You are on page 1of 9

Journal of Pereonality and Social Psychology Copyright 1986 by the American Psychological Association, lac.

1986, Vol. 51, No. 1,140-148 0022-3514/86/$00.75

Social Comparison and Depression: Company's Effect on Misery

Frederick X. Gibbons
Iowa State University

In this article, two studies examine the social comparison processes of depressed and nondepressed
college students. In the first study, subjects' preferences for information from others were assessed
after they had received a manipulation intended to improve or worsen their mood states. The re-
sponses of the depressed subjects provided evidence of downward comparison, as they indicated a
preference for information from people who were experiencing negative affect—but only when they
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

themselves were also experiencing relatively negative affect, and not when their moods had been
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

temporarily improved. In the second study, subjects' moods were assessed before and after they had
received information indicating another person was currently experiencing very negative affect. This
information had little effect on the nondepressed subjects, however, the mood states of the depressed
persons improved after they read the information. In general, the results indicate that realizing that
others are doing worse may help depressed persons to feel somewhat better.

Although continued interest in Festinger's (1954) social com- are depressed tend to be avoided (Coates, Wortman, & Abbey,
parison theory has resulted in a number of important modifi- 1980; Hammen & Peters, 1978), in part, says Coyne (1976a),
cations (cf. Snyder & Wicklund, 1981; Suls & Miller, 1977), the because they can make others feel bad. Coyne (1976b) found
basic tenets of the theory have not changed much in the last 30 that nondepressed subjects reported feeling worse after interact-
years. According to Festinger, when people choose targets for ing with depressed persons (also, cf. Howes, Hokanson, &
comparison, they usually prefer others who appear to have Loewenstein, 1985), and Hammen and Peters (1978) found the
slightly more of the trait or the ability in question than they same reaction to persons enacting a depressed role.
do—a tendency called upward drive or upward preference. The
reason for this preference is that people who are better off are
Downward Comparison
thought to be most likely to provide useful information that can
facilitate improvement on the dimension or the behavior being In one of the recent modifications of social comparison the-
compared. Festinger recognized, however, and more recent the- ory, it is suggested that the upward drive or tendency does not
orists have emphasized, that the selection process is not always hold for some people in some situations. Brickman and Bulman
done objectively or without bias. In particular, the need to know (1977), for example, have suggested that the opposite behavior,
may often be overridden by a need to know something avoidance of persons who are doing well, may occur when it is
positive—even if it is not entirely accurate—thereby protecting thought that the comparison might provide unfavorable infor-
the ego. Thus, comparison with others who are slightly better is mation. This is especially true for persons whose self-esteem is
usually preferred, because it has the potential to boost self-es- low. They may feel threatened by others who are better off and,
teem if the outcome happens to be favorable, and yet it is not as a result, choose to avoid such people and seek out others who
likely to pose much threat to the ego should the outcome be appear to be doing worse than they are—a process that has been
disappointing. termed downward comparison. In a recent review of the rele-
Although the topic has received relatively little empirical at- vant literature in this area, Wills (1981) came to the conclusion
tention, it seems likely that the same motivation might apply that this particular type of social comparison is most likely to
to comparisons involving affect or mood states. When given a occur when a person has experienced a decrease in subjective
choice, people should prefer and perhaps even seek out the com- well-being, and that it is most common among people who are
pany of others who are experiencing positive affect. Mood states "most unhappy and least fortunate."
tend to be contagious, and the appeal of persons who are in a Taylor, Wood, and Lichtman (1983) found anecdotal evi-
good mood is likely to increase their attractiveness (as an infor- dence of downward comparison among persons they inter-
mation source) in comparison situations. In contrast, it has viewed who had been victimized by disease, crime, or other
been demonstrated that people who are in a bad mood or who misfortunes. Similarly, more direct evidence of downward com-
parison among women adjusting to breast cancer is provided by
Wood, Taylor, and Lichtman (1985). The tendency was for these
people to seek out and socially compare with other persons who
The author would like to thank Sharon Brehm and Meg Oerrard for
had been more severely victimized. Thus, women who had had
their comments on this article.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Fred- lumpectomies might compare with those who had mastecto-
erick X. Gibbons, Department of Psychology, Iowa State University, mies, paraplegics with quadriplegics, and so forth. Taylor et al.
Ames, Iowa 50011. argue that although this may appear to be an unusual method

140
SOCIAL COMPARISON AND DEPRESSION 141

of coping with misfortune, it also seems to be an effective one. ments. The predictions were that (a) the nondepressed subjects
Seeing someone who is worse off may leave these distressed per- would prefer to read positive statements; whereas (b) the de-
sons with the impression that they are not so bad off themselves pressed subjects would prefer positive statements when their
with regard to either their own coping success or their relative mood states had been improved, but negative statements when
severity of affliction. As a result, some of their distress and de- their mood states had been worsened (cf. Wills, 1981). Thus a
pression may be alleviated. Type of Statement X Depression group interaction was antici-
pated on the choice variable. In the second study, the effect of
reading negatively toned statements on mood states was exam-
Downward Comparison and Affect
ined.
The downward comparison literature tends to complicate the
affect/social comparison relation discussed earlier, because it Method
suggests that the tendency to prefer happy people and avoid un-
happy persons (Coates et al., 1980; Gergen & Wishnow, 1965) Subjects
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

is not consistent. In particular, it may be the case that persons


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Twenty-five undergraduate males and 20 undergraduate females par-


