You are on page 1of 49

Journal of Mechanical Design.

Received June 30, 2018;


Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME
When Are Designers Willing To Take Risks?
How Concept Creativity and Prototype
Fidelity Influence Perceived Risk
(DETC2018-86092)

d
Elizabeth M. Starkey

ite
School of Engineering Design, Technology and Professional Programs
The Pennsylvania State University

ed
213 Hammond Building
University Park, Pennsylvania, 16802
ems413@psu.edu

py
ASME Member

Co
Jessica Menold
School of Engineering Design, Technology and Professional Programs

ot
The Pennsylvania State University
213 Hammond Building
tN
University Park, Pennsylvania, 16802
jdm5407@psu.edu
ASME Member
rip

Scarlett R. Miller
sc

School of Engineering Design, Technology and Professional Programs and the Department of
Industrial and Manufacturing Engineering
nu

The Pennsylvania State University


213 Hammond Building
Ma

University Park, Pennsylvania, 16802


shm13@psu.edu
ed

ABSTRACT
Building prototypes is an important part of the concept selection phase of the design process, where fuzzy ideas
pt

get represented to support communication and decision making. However, previous studies have shown that
prototypes generate different levels of user feedback based on their fidelity and aesthetics. Furthermore, prior
research on concept selection has shown that individual risk attitude effects how individuals select ideas, as creative
ce

ideas are perceived to be riskier in comparison to less creative ideas. While the role of risk has been investigated in
concept selection, there is lack of research on how risk is related to the selection of prototypes at various levels of
Ac

fidelity. Thus, the purpose of this study was to investigate the impact of prototype fidelity, concept creativity, and risk
aversion, on perceived riskiness and concept selection through a between-subjects study with 72 engineering
students. The results revealed that there was a “goldilocks” effect in which students choose concepts with “just the
right amount” of novelty, not too much and not too little, as long as quality was adequate. In addition, the prototype
fidelity of a concept had an interaction with uniqueness, indicating that unique concepts are more likely to be
perceived as less risky if presented at higher levels of fidelity.

Keywords: Decision Theory, Conceptual Design, Design Process, Design Theory and Methodology

!1 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME
INTRODUCTION

Large companies spend billions of dollars every year on research and development (R&D)

[1], in order to develop innovative products that are necessary to survive and thrive in a fast-

growing marketplace that demands product innovation. However, 40 to 50% of R&D

investments are lost each year due to projects that are canceled or fail in the market [2]. In order

d
ite
to reduce the risk of product failure, companies often rely on prototyping strategies to identify

ed
which concepts to move forward with or to “kill” [3]. This is because prototyping has been

py
shown to be an effective means of representing and communicating early phase concepts [4-7],

Co
gathering user feedback [8, 9], identifying problems [10], aiding in decision making [10, 11], and

improving design outcomes and functionality [12]. However, prototyping also accounts for the

ot
largest sunk cost during product development [1, 2]. Thus, there is a need to investigate how the
tN
type of prototype developed impacts design decision making in order to develop cost-effective
rip

prototyping strategies that promote the flow of creative ideas.


sc

While the use of prototypes is widespread in the design industry, prototypes can differ
nu

significantly in their purpose and in their fidelity. In this work, we adopt the broadest definition
Ma

of a prototype as “an early embodiment of a design concept” that is not a production stage design

[3, p. 650] meaning a prototype can range from low to high fidelity. For example, a low-fidelity
ed

prototype could represent an idea with limited function or capabilities of interaction [6][13] such
pt
ce

as through a sketch or a storyboard [14]. These prototypes which are often the least expensive to

produce [15] provide the designers with immediate feedback [16] and are typically used to gage
Ac

the usability [17] and desirability [18] of a design. On the other hand, medium fidelity prototypes

are used to illustrate the look and feel of a final design without the interactivity [19], and/or

understand the intuitiveness, usability, or basic functionality of a design [19-21]. Finally, high

!2 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME
fidelity prototypes which can be characterized as pre-production models [22] that serve as a

physical or digital instantiation of the finalized concept. High fidelity prototypes often offer a

fully interactive or functional representation of the entire system [22, 23], but this comes with a

high cost. While research has begun to explore the appropriateness of different prototype

fidelities for various design tasks, such as gathering user feedback [8, 10, 12, 13, 20, 24, 25],

d
there is little research on how prototype fidelity impacts go/ no-go decisions.

ite
In addition to our lack of understanding on prototype fidelity and design decision making, we

ed
also do not understand how one’s willingness to take risks relates to prototype fidelity and

py
concept creativity. Understanding the factors that impact this decision making process is critical

Co
as creative ideas are often associated with uncertainty and risk [26, 27]. A creative idea is

ot
typically considered one that is both novel and useful [28, 29]. In fact, this multifaceted
tN
relationship has been found to have an inverse effect, where novel ideas are discarded during the
rip

concept selection process, and more conventional alternatives are chosen to move forward with
sc

[30]. Previous work investigated the evolution of ideas throughout the design process in an 8
nu

week design project with engineering students and found that while students ended their design

projects with a high quality idea, the novelty of the ideas they generated at the beginning of the
Ma

project did not impact the novelty of their final design [30] . This indicates that novel ideas are
ed

not making it through the design process and that student designers are making decisions that
pt

favor more feasible, non-unique concepts. These results are in agreement with other work that
ce

found that individuals are prone to select traditional, less creative ideas over innovative, more
Ac

unique ideas during concept selection [28, 29]. This phenomenon can reduce the likelihood of

innovative products making it to market.

The current study was developed to investigate the role of individual risk preferences,

!3 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME
prototype fidelity, and creativity of ideas, on final concept choice. The results from an online

survey of 72 engineering students expand our understanding of the influence of prototype fidelity

on concept selection and perceived risk in an educational setting.

RELATED WORK

In order to provide a theoretical background for the proposed work, prior research was

d
ite
examined. The current section highlights these findings and lays the framework for the study

ed
design and the results.

py
Prototype Fidelity in Engineering Design Process

Co
Research points to the benefits of prototyping such as improved technical quality [7] and

ot
improved manufacturability [18] of final designs. Design teams have long struggled, however, to
tN
balance the fidelity of prototypes with budget and time constraints [20, 25]. With the large
rip

investment and risk of inadequate returns that prototyping presents [25], it is critical to
sc

understand what level of prototype fidelity is appropriate dependent upon design needs and
nu

context.
Ma

The prototype fidelity literature has focused extensively on which level of prototype (i.e.

low-, medium- or high-fidelity prototype) yields higher quality user feedback [7, 10, 13, 21, 24,
ed

31]. Specifically, low fidelity prototypes have been found to generate the equivalent amount of
pt

user feedback as high-fidelity prototypes [10, 31, 32] while leading to more efficient processes
ce

and outcomes [7]. In addition, Walker, Takayama, and Landay [13] found that low-fidelity
Ac

(sketched) and high-fidelity (HTML website) prototypes were equally effective in detecting

usability issues. While some have praised low fidelity prototypes for their ability to quickly

validate designs at a low cost [20, 26], other studies have suggested the effectiveness of low

!4 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME
fidelity prototypes depend on the purpose of its use. For example, in cases where interactivity is

essential to a concept, low-fidelity prototypes are insufficient for usability testing [8].

