You are on page 1of 6

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/299478546

Stereotype Threat

Chapter · January 2016


DOI: 10.1002/9781118528921.wbeaa134

CITATIONS READS
0 836

1 author:

Thomas M Hess
North Carolina State University
136 PUBLICATIONS   4,182 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Ageing as Future View project

Stereotype Threat View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Thomas M Hess on 06 September 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Stereotype Threat effect, however, tends to be larger when the
stereotype is only moderately explicit versus
Lauren E. Popham and Thomas M. Hess blatant or subtle (Nguyen & Ryan, 2008).
North Carolina State University, USA

Stereotype Threat in Older Adults


What is Stereotype Threat?
Stereotype threat was first identified by Steele
Stereotype threat occurs when one is con-
and Aronson (1995), who recognized that
fronted by a negative, self-relevant stereotype
African American students were under-
in a performance situation (Steele, 1997). Con-
performing on standardized academic tests
cerns about confirming the negative stereotype
because of the fear of confirming negative
result in performance decrements on cog-
racial stereotypes about their intellectual
nitive tests by members of the stereotyped
abilities. In an experiment, they found that
group. When the stereotype is deemphasized,
characterizing a standardized test as diagnostic
however, differences between members of
of intellectual abilities caused African Amer-
stereotyped and nonstereotyped groups are
icans to underperform compared to white
attenuated.
students, but characterizing it as diagnostic
of problem-solving abilities did not. Since
How is Stereotype Threat Induced? Steele and Aronson’s seminal experiment,
Self-relevant stereotypes become salient stereotype-based performance decrements
through situational cues, which then lead have been documented in numerous groups,
to threat-related decrements. Cues that activate including older adults.
stereotype threat may be blatant, moderately Negative aging stereotypes related to memory
explicit, or indirect and subtle (Nguyen & and cognition are typically endorsed by young,
Ryan, 2008). A blatant stereotype cue, for middle-aged, and even older adults (Hummert,
example, would be an experimenter telling an Garstka, Shaner, & Strahm, 1994). The work
older participant that younger adults tend to of Levy (1996, 2003) has revealed that priming
perform better than older adults on a cognitive older adults with negative aging stereotypes
task that they will subsequently be asked to reduces their memory performance and slows
perform. A moderately explicit cue would be their handwriting and walking speed, thereby
when individuals are reminded of subgroup providing evidence that stereotype threat
differences in ability but there is some ambigu- can operate beneath conscious awareness to
ity about the direction of these differences, such degrade performance in older adults. Using
as indicating that men and women generally an explicit stereotype manipulation, Hess,
perform differently on standardized math tests. Auman, Colcombe, and Rahhal (2003) tested
Indirect and subtle cues are those that involve the memory abilities of older and younger
subconscious priming of stereotype-based adults who were exposed to a negative aging
group differences, for example, by having stereotype versus a control group exposed to
people indicate age, race, or gender prior to a setting that deemphasized age differences in
taking a test or by emphasizing the diagnostic memory ability. Older adults exposed to the
nature of a high-stakes test. In each case, threat negative stereotype recalled significantly fewer
leads to underperformance on the cognitive words than the younger adults in the same con-
task, but only in those who are members of dition, whereas in the control condition older
the stereotyped group. The size of the threat adults recalled just as many words as younger

The Encyclopedia of Adulthood and Aging, First Edition. Edited by Susan Krauss Whitbourne.
© 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Published 2016 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
DOI: 10.1002/9781118528921.wbeaa134
2 STEREOTYPE THREAT