who are experiencing some kind of emotional or affect-related ticipated as part of an introductory psychology course extra credit op-
distress do not prefer the company of others who are in a good tion. The data from 2 women were not included in the final analysis
mood. Instead they may do just the opposite—seek out others because of procedural problems. In both experiments, subjects were
who are not doing well or not feeling particularly happy. In fact, administered the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck, Ward, Men-
there is some indirect evidence in the literature to support this delsohn, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961) during large mass-testing sessions
hypothesis. Coates and Winston (1983), for example, examined from 2 to 6 weeks prior to participation in the study. Only those with
BDI scores of 3 or less or 11 or more were chosen to participate. In the
self-help groups for persons experiencing physical illness and
first experiment, the mean BDI score of the low group was 1.81, and the
emotional distress, and suggested that depressed persons in
mean BDI score of the high group was 21.59. The majority of subjects
these groups seek out other depressed persons in order to obtain
in the latter group fell into Beck's (1978) "moderate-severe depression"
some information as to how well they themselves are doing. category (i.e., scores of 20-29) at the time of testing.
In another study by Coates (Coates & Peterson, 1982), de-
pressed college students gave some indication that they enjoyed
Procedure
talking with others who were instructed to act unhappy. Similar
results came from a survey of college students' reactions to dys- The PEQ. Subjects were run individually. Initial information pro-
phoria by Funabiki, Bologna, Pepping, and Fitzgerald (1980). vided by the experimenter (who was unaware of the subj ect's BDI score)
In that study, mildly depressed students were somewhat (i.e., indicated that the study concerned impression formation, or "the pro-
nonsignificantly) more likely to endorse what the authors la- cess whereby people get to know one another." Subjects were informed
that they would be providing some information about themselves and
beled as a "misery loves company" variable, which consisted of
reacting to information given to them in different forms by other people.
the items "When I am depressed . . . I want to be with other
After receiving the initial instructions, subjects were taken to a cubicle
depressed people," and "I get joy out of making others de-
where they were told they would have 10 min to perform the first task.
pressed." Finally, Bell (1978) found that subjects whose mood This consisted of writing a self-disclosure statement, or what was called
states had been temporarily depressed preferred subsequently a Personal Events Questionnaire (PEQ). In the positive condition, the
to work with a happy person, but were more willing than nonde- PEQ asked subjects to write about an event, which had happened to
pressed subjects to work with another who they thought was them in the last year, that had a significant positive effect on them; in
also feeling sad. Paradoxically, it may be that being in the pres- the negative condition, the word positive was replaced by the word nega-
ence of others who are also feeling bad (or worse) may help de- tive. At the end of the session, the experimenter returned, collected the
pressed persons to feel better. Although consistent with Wills's PEQ from the subject, and then left him or her alone in the cubicle. All
of the remaining instructions were presented via an intercom, and there
(1981) reasoning, this hypothesis remains unproven. The pur-
was no further contact between the experimenter and the subject.
pose of the current two studies was to examine this hypothesis
The questions. The experimenter told the subject to open up the first
directly.
folder as soon as she left and answer it. This questionnaire contained six
mood items (sad, cheerful, calm, worried, discouraged, and pleasant),
Study 1: Overview which were selected from the Multiple Affect Adjective Checklist (Zuck-
erman & Lubin, 1965). Each item was accompanied by a 9-point scale
In the first study, depressed and nondepressed college stu- (from not at all to very). Upon completion of this scale, subjects were
dents' preferences for social comparison partners were assessed. instructed to place it back in the folder and then they were allowed to
Subjects were first asked to write a self-disclosure statement wait alone for 8 min. The purpose of this waiting period was to give
about an event that had happened to them recently, which had subjects time to reflect back on the incident they had described. Al-
though no specific instructions were given to subjects to reflect, pilot
either a significant negative or a significant positive effect on
research had indicated that this period enhanced the effectiveness of the
them. Then, after their mood states had been assessed, they
PEQ manipulation.
were asked to choose one group of statements from a list of Statement selection and Mood 2. At the end of the waiting period,
seven categories that they had been presented, which they were subjects were instructed to take the questionnaire out of the next folder
to read and evaluate. The statements supposedly belonged to and answer it. This questionnaire contained one item, which was the
other subjects, and the categories were based on the type of primary dependent variable. It indicated that subjects would now be
affect (very positive to very negative) displayed in those state- asked to read and evaluate (i.e., provide their opinions of) a number of
142 FREDERICK X. GIBBONS

