You are on page 1of 13

This article was downloaded by: [The University of Manchester Library]

On: 31 October 2014, At: 07:14


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Roeper Review
Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/uror20

Social and Self-Perceptions of Adolescents Identified as


Gifted, Learning Disabled, and Twice-Exceptional
Carolyn Barber & Conrad T. Mueller
Published online: 30 Mar 2011.

To cite this article: Carolyn Barber & Conrad T. Mueller (2011) Social and Self-Perceptions of Adolescents Identified as Gifted,
Learning Disabled, and Twice-Exceptional, Roeper Review, 33:2, 109-120, DOI: 10.1080/02783193.2011.554158

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02783193.2011.554158

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the
Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and
are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and
should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for
any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever
or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of
the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
Roeper Review, 33:109–120, 2011
Copyright © The Roeper Institute
ISSN: 0278-3193 print / 1940-865X online
DOI: 10.1080/02783193.2011.554158

DUAL EXCEPTIONALITY

Social and Self-Perceptions of Adolescents


Identified as Gifted, Learning Disabled,
and Twice-Exceptional
Downloaded by [The University of Manchester Library] at 07:15 31 October 2014

Carolyn Barber and Conrad T. Mueller

The purpose of this study is to examine the social and self-perceptions of twice-exceptional
students, those students who meet criteria for being identified as both gifted and learning
disabled. In particular, we focus on how twice-exceptional students are similar to, or dif-
ferent from, students with only a learning disability or who are only identified as gifted.
Using data collected from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health, we identi-
fied a group of 90 twice-exceptional adolescents as well as three matched comparison groups.
Overall, twice-exceptional adolescents had less positive perceptions of maternal relationships
and self-concept than did gifted or nonidentified adolescents. Further, perceptions of mater-
nal relationships mediated and moderated group differences in self-concept. Implications for
adults working with twice-exceptional adolescents are discussed.
Keywords: adolescents, gifted, learning disability, parent relationships, peer relationships,
propensity score matching, school belonging, self-concept, social perceptions, twice-
exceptional

Twice-exceptional students are students who simultaneously found a potential for social difficulties (Kavale & Forness,
meet the definition for giftedness and for a learning diffi- 1996), and research on gifted students suggests that they may
culty. Though much research (including the current analysis) view themselves at a social disadvantage (Cross, Coleman, &
focuses on students simultaneously identified as gifted and Stewart, 1993), does this mean that twice-exceptional stu-
as having a learning disability, other twice-exceptional stu- dents are likely to feel doubly stigmatized? Or, do they
dents may exhibit other learning difficulties such as learning more closely resemble one group than another? These ques-
disorders, attention difficulties, or other learning differences. tions are especially important to consider among adolescent
Within the gifted population, it is estimated that up to 20% of students, given the increasing importance of social rela-
students may also meet the criteria for twice-exceptionality tionships on development during this time (e.g., Erikson,
(Ziemann, 2009). 1968).
To date, most of the literature on these students has In this analysis, we compare the self-perceptions and
focused on their identification and on subsequent adapta- social perceptions of four groups of students: students who
tion of curriculum and of school environments in order to meet criteria for a learning disability, students labeled as
maximize their potential to achieve despite their differences gifted, students who meet neither of these criteria, and stu-
in learning. However, there is also a social component to dents who meet both. Those students who meet both—who
twice-exceptionality that should be considered as well. If simultaneously meet the criteria for identification as gifted
research on students with a learning disability (LD) has and LD—are referred to as twice-exceptional. In particu-
lar, we focused on their perceptions of social relationships
in the school and home contexts. Limited research directly
Received 9 February 2009; accepted 6 January 2010.
Address correspondence to Carolyn Barber, Division of Counseling and
addresses the social experiences of students identified as
Educational Psychology, School of Education, University of Missouri— twice-exceptional. However, we developed hypotheses by
Kansas City, Suite 223 Education Building, 5100 Rockhill Road, Kansas analyzing literature on the social aspects of gifted and
City, MO 64110. E-mail: barberce@umkc.edu LD identification separately and by reporting the opinions
110 C. BARBER AND C. T. MUELLER

and speculations raised in discussions of research on other and nonidentified students can be significantly differentiated
aspects of twice-exceptional students’ experiences. according to these constructs.

SOCIAL AND SELF-PERCEPTIONS OF SOCIAL CHARACTERISTICS OF GIFTED


STUDENTS WITH LD STUDENTS

Students identified as LD are at risk for experiencing dif- Although research consistently finds that students with LD
ficulties in areas of life beyond academics. These include are at risk for social disadvantages when compared to other
but are not limited to, a potential for difficulty in relating students, theory and research on the social experiences of
to others socially. Much research has been done on this academically gifted students are more mixed. In some ways
area of the lives of students with LD in the past 15 years, giftedness, like a learning disability, has been viewed as
consistently finding evidence of greater difficulty in social a social disadvantage. Several foundational researchers felt
relationships in comparison to nonidentified peers. A meta- that the asynchronous development demonstrated by students
Downloaded by [The University of Manchester Library] at 07:15 31 October 2014

