for others, as in “I
Lead ⫽ 98°, 95% CI [89°, 108°]; Direct ⫽ 141°, 95% CI [131°, can help others with their needs without neglecting my own.” Thus
152°]; Balance ⫽ 180°, 95% CI [169°, 190°]; Restrain ⫽ 228°, it reflects in a mild way the “coldness” that is the negative anchor
95% CI [218°, 238°]; Cooperate ⫽ 272°, 95% CI [262°, 281°]; of the warm– cold dimension. In this sense, Balance seems related
Consider ⫽ 309°, 95% CI [301°, 317°]. The 95% CIs for each of to “mindfulness,” (Kabat-Zinn, 1990), as it reflects the tendency to
these estimates included the expected angular location for the be resilient in respect to social pressures and one’s own social
octant. Figure 2 demonstrates the circumplex model for the IIS-64. needs. The Restrain octant concerns recognizing and respecting
The circumplex structure in this sample was also assessed with others’ autonomy and needs for privacy, being thoughtful about
Tracey’s (1997) RANDALL, yielding p ⬍ .001 (correspondence taking action, and remaining constructive when aggravated by
index ⫽ .97), indicating an excellent fit. others. In this way, Restrain reflects positive aspects of the ten-
dency toward shyness or caution in social interactions. The Coop-
Discussion erate octant is a self-respecting, positive form of the submissive-
ness domain, lacking the abject or dominated qualities found in
The development of the IIS-64 yielded a measure with excellent other measures. Thus it includes working to facilitate others’ goals,
circumplex properties, including conforming angular locations and expressing gratitude for others’ generosity, and respecting others’
vector lengths and good score reliabilities for each of the eight needs. Finally, the Consider octant contains familiar ideas of
octants, so that in further studies it is likely that reliable measure- loyalty, gratitude, and pleasure in helping others.
ment may be conducted at the octant level. The octants themselves The use of outside groups to critique the measure during devel-
have good face validities that seem to fulfill the goal of identifying opment provided an important quality control on the items and
positive features for the entire interpersonal circle, including the their coverage of the interpersonal circumplex, as, of course, did
left side of the circle. The Connect octant consists of items cap- the actual responses of our participants. The age range of partici-
turing a warm and comfortable connection with others. The En- pants was wider than is typical for studies utilizing undergraduate
participants, which makes it more likely that the measure will be
valid in other populations.

Study 4
The study in this second section of the report examined the
validity of the IIS, including its relationships to other measures of
interpersonal features and social skills. We first examined the
distribution of respondents’ peak values on the IIS, calculated as
the circular mean of the individual’s octant scores. Grouping these
values by octants yields eight interpersonal styles. We compared
the overall IIS levels between these groups, expecting equal en-
dorsement of positive interpersonal features across interpersonal
styles. We then related the IIS to the interpersonal factors of the
Big Five (Hofstee et al., 1992). The Big Five interpersonal factors
should map onto IIS interpersonal space, indicating that the IIS
covers the full range of interpersonal characteristics. We examined
the discriminant and convergent validity of IIS in relation to the
IIP and the BIC, and its structural convergence to these measures
of the interpersonal circle. We related the IIS to measures of
Figure 2. Circumplex structure of the Inventory of Interpersonal interpersonal competence, expecting to find that their scales would
Strengths, 64-item version (IIS-64). have peak correlations on corresponding IIS octant scores. We
560 HATCHER AND ROGERS

examined the differential predictive power of the IIS in relation to situation requires it?” Main items for each category are accompa-
measures of life and relationship satisfaction. nied by alternative descriptors (e.g., dominant: commanding,
forceful; quarrelsome: uncooperative, hostile). Answers are re-
corded on a 7-point Likert-type scale, ranging from totally inca-
Method
pable to easily capable. Hofsess and Tracey (2005) demonstrated
Participants the circumplex properties of the BIC using octant scores created by
combining adjacent items. Although they found low internal con-
This study utilized participants from Studies 2 and 3. To max- sistency for the resulting two-item scales, the BIC showed ade-
imize coverage of validation measures, different waves of partic- quate circular ordering. Reliabilities for the current sample of 106
ipants in these two studies completed different measures. participants (followed by those from Hofsess and Tracey) were PA
(ambitious– dominant) ⫽ .51 (.64); BC (arrogant– calculating) ⫽
.56 (.38); DE (cold– quarrelsome) ⫽ .44 (.63); FG (aloof–
Measures
introverted) ⫽ .65 (.70); HI (lazy–submissive) ⫽ .49 (.49); JK
Inventory of Interpersonal Strengths. Participants completed (unassuming–ingenuous) ⫽ .16 (.09); LM (warm–agreeable) ⫽
the IIS as described. T tests compared means and standard devia- .36 (.56), and NO (gregarious– extraverted) ⫽ .62 (.73).
tions for the 64 IIS items embedded within the 149-item IIS that Other measures. The Interpersonal Competence Question-
was completed in Study 2 (N ⫽ 693) with those from the separated naire (ICQ; Buhrmester et al., 1988) assesses interpersonal com-
IIS-64 from Study 3 (N ⫽ 685). The overall IIS means and those petencies in five key “interpersonal task domains.” These domains
for six of the eight octants did not differ significantly, nor did any were (reporting alphas from the Buhrmester et al., 1988, study and
of the standard deviations (using Levene’s test for equality of the current study, N ⫽ 220) as follows: Initiation of Interactions
variances). The means differed on the Lead octant (4.19 vs. 4.30, and Relationships (.83, .91), Self-Disclosure of Personal Informa-
t ⫽ –2.20, p ⬍ .05, d ⫽ 0.12) and the Consider octant (4.66 vs. tion (.75, .89), Emotional Support of Others (.78, .84), Manage-
4.76, t ⫽ –2.31, p ⬍ .05, d ⫽ 0.12). The overall level of agreement ment of Interpersonal Conflicts That Arise in Close Relationships
and the small effect sizes of the two statistically significant dif- (.80, .89), and Asserting Influence”(.65, .86). The alpha for the
ferences were judged to justify combining the 64 items from the total scale in the current sample was .95. All items begin with the
IIS versions for comparison with other measures administered in phrase, “How good are you at . . .” followed by fairly specific
both Study 2 and Study 3. content, such as “carrying on conversations with new people that
Measure of Big Five factors. The Mini-Modular Markers 40 you would like to know better.” Its 5-point scale ranges from 1 ⫽
(3M40) is a Big Five measure developed by Saucier (2002) with I’m poor at this; I’d feel so uncomfortable and unable to handle
five 8-item scales: Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientious- this situation, I’d avoid it if possible to 5 ⫽ I’m EXTREMELY good
ness, Emotional Stability, and Intellect. Each scale has four items at this; I’d feel very comfortable and could handle this situation
representing the opposite poles of the dimensions (e.g., Introver- very well.
sion, Disagreeableness). Items are rated on a 9-point scale ranging The Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS; Diener, Emmons,
from extremely unlike me to extremely like me. Across multiple Larsen, & Griffin, 1985; Pavot & Diener, 1993, 2008) is a widely
samples, Saucier reported alphas for self-ratings from .77 to .82 for used 5-item scale that yields respondents’ overall evaluation of the
Extraversion, .71 to .76 for Agreeableness, .76 to .84 for Consci- quality of their lives. Rated on a 7-point Likert-type scale ranging
entiousness, .67 to .72 for Emotional Stability, and .67 to .73 for from strongly disagree to strongly agree, the scale has demon-
Intellect. In the current sample (N ⫽ 643), alphas were .83, .80, strated excellent reliability (alphas in the mid to high .80s) and
.79, .76, and .81, respectively. good convergent and discriminant validity across many studies. In
Measures linked to the interpersonal circumplex. The Inven- the current study (N ⫽ 1,321), alpha was .87.
tory of Interpersonal Problems (IIP) is an eight-octant, 64-item The Perceived Relationship Quality Components Inventory
measure with demonstrated circumplex properties (Alden et al., (PRQC; Fletcher, Simpson, & Thomas, 2000) is an 18-item ques-
1990; Horowitz, Alden, Wiggins, & Pincus, 2000). The IIP mea- tionnaire derived and confirmed factor analytically in a college
sures problems in each octant of the interpersonal circle, describ- student population. Using a 7-point scale from not at all to ex-
ing interpersonal behaviors as “hard to do” or done “too much,” tremely, it assesses six domains of relationship quality in the
utilizing a 5-point Likert-type scale (not at all to extremely). The individual’s current partner/relationship, including overall degree
reliability and validity of the IIP have been supported in many of relationship satisfaction, commitment, intimacy, trust, passion,
studies (e.g., Gurtman, 1996). The octants and their alpha reliabili- and love. Fletcher et al. (2000) recommend a short version with
ties in the current sample (N ⫽ 963) were as follows: Overly one item from each cluster. In our samples, the “love” item
Nurturant (.80), Intrusive (.75), Domineering (.77), Vindictive lowered the scale’s alpha score reliability to .67 from the .88 with
(.79), Cold (.84), Socially Avoidant (.87), Nonassertive (.88), and the five remaining items, presumably because many participants
Exploitable (.81). Reliability of the full scale was .95. were in more casual relationships. This item was dropped.
The 16-item Battery of Interpersonal Capabilities-Short Form
(BIC; Paulhus & Martin, 1987) is based on the Interpersonal
Results
Adjective Scales (IAS; Wiggins et al., 1988). Each BIC question
reflects a 22.5° section of the IAS circumplex. Respondents rate IIS Profile Peaks
their capability of enacting, in appropriate situations, each of 16
interpersonal behaviors. A global capability question is asked for Each respondent to the IIS has a peak value on the IIS circle,
each behavior: “How likely is it that you could be _______ if the calculated as a circular mean of the individual’s octant scores. This
INVENTORY OF INTERPERSONAL STRENGTHS 561