In addition to prototype fidelity, the aesthetics of a design and the interaction with fidelity

may play a crucial role in influencing user feedback [24, 27]. Sauer and Sonderegger [24]

investigated the interaction between prototype fidelity (paper, computer, fully operating

d
prototype) and levels of aesthetics (high vs. moderate) on user behavior, subjective evaluation,

ite
and emotion in usability tests, finding that participants overrated the aesthetic qualities of low-

ed
fidelity prototypes. In addition, they found that the fidelity level of prototypes did not affect

py
emotions or subjective user evaluation. Similarly, Macomber and Yang [27] investigated the

Co
influence of sketch finish and style in user responses to early design concepts, finding that users

ot
responded most positively to clean hand sketches rather than rough sketches or CAD drawings.
tN
This prior work indicate that variations in prototype fidelity can produce widely varying user
rip

feedback and recommendations.


sc

While this prior work highlights the impact of prototype fidelity on design outcomes and
nu

interactions with end users, little work to date has explored its effect on risk perceptions or

concept selection. We highlight this as a critical gap in the literature as failure to identify the
Ma

effect prototype fidelity has on perceived riskiness of concepts, during engineering design can
ed

lead to innovative or useful concepts being overlooked. Thus, this study aims to fill this
pt

knowledge gap by exploring the role of prototype fidelity during concept selection.
ce
Ac

Risk Taking in Concept Selection

While there is limited research investigating risk taking and prototyping, there has been

significant work investigating risk during concept selection. In fact, scholars have identified that

!5 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME
creative ideas are often associated with uncertainty and risk, which certain people have tendency

to avoid [28, 29]. Specifically, research has shown that men are more willing to take risks than

women [33] and that more narcissistic individuals are more likely to take risks [34]. In an

engineering context risk is defined as the “effect of uncertainty on objectives” [35]. When this is

applied to a creative design task, we can say that risk is uncertainties about whether potential

d
outcomes will be realized given creative effort [36, 37]. The willingness of an individual to take

ite
risks has been found to impact creative output in an R&D setting, with those who are more

ed
willing to take risks producing higher creative output [38]. In a study investigating choice with

py
architects, it was found that those who are more risk prone are willing to push through weaker

Co
code-compliant design [39]. These studies indicate that individuals may be more willing to move

ot
forward with ideas that are risky if they have a more risk prone attitude. While the role of risk
tN
has been investigated in concept selection, limited prior research has investigated how risk is
rip

driving selection of concepts and how the creativity of concepts impacts how risky they are
sc

perceived to be.
nu

While not studied in the context of prototyping, recent research has emerged to assess risk in

engineering domain specific tasks and to investigate the relationship between risk and creativity
Ma

during concept selection in engineering design. Specifically, to explore risk taking in an


ed

engineering contest the Engineering Domain Specific Risk Taking (E-DOSPERT) scale was
pt

developed by Van Bossuyt et al. [40] and found that risk taking attitudes are domain specific and
ce

can be trained. Barclift et al. [41] used this scale to investigate risk taking in an additive
Ac

manufacturing design task and found that the risk aversion sub-scale could predict the novelty of

ideas but not the quality of the ideas. While these studies show promise for the E-DOSPERT

scale, this scale has not been utilized in studies investigating the concept selection stage of the

!6 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME
design process and focuses on engineering process, product functionality, legal issues and ethics.

Because the “fuzzy" front end [42] of the design process is characterized by uncertainty and

unknown unknowns, the risks associated with the early stages of design are fundamentally

different than the risks associated with the later stages of design, when more design information

is known [43]. Since no scale existed to assess risk in concept selection during the engineering

d
design process, Toh and Miller adopted a financial risk-taking measure and found study, found

ite
that individual financial risk aversion was related to creative concept selection [44]. Risk

ed
aversion was measured using survey questions from behavioral economics where participants

py
were asked to answer 10 random questions on individual preference for a “fixed payoff of a

Co
specified value, or a gamble for fair odds with an uncertain payoff of a specified value” [44, p.

ot
215]. Follow up work by Toh and Miller may remedy this problem through the development of
tN
the Preferences for Creativity Scale (PCS), which assesses an individual’s preference for
rip

creativity in engineering design on four dimensions, including risk tolerance, but since this scale
sc

captures conscious risk attitudes, it may not capture one’s actual risk taking preference [45, 46].
nu

While the PCS results are promising, the scale has not been fully validated. At any case, these

studies indicate that risk taking impacts creative concept selection. However, these studies are
Ma

limited to investigating early phase design sketches and not the evolution of an idea over the
ed

course of a design project.


pt

While previous work has investigated the role of financial risk in concept selection, no study
ce

to date has looked at how financial risk is related to the selection of prototypes at various
Ac

fidelities. Prototyping represents the largest sunk cost of the product development process, and

thus could represent the largest financial risk a company may take during product development.

Understanding how perceived risk is associated with the selection of prototypes at different

!7 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME
fidelity levels is critical in understanding which concepts are more likely to gain stakeholder buy

in and progress further in the product development process. This knowledge gap sets the stage

for our investigation of the relationship between prototype fidelity, risk perceptions, and

creativity.

Research Objectives
The purpose of the current study was to investigate the role that risk, concept creativity, and

d
ite
prototype fidelity have on how an individual chooses which prototypes to move forward with.

ed
Specifically, we sought to explore the following research questions:

py
Co
RQ1: Is there a relation between likelihood to move forward with a concept, perceived risk of the

concept and individual financial risk aversion scores?

ot
tN
We hypothesized that individual financial risk aversion levels will impact how
rip

students select ideas, since Toh and Miller [44] found that financial risk is a factor
sc

impacting creative concept selection when working with sketches.


nu
Ma

RQ2: Is there a relation between likelihood to move forward with a concept and the creativity

(usefulness or uniqueness) of the concept?


ed
pt
ce

We hypothesized that the creativity of concepts in later stages of design would

impact the selection of ideas. Specifically, we hypothesized that the expert rated
Ac

uniqueness of the design, represented as a prototype, would negatively influence concept

selection while expert rated usefulness would positively influence concept selection.

!8 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018
RQ3:by Is
ASME
there a relation between perceived risk of a concept, prototype fidelity, and the creativity

(usefulness or uniqueness) of the idea?

We hypothesized that the fidelity of the prototype would impact students willing to

select creative ideas, since ideas that are more complete may have less risk associated

d
with them. This is based on previous studies that have found that prototype fidelity

ite
impacts selection [7]. We also hypothesized that prototype fidelity would impact the

ed
perceived risk of the design concepts because previous work has highlighted the impact

py
of prototype fidelity on stakeholder buy in and trust [47].