analysis conducted by Kavale and Forness (1996) showed identified as gifted, in which cognitive capacities develop
that students with LD were an average of two thirds of a stan- before other social and emotional capacities, put these stu-
dard deviation below their nonidentified peers on self-ratings dents at an increased risk of social alienation (Silverman,
of social skills, leaving them at about the 25th percentile. 2002). Although development is thought to be the most asyn-
More specifically, they reported that 7 out of 10 students with chronous during childhood, adolescents identified as gifted
LD self-report low social skills. More recent studies corrobo- report similar alienation from nonidentified peers. For exam-
rate these findings. Students with LD also experienced higher ple, interviews of adolescents identified as gifted conducted
levels of isolation (Hogan, McLellan, & Bauman, 2000; by Hertzog (2003) and Adams-Byers, Whitsell, and Moon
Stanovich, Jordan, & Perot, 1998), increased peer pressure, (2004) revealed that these students felt “different” from their
interpersonal difficulties, and loneliness (Hogan et al., 2000) nonidentified peers, regardless of whether they were enrolled
compared to nonidentified students. in academic programs specifically for the gifted. At the same
Kavale and Forness’s (1996) meta-analysis could shed time, Hertzog and Adams-Byers and colleagues reported that
light on why students with LD perceive themselves as less context may influence students’ perceptions of their rela-
socially skilled. A high percentage (over 80%) of students tionships with peers, because specialized academic programs
with LD reported deficits in nonverbal communication skills encourage contact with other students who think like them.
and limitations to social problem-solving skills. If such Students who do feel different from their nonidentified
deficits do indeed exist, it could explain why peers and teach- peers may develop coping strategies in order to feel as if they
ers report students with LD to have lower social skills, to fit in with their peers. Though many adapt positive strategies,
be less cooperative, and to be more rejected than nonidenti- such as using talents to help others, others develop cop-
fied students (Kavale & Forness; McDougal, DeWit, King, ing strategies that result in more negative social outcomes
Miller, & Killip, 2004). However, these perceptions that stu- (Swiatek, 1995). According to the Stigma of Giftedness
dents with LD hold do not appear to have an adverse impact paradigm (Cross et al., 1993), students who feel that their
on parental relationships as reported by parents (Haager & giftedness puts them at a social disadvantage will avoid
Vaughn, 1995). opportunities in which they feel that they will have to demon-
In addition to exploring mechanisms that may explain less strate their talent. In this paradigm, the perception of feeling
positive social perceptions among students with LD, Kavale different on the part of the gifted student is more central in
and Forness’s (1996) meta-analysis sheds light on a pos- driving behavior than the actual opinions of friends (Reis &
sible correlate of such difficulties: lower self-perceptions. Renzulli, 2004; Rimm, 2002). Therefore, one could hypoth-
Seventy to 80% of students with LD also report less social esize that students identified as gifted may report lower
competence, poor self-concept, and a lack of self-esteem. self-perceptions of their ability to relate to others in general,
Overall, prior research has suggested that students with even if their peers rate their interactions highly.
LD perceive a lack of ability to pick up on nonverbal cues Gifted students’ perceptions of relationships with adults
and problem-solve in social situations and as a result do may differ from the perceptions that they have of relation-
not participate in classroom activities to the same extent as ships with same-age peers. For example, students who are
their nonidentified peers. This appears to lower acceptance identified as gifted are more likely than nonidentified peers
and increase rejection by peers, leading to LD students feel- to seek out social support from older individuals, includ-
ing lonely and isolated. This could manifest itself further in ing older students and adults, with whom they feel that they
lower self-esteem and self-concept. Though this clearly is have more in common (Rimm, 2002). This may contribute to
not the case for every student with a LD, and differences feeling a sense of belonging to their school. Less is known
can be found when comparing students in different forms of about gifted students’ perceptions of relationships with par-
inclusion classrooms, the fact remains that students with LD ents; however, Olszewski-Kubilius (2008) reported that these
PERCEPTIONS OF TWICE-EXCEPTIONAL STUDENTS 111

relationships tend to be secure and are much of the time quite SOCIAL EXPERIENCES OF
harmonious. TWICE-EXCEPTIONAL STUDENTS
Some special tensions may arise in families of adoles- (IDENTIFIED AS GIFTED AND LD)
cents identified as gifted: articles written for parents of gifted
children have acknowledged that the propensity of these stu- Though the potential for social difficulties is mentioned in
dents to ask pointed, “probing” questions of their parents the literature on twice-exceptional students, there is a dearth
may strain relationships (Griffin, 2001). However, Griffin of empirical literature on the social characteristics and needs
then encouraged parents to allow their gifted adolescents an of these students. Because research has shown that students
environment in which they can try on adult roles and identi- with LD do show social skills deficits, and because some
ties and take on adult responsibilities in a safe manner. The believe that students identified as gifted face social con-
ability of parents to successfully create this environment for cerns as well, this group could be viewed as having a double
their children may have a bearing on how students perceive stigma attached to them. In fact, Coleman (2001) reported
the parent-child relationship. that students identified as twice-exceptional do report feel-
Research has also examined the association between ings of frustration due to pressure to perform (because of
Downloaded by [The University of Manchester Library] at 07:15 31 October 2014