Table 1 Statement Selection


Study 1: Mood Self-Report and Statement Selection
An analysis of subjects' responses regarding selection of PEQ
Variable statements revealed a main effect of type of statement written,
F(l, 39) = 5.44, p < .03, ij2 = .11, as subjects who had written
Subject N Mood 1" Mood 2 Choice"
positive statements preferred to read more positive statements
Negative statement 0 (M = 5.91 vs. 4.73; see Table 1). However, this main effect was
qualified by a Statement X Group interaction, F(l, 39) = 4.03,
Nondepressed 11 7.45, 8.27, 5.64,
p = .05, i)2 = .08.2 As expected, the depressed subjects who had
Depressed 11 0.00y -.37, 3.82,
written about a negative event requested the most negative PEQ
Positive statement" statements to read (ps for all comparisons < .05, according to
Newman-Keul's tests).
Nondepressed 10 13.00, 15.00, 5.80,
Depressed 11 4.00,y 7.54, 6.00,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Discussion
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Note. For each variable, cells without common subscripts differ at the
p < .05 level according to Newman-Keul's tests.a Mood indexes (self- In general, subjects did prefer to read the positive statements,
rating) = Sum of positive items — Sum of negative items. Range = —24 and this was especially true if they had written a positive state-
to 24. b Level of affect of statements subject chose to read. A low score
ment themselves. As expected, the only exception to this ten-
indicates preference for statements containing relatively negative affect
(Scale = 1 -7).c Valence of statement written by the subject. dency came from the depressed subjects, who had been exposed
to a mood lowering manipulation; these subjects did not ask for
positive affect statements. Thus, the results offer support for the
hypothesis and provide some evidence of downward social com-
PEQ statements from persons who had previously participated in the parison in terms of target preference: (a) with regard to affect
study. To facilitate this process, the statements had supposedly already and (b) among depressed college students, relative to nonde-
been read once (preliminarily) and then grouped into one of seven cate-
pressed students.
gories according to the type of affect that was displayed in them by the
Why downward comparison? Although the responses of the
authors. The categories were numbered from 1 (very negative) to 7 (very
depressed subjects suggest they were interested in engaging in a
positive). Subjects were told that statements from all seven categories
needed to be read eventually, but that each subject would have a choice form of downward comparison, the data provide little indica-
as to which single group of statements she or he wanted to read. As soon tion of why they wanted to do it. Researchers who have worked
as this item had been completed, subjects were instructed to answer the in this area (e.g., Taylor etal., 1983; Wills, 1981) have suggested
second mood scale, which was intended as a check on their mood states that people engage in downward comparison because it helps
at the time they answered the choice question. This scale consisted of them feel better, either through self-aggrandizement or simply
six mood items, which were the polar opposites of those on the first scale alleviation of negative mood. In the second study, the impact of
the subjects had filled out (i.e., happy, gloomy, upset, peaceful, encour- downward comparison on mood states was examined in order
aged, and irritated). Opposite adjectives were used on the second scale
to test this hypothesis directly.
in order to reduce subject suspicion regarding the mood variable. When
they finished this questionnaire, subjects were informed that they would
not be reading any more PEQ statements, and then they were debriefed Study 2: Overview
and dismissed.
Depressed and nondepressed subjects were asked to write
PEQ statements this time about any event, regardless of out-
Results

Mood Items 1
Although the mean Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) score of 21.6
was well above the cut-off score for depression (10) at the time subjects
Mood Indexes were calculated for both the first and second
took the scale, several researchers (e.g., Sacco, 1981) have indicated that
scales by subtracting the sum of the three negative items from
BDI scores tend to be unstable over time. Thus, although the differences
that of the three positive items. Intercorrelations among the ad- on the mood scales did indicate that the depressed group was experienc-
jectives of the positive and the negative indexes averaged .44 for ing more negative affect than was the nondepressed, it is possible that
the first scale and .52 for the second. Correlations between the some of these subjects may have moved out of the depression category
two scales and the BDI scores were -.40 and -.41 (JK < .004). by the time of their participation in the experiment. (It is also possible,
A 2 factor (Depression Group X Statement) unweighted means although not very likely, that some subjects moved up from the nonde-
analysis of variance was performed on this first mood index. pressed to the depressed category during this time interval.) Hence, it
This analysis revealed a significant main effect of depression may actually be technically more accurate to label these subjects as the
negative affect or dysphoric group. In short, the term depressed is used
group, and a slight trend toward a main effect of statement, as
to describe one group of subjects relative to the other and is not meant
the depressed subjects and those who wrote about a negative
to imply any type of clinical disorder.
event tended to report more negative affect, Fs(l, 39) = 8.32 2
Analyses for both studies were conducted a second time including
and 2.20, ps < .006 and .11, respectively (see Table 1). Both of sex of subject as an independent variable. These analyses produced re-
these main effects were highly significant on the second mood sults comparable to the present set, with no significant main effects or
scale, which was filled out after the waiting period and state- interactions involving this variable (all Fs < 1.6). Thus, data from both
ment selection, Fs( 1, 39) = 11.52 and 9.51, both ps < .004.' sexes are presented together.
SOCIAL COMPARISON AND DEPRESSION 143

