You are on page 1of 9

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Esthetic evaluation of dynamic smiles with


attention to facial muscle activity
Angela I-Chun Lin,a Thomas Braun,b James A. McNamara Jr,c and Geoffrey E. Gerstnerd
Ann Arbor, Mich

Introduction: Numerous studies of smile esthetics have used still photos. Photos, however, do not capture the
dynamics of a smile, an element that can contribute to overall smile esthetics. In this study, we assessed the
esthetics of dynamic smiles. Methods: Four facially balanced female dental students were trained to produce
8 distinct smiles using the facial action coding system. Videos of the models’ whole faces were presented to
2 panels of raters: dental students and nondental undergraduate students. Smile attractiveness was rated using
a Web-based survey. Results: The smile that used 4 labial muscles was rated significantly better than the smile
involving only the risorius muscle (P \0.05). The orbicularis oculi improved smile attractiveness (P \0.04), es-
pecially among smiles rated less favorably (P \0.05). Visibility of the models’ eyes, however, did not influence
the ratings (P .0.05), perhaps because orbicularis oculi activation altered activations in other muscles in such
a way that smile attractiveness was increased in the lower face. Conclusions: Smile esthetics increased with
increased recruitment of muscles involved in smile production. The results were robust across the models, sug-
gesting that objective rating methods of smile-dynamic esthetics could become an important clinical tool. (Am J
Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 2013;143:819-27)

S
miles play a critical role in esthetics and social be captured by still images. For instance, Tarantili
behavior.1 With the reemergence of the esthetic et al5 identified 3 phases in the motion of a spontaneous
paradigm in patient-centered care, smile smile: an initial attack phase, a sustaining phase, and
enhancement has become increasingly important.2 a fade-out phase. Smiles transition between these
However, a consensus on the criteria for smile esthetics phases through time; therefore, it is impossible to render
does not yet exist. In this study, we quantified the a complete smile in a single photograph.
esthetics of smiles and tested the reliability and repeat- Studies of smile dynamics suggest that facial
ability of these measures. movements are critical in esthetic assessments of a smile.
Most investigations of smile esthetics did not Rubenstein6 showed low correspondence between the
examine the facial movements involved in smiling. The attractiveness ratings of a face presented in the static vs
results were based on still images cropped to show the dynamic format. Static images probably relay
only the subject’s lips and dentition.3,4 A smile, structural facial information, whereas the dynamic
however, consists of dynamic movements that cannot format introduces nonstructural features such as
emotions. These observations suggest a critical need to
From the University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.
a
Postgraduate student, Graduate Orthodontic Program, Department of
develop objective methods to quantify and assess
Orthodontics and Pediatric Dentistry; private practice, Chelsea, Mass. dynamic smile esthetics. The facial action coding system
b
c
Assistant professor, Department of Biostatistics, School of Public Health. is a standardized method of describing facial muscular
Thomas M. and Doris Graber Endowed Professor of Dentistry, Department of
Orthodontics and Pediatric Dentistry, School of Dentistry; research professor,
activity during facial expressions.7,8 The system
Center for Human Growth and Development; private practice, Ann Arbor, Mich. identifies 46 distinct facial movements described as
d
Associate professor, Department of Biologic and Materials Sciences, School of action units. Most smiles involve contraction of the
Dentistry.
The authors report no commercial, proprietary, or financial interest in the
zygomaticus major muscle, action unit 12, and
products or companies described in this article. constriction of the eyes through contraction of the
Supported in part by the Delta Dental Foundation of Michigan. orbicularis oculi, action unit 6. Action unit 6, also called
Reprint requests to: Geoffrey E. Gerstner, Department of Biological and Material
Sciences, School of Dentistry, University of Michigan, 1011 N University Ave, Ann
“Duchenne’s marker,” has been associated with
Arbor, MI 48109-1078; e-mail, geger@umich.edu. spontaneous smiles or true enjoyment.9,10 Subjects
Submitted, September 2012; revised and accepted, January 2013. asked to use action unit 6 during smiling report feeling
0889-5406/$36.00
Copyright Ó 2013 by the American Association of Orthodontists.
more positive emotions, and they display concomitant
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ajodo.2013.01.017 changes in central nervous system activity.11
819
820 I-Chun Lin et al

Fig 1. Left: examples of smiles 1-4, with and without use of eyes (action unit 6). Right: depiction of the
main muscles involved in the study smiles. AU, Action unit.