social perceptions and self-perceptions among gifted stu- the gifted label) but not always having the ability (due to
dents. Though Kavale and Forness (1996) linked lower their LD status). This frustration could potentially lead to
social perceptions among students identified as LD with difficulties with social relationships both at home and in
lower self-perceptions, this connection is less established the academic setting due to the student feeling that fam-
for gifted students. For example, Hoogeveen, van Hell, ily, teachers, and peers having high expectations (due to the
and Verhoeven’s (2009) study of secondary-school students gifted label) that he or she is not able to live up to (due to
in The Netherlands found that participation in accelerated the LD). Vespi and Yewchuk (1992), in a phenomenologi-
programs had opposite effects on gifted students’ social cal study of four twice-exceptional 11-year-old boys, showed
perceptions and self-perceptions. Students participating in early support for this idea, finding that twice-exceptional
accelerated programs had higher global self-concepts, but students showed social skill difficulties and self-reported
lower social self-concepts, than did students who were not that they do not feel that they fit in with peers. Neihart
accelerated during the first 2 years of the program. (2008) spoke to this discrepancy as well, stating that twice-
Other studies that focused on multidimensional (as exceptional students typically do not show evidence of
opposed to global) self-concepts found that social percep- giftedness such as high levels of academic performance and
tions related to some areas of self-concept but not others. social skills but may more often display disruptive class-
In Swiatek’s (2001) study of the association between social room behaviors and an overall resistance to school and
coping and self-concept, for example, she found that vari- schoolwork.
ous social coping mechanisms (which, as described earlier, Part of this high incidence of disruptive behaviors may
are developed in line with students’ perceptions of fitting be due to the special difficulty that twice-exceptional stu-
in) related differently to various dimensions of self-concept. dents have finding “true peers” in the classroom context.
Using humor to downplay giftedness, for example, was Nielsen (2002) elucidated this point, saying that twice-
related to a strong feeling of peer acceptance but low global exceptional students have difficulty identifying with a peer
self-worth. Taken together, these findings suggest that stu- group due to difficulty fitting in with gifted, LD, or the
dents identified as gifted may not rely on the acceptance of general-education populations. In short, though gifted stu-
others as much when constructing their self-concepts. dents may have an easier time adapting socially by choosing
In summary, giftedness has the potential to be either a to associate with others who share their talents and interests,
social advantage or a social disadvantage. Although students twice-exceptional students may have a harder time finding
identified as gifted may be hard on themselves when ana- such an appropriate peer group. Neilson supported this asser-
lyzing their social situations, their sensitivity may in fact tion by referring to the limited empirical research on this
result in more positive peer relationships when compared topic, much of which focuses on younger students (e.g.,
to other students. These positive peer relationships are espe- Waldron, Saphire, & Rosenblum, 1987). More recent sup-
cially likely among students who cope with their giftedness port is also found in a qualitative study of twice-exceptional
in positive ways, such as helping out their peers and get- students by Trail (2008), which reported that these stu-
ting involved with others who share similar interests. Further, dents see the need for interactions with students with similar
their perceptions of the social context of their school may abilities to their own as necessary for positive peer relation-
be influenced by the presence of teachers who care about ships. This study also found that positive social support from
their students and who foster their talents. Parent relation- teachers and parents is essential for positive socioemotional
ships may prove more challenging, although research has development.
generally found positive family relationships among aca- This behavior could have additional implications for
demically gifted individuals. However, the extent to which twice-exceptional students’ perceptions of themselves as
these social perceptions relate to self-perceptions is less well as how they relate to others. Dole’s (2000) discussion
established.
112 C. BARBER AND C. T. MUELLER

of risk and resiliency factors for twice-exceptional students overview of the research on student populations identified
stated that these students possess socioemotional charac- as LD or gifted suggests that twice-exceptional students
teristics including “poor self-concept, poor self-efficacy, might be at an especially severe risk for compromised social
hypersensitivity, emotional lability, and high levels of frus- relationships with others.
tration, anxiety, and self-criticism” (p. 92). Although much When taking into consideration the Stigma of Giftedness
of the research cited by Dole focuses on students’ experi- paradigm (Cross et al., 1993) along with the social concerns
ences in the school, there is reason to learn more about their documented among students with LD, one could conclude
experiences at home and how they might relate to poten- that twice-exceptional students experience a dual disadvan-
tial risks associated with twice-exceptionality. Publications tage in social relationships in the academic setting. This
geared toward parents of gifted children such as Gifted Child could appear in twice-exceptional students as a limited abil-
Today discuss the importance of studying the social needs of ity to process and respond to social information coupled with
twice-exceptional children (Hishinuma, 1993). a heightened awareness of being different. The frustration
that twice-exceptional students feel from not being able to
live up to expectations from teachers and peers, not to men-
Downloaded by [The University of Manchester Library] at 07:15 31 October 2014

RELATION BETWEEN SOCIAL EXPERIENCES tion themselves, would compound these social issues. The
AND SELF-PERCEPTIONS question, then, becomes whether the relationships between
these social and self-perceptions mirror those of unidentified
When considering the relationship between social and self- students or whether students in this group exhibit a unique
perceptions, it becomes apparent that the two influence one relationship between the two.
another. Looking at these literatures together suggests two
potential ways in which social and self-perceptions may
relate to one another differently for populations of students RESEARCH QUESTIONS
who are identified as gifted, LD, both, or neither. On one
hand, observed group differences in social perceptions may Taking into account these perspectives, the purpose of this
be somehow related to differences in self-perceptions, such study is to begin to explore differences in the social and
that accounting for group differences in one explains group self-perceptions of twice-exceptional students to those of
differences in the other. Prior research on students with students identified as gifted, learning disabled, and noniden-
LD, for example, has suggested that this group experiences tified students. In particular, the following research questions
many social frustrations either directly or indirectly due to guide this study:
their disability, which shapes their self-perception (Kavale &
Forness, 1996). This process could be viewed as iterative 1. How do twice-exceptional students’ social perceptions
in nature, with each new experience solidifying the exist- differ from those of their peers identified as gifted,
ing schema that the student possesses, thus influencing future learning disabled, and nonidentified? Do perceptions
social interactions and perceptions. On the other hand, there of in-school relations and relationships with parents
may be reasons to think that self-perceptions and social per- differ between these groups?
ceptions relate differently to one another in different groups 2. How do twice-exceptional students differ from their
of students. Kavale and Forness further stated that students peers in their self-perceptions?
with LD may be at special risk of falling into this pattern 3. What are the similarities and differences between
because of a lack of higher-order cognitive skills that could twice-exceptional students and students in the other
buffer the effects of lower social perceptions. three groups in terms of the relationship between self-
If we are to assume that students identified as gifted are perceptions and perceptions of parent and in-school
more likely to have these skills (Reis & Renzulli, 2004), relationships? Do social perceptions mediate the rela-
this could allow for gifted students to maintain more posi- tionship between group membership and self-concept?
tive social and self-perceptions when compared to students Or is self-concept moderated by social perceptions?
with LD. Students labeled as gifted appear able to use the
advanced cognitive skills they possess to “fit in” socially
with other students through numerous coping strategies that
DATA AND METHODS
students with LD do not possess (Swiatek, 1995). Students
labeled as gifted also could maintain a higher self-concept
Data Source
regardless of social perceptions. Therefore, giftedness may
in fact be a protective factor against low social perceptions Data for the analysis come from the first wave of the National
that a student may have. It is unclear, however, how this Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (AddHealth;
relationship might look for students who meet both criteria Harris, 2008). The general purpose of AddHealth was to
simultaneously. Although there is limited previous research examine how contexts of development in the adolescent
on the social experiences of twice-exceptional students, an years (such as school, community, and family) influenced
PERCEPTIONS OF TWICE-EXCEPTIONAL STUDENTS 113