come, that had an impact on them. After writing their state- completed a questionnaire that concerned the extent of their
ments, subjects' moods were assessed, and then they were asked psychiatric problems as well as their responsibility for them. It
to read a false PEQ statement, supposedly written by their part- seems possible that asking subjects to ruminate on such issues
ner, that was very negative in tone. Mood states were then as- might have actually served as a mood manipulation of sorts.
sessed again after subjects had read their partner's PEQ. Once For this reason, the relation between chronic mood and self-
again the primary predictions involved the depressed subjects: focused attention was examined again in the current study. In
Based on the reasoning of Taylor et al. (1983) and Wills (1981), this case, half of the subjects responded to the mood questions
it was predicted that the realization that their partner was expe- in the presence of a mirror, but without any direct mood manip-
riencing negative affect—more negative than their own—would ulation. The prediction, once again, was that self-awareness
result in some alleviation of dysphoria for the depressed sub- would affect (i.e., exacerbate) subjects' chronic mood tenden-
jects. cies. In sum, the design of the study was a 2 X 2 (High/Low
Three additional groups of variables were also included in Depression x Self-Aware/Not Self-Aware) factorial.
this study, all of which were intended to assess subjects' reac-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tions to the PEQ statements that they had written and read. The
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Method
first group consisted of attributions (i.e., internal vs. external)
that subjects made for the event that they had described. In this Subjects
case, it was thought that the depressed subjects might differ
The subjects were 28 male and 29 female undergraduates. The mean
from the nondepressed in the amount of responsibility they ac-
score on the BDI for the low group was .48 and for the high group the
cepted for the event, especially if those events happened to be
mean was 17.11. The data from 2 subjects were not included in the final
negative in tone (cf. Gong-Guy & Hammen, 1980). This type
analysis: one because he did not read the partner's PEQ and one because
of attribution could also have an impact on their mood states, she doubted it's authenticity.
thereby making it difficult to determine how much of an effect
their partner's PEQ had on them. Similarly, subjects' percep-
tions of the intimacy of their statements were assessed. Again,
Procedure
the purpose here was to determine if subjects' perceptions of Subjects were run in same-sex pairs. Instructions were similar to
their own self-disclosure statements would be related to their those given in the first experiment, but with modifications reflecting the
mood states. Finally, the third dependent variable was subjects' fact that there were 2 subjects instead of 1. Initially they were greeted
assessments of the mood reflected in their partner's PEQ. This in a waiting room and told that the experiment concerned impression
was intended primarily as a check on the PEQ manipulation, formation. During the study they would be asked to provide informa-
and the prediction here was simply that all subjects would see tion about themselves to one another so that they could get to know
their partner's mood as being very negative, in fact, more nega- each other a little better. After determining that the subjects were not
acquainted, the experimenter led them to separate cubicles where they
tive than their own. However, evidence from two recent studies
were to fill out the PEQ.
suggests that this tendency might be stronger for the nonde-
The PEQ. This PEQ asked subjects to write about an event, which
pressed subjects. Tabachnik, Crocker, and Alloy (1983) re-
had happened to them in the recent past, that led to an outcome that
ported that nondepressed persons generally tend to view others had a "significant impact" on them. The favorability of the outcome
as being more depressed than they perceive themselves to be. was not specified, so that subjects could write about any type of event
Similarly, Kuiper and MacDonald (1982) found that nonde- they wanted to. Ten minutes were allowed for the PEQ. At the end of
pressed subjects rated themselves as less depressed than either the period, the statements were collected, and then one of the subjects
a generalized or a well-known other, whereas depressed subjects was randomly chosen to be moved into another cubicle, supposedly so
saw themselves as more depressed than a generalized, but not a that the first cubicle could be "set up for use later in the experiment."
well-known other. This cubicle contained a (64 cm X 120 cm) mirror along with a set of
folders, an intercom, and some excess electronic equipment (e.g., an
Self-focus and affect. There is another, secondary aspect of
oscilloscope). The experimenter indicated to the subject that the extra
this next study that concerns the relation between self-focused
equipment, including the mirror, belonged to another experimenter; no
attention and depression. In a recent study, Gibbons et al. subjects expressed any suspicions about the mirror.
(1985) examined the hypothesis that self-focused attention can Mood 1. Another minute was allowed (for the self-aware subject to
enhance awareness of chronic mood traits. As expected, clini- become adjusted to the cubicle), and then both subjects were instructed
cally depressed subjects in that study did report more negative to answer the first mood questionnaire, which consisted of a 6-item
affect when their attention was self-focused than when it was mood scale (happy, tense, peaceful, angry, apathetic, and secure).
not. Part of the significance of this finding centered around the Self PEQ Assessments. The next questionnaire asked subjects first to
fact that previous studies demonstrating emotional amplifica- make attributions for the events that they had described. For each event,
they indicated how much each of the following four factors contributed
tion under conditions conducive to self-focus (e.g., Scheier,
to the outcome: ability, effort, task difficulty, and luck. They were then
1976; Scheier, Carver, & Gibbons, 1981) all involved situations
asked to assess the intimacy of the statement in terms of how personal
in which subjects' mood states had been temporarily manipu-
it was, as well as how much they revealed about themself and how much
lated by a stimulus of some kind. However, because of a poten- affect they had displayed in it.
tial procedural problem in the Gibbons et al. study, the question Other PEQ. After completing this questionnaire, subjects were told
of how self-focused attention affects chronic mood traits has not that their PEQ statements would now be switched so that they would
been answered unequivocally. That is, the self-reported affect of have an opportunity to read and react to each other's statements. In
the psychiatric patients in that study was assessed after they had fact, all subjects were then presented with the same bogus PEQ. This
144 FREDERICK X. GIBBONS

Table 2
Study 2: Mood Assessments: Self and Other

Variable

Ability
Initial Other Final Mood attribution
Subject N mood* mood" mood change11 (self)"

Self-aware
Nondepressed 13 12.61, -10.08xy 13.84, 1.23 3.77
Depressed 15 2.33y -13.87, 5.93y 3.60 6.73

Nonself-aware
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Nondepressed 14 14.78, -12.35xy 13.00, -1.78 4.07


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Depressed 13 7.47ly -8.84,, 11.69, 4.22 5.62

Note. For each variable, cells without common subscripts differ at thep < .05 level according to Newman-Keul's tests.
• Scale for self = -24 to 24; for other = -25 to 15.
" Mood 2 - Mood 1; a positive number means a positive mood change. Main effect of depression = p < .05.
c
Attribution to factor of ability for incident described by subject. Scale = 1 (not at all important) to 9 (very important).