Co
METHODOLOGY

ot
To answer these research questions, a survey was conducted with 72 students. This section
tN
serves to summarize the methodological approaches of the study.
rip

Participants
sc
nu

Participants were recruited from a first-year undergraduate engineering design course at a

large northeastern university. In all, 72 students (52 males and 21 females) participated in the
Ma

survey.
ed

Procedure
pt
ce

At the start of the survey, a brief overview was provided and, per Institutional Review Board
Ac

requirements, informed consent was obtained. Participants were then asked to complete a survey

consisting of two parts on a computer, all participants used the same PC throughout the entirety

of the study. Participants were first asked to complete a financial risk aversion portion of the

survey, where they were asked 10 lottery questions to capture their financial risk based on

!9 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME
recommendations and validation with first-year students by Toh and Miller (See [44] for full list

of questions). The lottery questions took following form: “which would you prefer? A) $15 for

sure. B) a coin flip in which you get $X if it is heads and $0 if it is tails.” These questions were

presented to the students in random order with X ranging from $20 to $250 similarly to Toh and

Miller [44]. Next, the participants were given the following task prompt:

d
“You are an engineer on a project team that is trying to redesign a milk frother.

ite
Frothed milk is the warm, pourable, virtually liquid foam that tastes rich and sweet. It is

ed
an ingredient in many coffee beverages, especially espresso-based coffee drinks (Lattes,

py
Cappuccinos, Mochas). Frothed milk is made by incorporating very small air bubbles

Co
throughout the entire body of milk through some form of vigorous motion. As such,

ot
devices that froth milk can also be used in a number of other applications, such as for
tN
whipping cream, blending drinks, emulsifying salad dressing, and many others. In
rip

addition to the requirement of vigorous motion, frothed milk should be between 150° and
sc

160° because above 160° the proteins in the milk start to breakdown causing sugars to
nu

be released which ultimately changes the flavor of the milk. Currently your company has

the following product on the market:


Ma
ed
pt
ce
Ac

!10 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASMEThis product is battery operated and froths milk through a stirring motion. No

heating ability is incorporated into this device therefore the milk must be preheated in

order for this device to froth milk.

Your team has been tasked with designing a new milk frother that should be able to

d
be used by the consumer with minimal instruction. You have been tasked with

ite
determining which following 9 ideas should be brought to the board of directors for

ed
consideration to move forward with in the design process. “

py
Co
Participants were then randomly assigned to a fidelity condition (sketch = 23 participants,

ot
cardboard = 25, and CAD prototype = 24) and given images of 9 different prototypes with the
tN
assigned fidelity. Before completing the survey, participants were asked to review all of the 9 of
rip

the prototypes. Images in each fidelity set appeared randomly to remove the effect of order on
sc

concept choice. Images appeared twice in the survey, all 9 prototypes were displayed at the
nu

beginning of the survey in a group and then again each image was shown, larger and individually

and questions about each prototype followed directly below the image. See Appendix A for a
Ma

screen shot of the survey page. Students were then prompted to rate their feelings about the
ed

prototypes on a scale of 1-6 for multiple questions. A 6 point scale was used in order to avoid
pt

central tendency bias [48] and has been found to be reliable with participants with cognitive
ce

abilities of college level students [49]. Once they had reviewed these images they were prompted
Ac

as follows:

“On a scale of 1-6 with 6 being extremely likely and 1 being not likely at all, how

likely are you to present each of the ideas to the board of directors?”

11
! Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME
TABLE 1: ALL IDEAS AT ALL LEVELS OF QUALITY AND NOVELTY FOR LOW, MEDIUM,
AND HIGH-FIDELITY PROTOTYPES

d
ite
ed
py
Co
ot
tN
rip
sc
nu
Ma
ed
pt
ce
Ac

!12 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME

Prototypes were acquired from a sample of 932 ideas collected for a previous study by

Starkey, Hunter, and Miller [50]. Prototypes were chosen from the sample of ideas in order to

represent every combination of low, medium, and high uniqueness and usefulness using

Amabile’s Consensual Assessment Technique [51]. Using this technique ideas were rated by two

d
expert raters, who were chosen for their experience in design and assessment (4+ years) and for

ite
their extensive knowledge of milk frother designs, as well as creativity ratings, on a scale of 1-6

ed
for both uniqueness and usefulness. Prototypes were considered to have low uniqueness or

py
usefulness if they were rated as a 1 or 2 in the category, medium if they were rated as a 3 or 4,

Co
and high if they were rated as a 5 or 6 in the category. In all, 9 different ideas were chosen to

ot
represent each combination of level (low, medium, high) and category (uniqueness, usefulness).
tN
The ideas chosen are shown in Table 1. For a detailed look at the textual descriptions for each
rip

prototype see the example survey shown in Appendix A.


sc

Each of the ideas chosen for the study were taken from the gathered data and were recreated
nu

by the researchers, at each level (sketch, cardboard, and CAD) to ensure that participant

perceptions were not being determined based on the quality of the prototype. All annotations
Ma

were uniform across the fidelity levels.


ed

After rating the ideas for how likely they would be to present them to the board of directors,
pt

participants were prompted with the following question, “On a scale of 1-6 with 6 being
ce

extremely risky and 1 being not risky at all, how risky do you think each of the ideas presented by
Ac

your team are?” in order to gather student’s perceptions [52] of the risk of failure for the project.

As a reminder, in engineering, risk can be defined as “effect of uncertainty on objectives” [35].

As previously described, participants were prompted to “determine which of the following 9

!13 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 byshould
ideas ASME be brought to the board of directors for consideration to move forward with in the

design process” and were then asked to rate the perceived riskiness of each of the ideas on a

scale from 1 to 6. The prompt implies that performance will be measured by the success of the

product in the market and by the acceptance, or lack of acceptance, of the proposed idea(s) by

the “board of directors

d
Next, participants were presented with the prompt, “A unique idea can be defined as one that

ite
is rare, unusual, or uncommon. On a scale of 1-6 with 6 being extremely unique and 1 being not

ed
unique at all, how unique do you think each of the ideas presented by your team are?” This

py
question was intended to gather the perceived uniqueness of the ideas. Lastly, participants were

Co
prompted with the following question, “A useful idea can be defined as one that applies to the

ot
problem at hand, is an effective solution, and is implementable. On a scale of 1-6 with 6 being
tN
extremely useful and 1 being not useful at all, how useful do you think each of the ideas
rip

presented by your team are?” This was provided in order to gather the perceived usefulness of
sc

the ideas. For each question in the survey, participants were shown the ideas as they rated them,
nu

as shown in Appendix A.
Ma

Metrics
ed

The following metrics were utilized in this study to calculate the design creativity and the
pt

financial risk aversion.