physical and social well-being. As such, AddHealth is a use- twice-exceptional), and a control group of students who
ful source of data for our current study of social well-being were identified as neither gifted nor LD. In order to create
among students with gifted and LD identifications. these groups, we used assessments of intellectual functioning
Researchers associated with the AddHealth study drew conducted by AddHealth as well as parents’ and students’
the nationally representative sample of 12,105 students in the responses to interview items.
AddHealth study from schools in 80 communities. Although To assess giftedness, we used the AddHealth version
three waves of data are currently available through the of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary test (AHPVT). As was
AddHealth project, this study focuses only on data from described by Halpern, Joyner, Udry, and Suchindran (2000),
the first wave, when participants were enrolled in Grades the AHPVT is a shortened version of the full Peabody Picture
7 through 12. In Wave I, students were involved in both Vocabulary Test-Revised (PPVT-R; Dunn, 1981; cited in
in-school questionnaires and in-home interviews. The in- Halpern et al., 2000). A subset of AddHealth participants
school questionnaire, a self-administered instrument for- took both the AHPVT and the PPVT, with the two scores
matted for optical scanning, was administered to students correlated at r = .96. In addition, the PPVT itself is corre-
in Grades 7 to 12 from September 1994 through April lated with other measures of IQ such as the Stanford-Binet
Downloaded by [The University of Manchester Library] at 07:15 31 October 2014

1995, over the course of one 45- to 60-minute class period (r = .62) and the Weschler Intelligence Scale for Children
(Harris, 2008). Computer-based in-home interviews were (r = .64). However, the PPVT and AHPVT are especially
conducted between April and December 1995. These inter- suited for use in field surveys due to their ease of admin-
views took between 1 and 2 hours to complete, depending istration. The AHPVT has been used in other studies of
on the respondent’s age and experience. During the in- gifted adolescents conducted using the AddHealth survey
home interview, interviewers read less sensitive questions (e.g., Mueller, 2009).
and recorded responses, and respondents listened to more In the current study, respondents in the AddHealth study
sensitive questions through earphones and recorded answers were identified as gifted if their AHPVT score was at or
themselves. In this analysis, questions about adolescents’ above 120. This means that, for the purposes of this study,
perceptions of school relationships were asked by the inter- respondents were considered gifted if their intellectual func-
viewers, and questions about relationships with parents and tioning (as determined by AHPVT score) was 1.3 standard
self-perceptions were considered sensitive and therefore self- deviations from the mean of 100 and that 10% of students
recorded. Researchers also collected additional information in the nationally representative AddHealth sample could be
from respondents’ mothers or female heads of household considered gifted. First, use of a 90th percentile cutoff for
(or fathers/male heads if there was no female head) and defining giftedness is supported by gifted education theorists
from the administrators of the respondents’ schools in order (e.g., Gagné, 2004). Second, a criticism of IQ test scores as
to supplement information gained during the surveys and an assessment of giftedness in twice-exceptional students is
interviews. that a score of 130 (as was used in Mueller, 2009) is inappro-
Although Wave I of the AddHealth data was collected priately high (Lovett & Lewandowski, 2006; Nielsen, 2002).
over 10 years ago, it is still relevant to understanding the By using a more liberal (but still theoretically supported)
social experiences of twice-exceptional youth today. The cutoff for giftedness, we hope to identify a larger group of
breadth and depth of information collected in large-scale sur- twice-exceptional students. Finally, and more pragmatically,
vey programs such as AddHealth are time and labor intensive our study requires us to further divide our group of students
to collect, and data from one survey program can provide identified as gifted into groups of twice-exceptional students
researchers with years’ worth of opportunities for secondary and groups of gifted students. By using a somewhat more
data analysis. This is no less true when conducting analysis liberal cutoff for giftedness, we hope that enough twice-
in the field of gifted education. For example, Mueller (2009) exceptional students will be included in this sample to make
recently conducted a study on this same wave of AddHealth for meaningful between-group analyses.
data that shed new light on how contextual factors can pro- At the same time, respondents were considered to have
tect against depression in gifted adolescents. Further, data a learning disability if their parents reported that they had
collection for the AddHealth study is still ongoing, allowing a learning disability. As part of one of the supplementary
for further research on how these experiences as adolescents interviews, parents of respondents were asked to respond yes
impact behavior as young adults. or no to whether their child had a “specific learning dis-
ability, such as attention, dyslexia, or some other reading,
spelling, writing, or math disability” (Carolina Population
Measures Center, 1999, p. 7). This item has also been used by other
researchers interested in capturing students’ learning diffi-
Independent Variables: Creating Groups of Gifted,
culties (e.g., Crosnoe, Riegle-Crumb, & Muller, 2007). The
LD, and Twice-Exceptional Students
broadness of this single item made it preferable over other
In this study, we compared four groups of students: stu- items in the AddHealth interview used to capture learning
dents identified as gifted only, LD only, gifted and LD (i.e., difficulties. For example, Blum, Kelly, and Ireland (2001)
114 C. BARBER AND C. T. MUELLER