adults. Hess and colleagues also found that to assess the full extent of older adults’ memory
the older adults who were highly invested in abilities.
the stereotyped domain were most vulnerable
to threat effects on recall. Similarly, Kang and Mechanisms Underlying Stereotype
Chasteen (2009) observed that threat-related Threat
decrements on memory performance were
worst among the older adults who identi- What are the mechanisms underlying stereo-
fied with the stereotyped group. Although type threat effects? The social psychology
most research with older adults has focused literature includes an abundance of studies
on memory, stereotype threat effects have examining this question and two reliable
also been observed with math performance findings have been reported. First, stereotype
(Abrams et al., 2008) and in the workplace threat leads to evaluative concerns, which set
(Buyens, Van Dijk, Dewilde, & De Vos, 2009; in motion self-regulation processes such as
Von Hippel, Kalokerinos, & Henry, 2013). monitoring one’s behavioral expressions and
There are at least two important implications regulating worrisome task-related thoughts
of this work for the study of aging. The first (Schmader, Johns, & Forbes, 2008). Such
relates to the potential long-term consequences self-regulation processes require cognitive
that negative aging stereotypes and exposure resources, thereby depleting the availability of
to threat can have on older adults. Prolonged resources that are needed for performing well
exposure to threat may have negative effects on the task at hand. Schmader and Johns (2003)
in terms of both exposure to stress (e.g., in have found threat-related decrements in young
the workplace) and the consequences of non- adults’ working memory. Thus, it appears that
participation in contexts or activities in which the diversion of resources to processes other
threat might occur. For example, reducing than the task at hand is a primary mechanism
involvement in activities involving memory of threat effects in younger adults. Interest-
may not only serve to avoid threat, but may ingly, evidence regarding a similar mechanism
also negatively impact cognitive health. The underlying threat effects in older adults is weak.
second major implication of this research For example, Hess, Hinson, and Hodges (2009)
relates to assessment, and the possibility that and Popham and Hess (2015) found no evi-
age differences observed in performance on dence of working memory impairment in older
memory and other types of cognitive tasks adults subjected to threat (but see Mazerolle,
may not just reflect normative age changes in Régner, Morisset, Rigalleau, & Huguet, 2012).
ability. Specifically, inadvertent introduction Emotion regulation abilities have also been
of stereotype threat in testing contexts may found to moderate threat effects in younger
result in an overestimation of age differences adults, but not older adults (Popham & Hess,
in ability. Although it is highly improbable that 2015). That is, younger adults who reported
researchers would explicitly remind partici- low levels of emotional control were vulnerable
pants of aging stereotypes, subtle cues, such to threat effects on working memory, whereas
as having older participants indicate their age younger adults who reported high emotional
before a test or labeling tests as diagnostic of control did not experience working memory
memory ability, may prime aging stereotypes decrements under threat. Given that older
and thus activate threat at a subconscious adults tend to report higher levels of emotional
level. As mentioned previously, subtle stereo- control, this suggests the possibility that age
type cues can operate beneath awareness to differences in the underlying mechanisms are
influence physical and cognitive outcomes rooted in age-related improvements in emotion
in older adults (Bennet & Gaines, 2010). regulation.
Thus, it is advisable that memory researchers A second mechanism hypothesized to
attempt to minimize stereotype cues in order underlie stereotype threat effects relates to
STEREOTYPE THREAT 3