ce

Design Creativity: The ideas developed by the participants were evaluated for usefulness
Ac

and uniqueness by two expert raters using Amabile’s [51] Consensual Assessment Technique

using the guidelines put forth by [53] and [54]. Specifically, uniquness scores were founded on

perceptions of originality and surprise and usefulness scores were founded on perceptions of

!14 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME
value, logic, utility, and understandable of the idea. Raters provided a rating from 1 (low

uniquness or usefulness) to 6 (high uniquness or usefulness). The two expert raters assessed the

ideas independently and the scores from the raters were agregated. To qualify expertise, both

experts has at least 4 years of applied experience in design and assessment and had published at

least four papers in in design and creativity assessment. There was high level of agreement

d
among the expert raters for usefulness (α = 0.85) and uniqueness (α = 0.85).

ite
Financial Risk Aversion: Individual financial risk aversion was determined through a

ed
survey (completed online) containing 10 lottery questions (See [44] for full list of questions).

py
The financial risk aversion index for each individual was calculated according to Han et al. [55]

Co
using recommended payoff values validated by Toh and Miller [44]. The financial risk scores

ot
were normalized to range from 0 (risk prone) to 1 (risk averse). tN
DATA ANALYSIS AND RESULTS
rip

SPSS v. 25 was used to calculate the results and p = 0.05 was used in all analyses. While p-
sc

values are important to consider in statistical analysis, we must also consider the effect size of
nu

the significant relationships in order to understand the magnitude of the relationship. Therefore,
Ma

the results also include analysis of the effect size reported as β and η 2 . The results of our

statistical analysis are presented below as mean and standard error unless otherwise stated.
ed
pt

RQ1: Is There a Relation Between Likelihood To Move Forward With a Concept,


ce

Perceived Risk of the Concept and Individual Financial Risk Aversion Scores?
Ac

As an initial analysis, we sought to understand how much or how little individual financial

risk aversion scores impacted risk perception and concept choice. While not studied in the

context of prototyping, a recent study [44], found that individual financial risk aversion was

!15 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME

d
ite
ed
py
Co
FIGURE 1: LIKELIHOOD TO MOVE FORWARD SCORES BY LOW, MEDIUM, AND HIGH LEVELS
OF USEFULNESS AND UNIQUENESS.

ot
related to creative concept selection. Thus, we hypothesized that financial risk aversion would
tN
moderate the relationship between perceived riskiness of a concept and likelihood to move
rip

forward with a concept. Results from a regression model revealed that individual’s financial risk
sc

aversion did not significantly predict perceived riskiness of a concept f(1,646) = 0.369, p =
nu

0.544, β = -0.024, or an individual’s likelihood to move forward with a concept f(1,646) = 0.000,
Ma

p = 0.992, β < 0.001. These results led us to investigate other factors that may be impacting

perceived risk and likelihood to move forward with a concept. Specifically, the remaining
ed

research questions were focused on exploring the relationships between likelihood to move
pt

forward with a concept, perceived riskiness, creativity of the concept, and fidelity of the
ce

prototype.
Ac

RQ2: Is There a Relation Between Likelihood To Move Forward With a Concept,


Prototype Fidelity, and the Creativity (Usefulness or Uniqueness) of the Concept?

!16 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by
OurASME
second research question was developed to investigate if the likelihood to move forward

with an idea was based on the fidelity of the prototype and if this relationship was affected by the

creativity of the idea, where creativity is defined as a combination of usefulness and uniqueness.

Prior to conducting this analysis, a pre-analysis was conducted in order to understand how

student perceptions of uniqueness and usefulness lined up with expert ratings. The results of this

d
pre-analysis revealed a significant correlation between student perceptions and expert ratings for

ite
both uniqueness and usefulness, and therefore expert ratings were used to avoid multicollinearity

ed
of variables. A three-way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was conducted with likelihood to move

py
forward with the idea as the dependent variable, uniqueness, usefulness, and prototype fidelity as

Co
the independent variables of interest. Interactions that were not significant were subtracted from

ot
the model in order to arrive at the most accurate model. tN
The results revealed a statistically significant main effect for expert usefulness, f(2,633) =
rip

59.558, p < 0.0005, η 2 = 0.158. In addition, post-hoc analysis revealed that students were
sc

significantly more likely to move forward with ideas that were high in usefulness (3.90 ± 0.099),
nu

than those that were rated as medium usefulness (2.95 ± 0.109, p = 0.001) or low (2.44 ±

0.095, p < 0.0005). The results also showed that students were significantly more likely to move
Ma

forward with ideas that were rated as medium usefulness than those that had low usefulness
ed

scores (p < 0.0005). There was also a statistically significant main effect for expert uniqueness,
pt

f(2,633) = 14.204, p < 0.0005, η 2 = 0.043. Post-hoc analysis revealed that students were
ce

significantly more likely to move forward with ideas that had medium uniqueness scores (3.50 ±
Ac

0.116) than those that had a low (3.01 ± 0.105, p = 0.001) and high (2.78 ± 0.100, p < 0.0005)

uniqueness scores. Finally, the results revealed a statistically significant two-way interaction

between expert uniqueness and expert usefulness, f(2,633) = 7.791, p < 0.0005, η 2 = 0.047,

!17 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME
shown in Figure 1. Post-hoc analysis was conducted, revealing that for ideas with medium

usefulness, students were significantly more likely to move forward with ideas that had medium

uniqueness (3.90 ± .0.189) as compared to those with high uniqueness (2.51 ± 0.167, p < 0.0005)

and low uniqueness (2.44 ± 0.161, p < 0.0005). Participants were also significantly more likely

to move forward with ideas with medium uniqueness (4.22 ± 0.173) as compared to those with

d
high uniqueness (3.60 ± 0.169, p = 0.027). This is an interesting finding as it is indicative of a

ite
“Goldilocks” effect, in which students are choosing concepts with “just the right amount” of

ed
novelty. Not too much and not too little.

py
These results do not support our hypothesis that prototype fidelity would impact the

Co
likelihood to move forward with a concept. While previous work has shown that higher fidelity

ot
prototypes are more likely to garner support and buy-in from external stakeholders [40], our
tN
results indicate that prototype fidelity does not a play a significant role in the decision-making
rip

behaviors of internal design team members.


sc

RQ3: Is There a Relation Between Perceived Risk of a Concept, Prototype Fidelity, and the
Creativity (Usefulness or Uniqueness) of the Idea?
nu

Intuitively, perceived risk directly impacts likelihood to move forward with a concept and
Ma

previous work has found that prototype fidelity affects stakeholder buy in and trust. As a first
ed

step towards uncovering the relationship between prototype fidelity and willingness to move
pt

forward with a concept, the relationship between perceived risk and prototype fidelity was
ce

explored. A Spearman correlation was calculated between perceived risk and likelihood to move
Ac

forward, and perceived risk was significantly and negatively correlated with likelihood to move

forward with a concept, with a coefficient of -0.397, p < 0.0005. This highlights the negative

!18 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME between the perceived riskiness of
relationship a concept and the likelihood the concept is

chosen for further development.