statement concerned an accident which had happened to the author's subjects wrote appears not to have interfered with either the
nephew while she or he was (supposed to have been) babysitting the self-awareness or the partner PEQ manipulations.
child. In addition to broken Angers, the accident was apparently also
going to leave the boy with a permanent scar on his face, and it was
clear that the incident had had a negative impact on the author, who
Mood Variables
expressed some guilt and implied some responsibility for it. After read- Initial mood and other's mood. The average intercorrelations
ing the statement, subjects were asked to put it away and then evaluate
for the positive and negative adjectives of the first scale were .53
it in terms of how much affect was displayed in it. This was done on a 5-
and .25, and for the second scale they were .32 and .55.3 The
item scale (happy, upset, depressed, enthusiastic, and lonely). A different
correlation between the first scale and the BDI was —.44 (p <
scale was used, once again, in order to reduce suspicion regarding the
purpose of the mood questions. .001). An analysis of this first mood index revealed a main effect
Mood 2. The final questionnaire consisted of a second mood scale of depression group, F(\, 51) = 15.35, p< .001 (see Table 2).
containing the polar opposites of the six items in the first mood scale As expected, the mean of the self-aware depressed group was
(i.e., sad, calm, upset, good-natured, interested, and hopeless). A com- the lowest of all, but it was not significantly different from the
parison of subjects' responses to this second mood scale with those on nonself-aware depressed group, /(51) = 1.64, p< . 11. Looking
the first provided an indication of the impact that reading their partner's at subjects' assessments of their partner's affect (Table 2), it can
PEQ statement had on their mood states. Debriefing occurred as soon be seen that they did think she or he was in a worse mood than
as the final mood questionnaire was completed.
they were, even considering the fact that the scales were not
identical.4 An analysis of this variable revealed an unexpected
Results Self-Awareness X Group interaction, F(\, 51) = 4.53, p < .04,
but no main effect of depression,/; > .30. Thus, the expectation
PEQ Statements that nondepressed subjects would evaluate their partner's mood
more negatively (cf. Kuiper & MacDonald, 1982) was not sup-
All of the subjects' PEQ statements were read by a group of ported by the data. As can be seen in Table 2, the cell that was
10 undergraduate raters and then evaluated as to how personal discrepant with this prediction was that of the self-aware de-
they were, as well as the amount of positive and negative affect pressed subjects. This mean (—13.87) was quite low, in fact, sig-
displayed in them by the authors (the alphas for the three ratings
were .91, .95, and .93, respectively, suggesting reasonable agree-
3
ment). The 10 ratings were then averaged, and a score was as- In both cases there was an adjective that did not correlate well with
signed to each subject for each rating. Analyses of these scores the others (interested in the positive, Scale 1; apathetic in the negative,
revealed no differences between the depressed and the nonde- Scale 2). Analyses of variance conducted when these adjectives were
removed produced essentially the same results, however.
pressed subjects on any of the dimensions (all Fs < 1.0). This 4
Given that the self- and other mood scales did not contain the same
suggests two things: (a) the PEQ itself did not affect subjects'
(or opposite) adjectives, statistical comparison is not possible. However,
moods, thus the test of the impact of self-focused attention on the means on the two indexes are so discrepant (9.2 vs. -11.4 on the
chronic mood traits was not "contaminated" by a state mood combined index, 2.1 vs. 13.8 on the two 3-item negative scales) as to
manipulation; (b) subjects' responses to the mood scales most suggest a strong difference in subjects' perceptions on this variable.
likely reflected reactions to their partner's and not their own Moreover, a comparison of the item happy, which was on both scales,
PEQ statements. In short, the self-disclosure statement that did reveal a strong self/other contrast effect, 1^1, 51) = 94.63, p < .001.
SOCIAL COMPARISON AND DEPRESSION 145

15 Discussion
As expected, the negative PEQ statements did have the effect
SUBJECTS of improving the mood states of the depressed subjects. Thus,
10 Nondepressed the results of the second study help to answer a question raised
Depressed by the first study and also by previous research; that is, one
reason why depressed (or dysphoric) subjects choose to engage
5 in downward comparison on the dimension of affect is because
it helps them to feel better. The situation was quite different
for the nondepressed subjects. Reading about the misfortune of
Time 1 Time 2 their partner and his or her associated negative affect had little
MEASURE impact on their mood states. Thus, these results provided no
evidence that depressed persons' negative moods can rub off on
Figure 1. Study 2: Mood change after reading the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

others who are not feeling bad (as had been suggested by Coyne,
partner's Personal Events Questionnaire,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