regulatory focus (Higgins, 1997). Nega- Conclusions


tive stereotypes engage prevention-focused
While stereotype threat research continues to
motivation to not confirm the self-relevant
explore the different situations under which
stereotype. Threat-induced prevention focus threat degrades cognitive performance in
translates to task performance that is slow older adults as well as the moderators of threat
and cautious. This approach is maladaptive effects, there is also increasing interest in exam-
for many cognitive tasks that are structured ining the real-life and long-term implications
to reward the number of gains rather than of aging stereotypes.
the number of losses averted, which helps to Stereotypes about age can become embedded
explain why a prevention focus may lead to in a person’s worldview at an early age before
apparent threat-based decrements in some the stereotype applies to oneself (Bennet &
tasks. A series of studies by Seibt and Förster Gaines, 2010), and this can have negative con-
(2004) supported this hypothesis in younger sequences on cognitive and physical outcomes
adults. Recent research with older adults is also with age (Levy, 2009). In an examination of
consistent with a regulatory focus account of longitudinal data, Levy, Zonderman, Slade,
threat effects. For example, Hess, Emery, and and Ferrucci (2009) observed that people
Queen (2009) demonstrated that threat was who had internalized negative aging stereo-
types at midlife had increased probability of
associated with more conservative respond-
experiencing a cardiovascular event nearly 20
ing in older adults. Popham and Hess (2015)
years later. The behavioral pathway leading
expanded on this by demonstrating that threat
to greater cardiovascular risk appears to be
resulted in older adults adopting a more cau- that people who endorse aging stereotypes
tious approach to a task that resulted in slower believe that debilitating health problems are
responding but also reduced errors, both of inevitable in older adulthood. Such negative
which are reflective of a prevention-focused perceptions of aging manifest themselves in
approach to task. Although younger adults unhealthy lifestyle behaviors, and therefore
exhibited a tendency toward a similar type become a self-fulfilling prophecy (Levy, 2009).
of response, their behavior was more char- The physiological pathway may be related to
acterized by the aforementioned working the finding that older individuals have exag-
memory account of threat effects. Barber and gerated cardiovascular responses to stress after
Mather (2013) found similar results when negative versus positive age stereotype primes
they examined older adults’ working mem- (Levy, Hausdorff, Hencke, & Wei, 2000).
ory under threat versus no-threat conditions. Given the implications of internalizing aging
They manipulated the reward structure of the stereotypes on health and functioning, further
working memory test so as to differentiate the research is needed to examine other real-world
effects of aging stereotypes and interventions to
working memory and regulatory focus mech-
mitigate the detrimental effects of stereotypes
anisms: Half of the participants were given a
on older adults. In fact, Levy and colleagues
gain-based reward structure, whereas the other
(2000) have recommended that intervention
half were given a loss-based reward structure. programs aimed at improving cardiovascu-
Older adults’ performance was impaired when lar health and cognitive functioning among
the task had a gain-based reward structure, but older adults would benefit from emphasizing
their performance was enhanced when the task behaviors that help reinforce older individuals’
had a loss-based reward structure, indicating positive self-stereotypes and reduce harmful
that regulatory fit was achieved. This latter negative aging stereotypes. Certainly, educa-
result is interesting in suggesting that threat tion about the impact that negative stereotypes
may not always result in performance deficits. and beliefs about aging can have on one’s own
4 STEREOTYPE THREAT