The facial action coding system relies on subjective were recruited from the predoctoral dental classes at
interpretations by observers. Ekman and Friesen8 the University of Michigan School of Dentistry. Inclusion
attempted to overcome this subjectivity by calibrating criteria were (1) women, ages 20 to 30 years; (2) good
and certifying facial action coding system observers. facial and skeletal balance based on extraoral examina-
This calibration improved intrarater reliability; however, tions; (3) good general health; and (4) good dental
interrater reliability remained less than ideal. health with functional dentitions. The models were
Gracely et al12-14 developed a box scale to differentiate within 15% of the ideal body mass index for their age,
between sensory and affective components of pain. The and they had no history of organic disease, congenital
scale has demonstrated high correlations between disease, orofacial pain, dysfunction, arthritis, joint noises
sessions, groups, and experiments, and it has high or restrictions, and medications known to interfere with
internal and external validity. Because of its power and affective or motor parameters. The models were required
validity as a rating method, the scale was modified to to follow the instructions to learn to perform specific
provide esthetic ratings in our study. smiles on command. Only female models were selected
The aims of this study were to examine (1) whether spe- for this initial study to simplify interpretation of the
cific attributes of dynamic smiles were involved in esthetic results by controlling for sex effects.
ratings, (2) the role of the eyes in esthetic measurements, The exclusion criteria were based on the study of
and (3) the agreement between dental professionals and Kokich et al15 of altered dental esthetics: maxillary
laypeople on perceptions of dynamic smile esthetics. incisor angulation $2 mm from ideal, maxillary midline
$4 mm from the facial midline, open gingival embrasure
MATERIAL AND METHODS between the maxillary central incisors $2 mm (space
The study was approved by the University of between the tip of the interdental papilla and the inter-
Michigan's institutional review board. Smile models proximal contact point), maxillary incisal plane deviation

June 2013  Vol 143  Issue 6 American Journal of Orthodontics and Dentofacial Orthopedics
I-Chun Lin et al 821

Fig 2. Instruction page for the box-scale rating, part 3 of the survey. The actual survey Web page was
similar to the instruction page, except that the phrases in the top left corner were removed from the
survey pages. Shown is a snapshot of a video with the subject's eyes blocked and the vertical Gracely
box scale with numbered boxes, 0-20, along with descriptors placed adjacent to the scale in a manner
similar to that used in pain studies (see text). The raters could play the video repeatedly by pressing
a button at the bottom left of the video; however, as soon as a numbered box was clicked, the survey
proceeded automatically to the next page.

$1 mm, and gingiva-to-lip distance $2 mm at the The models practiced as many times as necessary to
maximum smile. The final 4 models were selected by achieve the desired movements, timing, and spontane-
an orthodontic expert (A.L.) from the original sample ity. The smiles were videotaped with a camcorder aligned
of 8 candidates, all of whom met the inclusion and with the model’s midsagittal plane to provide a full
exclusion criteria described above. frontal view of the face. The camera was located near
The 4 models were trained to produce 8 different the level of the subject’s Frankfort horizontal plane
smiles (Fig 1). The study smiles were carefully and 2 m from the subject’s face.
constructed using the facial action coding system to Upon command, the subjects produced each smile
control for variations in smiles. The activity in action starting from the neutral position, held the maximum
units was monitored visually to reduce unwanted position for 2 seconds, and then returned to the neutral
variations in smile dynamics. position. Hence, the total smile episode, from neutral
The action units used included (1) action unit back to neutral, lasted about 3 seconds, which is the
6, “cheek raise,” contraction of the orbicularis oculi; duration of many human motor events and their
(2) action unit 10, “upper lip raiser,” contraction of perceptions.16 Training typically lasted 15 to 30 minutes
the zygomaticus minor and levator labii superioris; for each model and involved having them watch
(3) action unit 12, “lip corner puller,” contraction of themselves in a mirror while performing each smile on
the zygomaticus major; (4) action unit 20, “lip stretch,” verbal commands. After training, 2 or 3 trials were
contraction of the risorius; and (5) action unit 25, “lips necessary to obtain a video clip in which a satisfactory
part,” used by the facial action coding system to indicate smile was performed. The videos then were screened
parting of the lips, thus showing the dentition (Fig 1). by the same investigator to identify the best clip to
All smiles were controlled for absence of nostril flare, represent each smile for each model.
chin and forehead muscle movements, eye movements, Videos were edited in i-Movie (Apple Inc, Cupertino,
movements at the glabella, blinking, and unwanted Calif) and converted into individual 3-second clips in
muscle movements involving the lips. MPEG 4 format. Each clip was 7 3 6.2 cm (2.8 3 2.44
The models were trained by 1 investigator (A.L.) to in) on the computer monitor and showed the entire
produce all 8 smiles naturally from the rest position. head and neck region of the model plus the top of the