point represents the individual’s salient interpersonal quality or range from .27 to .55, with six out of eight pairings matching
theme as measured by the IIS (Gurtman & Balakrishnan, 1998). expectations. The two that did not were off by one octant: the IIS
Formally known as the angular displacement of a circular profile, Consider octant had its largest correlation with IIP Overly Nur-
these points were calculated (Wiggins, Phillips, & Trapnell, 1989) turant (r ⫽ .41), one octant away from the expected IIP Exploit-
for the full sample (N ⫽ 1,378) and identified by their octant able, which had the next largest correlation (r ⫽ .34); the IIS
location. Peak values were fairly evenly distributed around the Connect octant had slightly larger correlations (r ⫽ .33 and .31) on
circle: Connect ⫽ 11% (IIS M ⫽ 4.33), Engage ⫽ 11% (4.51), the two IIP octants surrounding the expected IIP Overly Nurturant
Lead ⫽ 14% (4.24), Direct ⫽ 16% (4.18), Balance ⫽ 13% (4.48), octant (r ⫽ .27). Octant scores on one measure should also show
Restrain ⫽ 11% (4.42), Cooperate ⫽ 12% (4.32), and Consider ⫽ their largest negative correlations with the other measure’s octants
12% (4.33). A one-way ANOVA demonstrated significant differ- that are located on the opposite side of the circle. The largest
ences, though with small effect size, between these means F(7, negative correlations of the IIS on the IIP ranged from –.31 to –.55
1378) ⫽ 4.85, p ⬍ .001, ␩2 ⫽ .02. Post hoc comparisons (Scheffe) and matched expectations, except for the IIP Overly Nurturant
found significant differences only for Direct (lower) compared to octant, which had its largest correlation with IIS Direct (r ⫽ –.49),
Engage and Balance. Thus overall level of endorsement of IIS one octant away from the expected IIS Balance, which had the next
features did not vary much across profile peak groups. largest correlation (r ⫽ –.31). This pattern of correlations demon-
strates a somewhat closer circumplex matching between the IIS
Location of the IIS in Big Five Interpersonal Space and the IIP than was found by Alden et al. in their confirmatory
study of the IIP with the IAS.
The octants of the IIS were correlated with each of the Big Five This pattern of structural convergence is illustrated in Figure 4,
factors (see Table 2). The first two factors of the Big Five, which plots the IIP and IIS octants on common axes as determined
Extraversion–Introversion and Agreeableness–Disagreeableness, by principal components analysis (Alden et al., 1990). The two
define a widely recognized version of interpersonal space (Hofstee components accounted for 56% of the variance in the 16 combined
et al., 1992). The circumplex relationship between the IIS and octant scales. Table 4 provides the component loadings, commu-
these dimensions is shown in Table 2, but the sinusoidal relation- nalities, vector lengths (amplitude), rotated angular locations (IIP
ship that is the hallmark of a circumplex-based relationship is more Domineering set at 90°), and difference in angular locations for
readily perceived when graphed, as in Figure 3. The Extraversion each of the IIP and IIS octants. Each member of the octant pairs is
scale peaks as expected on IIS Engage (r ⫽ .56), and has its within half an octant (22.5°) of the other, except for Connect and
minimum on IIS Cooperate (r ⫽ .08), closely followed by the Overly Nurturant, which are 24.2° apart. The mean difference is
expected IIS Restrain (r ⫽ .09). The Agreeableness scale has its 12.3°. Similar to the findings of Alden et al. (1990) for the IIP and
predicted minimum on IIS Direct (r ⫽ .08), and its maximum is at the IAS, communalities range from .46 to .63. These results
IIS Cooperate (r ⫽ .53), closely followed by IIS Consider (r ⫽ demonstrate that the two octant sets share a common circular
.52). The remaining Big Five Factors showed a common pattern of structure.
small correlations with all IIS octants, with slightly larger corre- The mean IIP and IIS scores, reflecting an overall assessment of
lations on the left side of the interpersonal circle. interpersonal distress and endorsement of more positive interper-
sonal features, respectively, correlated –.43. Persons who more
Structural Convergence of the IIP and IIS strongly endorse interpersonal features on the IIS show lower
levels of interpersonal distress, thus demonstrating the relative
The correlations between the octants of the IIS and the IIP in
positiveness and adaptiveness of IIS items.
Table 3 help confirm the structural convergence of these two
interpersonal measures. Following Alden et al. (1990), we used
ipsitized scores for both measures. The diagonal of the correlation Relationship of the IIS to Measures of Social
matrix compares IIS and IIP octants that are expected to occupy Skills/Competence
the same space on the interpersonal circle. Structural convergence
is demonstrated by peak positive correlations on corresponding The 16-item Battery of Interpersonal Capabilities (BIC) was
octants and peak negative correlations on opposite octants. Corre- converted to eight octant scales following Hofsess and Tracey
lations of the IIS on the criterion IIP along the positive diagonal (2005). Because the BIC is based on the IAS, with structure