Our third research question was focused on unpacking the relationship between perceived

risk, prototype fidelity, and creativity of the idea. In order to answer this research question, a

three-way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was conducted with perceived risk of the idea as the

d
dependent variable, expert ratings of uniqueness, expert ratings of usefulness, and prototype

ite
fidelity as the independent variables. Interactions that were not significant were subtracted from

ed
the model in order to arrive at the most accurate model. Results revealed a statistically significant

py
main effect for expert usefulness, f(2,633) = 11.535, p < 0.0005, η 2 = 0.035. Post-hoc analysis

Co
revealed that ideas rated high in usefulness (2.96 ± 0.092), were perceived to be significantly less

ot
risky than those that had a medium (3.48 ± 0.099, p = 0.001) or low (3.58 ± 0.107, p < 0.0005)
tN
usefulness score. These results agree with the previous findings that found students perceive
rip

ideas with higher levels of usefulness to be inherently less risky [37]. Our findings extend this
sc

body of work into the prototyping phases of the design process, demonstrating that concepts with
nu

higher levels of usefulness are perceived to be less risky, even when presented in various

prototype fidelities.
Ma

Results revealed a statistically significant main effect for expert uniqueness, f(2,633) =
ed

4.645, p = 0.01, η 2 = 0.014. Post-hoc analysis revealed that ideas that were high in uniqueness
pt

(3.56 ± 0.097) were perceived to be significantly riskier than those that had a medium (3.14 ±
ce

0.101, p = 0.009) uniqueness scores. This is not unexpected since novel ideas are perceived to be
Ac

inherently risky. Once again, our findings extend previous work into the prototyping phase of

design, demonstrating that risk perception is much more dependent on concept creativity versus

prototype fidelity.

!19 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by
In ASME
addition, there was a statistically significant two-way interaction between expert

uniqueness and expert usefulness, f(2,633) = 3.111, p = 0.015, η2 = 0.019. Post-hoc analysis was

conducted revealing that for ideas with low uniqueness, those with high usefulness (2.597 ±

0.171) had much lower perceived risk than those with medium usefulness (3.764 ± 0.171, p <

0.0005 ) and low usefulness (3.611 ± 0.171, p < 0.0005 ). This is an interesting finding as it is

d
indicative of the summative nature of usefulness and uniqueness in terms of perceived risk. As

ite
previously demonstrated, increased usefulness (in general) decreases the perceived risk of an

ed
idea. Yet, increased uniqueness increases the perceived risk of an idea. The above two-way

py
interaction indicates that these two facets of a concept are not independent but work to impact

Co
perceived risk together, thus ideas with low uniqueness, but high usefulness are perceived to be

ot
the least risky. tN
There was a statistically significant two-way interaction between uniqueness and prototype
rip

fidelity f(2,633) = 2.718, p = 0.013, η 2 = 0.025. Pairwise comparisons were conducted with a
sc

Bonferroni correction, revealing that for ideas with low uniqueness, low fidelity prototypes
nu

(3.087 ± 0.171) were perceived to be significantly less risky than high fidelity prototypes (3.708
Ma
ed
pt
ce
Ac

FIGURE 2: PERCEIVED RISKINESS SCORES BY LOW, MEDIUM, AND HIGH


!20 Copyright © 2018 by ASME
UNIQUENESS FOR LOW, MEDIUM AND HIGH FIDELITY
Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use
Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME
± 0.177, p = 0.037). In contrast, for ideas with high uniqueness low fidelity prototypes (3.913 ±

0.177) were perceived to be significantly more risky than high fidelity prototypes (3.250 ± 0.173,

p = 0.023), shown in figure 2 below. This interaction reveals that when ideas have high levels of

uniqueness, higher fidelity prototypes could be used to reduce the perceived risk of the idea. This

is a critical finding as it indicates that truly novel ideas may be able to “fake” their way to

d
stakeholder buy-in (i.e. reduce the perceived risk) through higher fidelity prototypes.

ite
DISCUSSION

ed
py
The main goal of this study was to investigate the role that individual financial risk aversion,

Co
idea creativity, and prototype fidelity had on individual concept selection and perceived risk. Our

main results were as follows:

ot
tN
• An individual’s perceived riskiness of an idea was not related to their financial risk
rip

aversion score
sc

• Individuals are more likely to move forward with concepts rated higher in usefulness.
nu

• Individuals are more likely to move forward with concepts rated at medium levels of
Ma

uniqueness as compared to concepts with high or low levels of uniqueness.

• Ideas with low uniqueness presented as low fidelity prototypes were perceived to be
ed

significantly less risky than their high-fidelity counterparts


pt


ce

Ideas with high uniqueness presented as high fidelity prototypes were perceived to be

significantly less risky than their low fidelity counterparts


Ac

Our first research question was developed to understand the relationship between financial

risk aversion, perceived risk, and likelihood to move forward with a concept. Our results found

!21 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME
no statistically significant relationship between financial risk aversion and perceived risk or

likelihood to move forward with a concept. These results indicate that the financial risk aversion

scores are not strongly connected to the perceived riskiness of a concept or the likelihood to

move forward with a concept. This finding is in contradiction to prior research that suggested a

relationship between financial risk aversion and creative concept selection in the engineering

d
design process [44]. We speculate that this unexpected result may be due to a lack of variability

ite
in the financial risk aversion scores within our sample. Our sample was skewed, and the majority

ed
of the students were very risk prone on the financial risk scale. We also question the validity of

py
the financial risk aversion scale to be indicative of individual risk aversion in the context of

Co
engineering design. Prior work that investigated the risk aversion and perceived risk in software

ot
development found that these two measures were not related [52]. We highlight the need for
tN
alternative risk assessment measures, like the Preferences for Creativity Scale [56], to be
rip

investigated for their ability to accurately measure risk aversion constructs in the context of
sc

engineering design.
nu

Our second research question sought to understand the relationship between the creativity of

a concept, fidelity of the prototype, and the likelihood to move forward with a concept in later
Ma

stages of the design process. Our results suggest that as usefulness of an idea increases the
ed

likelihood to move forward with that idea also increases. Additionally, we found that concepts
pt

with medium levels of uniqueness were more likely to be chosen to move forward as compared
ce

to concepts with high or low levels of uniqueness. We also found a significant two-way
Ac

interaction between usefulness and uniqueness. We categorize this effect as a Goldilocks effect in

which students were more likely to move forward with ideas that had medium levels of novelty

and high to medium levels of quality. We hypothesize that this is due to a balance between the

!22 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME
inherent riskiness of truly novel ideas, yet the social biases that may pressure students to choose

“innovative” concepts. Another possible explanation for this effect is that complexity of the ideas

may relate to their uniqueness. Importantly, we found no evidence to suggest that prototype

fidelity affected the go or no-go decisions to move forward with concepts.