considered current involvement in special education when have fallen under scrutiny (Lovett & Lewandowski; Neihart,
identifying a group of learning disabled students from the 2008). In addition, the PPVT (which serves as the basis
AddHealth sample. This criterion presents two important for the AHPVT), like many other normed-referenced tests
problems when using it in a sample of gifted students: first, of intelligence, has been critiqued in its ability to assess
schools may not feel that a gifted student warrants special giftedness due to the limited information that such tests
education for learning difficulties due to a disability (i.e., provide about students scoring at either extreme of its dis-
the masking hypothesis); and second, when answering this tribution (Wright, 1983). Further, though we feel that the
item parents may consider gifted education programming as parent report of a learning disability is the best measure
a form of special education. for our purposes available in the AddHealth data, it is far
Respondents who met only the AHPVT criterion for gift- from perfect. There is no opportunity to cross-validate par-
edness were considered in the gifted group in this study, and ent reports and no specification in the question administered
respondents with AHPVT scores within one standard devi- to parents that the learning difficulty diagnosis came from a
ation of the average (i.e., between 85 and 115) but whose trained professional. In short, there is no best way to identify
parents reported that they had learning disabilities were con- twice-exceptional students; however, we hope that our meth-
Downloaded by [The University of Manchester Library] at 07:15 31 October 2014

sidered in the LD category. Combining these two criteria for ods help to shed some initial light on the social experiences
gifted and LD identification, we formed a group of twice- of this group of students compared to students with a single
exceptional students. Altogether, 90 students had AHPVT exceptionality or no exceptionality at all.
scores at or above 120 and were identified by parents as
having a learning disability. Finally, we also created a fourth Propensity Score Matching
group of nonidentified students to serve as a control group.
Because the twice-exceptional group was considerably
These students had average AHPVT scores and were not
smaller than any other group formed, we subsampled the
identified as learning disabled by their parents.
gifted, LD, and nonidentified groups so that each group had
an equal number of respondents in it using propensity score
Strengths and Weaknesses of Definition Approach matching. The propensity score equation was calculated
using gender, race/ethnicity, age, and maternal education
Our methods for identifying groups of twice-exceptional
as predictors of the log-likelihood that a respondent met
students align with several of the recommendations outlined
our criteria for being identified as twice-exceptional. Exact
in Lovett and Lewandowski’s (2006) review of assessment
matches on the propensity score variable were obtained
practices in this group. First, as Brody and Mills (1997) sug-
whenever possible; if not, a single nearest neighbor was ran-
gested, our identification criteria capture both evidence of
domly selected. Overall, our analytic sample consisted of
outstanding talent (in the form of high PPVT score) as well
360 students, with 90 students in each group. Demographic
as a discrepancy in aptitude and achievement (in the form
characteristics of the four groups before and after matching
of an identified LD). Second, McCoach, Kehle, Bray, and
are provided in Table 1.
Siegle (2001) suggested that twice-exceptional students be
required to meet the same criteria for LD as other students
Dependent Variables: Social and Self-Perceptions
until more is known about how LD manifests itself specifi-
cally in twice-exceptional populations. Although we do not We also used data from the respondent at-home interviews
know the exact procedures used to identify the LD in each to create measures of adolescents’ perceptions of their rela-
case, we are still using the same criterion (parent report of tionships with others. To accomplish this, we identified three
LD) for both twice-exceptional and students with LD. Third, batteries of interview questions that related most closely to the
although several scholars advocate for the use of multiple goals of our study. These batteries were (a) sense of belonging
criteria to best identify groups of twice-exceptional students at school, (b) perceptions of relationships with parents, and
(e.g., Neihart, 2002), the use of a single set of identification (c) self-concept. For each battery of questions, we conducted
criteria is a strength in this particular exploratory analysis. principal components analysis to assess the unidimension-
Research by Barber and Torney-Purta (2008) has suggested ality of the items presented. In cases where more than one
that social relationships differ between groups of gifted stu- component was identified in the battery of items, we used
dents who meet test-based criteria of giftedness and those varimax rotation to identify the items that loaded onto each
who meet teacher-nomination criteria. By choosing a single of the two dimensions. Once unidimensional groups of items
set of criteria, we are controlling for possible variations in were identified, we computed Cronbach’s alphas to assess
social experiences due to differences in identification. the reliability of each group of items.
At the same time, there are noteworthy limitations to con- In looking first at the battery of items, we identified one
sider as well. Several approaches discussed by Lovett and scale that captured students’ senses of belonging at school
Lewandowski (2006) advocate the use of subscales in anal- (α = .765). Factor loadings of items on this scale ranged
ysis of scatters or profiles. Such information is not available from .581 to .798. This scale, which encompasses percep-
in the AddHealth database. However, even these methods tions of relationships with both peers and teachers in the
PERCEPTIONS OF TWICE-EXCEPTIONAL STUDENTS 115

TABLE 1
Descriptive Statistics Before and After Propensity Score Matching

Before matching After matching

Learning Learning
Twice-exceptional Gifted disability Control Twice-exceptional Gifted disability Control
(n = 90) (n = 1,688) (n = 1,341) (n = 10,076) (n = 90) (n = 90) (n = 90) (n = 90)