Table 2
Correlations of IIS Subscales With Big Five Factors

IIS subscale

Big Five factor Connect Engage Lead Direct Balance Restrain Cooperate Consider IIS mean

Extraversion .45 .56 .50 .30 .17 .09 .08 .25 .39
Agreeableness .46 .31 .16 .08 .20 .37 .53 .52 .40
Conscientiousness .01 ⫺.01 .15 .19 .19 .20 .09 .08 .14
Emotional Stability ⫺.04 .02 .11 .17 .24 .18 .05 ⫺.04 .10
Openness .01 .08 .17 .18 .14 .23 .14 .11 .16

Note. N ⫽ 663. rs ⬎ .07 are significant at p ⬍ .05. rs ⬎ .10 are significant at p ⬍ .01. IIS ⫽ Inventory of Interpersonal Strengths.
562 HATCHER AND ROGERS

Figure 3. The circumplex features of the correlations between the Inventory of Interpersonal Strengths (IIS)
and Big Five Extraversion and Agreeableness.

described as bipolar by Wiggins (1979), we expected to find endorse being capable of acting in a crafty, cunning, sly, and
positive correlations between octants on the upper right quadrant, calculating manner.
corresponding to IIS Lead (PA), Engage (NO), and Connect (LM). To clarify the relationships between the IIS and BIC octants we
We expected negative correlations between the IIS octants Coop- examined interoctant correlations (see Table 5). Here we find BIC
erate (JK), Restrain (HI), Balance (FG), and their counterparts on Warm–Trusting has its peak correlation with IIS Connect (r ⫽
the BIC, which mirror the generally negative adjectives on the left .28), BIC Extraverted–Gregarious has its peak correlation with IIS
side of the IAS interpersonal circle. For example, the BIC Meek- Engage (r ⫽ .63), and BIC Dominant–Assured has its peak cor-
Submissive scale refers to how capable the respondent is of be- relation with IIS Lead (r ⫽ .48), all as expected. BIC Crafty–
having in a submissive, forceless, yielding, weak, timid, or shy in Boastful has its peak correlation with IIS Direct (r ⫽ .24), and BIC
manner, when the social situation requires it. We were less con- Cold–Quarrelsome has overall negative correlations, with the least
fident about how the two IIS octants on the borderline between negative of these with its counterpart, IIS Balance (r ⫽ –.01). BIC
these sets, Consider and Direct, would relate to the BIC. IIS Introverted–Aloof is negatively correlated with all IIS octants,
Consider has many elements that would make it relate more with the least negative of these with IIS Cooperate (r ⫽ –.17)
strongly to BIC Warm–Trusting and Extraverted–Gregarious, and instead of with its counterpart, IIS Restrain (r ⫽ –.28). BIC
individuals endorsing IIS Direct items, we believed, might well Meek–Submissive has its most positive correlation with its coun-

Table 3
Correlations of IIP and IIS Subscales and Mean Scores

IIS subscale

IIP subscale Connect Engage Lead Direct Balance Restrain Cooperate Consider IIS mean

Overly Nurturant .27 .15 ⫺.15 ⫺.49 ⫺.31 ⫺.04 .14 .41 ⫺.13
Intrusive .33 .50 .25 .00 ⫺.28 ⫺.47 ⫺.42 ⫺.05 ⫺.18
Domineering ⫺.11 .19 .46 .44 .09 ⫺.29 ⫺.55 ⫺.33 ⫺.26
Vindictive ⫺.20 .05 .35 .55 .18 ⫺.14 ⫺.38 ⫺.48 ⫺.36
Cold ⫺.35 ⫺.23 .17 .41 .27 .16 ⫺.07 ⫺.34 ⫺.39
Socially Avoidant ⫺.36 ⫺.54 ⫺.23 .12 .30 .38 .38 .07 ⫺.46
Nonassertive .13 ⫺.16 ⫺.46 ⫺.40 ⫺.04 .25 .48 .30 ⫺.40
Exploitable .31 .11 ⫺.34 ⫺.57 ⫺.20 .08 .31 .34 ⫺.27
IIP mean ⫺.30 ⫺.32 ⫺.38 ⫺.44 ⫺.44 ⫺.34 ⫺.28 ⫺.25 ⫺.43