L976b, and Hammen & Peters, 1978). In this case, however,


there was no direct contact between the nondepressed person
and the depressed partner. It may be that direct contact is
nificantly lower than that of the nonself-aware depressed sub- needed in order for mood states to be affected in this way (cf.
jects (-8.84), and lower (but not significantly) than that of the Gotlib & Robinson, 1982; Howes etal., 1985).
(two) nondepressed cells (-11.26).
Final mood and mood change. The same pattern of results Choice Versus Impact
appeared on the second as on the first mood assessment. An
analysis of this index revealed only a main effect of depression There was no mood manipulation in the second experiment;
group, F(\, 51) = 5.41, p < .03. However, this time the self- thus, the results indicate that depressed subjects mood states do
aware depressed group did report more negative affect than did not have to be exacerbated in order for downward comparison
the nonself-aware depressed subjects (M = 5.93 vs. 11.69; t = to have a favorable effect on them. Apparently, even in the ab-
2.07, p < .05), and more than the other two groups of subjects sence of such a manipulation, their mood states are typically
(both p < .05 according to Newman-Keul's tests). As can be negative enough that there is plenty of room for improvement.
seen in Table 2, a comparison of the first and second mood as- However, because there was not a (no-mood manipulation) con-
sessments indicated that there was a slight decline overall in the trol condition in Experiment 1, it is not possible to tell for sure
self-reported moods of the nondepressed subjects (M change = whether depressed subjects will still choose to downward com-
-.24). However, as expected, there was a significant improve- pare in the absence of a mood manipulation—even though their
ment in the moods of the depressed subjects after reading the mood states are improved when such comparison does happen
negative PEQ (Mchange = 3.92). A repeated measures analysis (as was the case in Experiment 2). Likewise, these data do not
of variance indicated that the increase in mood scale scores was indicate whether involuntary downward comparison will im-
significant for both depressed groups (rs[ 13 and 15] = 2.38 and prove the mood states of depressed persons who are already ex-
2.17, ps < .05; see Figure 1). In addition this difference in mean periencing relatively positive affect. Although it can be con-
change scores between the depressed and nondepressed groups cluded from the results of Experiment 1 that depressed persons
was also significant, F(l, 51) = 5.74, .p < .02, T,2 = .10. will not choose to downward compare when they are in a rela-
tively good mood, that does not mean, of course, that their
PEQ self-assessments. With one exception, there were no
moods would not be improved were they to engage in it for some
significant effects on the self-attribution items (all/? > .25), The
reason.
exception occurred on the ability variable, which the depressed
subjects saw as much more relevant to the incident they had
described, F( 1,51) = 9.69, p < .003. In addition, a multivariate
Self-Awareness and Perceptions of Others
analysis of variance was performed on the three questions per- Subjects' assessments of their partner's mood states were gen-
taining to the intimacy of subjects' (own) statements (personal, erally very negative and, for the most part, consistent with ex-
reveal, and affect displayed). This analysis revealed only a sig-
nificant main effect of depression, as the depressed subjects gen- 5
All of the PEQ statements were also evaluated by raters as to the
erally thought their statements were more intimate, F{3,49) = number of self-references contained in them. An analysis of these rat-
3.05, p < .04. Although it might be interesting to speculate on ings indicated that the depressed subjects made more self references
the antecedents of both of these effects, neither one of them was (p < .06) than did the nondepressed group. The same was true in the
included in the prediction, so they will not be discussed further.5 first study (p < .02; see Gibbons, in press). Thus it is possible that the
More importantly, correlational analyses indicated that neither results on the ability attribution item and on the self-assessments of
intimacy may both have reflected a general tendency toward self-preoc-
the self-perceptions nor the attributions were related to the pri- cupation (cf. Gibbons, in press) and, relatedly, toward internal attribu-
mary dependent variables, which were the affect items (all p > tions (cf. Coyne & Gotlib, 1983) by the depressed subjects. Once again,
.20). Thus, the concern that subjects' perceptions of their own however, there was no evidence that either of these unexpected findings
PEQ might have affected their mood states appears to have been was related to the primary concern of this study, which was the subjects'
unfounded. mood reports.
146 FREDERICK X. GIBBONS

pectations. The exception here was the self-aware depressed sponses of the depressed subjects support Wills's (1981) conten-
group who imputed much more negative affect to their partner tion that downward comparison is primarily a habit of people
than was expected. Because this topic has generated very little who are "most unhappy" and is most likely to occur when they
research (or theorizing), it is difficult to speculate on why this have experienced a "decrease in subjective well-being."
happened. One possibility, however, is that just as self-focused
attention intensifies depressed persons' awareness of their own
Why
mood states, it may also intensify awareness of, or perhaps sen-
sitivity to, affect in others. Another, similar possibility is that Taken together, the two studies indicate that downward com-
self-awareness in this type of small group setting may enhance parison does occur among depressed persons, and they offer
projection. Thus, the negative affect that the depressed subjects some evidence as to why it happens. For people who are de-
saw in themselves, they may also have seen, perhaps to a greater pressed, realizing that there are others who are also experienc-
extent, in their partners. Of course, this reasoning is very specu- ing negative affect can provide some useful comparative infor-
lative. However, given that there are a number of potentially mation, either in terms of evidence of a relative lack of coping
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

important clinical implications surrounding this issue, espe- success (Coates & Winston, 1983), or, in some cases, evidence
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

cially with regard to group therapy techniques, then it would that "things could be worse" (Taylor et al., 1983). In the current
appear that the topic of self-focused attention and perceptions studies, we have dealt more directly with the latter type of infor-
of others also deserves further investigation. mation than the former, because the statements subjects re-
ceived concerned their partner's affective reaction to the prob-
General Discussion lem she or he was facing and contained little information about
how they were dealing with it. Moreover, looking at subjects'
Downward Comparison and Affect self- and other mood ratings, it can be seen that even the de-
pressed subjects believed that their partner was feeling worse
Who
than they were. With regard to downward comparison and cop-
In terms of social comparison, the nondepressed subjects in ing, however, even though the results do not provide any direct
these two studies responded very differently from the depressed evidence, they do have some implications for the study of coping
subjects. However, the responses of the former group were sim- mechanisms.
ilar to those of other nondepressed subjects in previous research
on at least one dimension, and that is they did show a clear Downward Comparison Outside the Laboratory
preference for the positive affect statements and virtually no
interest in the negative—further evidence of a general desire or The fact that the depressed subjects in Study 1 were choosing
tendency to avoid people who are experiencing negative affect to socially compare with other (relatively) depressed persons
(Coates et al., 1980; Hammen & Peters, 1978). This occurred does not necessarily mean that they would prefer to affiliate
even when their mood states had been lowered as a result of with other depressed persons outside the laboratory. It does ap-
writing the negative self-disclosure statement. The lack of re- pear, however, that they are more willing than nondepressed
sults among the nondepressed subjects on the choice variable persons to associate with others who are having similar prob-
and on the mood change variable in Study 2, suggest that per- lems. Moreover, there are some specific circumstances under
sons who are not experiencing chronic negative mood do not which this willingness might translate into behavioral prefer-
seek to downward compare on the dimension of affect. More- ence. One such circumstance might occur when a person de-
over; even when it does happen (as in Study 2), it apparently has cides to join a self-help or support group. The literature in this
little effect on them. There is no reason to believe, however, that area has indicated that one reason why people join these groups
downward comparison has a mood amelioration effect only for is because of the "shared stress" atmosphere they offer
depressed or even negative affect persons. It seems quite possi- (Schachter, 1959; Zimbardo & Formica, 1963). Another rea-
ble that anxious persons, or alcoholics, or most any distressed son, apparently, is because of the opportunity that the group can
group, might be heartened to learn that there are others who provide for downward comparison. In this regard, the results
have an equal or even more severe affliction. Generalization of of the current study suggest that these support groups can be
the findings to other such groups shall have to remain for future effective in helping to alleviate negative affect of the partici-
research. pants, but perhaps only when they believe there are others in
the group who are doing at least as poorly, if not worse than
they are. In fact, a persual of the literature in the area suggests
When
that self-help groups are certainly not always effective in allevi-
In contrast to the nondepressed group, the depressed subjects ating negative affect or depression (e.g., Videka-Sherman, 1982;
in the first study showed a definite preference for the more nega- Wortman&Dunkel-Schetter, 1979).
tive statements. This occurred, however, only after they them- It would appear, then, that the participant is taking a risk by
selves had written a negative statement and were feeling bad. joining this type of group. On the one hand, it may very well be
The fact that the depressed subjects did not choose negative that the group does provide its members with an opportunity
statements after their mood had been temporarily elevated indi- for self-aggrandizement through comparison with others who
cates that they were not interested in downward comparison are doing worse. On the other hand, it could also be the case
when they were not experiencing dysphoria. In general, the re- that individuals in the group might perceive there are others
SOCIAL COMPARISON AND DEPRESSION 147