Race/ethnicitya
% Latino/a 7 9 11 14 7 8 6 7
% White 78 70 67 56 78 81 81 78
% Other 15 21 22 30 15 11 13 15
Age 15.23 (1.50) 15.22 (1.58) 15.60 (1.69) 15.57 (1.69) 15.23 (1.50) 15.16 (1.54) 15.29 (1.49) 15.24 (1.49)
Mother’s educationb 4.30 (1.12) 4.29 (1.22) 3.64 (1.13) 3.75 (1.19) 4.30 (1.12) 4.32 (1.17) 4.30 (1.15) 4.28 (1.12)
% Female 27 50 33 50 27 28 26 27
Downloaded by [The University of Manchester Library] at 07:15 31 October 2014

Note. Standard deviations appear in parentheses for continuous variables.


a Categorical race/ethnicity variables also included African American, Native American, multiracial, and “other” as categories. Due to small cell sizes,
these are collapsed into the other category when reported here. We did, however, consider African American ethnicity when computing propensity scores.
b Mother’s education is measured on a 7-point scale: 1, didn’t go to school; 2, eighth grade or less; 3, some high school; 4, high school degree or equivalent;
5, some postsecondary education; 6, college graduate; 7, graduate or professional training.

school context, consisted of respondents’ agreement on a This comparison of means allowed us to address Research
5-point scale that they: (a) feel close to people at school, Question 1 by examining whether twice-exceptional students’
(b) feel part of school, (c) feel happy at school, (d) [feel that] perceptions of their relationships at school and of their rela-
teachers treat students fairly, and (e) feel safe at school. tionships with mothers were significantly more or less positive
When we analyzed the second battery of items relat- than those of their gifted, LD, or nonidentified peers. It also
ing to respondents’ perceptions of their relationships with allowed us to address Research Question 2 by examining
parents, we found that attitudes lay along two dimensions: whether their self-conceptions of feeling loved and accepted
perceptions of relationships with mother and perceptions of were significantly different from other students. To correct
relationships with father. For the purposes of this analysis, for inflated Type I error due to multiple comparisons, we used
we focus only on those items that capture perceptions of Holm-corrected alpha levels for individual ANOVAs. Next,
maternal relationships (α = .898). Factor loadings for items a series of three planned contrasts per analysis allowed us
on this scale ranged from .801 to .894. Respondents were to compare average scores for the twice-exceptional group
asked to indicate how strongly they agreed on a 5-point scale against each other group individually.
with the following statements about their relationship with This method was preferred over post hoc analyses because
their mother: it allowed us to focus on testing the pairwise comparisons
most pertinent to this study (i.e., the comparisons of the
● [The respondent is] close to mom twice-exceptional group to other groups) without simultane-
● Mom is warm/loving ously testing additional unnecessary comparisons.
● [The respondent has an] overall good relationship with After we calculated group averages, we conducted hier-
mom archical regression analyses to determine whether parent
● [The respondent has] good communication with mom relationships or sense of belonging mediated or moderated
the association between being identified as gifted/LD and
The final battery of items yielded one scale that captured
self-concept. In addition, these analyses determined whether
students’ self-perceptions, including the extent to which they
maternal relationships similarly influenced group differences
see themselves as loved, accepted, and proud (α = .813).
in sense of belonging at school. This allowed us to examine
Factor loadings for the items on this scale ranged from .730
whether the association of self-perceptions to perceptions of
to .809. Respondents were asked to indicate how strongly
relationships with others was different for twice-exceptional
they agreed on a 5-point scale that they (a) have lots of good
students, thus addressing Research Question 3.
qualities, (b) have a lot to be proud of, (c) like themselves
as they are, (d) do everything just right, (e) feel socially
acceptable, and (f) feel loved and wanted.
RESULTS
Statistical Analysis
Differences in Social and Self-Perceptions
Using these scales, we conducted two types of statistical anal-
ysis. First, each group’s average scores on each of the three Average respondent perceptions of sense of belong-
scales were compared using analyses of variance (ANOVAs). ing at school, quality of relationship with mother, and
116 C. BARBER AND C. T. MUELLER

TABLE 2 TABLE 3
Average Social and Self-Perceptions Among Twice-Exceptional, Association Between Perception of Relationship With Mother
Gifted, LD, and Control Group Students and Self-Concept Among Twice-Exceptional, Gifted, LD,
and Control Group Students
Twice-
exceptional Gifted LD Control Step 1 2 3

Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Constant 3.91 (.07) 2.16 (.15) 2.52 (.24)
Gifted 0.20∗ (.09) 0.06 (.08) −1.01 (.42)
Sense of school 3.53 (0.75) 3.74 (0.83) 3.56 (0.77) 3.80 (0.77) LD 0.03 (.09) −0.03 (.08) −0.67 (.38)
belonging Control 0.27∗∗ (.10) 0.13 (.08) 0.18 (.42)
Mother 4.01 (0.94) 4.34 (0.70) 4.14 (0.79) 4.33 (0.72) Mother relationship 0.44∗∗ (.04) 0.35∗∗ (.06)
relationship Gifted × Mother 0.25∗ (.10)
Self-concept 3.90 (0.58) 4.11 (0.64) 3.95 (0.69) 4.16 (0.55) LD × Mother 0.16 (.09)
Control × Mother −0.01 (.10)
Adjusted R2 .32 .33
self-perceptions are listed in Table 2. In comparing these Change to R2 .02∗∗ .30∗∗ .02∗
Downloaded by [The University of Manchester Library] at 07:15 31 October 2014

means, we are able to answer Research Questions 1 and 2.