Note. N ⫽ 985. rs ⬎ .06 are significant at p ⬍ .05. rs ⬎ .09 are significant at p ⬍ .01. IIP ⫽ Inventory of Interpersonal Problems; IIS ⫽ Inventory of
Interpersonal Strengths.
INVENTORY OF INTERPERSONAL STRENGTHS 563

features were most involved in interpersonal competence for ad-


olescents (Buhrmester et al., 1988). The ICQ was analyzed by
Gurtman (1999) for its range of coverage of interpersonal features
using the IIP as an indicator of the interpersonal circle. Based on
his findings and our understanding of the IIS, we expected the
following peak correlations with IIS octants: Emotional Support of
Others (IIS Consider), Initiation of Interactions and Relationships
(Engage), Asserting Influence (Lead/Direct), Self-Disclosure of
Personal Information (Engage), and Management of Interpersonal
Conflicts That Arise in Close Relationships (Consider). The cor-
relations of IIS octants and ICQ scales are presented in Table 5.
ICQ Emotional Support had its peak correlation on Consider (r ⫽
.60). Initiation of Interactions had its peak with the expected
Engage (r ⫽ .60). Asserting Influence had, as expected, its highest
correlations with Lead (r ⫽ .61) and Direct (r ⫽ .57), and Self-
Disclosure peaked on Engage (r ⫽ .48) as predicted. Management
of Conflict did not have a distinct peak on the IIS octants, with
evenly spread correlations in the .44 to .47 range across Balance,
Restrain, Cooperate, and Consider. These results confirm Gurt-
man’s IIP-based analysis of the ICQ’s interpersonal locations and
lend further validation to the IIS as a measure of the interpersonal
circle. The correlation between the mean scores of the ICQ and the
Figure 4. Structural convergence of the octants of the Inventory of IIS (r ⫽ .60) demonstrated a good level of convergent validity with
Interpersonal Strengths (IIS) and Inventory of Interpersonal Problems (IIP) this measure of adaptive, positive interpersonal capabilities.
on shared axes. Standard general octant labels (e.g., PA, NO) identify
equivalent IIS and IIP octants.
Differences in Peak Profile Octant Groups on the IIP
and ICQ
terpart IIS Cooperate (r ⫽ .01). Finally, BIC Humble–Agreeable If the IIS measures positive interpersonal features in all facets of
has mostly low positive correlations with IIS octants, with its peak the interpersonal circle, we would expect few to no differences
on IIS Cooperate (r ⫽ .25) instead of with its counterpart, IIS between peak profile groups on measures of interpersonal distress
Consider (r ⫽ .12). These results are not surprising. We expected and interpersonal competence. Two one-way ANOVAs were con-
that the BIC’s positive capabilities would relate strongly to the ducted for the eight peak profile groups with IIP (N ⫽ 984) and
positive features identified by the IIS, and that the capabilities tied ICQ (N ⫽ 410) mean scores as the dependent variables. For the
to what we would consider relatively negative features (e.g., quar- IIP, the analysis of variance (ANOVA() was significant, F(7,
relsomeness) would correlate negatively. This result is reflected in 976) ⫽ 5.81, p ⬍ .001, ␩2 ⫽ .04. In post hoc analyses (Scheffe)
the lack of correlation of mean BIC and IIS scores (r ⫽ .00). the Connect group showed greater IIP distress than Direct ( p ⬍
Despite the divergence in orientation between these two mea- .05), and Balance showed less distress than Connect ( p ⬍ .001),
sures, they in fact share the interpersonal circle as a common Cooperate ( p ⬍ .01), and Consider ( p ⬍ .05). For the ICQ, the
conceptual space. We projected the two measures onto common ANOVA was not significant, F(7, 402) ⫽ 1.78. Together these
interpersonal axes, accounting for 51% of the overall variance, to results point to a uniform distribution of positive interpersonal
assess the overlap in interpersonal space of the two measures (see capabilities for persons with profile peaks in all eight octants, and
Table 4). Here we find somewhat more variation in communality, rather minor differences in interpersonal distress.
ranging from .36 to .66, and somewhat greater interoctant differ-
ences in angular location, with a mean difference of 17.2°, than Accounting for Life Satisfaction and Relationship Quality:
that found in the IIS–IIP comparison. All the differences except Positive Interpersonal Features Versus Interpersonal
one indicate that the BIC octants seem to be rotated approximately
Problems and IAS-Based Capabilities
15° clockwise from the IIS octants, perhaps resulting from the way
the items of the BIC scales are distributed across its 16 scales. For An important proposition of interpersonal psychology is that
example, the Trusting component of BIC Warm–Trusting (NO) deficits are not the opposite of strengths (Carson, 1969; Horowitz,
includes the descriptors trusting, with synonyms gullible and na- 2004; Kiesler, 1996). Positive interpersonal features are consid-
ı̈ve. These items are closer to the content of the IIS Consider octant ered indicators of adaptive, interpersonally creative characteristics
(LM) and the IIP Overly Nurturant Octant (LM). This likely that exceed the absence of deficit (Peterson, 2006a). To demon-
accounts for the 17.2° clockwise shift of the BIC relative to the IIS. strate that the IIS brings added value in accounting for variation in
Overall, we found that the BIC and IIS share common interper- life satisfaction and relationship quality, separate regressions were
sonal space. conducted with the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) and the
Unlike the BIC, the Interpersonal Competence Questionnaire Perceived Relationship Components Questionnaire (PRCQ) as de-
(ICQ) was not designed to assess competence in all eight inter- pendent variables and the mean IIP and the mean IIS as indepen-
personal domains. Rather, it was based on research showing which dent variables. Hierarchical regression for these variables, with IIP
564 HATCHER AND ROGERS

Table 4
Factor Loadings, Communality Values, Vector Lengths, Rotated Angles, and Angular Differences
for IIS, IIP, and BIC Circumplex Scales

Octant and scales Factor 1 Factor 2 h2 Vector Angle Difference

IIS–IIPa
LM
IIS Connect ⫺.363 ⫺.626 .52 .72 353.2
IIP Overly Nurturant ⫺.573 ⫺.411 .50 .71 329.0 24.2
NO
IIS Engage .062 ⫺.766 .59 .77 27.9
IIP Intrusive .159 ⫺.769 .62 .79 35.0 7.1
PA
IIS Lead .615 ⫺.314 .48 .69 86.2
IIP Domineering .729 ⫺.314 .63 .79 90.0 3.8
BC
IIS Direct .777 .234 .66 .81 130.1
IIP Vindictive .781 .045 .61 .78 116.6 13.5
DE
IIS Balance .332 .580 .45 .67 173.5
IIP Cold .532 .483 .52 .72 155.5 17.9
FG
IIS Restrain ⫺.216 .672 .50 .71 221.1
IIP Socially Avoidant ⫺.077 .768 .60 .77 209.0 12.1
HI
IIS Cooperate ⫺.625 .487 .63 .79 255.4
IIP Nonassertive ⫺.674 .320 .56 .75 267.9 12.5
JK
IIS Consider ⫺.677 ⫺.074 .46 .68 299.6
IIP Exploitable ⫺.754 ⫺.180 .60 .78 306.7 7.1
Mean angular difference 12.3