Our last research question was focused on investigating the influence of prototype fidelity

d
and concept creativity has on the perceived riskiness of an idea. The significant main effects for

ite
both expert rated uniqueness and usefulness on the perceived riskiness of an idea confirms

ed
existing theory and research within creativity and concept selection [44]. The interesting results

py
from this study was the interaction terms we found between prototype fidelity and concept

Co
perceived riskiness. Our results indicate that prototype fidelity does impact concept perceived

ot
riskiness, dependent upon the level of uniqueness or usefulness of the idea. For example, we
tN
found that for ideas with high levels of uniqueness, higher fidelity prototypes were viewed as
rip

less risky. Our results point to the influential nature of creativity in concept selection. We did not
sc

find any significant main affects for prototype fidelity on the perceived riskiness of ideas but
nu

found significant two-way interactions. In other words, prototype fidelity only affects the

perceived riskiness of a concept at certain levels of creativity. This is an important finding as


Ma

products in general can be classified as evolutionary and revolutionary. Evolutionary products


ed

are minor or incremental improvements on existing products, where revolutionary products are
pt

paradigm shifting and tend to be more novel or more unique. Our work found that when products
ce

are very unique, or revolutionary, higher fidelity prototypes are perceived as less risky; this is an
Ac

important finding for startups or intrapreneurs looking to pitch revolutionary new concepts.

Getting stakeholder buy in to secure resources, time, money, etc is critical, and ensuring that

your product or idea is not seen as a risk is key. It is important to note that in the current study,

!23 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018
levelbyof
ASME
fidelity was defined by the look of concepts presented to participants. In the lowest

fidelity level sketched concepts were presented, in the middle level concepts were built from

cardboard and photographs of final builds were presented, and in the highest level concepts were

generated using 3D modeling software and screenshots were presented. While the higher levels

of fidelity may have provided participants with more information about material choice or

d
product geometry, designer supplied information was kept consistent. All annotations made to

ite
the original sketch by designers were presented at every level of fidelity. Results indicating that

ed
higher fidelity prototypes may be perceived as less risky for more novel ideas, give us insights

py
into how appearance can impact the perception of risk,

Co
CONCLUSION
The main goal in this study was to investigate how prototype fidelity affects perceived risk

ot
and choice of concepts during creative concept selection. This study found that financial risk
tN
aversion scores taken from behavioral economics do not accurately predict risk aversion in
rip

engineering design. Our results highlight the potential impact of prototype fidelity on creative
sc

concept selection and the need for expanding research on the interaction between creative
nu

concept selection, perceived risk, and prototype fidelity.


Ma

Our study was limited due to the nature of the experiment, specifically the mode of data

collection. Because data was gathered through an online survey and prototypes were presented to
ed

participants in still photo, our findings might not accurately represent the actual process in
pt
ce

concept selection. Further research is needed to explore the relationship between the mode that

prototypes are presented to participants (in person, online, video, still photo) and the effect this
Ac

has on perceived risk and concept selection. Future work will explore the usefulness of online

rating systems in rating and selecting physical products.


While this study looked to uncover the perceived riskiness of the ideas to students and the

!24 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME
prompt implies that performance will be measured by the success of the product in the market

and by the acceptance, or lack of acceptance, of the proposed idea(s) by the “board of directors,”

this was not explicitly defined to the students. We recognize this as a limitation of the study, as

we did not explicitly ask students to define what riskiness meant nor, did we define risk at the

start of the study. In future work, either risk should be explicitly defined for students or

d
additional survey questions should be asked in order to understand whether students were using

ite
similar mental models of risk when evaluating concepts.

ed
In addition to the mode of data collection, the pool of participants was limited to freshmen

py
engineering students in this study. Therefore, these results cannot be generalized for the general

Co
population. Specifically prior work has found that men are more willing to take risks than

ot
women [33], more narcissistic individuals take more risks [34], and level of training can impact
tN
risk attitudes [41]. Further exploration of higher level students and professionals is needed as
rip

well as an investigation of how personality types and gender play a role. Future work should
sc

investigate not only how industry professionals perform in these risk taking scenarios, but also
nu

how an individual’s background (i.e. financial history) impact these scores. Through this future

work, different risk taking scales should be investigated for their potential to identify risk taking
Ma

attitudes in the creative concept selection stages of the design process. While financial risk taking
ed

[55] and the engineering domain specific risk taking [40] scales exist, they have not been
pt

thoroughly investigated for their ability to explain variations in the fuzzy front end of the design
ce

process and should be investigated, along with the preferences for creativity scale (PCS) [56], for
Ac

their influence on this portion of the design process.

Another limitation of our study was the chosen experts for the creativity ratings. While these

experts were experienced in creativity ratings and milk frother designs, they are not industry

!25 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME
experts. Future work should investigate how the ratings of creativity change between experts in

academia and industry, and if this impacts the relationship between likelihood to move forward

and creativity.

Lastly, there are several aspects of the designs that this study did not explore. For instance,

while the design information provided to the participants was kept consistent for each fidelity

d
level, additional information may have been encoded in the images through the use of different

ite
materials. Future work should investigate the factors that make up a higher fidelity prototype and

ed
which of the factors can reduce the perceived riskiness of the idea. In addition, the complexity of

py
the design ideas were not investigate, and therefore we do not know if the complexity is a factor

Co
impacting likelihood to move forward. This is a factor worth investigating in the future since

ot
more unique ideas may be more complex to understand, and therefore may need a higher fidelity
tN
model to communicate them effectively. Lastly, since students were given very little information
rip

about the products they were choosing to move forward with, we do not know how knowing
sc

factors such as cost of production, cost of final product, and time to market would affect their
nu

willingness to move forward with a particular idea.

There is little research connecting the concept generation phases of design to the later stages
Ma

of development, such as prototyping and testing. This work explored the impact of prototype
ed

fidelity on concept selection and perceived risk in order to highlight the importance of prototype
pt

fidelity in the design process. Prototyping represents one of the largest sunk costs of the product
ce

development process, and thus it is critical for companies to understand how frequently and at
Ac

what level prototypes should be created. The work presented here, along with related future

work, contributes to the understanding of prototype fidelity, concept selection, and perceived

risk.