American Journal of Orthodontics and Dentofacial Orthopedics June 2013  Vol 143  Issue 6
822 I-Chun Lin et al

Fig 3. Instruction page of the forced comparison, part 4 of the survey. The actual survey Web pages
looked similar to that shown except for the phrasing in the top left corner. The raters could play the
videos repeatedly if desired by pressing the appropriate button in the bottom left corner of the videos;
however, the computer algorithm accepted the first click on the box below the video as the rater’s
response. The raters then clicked on the “next” box, bottom of the screen, to proceed with the survey.

In part 1, rater participant data, the raters’


Table I. Demographic data on the raters by panel
demographic information including sex, age, ethnicity,
Age (y) occupation, and sexual orientation were collected. The
Sex Ethnicity
Panel (F:M) \21 21-25 .25 (W:As:Af:H:I:U)
participants also indicated whether they knew any of
Dental 31:21 0 42 10 32:14:5:0:1:0 the models before this study. It was determined that
Lay 15:16 25 6 0 25:2:0:2:0:2 the raters gave higher scores to models whom they
Total 46:37 25 48 10 57:16:5:2:1:2 knew than to those they did not know (P \0.001).
Therefore, data representing models whom raters knew
F, Female; M, male; W, white; As, Asian or Asian American;
Af, African or African American; H, Hispanic; I, Indian; U, unknown. were removed from analyses.
In part 2, overall facial attractiveness of the models,
shoulders. The clips were duplicated, and the copy was photographs of the 4 models were presented on 1 Web
edited to include a rectangular block (7 3 1.9 cm, or 2.8 page. The pictures showed only the models’ heads, and
3 0.75 in) that covered the model’s eyes and nose (Figs all models were performing smile 4 in the photos. The
2 and 3). This approach resulted in 16 video clips (8 smiles participants were asked to rate each model’s overall
with and without the rectangular block) for each model. facial attractiveness on a scale of 1 to 10, with 1 the least
Two rater panels were recruited from the University attractive and 10 the most attractive.
of Michigan and signed informed consent forms to Based on the ratings in the preliminary pilot study,
participate. The dental panel consisted of dental the attractiveness ratings of models C and H were similar
students in their first to fourth years. The lay panel to each other, whereas the ratings of models K and S were
consisted of students from the Department of Psychol- similar to each other. As a result, only one of models C
ogy research subject pool. Anyone over 18 years old and H and only one of models K and S were assigned ran-
was invited to participate. The dental panel consisted domly to each rater for parts 3 and 4 of the survey.
of 52 raters (31 women, 21 men), and the lay panel In part 3, box scale, all 32 videos (16 per model 3 2
included 31 subjects (15 women, 16 men; Table I). models) were presented to a rater 3 times in random
A Web-based survey was created containing 5 parts. order, requiring 96 total responses (3 3 32). The raters se-
Each part had detailed instructions and examples of the lected the number in the box scale corresponding to their
tasks to be completed. The raters were allowed to watch impression of the attractiveness of each smile (Fig 2).
the smile videos as many times as needed to make their In part 4, forced comparison, 2 videos were presented
decisions. They were not allowed to skip responses or simultaneously, and the raters were forced to choose
change their responses once answered. which of the 2 smiles was more esthetic (Fig 3). The paired