IIS–BICb
LM
IIS Connect ⫺.367 .475 .36 .60 10.7
BIC Warm–Trusting ⫺.226 .630 .45 .67 352.8 17.9
NO
IIS Engage ⫺.728 .111 .54 .74 54.3
BIC Extraverted–Gregarious ⫺.733 .344 .66 .81 37.8 16.5
PA
IIS Lead ⫺.555 ⫺.587 .65 .81 109.6
BIC Dominant–Assured ⫺.636 ⫺.325 .51 .71 90.0 19.6
BC
IIS Direct ⫺.009 ⫺.783 .61 .78 152.3
BIC Crafty–Boastful ⫺.295 ⫺.707 .59 .77 130.4 21.9
DE
IIS Balance .606 ⫺.302 .46 .68 216.5
BIC Cold–Quarrelsome .441 ⫺.373 .33 .58 202.8 13.7
FG
IIS Restrain .561 .009 .31 .56 243.9
BIC Introverted–Aloof .760 ⫺.197 .62 .78 228.4 15.5
HI
IIS Cooperate .602 .464 .58 .76 280.6
BIC Meek–Submissive .770 .157 .62 .79 254.5 26.1
JK
IIS Consider .026 .720 .52 .72 330.9
BIC Humble–Agreeable ⫺.039 .619 .38 .62 336.6 5.7
Mean angular difference 17.1

Note. h2 ⫽ communality; Angle ⫽ rotated angle, in degrees; Diff. ⫽ angular difference, in degrees; IIS ⫽
Inventory of Interpersonal Strengths; IIP ⫽ Inventory of Interpersonal Problems; BIC ⫽ Battery of Interpersonal
Capabilities.
a
N ⫽ 984. b N ⫽ 106.

and IIS means entered sequentially, are displayed in Table 6. The correlations were found between IIS octants on the left side of the
mean IIS score accounts for a significant increment in variance for interpersonal circle and mean SWLS: Direct (.28), Balance (.32),
both life satisfaction and relationship quality beyond the mean IIP. and Restrain (.24). Similar, though smaller, positive correlations
Correlations between IIS octants and mean SWLS (see Table 5) were found between IIS octants and the PRCQ (see Table 5),
ranged from .17 (Cooperate) to .33 (Engage); as predicted, positive ranging from .12 (Direct) to .20 (Consider). All correlations were
INVENTORY OF INTERPERSONAL STRENGTHS 565

Table 5
Correlations of IIS Subscales and Criteria Measures

IIS subscale

Criteria measure Connect Engage Lead Direct Balance Restrain Cooperate Consider IIS mean

BICa
Warm–Trusting .28ⴱⴱ .20ⴱ .08 .00 .17 .15 .27ⴱⴱ .25ⴱⴱ .22ⴱ
Extraverted–Gregarious .55ⴱⴱ .63ⴱⴱ .49ⴱⴱ .37ⴱⴱ .21ⴱ .24ⴱ .32ⴱⴱ .39ⴱⴱ .50ⴱⴱ
Dominant–Assured .23ⴱ .28ⴱⴱ .48ⴱⴱ .37ⴱⴱ .11 .13 .09 .06 .27ⴱⴱ
Crafty–Boastful ⫺.03 .04 .20ⴱ .24ⴱ ⫺.04 ⫺.16 ⫺.18 ⫺.21ⴱ ⫺.02
Cold–Quarrelsome ⫺.13 ⫺.20ⴱ ⫺.16 ⫺.01 ⫺.01 ⫺.19 ⫺.06 ⫺.21ⴱ ⫺.15
Introverted–Aloof ⫺.40ⴱⴱ ⫺.50ⴱⴱ ⫺.39ⴱⴱ ⫺.24ⴱ ⫺.18 ⫺.28ⴱⴱ ⫺.17 ⫺.31ⴱⴱ ⫺.39ⴱⴱ
Meek–Submissive ⫺.22ⴱ ⫺.43ⴱⴱ ⫺.40ⴱⴱ ⫺.27ⴱⴱ ⫺.14 ⫺.24ⴱ .01 ⫺.24ⴱ ⫺.30ⴱⴱ
Humble–Agreeable .16 .04 ⫺.04 ⫺.05 .08 .11 .25ⴱⴱ .12 .10
BIC mean .05 ⫺.04 .02 .08 .01 ⫺.09 .08 ⫺.08 .00
ICQb
Emotional Support .49ⴱⴱ .40ⴱⴱ .34ⴱⴱ .27ⴱⴱ .30ⴱⴱ .39ⴱⴱ .50ⴱⴱ .60ⴱⴱ .51ⴱⴱ
Initiating Interactions .54ⴱⴱ .60ⴱⴱ .43ⴱⴱ .28ⴱⴱ .23ⴱⴱ .24ⴱⴱ .23ⴱⴱ .34ⴱⴱ .45ⴱⴱ
Asserting Influence .32ⴱⴱ .46ⴱⴱ .61ⴱⴱ .57ⴱⴱ .45ⴱⴱ .36ⴱⴱ .25ⴱⴱ .29ⴱⴱ .51ⴱⴱ
Self-Disclosure .39ⴱⴱ .44ⴱⴱ .28ⴱⴱ .23ⴱⴱ .18ⴱⴱ .18ⴱⴱ .25ⴱⴱ .36ⴱⴱ .36ⴱⴱ
Management of Conflict .36ⴱⴱ .38ⴱⴱ .36ⴱⴱ .34ⴱⴱ .45ⴱⴱ .48ⴱⴱ .44ⴱⴱ .45ⴱⴱ .50ⴱⴱ
ICQ mean .55ⴱⴱ .60ⴱⴱ .51ⴱⴱ .43ⴱⴱ .41ⴱⴱ .41ⴱⴱ .42ⴱⴱ .52ⴱⴱ .60ⴱⴱ
SWLSc .30ⴱⴱ .33ⴱⴱ .29ⴱⴱ .28ⴱⴱ .32ⴱⴱ .24ⴱⴱ .17ⴱⴱ .23ⴱⴱ .34ⴱⴱ
PRQCd .18ⴱⴱ .14ⴱⴱ .13ⴱⴱ .12ⴱⴱ .14ⴱⴱ .15ⴱⴱ .13ⴱⴱ .20ⴱⴱ .22ⴱⴱ

Note. IIS ⫽ Inventory of Interpersonal Strengths; BIC ⫽ Battery of Interpersonal Capabilities; ICQ ⫽ Interpersonal Competence Questionnaire; SWLS ⫽
Satisfaction With Life Scale; PRQC ⫽ Perceived Relationship Quality Components Inventory.
a
N ⫽ 108. b N ⫽ 410. c N ⫽ 1,315. d N ⫽ 1,277.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01.