Note. Standard errors are in parentheses. Reference group = twice-
exceptional.
Turning first to Research Question 1, which focuses on ∗ p < .05. ∗∗ p < .01.

differences in twice-exceptional students’ social perceptions,


we see a statistically significant difference among the groups gifted and control groups reporting significantly higher
in the perceptions of their relationships with their mothers, levels of self-concept compared to the twice-exceptional
F(3, 346) = 3.64, p = .013. More specifically, using Holm’s group. However, when controlling for perceptions of mater-
step-down procedure to correct alpha for multiple contrasts nal relationships, the group differences in self-concept disap-
within an ANOVA, one of our three planned contrasts was pear. More specifically, a statistically significant difference
statistically significant. Students in the twice-exceptional between the twice-exceptional group and the control group
group on average had less positive perceptions of their rela- at step 1 is no longer significant in step 2 with the inclusion
tionship with their mothers than did students in the gifted of the maternal relationship variable. As well, the signifi-
group, t(160.65) = −2.71, p = .007, d = 0.41. The analysis cant difference between the twice-exceptional group and the
of variance to assess group differences in sense of belonging gifted group at step 1 is lost in step 2.
in school was not statistically significant. In adding the interaction terms in step 3, however, statis-
Turning next to Research Question 2, there were sta- tically significant interactions (and main effects) are seen in
tistically significant differences among the four groups in comparing the twice-exceptional group to the gifted group.
their self-concepts, F(3, 356) = 3.68, p = .012. Analysis of There is a statistically significant difference between the
planned contrasts revealed that the twice-exceptional group average self-concept of students in the twice-exceptional and
had significantly lower self-concepts than did the control gifted groups, such that twice-exceptional students on aver-
group, t(356) = −3.03, p = .003, d = 0.45. age have a higher self-concept than do students identified
as gifted (after controlling for maternal support). However,
there is also a statistically significant and positive interaction
Differences in the Relation Between Social and between being in the gifted group and maternal relationships.
Self-Perceptions As illustrated in Figure 1, this indicates that maternal support
Research Question 3 asked whether the relation between is more strongly associated with a positive self-concept for
self and social perceptions differed for twice-exceptional
5
students when compared to other students. To answer this
question, we ran three hierarchical regression analyses (one
for each social perception outcome) in which group mem-
Mother Relationship

bership (gifted, LD, or control, with twice-exceptional as 4

the reference group) was entered in the first block, a social 2X


perception measure was entered in the second block, and Gifted
LD
self-perception by group membership interaction terms was 3
Control
entered in the third block as predictors.
Of these analyses, several yielded statistically significant
results for our interaction terms; however, only one model 2
significantly improved the amount of variance explained: 2 3 4 5
Self-Concept
group membership and maternal relationship predicting self-
concept (see Table 3). The first block of the regression FIGURE 1 Illustration of interaction between perception of relationship
model confirmed results of the ANOVA, with both the with mother and gifted/LD status as it predicts self-concept.
PERCEPTIONS OF TWICE-EXCEPTIONAL STUDENTS 117

students identified as gifted than it is for twice-exceptional environments (particularly as related to support from their
students. The few gifted students with negative perceptions mothers) as supportive.
of maternal relationships have extremely low self-concept; Although this finding speaks to differences in percep-
however, increased maternal support is more strongly asso- tions of the family context, it nonetheless has important
ciated with increased positive self-concept in this group. implications for the education and well-being of this group
In other words, this analysis indicates that maternal rela- of students. A relatively lower level of home support for
tionships have a stronger, more protective association with twice-exceptional students could have especially dire conse-
self-concept for students identified as gifted than for twice- quences, considering the extent to which current recommen-
exceptional students. dations for meeting the needs of twice-exceptional students
A similar interaction between control group status and involve parental support (Dole, 2000; King, 2005; Trail,
social perceptions is not statistically significant. This sug- 2008). This analysis suggests that twice-exceptional students
gests that the quality of students’ relationships with their do not perceive the same level of support from their mothers
mothers mediates, rather than moderates, the differences as do other students, although they may be in the most need
in self-concept observed between students in the control of such support. It is also important, therefore, that teach-
Downloaded by [The University of Manchester Library] at 07:15 31 October 2014

and Twice-Exceptional groups. Further statistical analysis ers and counselors consider the role that they could possibly
supports this assertion, Sobel test statistic = 2.81, p = .005. play in providing an additional source of support for those
In other words, differences in average levels of self-concept students who may not perceive it at home. Teachers and
observed between the twice-exceptional and control groups counselors may especially be suited for this if they are famil-
can be accounted for by differences in social perceptions. iar with the unique needs of twice-exceptional students in
ways that parents might not be.
In a sense, however, it is still somewhat surprising to find
DISCUSSION that significant differences exist between twice-exceptional
students in terms of maternal relationships but not in the per-
Overall, this analysis has provided preliminary evidence to ception of in-school relationships. Given that the LD and
suggest that twice-exceptional students do differ in some gifted labels are conferred upon by the school, one might
ways from other students in their perceptions of social inter- expect to see the most dramatic differences for perceptions
actions in their school and family contexts. As expected, of relationships in school contexts. At the same time, this
twice-exceptional students’ social and self-perceptions most finding sheds light on ways in which exceptionalities have
closely resembled perceptions of students with LD, with the potential to influence social well-being outside of the
one important exception: the positive association of strong academic context. Based on this, additional work should be
maternal relationships to positive self-concept found among conducted specifically on twice-exceptional students’ social
students with LD is not shared to the same extent by experiences outside of the school, because social difficulties
twice-exceptional students. do not appear to be limited to this context alone. Further,
In particular, students who are identified both as gifted it is important to remember that the items in this measure
and as having an LD have significantly more negative per- of school belonging focus primarily on relationships among
ceptions of their relationships with their mothers than do students in a school, with only one item devoted to whether
other students. Reasoning for this pattern can be inferred teachers generally treat students fairly. As such, it provides
from the existing twice-exceptional literature, specifically us more information on relationships among students than it
the frustrations experienced overall by twice-exceptional does on teacher–student relationships. Given that the find-
students and the tendency of others (including parents) to ings here suggest the importance of support from close
view them as not living up to their potential (Coleman, adults such as parents, more research should be conducted
2001). However, additional support or information is lim- focusing on the propensity of teachers to support students
ited, due to differences in what most previous work has specifically.
focused on. Instead of looking at within-group differences twice-exceptional students also demonstrate a less posi-
in relationships with parents, much of the very limited tive self-concept than do gifted and nonidentified students
available research (e.g., Waldron et al., 1987) has focused (but not students identified as LD), providing additional
on comparing the social relationships of twice-exceptional empirical evidence to back assertions made by theorists and
students in the family context to those in the school con- educators (e.g., King, 2005). Thus, in this regard, twice-
text. Like Waldron et al.’s study, this analysis found that exceptional students seem to have more in common with
the average level of maternal support is higher than the their LD peers than with their gifted peers. This analysis
average sense of belonging in school experienced by twice- further reveals one possible factor in the low self-concept
exceptional students. However, other students are similar of twice-exceptional students: low feelings of support from
to twice-exceptional students in their lower average sense home, specifically from mothers. The results of this analysis
of belonging. Where twice-exceptional students differ from did show that the higher levels of self-concept found in the
their peers is in the extent to which they see their home control group compared to twice-exceptional students can
118 C. BARBER AND C. T. MUELLER