June 2013  Vol 143  Issue 6 American Journal of Orthodontics and Dentofacial Orthopedics
I-Chun Lin et al 823

videos were selected randomly using the following rules: As an example, to compare smile types while holding
(1) the pair represented the same model; and (2) the pair “use of action unit 6” and “blocking of eyes” constant,
differed from each other in only one of 3 ways—type of we first determined for each rater the number of times
smile (smile 1, 2, 3, or 4), use of the eyes (action unit 6 each smile type was selected in the forced comparison
used or not used), or blocking of the eyes (eyes blocked with every combination of eye use and eye block. The
or eyes visible). This protocol resulted in 40 paired video frequencies of the 4 smile types then were modeled
presentations for each model: ie, 2 eye-use conditions with multinomial logistic regression as a function of
times 2 eye-blocking conditions times 10 smile pairs— the participant’s sex, age, panel, smile, use of action
smiles 1 and 1, 1 and 2, 1 and 3, 1 and 4, 2 and 2, 2 unit 6, and blocking of eyes. Because each rater
and 3, 2 and 4, 3 and 3, 3 and 4, and 4 and 4. evaluated a given smile many times during the survey,
To control for spatial order effects, each of the all P values were computed using bootstrap methods
40 video pairs was presented twice using an algorithm to adjust for this repeated-measure dependency.
that changed the left-to-right ordering randomly on
the screen. This resulted in the raters viewing a total of RESULTS
80 (40 3 2) pairs of smile videos for each model. Since For part 1, rater participant data, 83 raters took the
each rater viewed the smiles from 2 models, 160 total survey: 52 in the dental panel and 31 in the lay panel
presentations were rated. (Table I). Although the lay panel was younger than the
In part 5, overall facial attractiveness of models, at dental panel (P \0.001), the age range (18-26 years)
the end of the survey, the raters were asked once again was young adult in both panels; therefore, age was
to rate each model’s overall facial attractiveness on a not deemed a relevant factor for investigation in our
scale of 1 to 10. The presentation of this part was study. All but 1 rater indicated “heterosexual” for sexual
identical to part 2. orientation. Only raters on the dental panel were
The raters took the survey at the walk-in computer acquainted with the models (Table I).
center in the University of Michigan School of Dentistry For part 3, box scale, a quantile-quantile plot showed
with the Mozilla Firefox web-browser software (Mozilla a normal distribution for the box-scale scores for each of
Foundation, Mountain View, Calif) on personal com- the 4 smiles, pooled across models and eye-related fac-
puter desktops linked to a high-speed Internet cable. tors. Therefore, parametric statistics were used to ana-
lyze the box-scale data.
Statistical analysis Each smile was presented 3 times in the box-scale
Descriptive statistics were calculated for the raters portion of the survey. An analysis of order effects
including sex, age, ethnicity, and sexual orientation. showed no significant differences in presentation order
Parametric tests were used to analyze the box-scale for any of the 4 smiles; therefore, scores from the second
data (part 3, above). The mean box score for each smile presentation were used in analyses.
was plotted to identify rating trends that might have Also, for model S, the difference between all 4 smiles
existed across the 3 presentations of each video. Data was insignificant; therefore, the data for model S were
from the second video presentation were chosen for excluded from further analysis.
analysis using a univariate analysis of variance (SPSS For all raters, smile 4 was rated the best, followed by
version 17.0.1; SPSS, Chicago, Ill) to study effects from smiles 3, 1, and 2 (Fig 4). For both rater groups, the
smile type, use of action unit 6, blocking of eyes, rater difference between smiles 4 and 2 was significant
panel, rater age, and rater sex. for models H (P 5 0.045) and K (P 5 0.005), but
Data from the forced comparison (part 4) were insignificant for model C (Fig 4).
analyzed using multinomial logistic regression. Logistic Among lay and dental raters, contracting the orbicu-
regression is used to predict the probability of the occur- laris oris caused a significant increase in the scores for
rence of an event based on a set of dependent and inde- models H (P 5 0.038) and K (P 5 0.004) for the less
pendent variables. attractive smiles (Fig 5). Use of the eyes also improved
For this study, 3 forced-comparison analyses were the scores for model C, but the effect was statistically
performed using multinomial logistic regression: insignificant.
(1) comparing the 4 smile types while holding “use of Blocking the eyes (Fig 2) had no significant effect on
action unit 6” and “blocking of eyes” constant; (2) com- the scores involving any of the models or either rater group.
paring “use of action unit 6” while holding smile type The dental panel gave significantly higher scores to
and “blocking of eyes” constant; and (3) comparing all 3 models than did the lay panel (P \0.001).
“blocking of eyes” while holding smile type and “use Among all raters, women gave significantly
of action unit 6” constant. higher scores to models C and K than did the men