positive and significant, including those on the left side of the SWLS. The BIC capability octants on the left side of the circle, we
interpersonal circle. As part of the effort to show that the IIS also note, are negatively associated with SWLS.
indicates positive, adaptive interpersonal characteristics at all Finally, we were interested in persons whose endorsements of
points around the interpersonal circle, we compared the octants of positive interpersonal features are more uniformly distributed.
the IIS and the BIC, a measure of interpersonal capabilities, in Some interpersonal theorists suggest that such persons should have
relation to the SWLS with a series of hierarchical multiple regres- more ready and flexible access to a range of alternative ways of
sions (see Table 7). Here we find that seven of the eight IIS octants processing interpersonal experience, and on the basis of this rea-
are associated with significant incremental variance in the SWLS soning, we expected that they would report fewer interpersonal
beyond those of the BIC. The IIS octant Cooperate is eclipsed by problems, greater life satisfaction, and better relationship-forming
BIC Meek–Submissive, which is negatively associated with skills (Leary, 1957; Wiggins et al., 1989), although this claim has
been questioned (Gurtman & Balakrishnan, 1998; Pincus & Gurt-
man, 2006). To test this hypothesis, we utilized a measure of octant
Table 6 variability (amplitude; Gurtman & Balakrishnan, 1998), where
Hierarchical Regression Analyses Predicting Satisfaction With greater variability is presumed to indicate less flexibility. We
Life and Relationship Quality found that octant variability had small correlations in the expected
directions with interpersonal problems (IIP; r ⫽ .19, p ⬍ .001,
Variable R2 ⌬R2 B SE B ␤ N ⫽ 984); life satisfaction (SWLS; r ⫽ –.12, p ⬍ .001, N ⫽
1,314); relationship-forming ability (ICQ; r ⫽ –.15, p ⬍ .001, N ⫽
Satisfaction with lifea 410) and the IIS mean score (r ⫽ –.15, p ⬍ .001, N ⫽ 1,379).
Step 1
IIP mean .10 ⫺.79 .08 ⫺.31ⴱⴱⴱ
Step 2 General Discussion
IIP mean ⫺.53 .08 ⫺.21ⴱⴱⴱ
ⴱⴱⴱ
IIS mean .15 .05 .48 .06 .24ⴱⴱⴱ The Inventory of Interpersonal Strengths (IIS) was developed to
Relationship qualityb provide a circumplex-based measure of positive interpersonal fea-
Step 1
IIP mean .05 ⫺.62 .09 ⫺.23ⴱⴱⴱ tures for the full interpersonal circle. We conceptualized these
Step 2 positive features as interpersonal strengths, encompassing a wide
IIP mean ⫺.45 .09 ⫺.17ⴱⴱ range of qualities that include interpersonal competencies, traits,
ⴱⴱⴱ
IIS mean .07 .02 .31 .07 .15ⴱⴱⴱ and values. Items reflect the positive aspects of each octant’s
Note. IIP ⫽ Inventory of Interpersonal Problems.
particular interpersonal quality, including octants heretofore gen-
a
N ⫽ 973. b N ⫽ 948. erally characterized as negative. For example, the general tone of
ⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001. the “introverted–inhibited” octant, of holding back in interpersonal
566 HATCHER AND ROGERS

Table 7
Hierarchical Regression Analyses Predicting Satisfaction With Life: BIC Versus IIS Octants

Satisfaction with life R2 ⌬R2 B SE B ␤

Step 1
BIC Warm–Trusting .02 .14 .11 .13
Step 2
BIC Warm–Trusting .08 .11 .07
IIS Connect .05 .04ⴱⴱ .25 .12 .20ⴱⴱ
Step 1
BIC Extraverted–Gregarious .08 .26 .08 .29
Step 2
BIC Extraverted–Gregarious .09 .11 .10
IIS Engage .14 .05ⴱⴱ .35 .14 .30ⴱⴱ
Step 1
BIC Dominant–Assured .02 .10 .08 .13
Step 2
BIC Dominant–Assured ⫺.02 .09 ⫺.02
IIS Lead .09 .08ⴱⴱⴱ .38 .13 .31ⴱⴱⴱ
Step 1
BIC Crafty–Boastful .00 ⫺.05 .07 ⫺.07
Step 2
BIC Crafty–Boastful ⫺.10 .07 ⫺.13
IIS Direct .06 .06ⴱⴱ .34 .13 .25ⴱⴱ
Step 1
BIC Cold–Quarrelsome .05 ⫺.18 .08 ⫺.22
Step 2
BIC Cold–Quarrelsome ⫺.10 .07 ⫺.13
IIS Balance .10 .06ⴱⴱ .36 .14 .23ⴱⴱ
Step 1
BIC Introverted–Aloof .04 ⫺.14 .07 ⫺.11
Step 2
BIC Introverted–Aloof ⫺.08 .07 ⫺.11
IIS Restrain .13 .09ⴱⴱⴱ .50 .15 .31ⴱⴱⴱ
Step 1
BIC Meek–Submissive .04 ⫺.14 .07 ⫺.19
Step 2
BIC Meek–Submissive ⫺.14 .07 ⫺.19ⴱⴱ
IIS Cooperate .05 .01 .17 .14 .12
Step 1
BIC Humble–Agreeable .01 .08 .10 .08
Step 2
BIC Humble–Agreeable .05 .10 .05
IIS Consider .08 .07ⴱⴱⴱ .30 .11 .26ⴱⴱⴱ

Note. N ⫽ 105. BIC ⫽ Battery of Interpersonal Capabilities; IIS ⫽ Inventory of Interpersonal Strengths.
ⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.

situations, is reflected in a positive form in items such as “I respect We found that peak profile locations showed a reasonably
others’ needs for privacy.” Overall, we have provided evidence uniform distribution of central interpersonal themes for study
that these strengths, as measured by the IIS, are associated with participants across all octants around the IIS circle. There were no
better life outcomes, fewer interpersonal problems, more interper- detected differences in the levels of interpersonal competence
sonal competence, and more emotional stability. among these groups, and just a few differences in the levels of
The difficulty in finding positive adjectives for the left side of interpersonal distress. This supports the claim that the IIS has
the circle, evident in the IAS and the Big Five circumplex scales, identified a uniform set of positive, adaptive interpersonal features
led us to conclude that sentences would better describe positive around the interpersonal circle. Overall, these results show that the
interpersonal features, as exemplified by the IIP for problematic IIS can help identify large numbers of people whose positive,
features. The process of item development and selection produced adaptive modes of interpersonal engagement may not have previ-
a measure with strong circumplex properties. Locating the IIS ously been recognized. Further, the positive association with in-
within the interpersonal nomological net defined by the Big Five, terpersonal competence (ICQ) provided convergent validation of
the IIP, and the BIC demonstrated that this instrument participates the IIS, and the negative association with the IIP, divergent vali-
fully and consistently in the domain of interpersonal measurement. dation. The location of ICQ subscales on the IIS circle was as
Correlations with the non-interpersonal Big Five dimensions were predicted based on Gurtman’s (1999) interpersonal analyses of the
small and low compared to those of the Big Five interpersonal ICQ. BIC items measuring positive capabilities (on the right side
dimensions. of the circle) and negative capabilities (on the left) generally
INVENTORY OF INTERPERSONAL STRENGTHS 567