!26 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This material is based upon work supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant

No. 1351493. We would also like to thank Dr. Sarah Ritter for her help with gathering

participants for our study.

d
ite
ed
py
Co
ot
tN
rip
sc
nu
Ma
ed
pt
ce
Ac

!27 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME

APPENDIX A: EXAMPLE OF THE PROTOTYPING SURVEY

d
ite
ed
py
Co
ot
tN
rip
sc
nu
Ma
ed
pt
ce
Ac

!28 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME

d
ite
ed
py
Co
ot
tN
rip
sc
nu
Ma
ed
pt
ce
Ac

!29 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME

d
ite
ed
py
Co
ot
tN
rip
sc
nu
Ma
ed
pt
ce

!
Ac

!30 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME

d
ite
ed
py
Co
ot
tN
rip
sc
nu
Ma
ed
pt
ce

!
Ac

!31 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME

d
ite
ed
py
Co
ot
tN
rip
sc
nu
Ma
ed
pt
ce
Ac

!32 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME

d
ite
ed
py
Co
ot
tN
rip
sc
nu
Ma
ed
pt
ce

!
Ac

!33 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME

d
ite
ed
py
Co
ot
tN
rip
sc
nu
Ma
ed
pt
ce

!
Ac

!34 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME

d
ite
ed
py
Co
ot
tN
rip
sc
nu
Ma
ed
pt
ce
Ac

!35 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME

d
ite
ed
py
Co
ot
tN
rip
sc
nu
Ma
ed
pt
ce

!
Ac

!36 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME

d
ite
ed
py
Co
ot
tN
rip
sc
nu
Ma
ed
pt
ce

!
Ac

!37 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME

d
ite
ed
py
Co
ot
tN
rip
sc
nu
Ma
ed
pt
ce
Ac

!38 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME

d
ite
ed
py
Co
ot
tN
rip
sc
nu
Ma
ed
pt
ce

!
Ac

!39 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME

d
ite
ed
py
Co
ot
tN
rip
sc
nu
Ma
ed
pt
ce

!
Ac

!40 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME
BIBLIOGRAPHY

1. Fox, J. The Big Spenders on R&D. Tech 2016 [cited 2017 02/09/2017]; Available from:

https://www.bloomberg.com/view/articles/2016-04-29/amazon-and-facebook-are-big-spenders-

on-r-d.

2. Cooper, R.G., Winning at new products: Accelerating the process from idea to launch.

d
ite
2001, New York, NY: Perseus Books.

ed
3. Cooper, R.G., Perspective: The stage-gate® idea-to-launch process—update, what's

py
new, and nexgen systems. Journal of product innovation management, 2008. 25(3): p. 213-232.

Co
4. Kolodner, J.L. and L.M. Wills, Powers of observation in creative design. Design Studies,

1996. 17(4): p. 385-416.

ot
5. Brereton, M. and B. McGarry. An observational study of how objects support engineering
tN
design thinking and communication: implications for the design of tangible media. in
rip

Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. 2000. ACM.
sc

6. Buchenau, M. and J. Fulton Suri, Experience Prototyping, in Designing interactive


nu

systems: processes, practices, methods, and techniques. 2000: Brooklyn, NY. p. 424-433.
Ma

7. Yang, M.C. and D.J. Epstein, A study of prototypes, design activity, and design outcomes.

Design Studies, 2005. 26(2005): p. 649-669.


ed

8. Liu, L. and P. Khooshabeh. Paper or interactive?: a study of prototyping techniques for


pt
ce

ubiquitous computing environments. in CHI'03 extended abstracts on Human factors in


Ac

computing systems. 2003. ACM.

9. Dow, S.P., et al., The effect of parallel prototyping on design performance, learning, and

self-efficacy. 2009, Stanford Tech Report.

!41 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by
10.ASME
McCurdy, M., et al. Breaking the fidelity barrier: an examination of our current

characterization of prototypes and an example of a mixed-fidelity success. in Proceedings of the

SIGCHI conference on Human Factors in computing systems. 2006. ACM.

11. Doke, E.R. and N. Swanson, Decision variables for selecting prototyping in information

systems development: A Delphi study of MIS managers. Information & Management, 1995.

d
29(4): p. 173-182.

ite
12. Viswanathan, V.L., JS, Physical Models and Design Thinking: A Study of functionality,

ed
Novelty, and Variety of Ideas. Journal of Mechanical Design, 2012. 134.

py
13. Walker, M., L. Takayama, and J.A. Landay. High-fidelity or low-fidelity, paper or

Co
computer? Choosing attributes when testing web prototypes. in Proceedings of the human factors

ot
and ergonomics society annual meeting. 2002. SAGE Publications Sage CA: Los Angeles, CA.
tN
14. Elsen, C., et al., Representation in Early Stage Design: An Analysis of the Influence of
rip

Sketching and Prototyping in Design Projects, in Design Engineering Technical Conferences.


sc

2012: Chicago, IL. p. 737-747.


nu

15. Neeley, W.L., et al. Building fast to think faster: exploiting rapid prototyping to accelerate

ideation during early stage design. in ASME 2013 International Design Engineering Technical
Ma

Conferences and Computers and Information in Engineering Conference. 2013. American


ed

Society of Mechanical Engineers.


pt

16. Dow, S.P., et al., Parallel prototyping leads to better design results, more divergence, and
ce

increased self-efficacy. ACM Transactions on Computer-Human Interaction (TOCHI), 2010.


Ac

17(4): p. 18.

17. Bailey, B., Paper prototypes work as well as software prototypes. Usability. gov. 2005.

2010.

!42 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by
18.ASME
Menold, J., T.W. Simpson, and K.W. Jablokow. The Prototype for X (PFX) Framework:

Assessing the Impact of PFX on Desirability, Feasibility, and Viability of End Designs. in ASME

2016 International Design Engineering Technical Conferences and Computers and Information

in Engineering Conference. 2016. American Society of Mechanical Engineers.

19. Catani, M.B. and D.W. Biers. Usability evaluation and prototype fidelity: Users and

d
usability professionals. in Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual

ite
Meeting. 1998. SAGE Publications Sage CA: Los Angeles, CA.

ed
20. Rudd, J., K. Stern, and S. Isensee, Low vs. high-fidelity prototyping debate. interactions,

py
1996. 3(1): p. 76-85.

Co
21. Sauer, J., K. Seibel, and B. Rüttinger, The influence of user expertise and prototype

ot
fidelity in usability tests. Applied ergonomics, 2010. 41(1): p. 130-140.
tN
22. Ulrich, K.T., S.D. Eppinger, and A. Goyal, Product design and development. 2011, New
rip

York, NY: McGraw-Hill.


sc

23. Wall, M.B., K.T. Ulrich, and W.C. Flowers, Evaluating prototyping technologies for
nu

product design. Research in Engineering Design, 1992. 3(3): p. 163-177.

24. Sauer, J. and A. Sonderegger, The influence of prototype fidelity and aesthetics of design
Ma

in usability tests: Effects on user behaviour, subjective evaluation and emotion. Applied
ed

ergonomics, 2009. 40(4): p. 670-677.


pt

25. Christie, E.J., et al. Prototyping strategies: literature review and identification of critical
ce

variables. in American Society for Engineering Education. 2012. American Society for
Ac

Engineering Education.

!43 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by
26.ASME
Virzi, R.A., J.L. Sokolov, and D. Karis. Usability problem identification using both low-

and high-fidelity prototypes. in Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in

Computing Systems. 1996. ACM.