American Journal of Orthodontics and Dentofacial Orthopedics June 2013  Vol 143  Issue 6
824 I-Chun Lin et al

Fig 4. Box-scale ratings for the 4 types of smiles for Fig 5. Effect of eye use on box ratings for the 4 types of
models K, H, and C. Smile numbers 1-4 correspond to smiles for models K (top), H (middle), and C (bottom).
those represented in Figure 1. *P \0.05; ***P \0.005. Smile numbers 1-4 correspond to those in Figure 1. No,
No eyes; yes, eyes used.
(P \0.001). No significant sex differences were found
involving model H. use of eyes, and blocking of eyes. Since each rater
Table II shows the raw forced comparison data from made many forced comparisons, repeated measures
1 rater. The numbers in the 4 smile columns indicate the were modeled as well.
number of times each smile type and eye condition were The results for the multinomial logistic regression
selected by this rater; eg, the smiles in the first row all appear in Table III, which represents the combined
have eyes blocked with no use of eyes. The data from all 83 raters and shows the probabilities for
frequencies of the 4 smile types were modeled as selection of a given smile type. Table III shows that smile
a function of the participant’s sex, panel, smile type, 4 was preferred the most (probability of selection, 35%

June 2013  Vol 143  Issue 6 American Journal of Orthodontics and Dentofacial Orthopedics
I-Chun Lin et al 825

Table II. Results from 1 rater


Number of times smile selected

Sex Age (y) Panel Use of eyes Blocking of eyes Smile 1 Smile 2 Smile 3 Smile 4
Male 21-25 Dental No Yes 6 2 8 8
Yes Yes 4 3 8 9
No No 6 2 8 8
Yes No 5 3 9 7

A consistent pattern of differential preference


Table III. Forced comparison results: fitted probability
emerged from the box scores and the forced comparison
for the 4 smile types while controlling for “use of eyes”
data; smile 4 was most liked, followed by smiles 3, 1, and
and “blocking of eyes”
2. How do these results compare with established criteria
Probability that each smile type was selected for ideal smile esthetics? The smile index (ratio between
intercommissure width and interlabial gap) is one of the
Use of eyes 1 2 3 4 Total
No 0.22 0.10 0.32 0.35 1.00
few measurements that focus on the overall shape of the
Yes 0.25 0.16 0.28 0.31 1.00 lips as dictated by the movement of facial musculatures.
According to Ackerman et al17 and Ackerman and
Ackerman,18 an ideal smile has an index between
without eyes and 31% with eyes), followed by smile 3, 5 and 6. Smile index values for the 4 smile types in all
then smile 1, and finally smile 2. 3 models were measured and calculated.19 No presenta-
Table III demonstrates that the probability of select- tions of the most-preferred smile, smile 4, had the
ing smile 1 increased by 3% (0.25 0.22 5 0.03) with “ideal” smile index. By contrast, 2 presentations of smile
eye use. Similarly, the probability that smile 2 would 2, the least-preferred smile, had the “ideal” index of 6.
be selected increased by 6% with inclusion of eyes These results clearly contradict the established
(0.16 0.10 5 0.06). On the other hand, the probability orthodontic ideal of the smile index, suggesting the
of selecting smile 3 or smile 4 decreased by 4% when the inadequacy of examining esthetics using static and
eyes were used. The changes in probabilities with eye use cropped images of smiles.
over all 4 smiles was significant (P 5 0.001). However, The use of the orbicularis oculi during smiling had
whether eyes were included did not affect the smile a positive effect on all smiles. As shown by the forced
preference order (4 .3 .1 .2). Blocking of the eyes comparison data, the positive effect was even more
had no impact on the probabilities (P .0.05). significant for smiles 1 and 2, the 2 least-preferred
Model attractiveness ratings varied significantly smiles. This change in preferential distribution suggests
depending on the rater’s sex. Female raters gave that people already with attractive smiles might benefit
significantly higher ratings than did the men for models some but not greatly from the use of eyes. On the other
C and H (P \0.001) but only marginally significantly hand, those with less attractive smiles can improve their
higher ratings for model K (P 5 0.067). On the other esthetics with some help from the orbicularis oculi.
hand, the rater’s panel affiliation did not significantly This finding of the positive eye effect complemented
impact model rating. the results of another study, where the use of the orbicu-
laris oculi was identified as intensifying the emotional
DISCUSSION valence.20 In our study, the participants not only picked
Smile esthetics are influenced by the subjective up on the use of the orbicularis oculi, but also related
preferences of a viewer and by objective measures of this muscle activity to better smile esthetics. This finding
a smile. In this study, we aimed to separate the objective suggests an important relationship between smile
model factors from the subjective rater factors to attractiveness and positive emotional valence.
establish objective criteria of dynamic smile esthetics Our method of blocking the eyes involved covering
that can be used in orthodontic diagnosis and treatment most of the midface. It was, therefore, surprising that
planning. blocking the eyes had little effect on the ratings. This
The results of this study identified the type of smile, finding suggested that actions of the orbicularis oculi
the use of eyes, and the blocking of eyes as model showed through the smile regardless of whether a rater
factors. These factors were intrinsic to the smiles and could directly see the model’s eyes and midface. During
common across all models and all raters, regardless of the training sessions, the models had difficulty producing
the raters’ sex, panel, or knowledge of the models. consistent smiles when activation of the orbicularis oculi