followed expectations for their associations with IIS octants, ex- scured if emphasis is given to deficits and symptoms. Such infor-
cept for IIS Direct and BIC Crafty–Boastful, which had a small mation could then contribute to providing feedback during assess-
positive correlation. This finding suggests that persons endorsing ment and to establishing goals for treatment. The measure may
higher levels of IIS Direct features, such as “I know how to look prove to be a valuable predictor of success in settings where
after my own interests,” or “I can make a decision even if others positive interpersonal features are important to effective function-
disagree,” are more likely to feel capable of being “boastful/ ing, such as in the workplace or in family relationships. Finally, the
conceited, arrogant” and “crafty/cunning, sly, calculating,” when IIS may be a valuable measure of change in any setting that aims
the situation requires it. It remains unclear what situation would to improve interpersonal functioning.
require being boastful, conceited, or arrogant, however.
Hierarchical regressions of IIP and IIS mean scores on measures References
of life satisfaction and relationship quality indicated that the IIS as
a measure of positive interpersonal features adds explanatory Acton, G. S., & Revelle, W. (2002). Interpersonal personality measures
power over and above the relative absence of interpersonal dis- show circumplex structure based on new psychometric criteria. Journal
of Personality Assessment, 79, 446 – 471.
tress. Hierarchical regressions of BIC and IIS octant scores on the
Alden, L. E., Wiggins, J. S., & Pincus, A. L. (1990). Construction of
measure of life satisfaction showed that the IIS, and particularly circumplex scales for the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems. Journal
the IIS octant scores on the left side of the interpersonal circle, of Personality Assessment, 55, 521–536.
provided greater explanatory leverage than a measure of interper- Browne, M. W. (1992). Circumplex models for correlation matrices. Psy-
sonal capabilities. It should be noted that the reliabilities of the chometrika, 57, 469 – 497.
BIC are often suppressed due to the two-item scales, and as found Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1992). Alternative ways of assessing model
in previous research. The results of the hierarchical regression fit. Sociological Methods and Research, 21, 230 –258.
must be interpreted with this issue in mind. Buhrmester, D., Furman, W., Wittenberg, M. T., & Reis, H. T. (1988). Five
Finally, the idea that more uniform access to the full range of domains of interpersonal competence in peer relationships. Journal of
positive interpersonal features would be associated with greater Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 991–1008.
Carson, R. C. (1969). Interaction concepts of personality. Chicago, IL:
life satisfaction, fewer interpersonal problems, and greater inter-
Aldine.
personal competence received some support in this study. Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The
Satisfaction With Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49,
Study Limitations 71–75.
Dryer, D. C., & Horowitz, L. M. (1997). When do opposites attract?
This study was based on undergraduate samples. Other inter- Interpersonal complementarity versus similarity. Journal of Personality
personal measures, such as the IIP and the IAS, were initially and Social Psychology, 72, 592– 603.
developed in similar samples, and have over time proved to be Fletcher, G. J. O., Simpson, J. A., & Thomas, G. (2000). The measurement
robust in broader samples. But this remains to be demonstrated for of perceived relationship quality components: A confirmatory factor
the IIS. In addition, the study is based entirely on self-report analytic approach. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 340 –
354.
instruments. Future studies could include interpersonal strength
Freedman, M. B. (1985). Interpersonal circumplex models (1948 –1983).
ratings made by others and examine the IIS in relation to external Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 622– 625.
variables. Freedman, M. B., Leary, T. F., Ossorio, A. G., & Coffey, H. S. (1951). The
interpersonal dimension of personality. Journal of Personality, 20, 143–
Future Directions 161.
Goldberg, L. R. (1992). The development of markers for the Big-Five
Confidence in the validity of the IIS will be enhanced by further factor structure. Psychological Assessment, 4, 26 – 42.
study of its relationships with other, more focused measures of Gurtman, M. B. (1992). Construct validity of interpersonal personality
positive interpersonal constructs. The ability of the IIS to demon- measures: The interpersonal circumplex as a nomological net. Journal of
strate incremental validity relative to these measures is an impor- Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 105–118.
Gurtman, M. B. (1996). Interpersonal problems and the psychotherapy
tant factor in selecting methods to assess interpersonal functioning.
context: The construct validity of the Inventory of Interpersonal Prob-
For example, investigation of the differences between strengths as lems. Psychological Assessment, 8, 241–255.
assessed by the IIS and efficacies as assessed by the CSIE will Gurtman, M. B. (1999). Social competence: An interpersonal analysis and
offer valuable insight into the relationship of these two measures reformulation. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 15, 233–
and their underlying constructs. 245.
It would be valuable to examine the links between the IIS and Gurtman, M. B., & Balakrishnan, J. D. (1998). Circular measurement
additional desirable interpersonal outcomes such as satisfaction in redux: The analysis and interpretation of interpersonal circle profiles.
couples, positive outcomes in work situations, health-related out- Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 5, 344 –360.
comes, and outcomes in psychotherapy and other helping relation- Gurtman, M. B., & Pincus, A. L. (2000). Interpersonal adjective scales:
ships. Confirmation of circumplex structure from multiple perspectives. Per-
sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 374 –384.
We anticipate that the IIS could make a valuable contribution in
Hofsess, C. D., & Tracey, T. J. G. (2005). The interpersonal circumplex as
a variety of research and applied settings. The measure may a model of interpersonal capabilities. Journal of Personality Assessment,
provide researchers with information relevant to investigations of 84, 137–147.
personality, psychotherapy process, and treatment outcome. Prac- Hofstee, W. K. B., de Raad, B., & Goldberg, L. R. (1992). Integration of
titioners could utilize the IIS to gather information about clients’ the Big Five and circumplex approaches to trait structure. Journal of
adaptive interpersonal functioning that might otherwise be ob- Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 146 –163.
568 HATCHER AND ROGERS