behavior and that of others will help reduce the memory performance. Journals of Gerontology
impact of threat in everyday life. In addition, Series B: Psychological Sciences & Social Sciences,
characteristics of one’s environment may also 64, 482–486. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp044
have an important impact on experiences of Hess, T. M., Hinson, J. T., & Hodges, E. A. (2009).
stereotype threat. For example, Abrams and Moderators of and mechanisms underlying
colleagues (Abrams et al., 2008; Abrams, Eller, stereotype threat effects on older adults’ memory
performance. Experimental Aging Research, 35,
& Bryant, 2006) found that threat effects in
153–177. doi: 10.1080/03610730802716413
older adults were attenuated when older adults Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain.
had high levels of contact with individuals American Psychologist, 52, 1280–1300.
of younger generations. Such findings have Hummert, M. L., Garstka, T. A., Shaner, J. L., &
important implications for understanding Strahm, S. (1994). Stereotypes of the elderly held
both the mechanisms of stereotype threat by young, middle-aged, and elderly adults.
and the factors that may help reduce its Journal of Gerontology, 49, 240–249.
impact. doi: 10.1093/geronj/49.5.P240
Kang, S. K., & Chasteen, A. L. (2009). The
SEE ALSO: Ageism; Memory moderating role of age-group identification and
perceived threat on stereotype threat among
older adults. International Journal of Aging &
References Human Development, 69, 201–220.
Abrams, D., Crisp, R. J., Marques, S., Fagg, E., Levy, B. R. (1996). Improving memory in old age
Bedford, L., & Provias, D. (2008). Threat through implicit self-stereotyping. Journal of
inoculation: Experienced and imagined Personality and Social Psychology, 71,
intergenerational contact prevents stereotype 1092–1107. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.71.6.1092
threat effects on older people’s math Levy, B. R. (2003). Mind matters: Cognitive and
performance. Psychology and Aging, 23, physical effects of aging self-stereotypes. Journals
934–939. doi: 10.1037/a0014293 of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences &
Abrams, D., Eller, A., & Bryant, J. (2006). An age Social Sciences, 58, P203–P211. doi: 10.1093/
apart: The effects of intergenerational contact and geronb/58.4.P203
stereotype threat on performance and intergroup Levy, B. R. (2009). Stereotype embodiment: A
bias. Psychology and Aging, 21, 691–702. psychosocial approach to aging. Current
doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.4.691 Directions in Psychological Science, 18(6),
Barber, S. J., & Mather, M. (2013). Stereotype threat 332–336. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01662.x
can enhance, as well as impair, older adults’ Levy, B. R., Hausdorff, J. M., Hencke, R., & Wei, J. Y.
memory. Psychological Science, 24(12), (2000). Reducing cardiovascular stress with
2522–2529. doi: 10.1177/0956797613497023 positive self-stereotypes of aging. Journals of
Bennet, T., & Gaines, J. (2010). Believing what you Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and
hear: The impact of aging stereotypes upon the Social Sciences, 55, 205–213. doi: 10.1093/
old. Educational Gerontology, 36, 435–445. geronb/55.4.P205
doi: 10.1080/03601270903212336 Levy, B. R., Zonderman, A., Slade, M. D., &
Buyens, D., Van Dijk, H., Dewilde, T., & De Vos, A. Ferrucci, L. (2009). Negative age stereotypes held
(2009). The aging workforce: Perceptions of earlier in life predict cardiovascular events in
career ending. Journal of Managerial Psychology, later life. Psychological Science, 20, 296–298.
24, 102–117. doi: 10.1108/02683940910928838 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02298.x
Hess, T. M., Auman, C., Colcombe, S., & Rahhal, T. Mazerolle, M., Régner, I., Morisset, P., Rigalleau, F.,
(2003). The impact of stereotype threat on age & Huguet, P. (2012). Stereotype threat
differences in memory performance. Journals of strengthens automatic recall and undermines
Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences & controlled processes in older adults. Psychological
Social Sciences, 58, P3–P11. doi: 10.1093/geronb/ Science, 23, 723–727. doi: 10.1177/
58.1.P3 0956797612437607
Hess, T. M., Emery, L., & Queen, T. L. (2009). Task Nguyen, H.-H. D., & Ryan, A. M. (2008). Does
demands moderate stereotype threat effects on stereotype threat affect test performance of
STEREOTYPE THREAT 5

minorities and women? A meta-analysis of Seibt, B., & Förster, J. (2004). Stereotype threat and
experimental evidence. Journal of Applied performance: How self-stereotypes influence
Psychology, 93(6), 1314–1334. doi: 10.1037/ processing by inducing regulatory foci. Journal of
a0012702 Personality and Social Psychology, 87(1), 38–56.
Popham, L. E., & Hess, T. M. (2015). Age doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.1.38
differences in the underlying mechanisms of Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How
stereotype threat effects. Journals of Gerontology stereotypes shape intellectual identity and
Series B: Psychological Sciences & Social Sciences, performance. American Psychologist, 52,
70(2), 223–232. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt093 613–629. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.52.6.613
Schmader, T., & Johns, M. (2003). Converging Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype
evidence that stereotype threat reduces working threat and the intellectual test performance of
memory capacity. Journal of Personality and African Americans. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 85, 440–452. doi: 10.1037/ Social Psychology, 69, 797–811. doi: 10.1037/
0022-3514.85.3.440 0022-3514.69.5.797
Schmader, T., Johns, M., & Forbes, C. (2008). An Von Hippel, C., Kalokerinos, E. K., & Henry, J. D.
integrated process model of stereotype threat (2013). Stereotype threat among older
effects on performance. Psychological Review, employees: Relationship with job attitudes and
115(2), 336–356. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X. turnover intentions. Psychology and Aging, 28,
115.2.336 17–27. doi: 10.1037/a0029825

View publication stats

You might also like