American Journal of Orthodontics and Dentofacial Orthopedics June 2013  Vol 143  Issue 6
826 I-Chun Lin et al

was added; the use of the orbicularis oculi tended to make were used, regardless of whether the eyes were visible
each smile larger. Despite efforts to control this variation, to the raters, suggesting that eye use influenced the
perhaps the raters still picked up on subtle differences actions of other facial muscles. The results were robust
produced when the orbicularis oculi were used. across the models, suggesting that objective rating
Sex, panel, and knowledge effects were the rater methods of smile-dynamic esthetics could become an
factors in this study. These factors represent rater- important clinical tool. The discrepancy between the
dependent preferences or tastes in beauty. Even though traditional ideal smile index and the smile index of the
the 2 rater groups were unequal in size—51 dental raters preferred smile (smile 4) in this study suggested that
vs 31 lay raters—we do not believe that these inequalities static smile images are perceived differently from
impacted the results reported below. dynamic smile videos. Therefore, it is important to
In this study, female raters gave higher esthetic scores reexamine the criteria for evaluating smile esthetics us-
than did the men in almost all cases. Two speculations ing smile videos.
can be made with regard to this sex difference:
We thank Richard Gracely for extensive contributions
(1) women might be less critical of facial attractiveness
to the assessment tool designs, Clayton Gerstner for
of other women than are men, or (2) the sex difference
creating the survey and algorithms necessary to conduct
observed might be more a reflection of the differential
the survey online, Beth Crane for help with facial action
sex preference in various facial attributes: ie, the models
coding, and Colleen Cunningham for reconstructing the
might have had certain facial features that appealed
figures for reproduction.
more to the female than to the male participants. The
latter conclusion is supported by the study of Meerdink
et al,21 who stated that men and women focus on REFERENCES
different features when assessing the attractiveness of 1. Mehu M, Dunbar R. Naturalistic observations of smiling and laugh-
faces. Furthermore, Flores-Mir et al22 demonstrated ter in human group interactions. Behaviour 2008;145:1747-80.
that men are less critical than women in rating facial 2. Ackerman JL, Proffit WR, Sarver DM. The emerging soft tissue
paradigm in orthodontic diagnosis and treatment planning.
attractiveness when presented with still images of smiles,
Clin Orthod Res 1999;2:49-52.
suggesting that the first speculation, above, is not 3. Witt M, Flores-Mir C. Laypeople's preferences regarding frontal
a general finding. dentofacial esthetics: periodontal factors. J Am Dent Assoc
The dental panel liked all 3 models more than the lay 2011;142:925-37.
panel did. Recent studies indicate cultural differences in 4. Janson G, Branco NC, Fernandes TM, Sathler R, Garib D, Lauris JR.
Influence of orthodontic treatment, midline position, buccal
the perceptions of smile esthetics among various lay
corridor and smile arc on smile attractiveness. Angle Orthod
populations.23 If dental and nondental raters interpret 2011;81:153-61.
esthetics differently, it will be important to identify 5. Tarantili VV, Halazonetis DJ, Spyropoulos MN. The spontaneous
what are the specific differences that patients and smile in dynamic motion. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 2005;
practitioners can agree on for treatment outcomes. 128:8-15.
6. Rubenstein AJ. Variation in perceived attractiveness: differences
The box scales and forced comparisons hold great
between dynamic and static faces. Psychol Sci 2005;16:759-62.
promise to objectify and quantify esthetic measures. 7. Ekman P, Friesen WV. Nonverbal leakage and clues to deception.
Because we used specifically defined rater groups—lay Psychiatry 1969;32:88-106.
and dental students—its external validity is limited; there- 8. Ekman P, Friesen WV. A tool for the analysis of motion picture film
fore, future studies should be conducted to further test the or video tape. Am Psychol 1969;24:240-3.
9. Ekman P. Facial expressions of emotion: an old controversy and
external and ecologic validity and psychometrics of these
new findings. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1992;335:63-9.
methods in orthodontics. Also, since esthetics and their in- 10. Steiner F. Differentiating smiles. In: Branninger-Huber E,
terpretations can vary depending on demographic factors, Steiner F, editors. FACS in psychotherapy research. Zurich,
future studies with models and raters representing Switzerland: Department of Clinical Psychology, Universitat
broader demographic attributes will be required to deter- Zurich; 1986. p. 139-48.
11. Ekman P, Davidson RJ, Friesen WV. The Duchenne smile:
mine how the findings generalize.21-25
emotional expression and brain physiology. II. J Pers Soc Psychol
1990;58:342-53.
CONCLUSIONS
12. Gracely RH, Dubner R, McGrath P, Heft H. New methods of pain
This study was one of the first investigations to use measurement and their application to pain control. Int Dent J
video clips of smiles to look for objective criteria in smile 1978;28:52-65.
13. Gracely RH, McGrath F, Dubner R. Ratio scales of sensory and
esthetics. Smile esthetics increased with increased
affective verbal pain descriptors. Pain 1978;5:5-18.
recruitment of muscles involved in smile production: 14. Gracely RH, McGrath P, Dubner R. Validity and sensitivity of
smile 4 was more attractive than smiles 3, 2, and 1. ratio scales of sensory and affective verbal pain descriptors:
The ratings were higher when the eyes (orbicularis oris) manipulation of affect by diazepam. Pain 1978;5:19-29.