Horowitz, L. M. (2004). Interpersonal foundations of psychopathology. strengths. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), A life
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. worth living: Contributions to positive psychology (pp. 29 – 48). New
Horowitz, L. M., Alden, L. E., Wiggins, J. S., & Pincus, A. L. (2000). York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Inventory of Interpersonal Problems. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Pincus, A. L., & Gurtman, M. B. (2006). Interpersonal theory and the
Corporation. interpersonal circumplex: Evolving perspectives on normal and abnor-
Horowitz, L. M., Rosenberg, S. E., Baer, B. A., Ureño, G., & Villasenor, mal personality. In S. Strack (Ed.), Differentiating normal and abnormal
V. S. (1988). Inventory of Interpersonal Problems: Psychometric prop- personality (2nd ed., pp. 83–111). New York, NY: Springer.
erties and clinical applications. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Riggio, R. E. (1986). Assessment of basic social skills. Journal of Per-
Psychology, 56, 885– 892. sonality and Social Psychology, 51, 649 – 660.
Horowitz, L. M., Turan, B., Wilson, K. R., & Zolotsev, P. (2008). Inter- Rose-Krasnor, L. (1997). The nature of social competence: A theoretical
personal theory and the measurement of interpersonal constructs. In G. J. review. Social Development, 6, 111–135.
Boyle, G. Matthews, & D. H. Saklofske (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of Saucier, G. (2002). Orthogonal markers for orthogonal factors: The case of
personality theory and assessment, Vol. 2: Personality measurement and the Big Five. Journal of Research in Personality, 36, 1–31.
testing (pp. 420 – 439). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Tracey, T. J. G. (1997). RANDALL: A Microsoft FORTRAN program for
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: The program of the Stress a randomization test of hypothesized order relations. Educational and
Reduction Clinic at the University of Massachusetts Medical Center. Psychological Measurement, 57, 164 –168.
New York, NY: Delta.
Tracey, T. J. G. (2000). Analysis of circumplex models. In H. E. A. Tinsley
Kiesler, D. J. (1996). Contemporary interpersonal theory and research.
& S. D. Brown (Eds.), Handbook of applied multivariate statistics and
New York, NY: Wiley.
mathematical modeling (pp. 641– 664). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Leary, T. (1957). Interpersonal diagnosis of personality. New York, NY:
Tracey, T. J. G., Rounds, J., & Gurtman, M. (1996). Examination of the
Ronald Press.
general factor with the interpersonal circumplex structure: Application
Locke, K. D. (2000). Circumplex scales of interpersonal values: Reliabil-
to the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems. Multivariate Behavioral
ity, validity, and applicability to interpersonal problems and personality
Research, 31, 441– 466.
disorders. Journal of Personality Assessment, 75, 249 –267.
Wiggins, J. S. (1979). A psychological taxonomy of trait descriptive terms:
Locke, K. D., & Sadler, P. (2007). Self-efficacy, values, and complemen-
tarity in dyadic interactions: Integrating interpersonal and social- The interpersonal domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
cognitive theory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 94 – ogy, 37, 395– 412.
109. Wiggins, J. S. (1995). Interpersonal adjective scales professional manual.
Paulhus, D. L., & Martin, C. L. (1987). The structure of personality Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
capabilities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 354 – Wiggins, J. S. (1996). An informal history of the interpersonal circumplex
365. tradition. Journal of Personality Assessment, 66, 217–233.
Paulhus, D. L., & Martin, C. L. (1988). Functional flexibility: A new Wiggins, J. S., & Broughton, R. (1985). The interpersonal circle: A
conception of interpersonal flexibility. Journal of Personality and Social structural model for the integration of personality research. In R. Hogan
Psychology, 55, 88 –101. & W. H. Jones (Eds.), Perspectives in personality (Vol. 1, pp. 1– 47).
Pavot, W., & Diener, E. (1993). Review of the Satisfaction With Life Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Scale. Psychological Assessment, 5, 164 –172. Wiggins, J. S., Phillips, N., & Trapnell, P. (1989). Circular reasoning about
Pavot, W., & Diener, E. (2008). The Satisfaction With Life Scale, and the interpersonal behavior: Evidence concerning some untested assumptions
emerging construct of life satisfaction. Journal of Positive Psychology, underlying diagnostic classification. Journal of Personality and Social
3, 137–152. Psychology, 56, 296 –305.
Peterson, C. (2006a). A primer in positive psychology. New York, NY: Wiggins, J. S., Trapnell, P., & Phillips, N. (1988). Psychometric and
Oxford University Press. geometric characteristics of the Revised Interpersonal Adjective Scales
Peterson, C. (2006b). The Values in Action (VIA) classification of (IAS-R). Multivariate Behavioral Research, 23, 517–530.
INVENTORY OF INTERPERSONAL STRENGTHS 569

Appendix

Inventory of Interpersonal Strengths

Connect Balance
I enjoy being with other people. I recover quickly when people hurt my feelings.
I feel good when I’m with other people. I realize that I don’t have to be friends with everyone.
I’m warm with other people. I can help others with their needs without neglecting my own.
I look forward to spending time with people. I can say “no” to others.
I make time to be with others. I like to be clear on my agreements with other people.
I’m excited about meeting new people. I can rely on myself when I’m having problems with others.
I approach other people with friendliness. I’m ok with not being included in all activities.
I like asking people about their lives. I know how to be angry without pushing people away.

Restrain
Engage
It’s important to me to be honest even when it’s difficult.
I can really shine in the spotlight. When someone irritates me, I look for a constructive solution.
I feel comfortable being open about myself. I can listen and think before I act in relationships.
I put myself out there in order to connect with others. I hesitate to express opinions about others without all the facts.
I try to regain contact with people with whom I’ve lost touch. I recognize when others need privacy.
I enjoy mingling at parties. I can be interested in others without being nosy.
I try to help people to loosen up. I can resist others’ tempting me to indulge myself.
I can make people laugh. I’m respectful of others’ need for time to themselves.
I put other people at ease.
Cooperate
Lead I enjoy learning from people who have more experience than I do.
I’m pretty even-tempered with others.
I don’t give up easily in competitive situations. I am able to compromise.
I feel confident in front of other people. I show my gratitude for what others do for me.
I enjoy lively competition with others. I work really well as an assistant.
I can be very persuasive. I’m a good listener.
I am a strong but fair leader. I’m cooperative.
I can ask other people for what I want. I don’t ask others for more than they are comfortable giving.
I can take charge in a group.
I am able to be assertive with other people. Consider
I enjoy celebrating others’ achievements.
Direct Offering other people emotional support is important to me.
I stick by my friends when they’re in trouble.
I can assert my needs even when it’s not agreeable to others. It makes me happy when others are happy.
I can make a decision even if others disagree. My feelings of gratitude warm my relationships with others.
I argue effectively with others. When friends ask for favors, I’m delighted to help them out.
I know how to look after my own interests. I feel enriched by helping others.
I can take care of myself, even when others’ needs feel pressing. I enjoy complimenting others.
I can let other people know when I think that they’re asking for
too much from me. Received September 20, 2008
I’m comfortable disagreeing with others. Revision received July 9, 2009
When others get me down, I can bounce back. Accepted July 13, 2009 䡲

View publication stats

You might also like