with similar or even more serious afflictions who appear to be References


doing as well or even better than they are. Although the impact
Beck, A. T. (1978). The Beck Depression Inventory. Philadelphia: Cen-
of this type of involuntary "upward comparison" on the mood
ter for Cognitive Therapy.
states of depressed or victimized people has never been assessed,
Beck, A. X, Ward, C. H., Mendelsohn, M, Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J.
it seems likely that such an effect might be different from that (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General
associated with the situation in the current study. Both types of Psychiatry, 4, 561-571.
comparison are likely to take place in groups of this nature but, Bell, P. A. (1978). Affective state, attraction and affiliation. Personality
presumably, with different results. and Social Psychology Bulletin, 4, 616-619.
Brickman, P., & Bulman, R. J. (1977). Pleasure and pain in social com-
parison. In J. M. Suls & R. L. Miller (Eds.), Social comparison pro-
cesses: Theoretical and empirical perspectives. Washington, DC:
Self-Awareness and Depression
Hemisphere.
Coates, D., & Peterson, B. A. (1982). Depression and deviance. In G.
The results of the second study offer further evidence that
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Weary & H. L. Mirels (Eds.), Integrations of clinical and social psy-


self-awareness (in and of itself) can exacerbate the mood states
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

chology. New York: Oxford University Press.


of people who are already experiencing prolonged negative Coates, D., & Winston, T. (1983). Counteracting the deviance of depres-
affect. Based on similar results, Gibbons et al. (1985) concluded sion: Peer support groups for victims. Journal of Social Issues, 39,
that caution is warranted when therapists use self-confrontation 169-194.
techniques that involve some self-focusing by depressed clients Coates, D., Wortman, C., & Abbey, A. (1980). Reactions to victims. In
(e.g., videotape playback). In particular, they suggested that I. H. Frieze, D. Bar-Tal, & J. S. Carroll (Eds.), New approaches to
self-confrontation by itself, without the direct guidance of a social problems. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
therapist, is likely to enhance the dysphoria that the client is Coyne, J. C. (1976a). Toward an interactional description of depression.
Psychiatry, 39, 28-40.
experiencing at the time. The current results support this con-
Coyne, J. (1976b). Depression and the response of others. Journal of
tention and suggest another caveat, also relevant to therapeutic
Abnormal Psychology, 85,186-193.
concerns, which involves the potential impact of self-focused
Coyne, J. C., & Gotlib, I. H. (1983). The role of cognition in depression:
attention on members of self-help groups. According to self- A critical appraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 472-505.
awareness theory (Duval & Wicklund, 1972; Wicklund, 1975), Duval, S., & Wicklund, R. A. (1972). A theory of objective self-aware-
being the primary focus of attention in a group is likely to lead ness. New York: Academic Press.
to an increase in self-focused attention. This means that mem- Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human
bers of self-help or support groups would be expected to experi- Relations. 7, 117-140.
ence some heightened self-awareness, which presumably, would Funabiki, D., Bologna, N. C., Pepping, M., & Fitzgerald, K. C. (1980).
Revisiting sex differences in the expression of depression. Journal of
be associated with an intensification of negative affect as well.
Abnormal Psychology, 89, 194-202.
Although probably more of a problem for some clients than
Gergen, K. J., & Wishnow, B. (1965). Others' self-evaluation and inter-
for others, in most cases, this exacerbation of mood is likely to
action participation as determinants of self-presentation. Journal of
interfere with the groups' therapeutic goals.
Personality and Social Psychology, 2, 348-358.
Another finding from the current study is also relevant here. Gibbons, F. X. (in press). Mild depression and self-disclosure intimacy:
The fact that the self-focused depressed subjects tended to read Self and other's perceptions. Cognitive Therapy and Research.
more negative affect into their partner's statement suggests that Gibbons, F. X., Smith, T. W., Ingram, R. E., Pearce, K., Brehm, S. S.,
they might have been projecting some of their own negative & Schroeder, D. (1985). Self-awareness and self-confrontation:
moods onto their depressed partner or, perhaps, that they were Effects of self-focused attention on members of a clinical population.
experiencing some feelings of similarity with him or her— Journal of Personality of Social Psychology, 48, 662-675.
Gong-Guy, E., & Hammen, C. L. (1980). Causal perceptions of stressful
feelings that were enhanced by their self-focused state (cf. Ste-
life events in depressed and nondepressed outpatients. Journal of Ab-
phenson & Wicklund, 1983, 1984). To the extent that the latter
normal Psychology, 89, 662-669.
explanation is accurate, then it suggests that self-focus in this
Gotlib, I. H., & Robinson, L. A. (1982). Responses to depressed individ-
type of situation may, in fact, be a two-edged sword. On the one
uals: Discrepancies between self-report and observer-rated behavior.
hand, it could have the effect of increasing empathy or identifi- Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 91, 231-240.
cation with other members of the group who are seen as similar. Hammen, O. L., & Peters, S. D. (1978). Interpersonal consequences of
Presumably, this effect would be beneficial in a setting of this depression: Responses to men and women enacting a depressed role.
nature. Once again, however, it is also possible that it could ex- Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 322-332.
acerbate the person's dysphoria, or their insecurity, fears, and Howes, M. J., Hokanson, J. E., & Loewenstein, D. A. (1985). Induction
self-esteem—or whatever problem caused them to be in the of depressive affect after prolonged exposure to a mildly depressed
individual. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1110-
group in the first place. The conclusion to be drawn here, then,
1113.
appears to be similar to that reached by Gibbons et al. (1985):
Kuiper, N. A., & MacDonald, M. R. (1982). Self and other perception
The effect of self-focus in therapeutically relevant situations is
in mild depressives. Social Cognition, 3, 223-239.
complicated, and it can be harmful as well as beneficial. Given Sacco, W. P. (1981). Invalid use of the Beck Depression Inventory to
the popularity of self-confrontation techniques and the extent identify depressed college student subjects: A methodological com-
of involvement of self-awareness in them, it seems clear that ment. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 5,143-147.
more investigation into the self-focus/therapy relation is Schachter, S. (1959). The psychology of 'affiliation. Stanford, CA: Stan-
needed. ford University Press.
148 FREDERICK X. GIBBONS