June 2013  Vol 143  Issue 6 American Journal of Orthodontics and Dentofacial Orthopedics
I-Chun Lin et al 827

15. Kokich VO Jr, Kiyak HA, Shapiro PA. Comparing the perception of 21. Meerdink JE, Garbin CP, Leger DW. Cross-gender perceptions of
dentists and lay people to altered dental esthetics. J Esthet Dent facial attributes and their relation to attractiveness: do we see
1999;11:311-24. them differently than they see us? Percept Psychophys 1990;48:
16. Poppel E. The brain’s way to create ‘nowness’. New York: Springer; 227-33.
1997. 22. Flores-Mir C, Silva E, Barriga MI, Lagravere MO, Major PW. Lay
17. Ackerman JL, Ackerman MB, Brensinger CM, Landis JR. A morpho- person's perception of smile esthetics in dental and facial veiws.
metric analysis of the posed smile. Clin Orthod Res 1998;1:2-11. J Orthod 2004;31:204-9.
18. Ackerman MB, Ackerman JL. Smile analysis and design in the dig- 23. McLeod C, Fields HW, Hechter F, Wiltshire W, Rody W Jr,
ital era. J Clin Orthod 2002;36:221-36. Christensen J. Esthetics and smile characteristics evaluated by
19. Lin AIC. Smile esthetics: evaluating smile esthetics through laypersons. Angle Orthod 2011;81:198-205.
attention to facial muscular actvity [thesis]. Orthodontics. 24. Houstis O, Kiliaridis S. Gender and age differences in facial
Ann Arbor: University of Michigan; 2008. p. 127. expressions. Eur J Orthod 2009;31:459-66.
20. Messinger DS, Mattson WI, Mahoor MH, Cohn JF. The eyes have 25. Kawamura S, Komori M, Miyamoto Y. Smiling reduces masculinity:
it: making positive expressions more positive and negative principal component analysis applied to facial images. Perception
expressions more negative. Emotion 2012;12:430-6. 2008;37:1637-48.

American Journal of Orthodontics and Dentofacial Orthopedics June 2013  Vol 143  Issue 6

You might also like