27. Macomber, B. and M. Yang. The role of sketch finish and style in user responses to early

stage design concepts. in ASME 2011 International Design Engineering Technical Conferences

d
and Computers and Information in Engineering Conference. 2011. American Society of

ite
Mechanical Engineers.

ed
28. Sternberg, R., Handbook of Creativity. 1999, New York: Cambridge University Press.

py
29. Runco, M.A. and G.J. Jaeger, The standard definition of creativity. Creativity Research

Co
Journal, 2012. 24(1): p. 92-96.

ot
30. Starkey, E., C.A. Toh, and S.R. Miller, Abandoning creativity: The evolution of creative
tN
ideas in engineering design course projects. Design Studies, 2016.
rip

31. Lim, Y.-k., et al. Comparative analysis of high-and low-fidelity prototypes for more valid
sc

usability evaluations of mobile devices. in Proceedings of the 4th Nordic conference on Human-
nu

computer interaction: changing roles. 2006. ACM.

32. Lim, Y.-K., E. Stolterman, and J. Tenenberg, The anatomy of prototypes: Prototypes as
Ma

filters, prototypes as manifestations of design ideas. ACM Transactions on Computer-Human


ed

Interaction (TOCHI), 2008. 15(2): p. 7.


pt

33. Harris, C.R. and M. Jenkins, Gender differences in risk assessment: why do women take
ce

fewer risks than men? 2006.


Ac

34. Foster, J.D., J.W. Shenesey, and J.S. Goff, Why do narcissists take more risks? Testing

the roles of perceived risks and benefits of risky behaviors. Personality and Individual

Differences, 2009. 47(8): p. 885-889.

!44 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by
35.ASME
Purdy, G., ISO 31000: 2009—setting a new standard for risk management. Risk Analysis:

An International Journal, 2010. 30(6): p. 881-886.

36. Heath, C. and A. Tversky, Preferences and Beliefs: Ambiguity and Competence in Choice

Under Uncertainty. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 1991. 2: p. 5-35.

37. Sitkin, S.B. and A.L. Pablo, Reconceptualizing the determinants of risk behavior.

d
Academy of Management Review, 1992. 17(1): p. 9-38.

ite
38. Dewett, T., Linking intrinsic motivation, risk taking, and employee creativity in an R&D

ed
environment. R&D Management, 2007. 37(3): p. 197-208.

py
39. Liu, M., Y. Wen, and S.A. Burns, Life cycle cost oriented seismic design optimization of

Co
steel moment frame structures with risk-taking preference. Engineering Structures, 2004. 26(10):

ot
p. 1407-1421. tN
40. Van Bossuyt, D., I. Tumer, and S. Wall, A case for trading risk in complex conceptual
rip

design trade studies. Research in Engineering Design, 2012: p. 1-17.


sc

41. Barclift, M., et al. An Investigation Into the Driving Factors of Creativity in Design for
nu

Additive Manufacturing. in ASME 2017 International Design Engineering Technical

Conferences and Computers and Information in Engineering Conference. 2017. American


Ma

Society of Mechanical Engineers.


ed

42. Khurana, A. and S.R. Rosenthal, Integrating the fuzzy front end of new product
pt

development. IEEE Engineering Management Review, 1997. 25(4): p. 35-49.


ce

43. Jensen, M.B., C.W. Elverum, and M. Steinert, Eliciting unknown unknowns with
Ac

prototypes: Introducing prototrials and prototrial-driven cultures. Design Studies, 2017. 49: p.

1-31.

!45 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by
44.ASME
Toh, C.A. and S.R. Miller, Choosing Creativity: The Role of Individual Risk and

Ambiguity Aversion on Creative Concept Selection in Engineering Design. Research in

Engineering Design, 2016. 27: p. 195-219.

45. Rietzchel, E.F., B.A. Nijstad, and W. Stroebe, Productivity is not enough: a comparison

of interactive and nominal groups in idea generation and selection. Journal of Experimental

d
Social Psychology, 2006. 42(2): p. 244-251.

ite
46. Mueller, J.S., S. Melwani, and J.A. Goncalo, The Bias Against Creativity: Why People

ed
Desire But Reject Creative Ideas. Psychological Science, 2011. 20(10): p. 1-5.

py
47. Greenberg, M.D., et al. Crowdfunding support tools: predicting success & failure. in

Co
CHI'13 Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems. 2013. ACM.

ot
48. Dawis, R.V., Scale construction. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1987. 34(4): p. 481.
tN
49. Weng, L.-J., Impact of the number of response categories and anchor labels on coefficient
rip

alpha and test-retest reliability. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 2004. 64(6): p.
sc

956-972.
nu

50. Starkey, E., et al., Dissecting Creativity: How Dissection Virtuality, Analogical Distance,

And Product Complexity Impact Creativity And Self-Efficacy, in The 7th International
Ma

Conference on Design Computing and Cognition. 2016: Evanston, IL. p. 10.


ed

51. Amabile, T., Creativitiy in Context. 1996, Boulder, Colorado: Westview Press.
pt

52. Keil, M., et al., An investigation of risk perception and risk propensity on the decision to
ce

continue a software development project. Journal of Systems and Software, 2000. 53(2): p.
Ac

145-157.

!46 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by
53.ASME
Besemer, S.P., Creative Product Analysis Matrix: Testing the Model Structure and a

Comparison Among Products--Three Novel Chairs. Creativity Research Journal, 1998. 11(4): p.

333-346.

54. Besemer, S.P. and K. O'Quin, Confirming the Three-Factor Creative Product Analysis

Matrix Model in an American Sample. Creativity Research Journal, 1999. 12(4): p. 287-296.

d
55. Han, S.D., et al., Neural intrinsic connectivity networks associated with risk aversion in

ite
old age. Behavioral Brain Research, 2012. 227: p. 233-240.

ed
56. Toh, C.A. and S.R. Miller. The Preferences for Creativity Scale (PCS): Idenfifying the

py
underlying constructs of creative concept selection. in Proceedings of ASME 2016 International

Co
Design Engineering Technical Conference & Design Education. 2016. Charlotte, NC.

ot
tN
rip
sc
nu
Ma
ed
pt
ce
Ac

!47 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME
Figure Cap+ons List

Figure 1 Likelihood to Move Forward Scores by Low, Medium, and High


Levels of Usefulness and Uniqueness.

Figure 2 Perceived Riskiness Scores by Low, Medium, and High Uniqueness


for Low, Medium and High Fidelity

d
ite
ed
py
Co
ot
tN
rip
sc
nu
Ma
ed
pt
ce
Ac

!48 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use


Journal of Mechanical Design. Received June 30, 2018;
Accepted manuscript posted December 21, 2018. doi:10.1115/1.4042339
Copyright (c) 2018 by ASME
Table Cap+ons List

Table 1 All ideas at all levels of quality and novelty for low, medium, and
high-fidelity prototypes

d
ite
ed
py
Co
ot
tN
rip
sc
nu
Ma
ed
pt
ce
Ac

!49 Copyright © 2018 by ASME

Downloaded From: https://mechanicaldesign.asmedigitalcollection.asme.org on 01/10/2019 Terms of Use: http://www.asme.org/about-asme/terms-of-use

You might also like