Scheier, M. F. (1976). Self-focused attention and aggression. Journal of Videka-Sherman, L. (1982). Effects of participation in a self-help group
Personality, 44, 627-644. for bereaved parents: Compassionate friends. In L. P. Boman, L. E.
Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Gibbons, F. X. (1981). Self-focused at- Borck, R. Hess, & F. L. Pasquale (Eds.), Helping people to help them-
tention and reactions to fear. Journal of Research in Personality, 15, selves: Self-help and prevention. New York: Haworth Press.
1-15. Wicklund, R. A. (1975). Objective self-awareness. In L. Berkowitz
Snyder, M. L., & Wicklund, R. A. (1981). A theory of attribute ambigu- (Ed.), Advanced in experimental social psychology (Vol. 8, pp. 465-
ity. In J. Harvey, W. Ickes, & R. F. Kidd (Eds.), New directions in 507). New York: Academic Press.
attribution research. Erlbaum. Wills, T. A. (1981). Downward comparison principles in social psychol-
Stephenson, B. Q, & Wicklund, R. A. (1983). Self-directed attention ogy. Psychological Bulletin, 90,245-271.
and taking the other's perspective. Journal of Experimental Social Wood, J. V., Taylor, S. E., &Lichtman, R. R. (1985). Social comparison
Psychology, 19, 58-77. in adjustment to breast cancer. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psy-
Stephenson, B. Q, & Wicklund, R. A. (1984). The contagion of self- chology, 49. 1169-1183.
focus within a dyad. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Wortman, C. B., & Dunkel-Schetter, C. (1979). Interpersonal relation-
ships and cancer: A theoretical analyses. Journal of Social Issues, 35,
46, 163-168.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

120-155.
Suls, J., & Miller, R. L. (Eds.). (1977). Social comparison processes:
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Zimbardo. P. G., & Formica, R. (1963). Emotional comparison and


Theoretical and empirical perspectives. New York: Wiley.
self-esteem as determinants of affiliation. Journal ofPersonality, 31,
Tabachnik, K, Crocker, J., & Alloy, L. (1983). Depression, social com-
141-162.
parison, and the false-consensus effect. Journal of Personality and So-
Zuckerman, M., & Lubin, B. (1965). Multiple Affect Adjective Check-
cial Psychology, 45, 688-699.
list. San Diego: Educational and Industrial Testing Service.
Taylor, S. E., Wood, J. V., & Lichtman, R. R. (1983). It could be worse:
Selective evaluation as a response to victimization. Journal of Social Received April 12, 1985
Issues, 39, 19-40. Revision received July 3, 1985 •

Integrating Personality and Social Psychology: Call for Papers

The editors intend to publish a special issue devoted to papers demonstrating that social behav-
ior is best understood by integrating the diverse concerns of the three sections ofJPSP. Papers
are invited that deal with particular substantive issues crossing the boundaries of individual
differences, social cognition, and interpersonal relations. Papers should represent practical
demonstrations that the diverse concerns of this journal belong together in a full understanding
of social behavior. We seek previously unpublished contributions, primarily empirical studies,
but we are also amenable to syntheses of long-term research programs and to innovative theo-
retical statements.

Contributions intended for the special issue should be sent to the guest editor:

John F. Kihlstrom, PhD


W. J. Brogden Psychology Building
University of Wisconsin
1202 West Johnson Street
Madison, Wisconsin 53706

Interested authors should send an abstract of their article to the guest editor by September 1,
1986 and plan to submit a completed manuscript by December 1, 1986, at which time the
paper will become subject to the usual peer review process.

Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology editors:

Attitudes and Social Interpersonal Relations and Personality Processes and


Cognition Group Processes Individual Differences
Steven J. Sherman Harry T. Reis Irwin G. Sarason
Charles M. Judd Norbert L. Kerr Edward F. Diener
Warren H. Jones

You might also like