be explained by the stronger relationship with their mothers. school system, those who are identified as both, and those
In other words, differences in self-concept between students students whose giftedness and LD serve to cancel each other
identified as neither gifted nor having a learning disability out, resulting in no diagnosis either way (i.e., the masking
and those identified as having both can be explained by con- hypothesis of twice-exceptionality). Students in this sample
sidering different levels of perceived quality in relationships could fall into the first or the third categories but not into the
with mothers. This provides further evidence for a close second or fourth. Although students in all of these categories
link between perceived support from parents and positive can be considered twice-exceptional, the resulting curricular
self-perceptions. adjustments or labeling effects may mean that students in
However, the analysis also suggests that, though at-home each of these groups have vastly different experiences both
support from mothers may be especially helpful in fostering in school and with their parents.
positive self-concept for students with a single exceptional- Given the social risk factors associated with LD, one may
ity (i.e., giftedness), this benefit is not extended to students speculate that it is the twice-exceptional student whose LD is
who are twice-exceptional. There is a stronger associa- severe enough to warrant recognition that is at greatest risk
tion between maternal support and sense of belonging at for social difficulties. Therefore, although the extent to which
Downloaded by [The University of Manchester Library] at 07:15 31 October 2014

school among gifted students than among twice-exceptional a masking effect exists has been called into debate (McCoach
students. Some of this may be due to extremely low percep- et al., 2001), it is important to acknowledge that these find-
tions of support from a subset of gifted youth (discussed ings cannot be generalized to twice-exceptional students who
in Olszewski-Kubilius, 2008); however, most have much have not received an LD diagnosis. Further research should
more positive perceptions of their mothers. More generally, use alternate definitions of twice-exceptionality to deter-
researchers interested in the issues of students either iden- mine whether similar relationships still exist even among
tified as gifted (Swiatek, 2001) or LD (Kavale & Forness, students whose giftedness may have masked their LD.
1996) have acknowledged the extent to which feelings of For example, although the use of ability/achievement gaps
support are important for the development of positive self- has recently been contested in the larger LD community
perceptions. What this current research suggests is that (and in some cases by those interested in twice-exceptional
parents (especially mothers) may not be as effective in students as well), they may, in some cases, still prove
preventing risks associated with students who exhibit mul- useful for the identification of gifted students for whom
tiple exceptionalities. Perhaps we see this because they even average achievement is still below what is expected
do not know how to prevent such risks. Thus, the major given their levels of aptitude (e.g., Assouline, Nicpon, &
implication of this finding is the same as those for previ- Whiteman, 2007).
ous findings: parents need to become more knowledgeable Finally, it is also important to acknowledge the age of the
of the issues that their twice-exceptional students face so AddHealth data when addressing limitations of this study.
that they can more effectively serve as social support and Although this rich data set provides new information on the
advocates for their child. Turning again to implications for experiences of twice-exceptional students, it is likely that
teachers and counselors, the support that they can provide the experiences of these adolescents have changed since
(even when parents cannot) may additionally serve to bol- the time these data were collected due to shifts in educa-
ster positive self-perceptions of twice-exceptional students, tional policies. Further research should seek to collect new
especially if they are versed in the unique needs, risks, and data to see whether the preliminary patterns explored here
potentials that this group of students possesses. However, still hold.
more research is needed to determine whether this is indeed
the case.
CONCLUSION
Limitations
In summary, this analysis preliminarily illustrates the social
In considering the generalizability of these findings, it is difficulties that twice-exceptional students face, especially
especially important to remember the method this study used when compared to their gifted and their nonidentified peers.
to identify twice-exceptional students. Students were con- Although previous research has focused primarily on what
sidered twice-exceptional in this study if they had AHPVT this means for the school environment, these challenges are
scores over 120 and if their parents reported that they were by no means limited to the school environment. In fact, some
diagnosed with a learning disability. As Brody and Mills of the most dramatic findings were seen when examining
(1997) acknowledged, however, not all twice-exceptional twice-exceptional students’ relationships with their moth-
students may be diagnosed with a learning disability. More ers. Lower self-concept among twice-exceptional students
specifically, Brody and Mills identified four categories of appears to be linked to lower perceived support from moth-
twice-exceptional students: those whose LD is pronounced ers; additionally, the protective factor of strong maternal sup-
enough to warrant an LD diagnosis, those who are academ- port demonstrated by students with a single exceptionality is
ically successful enough to be identified as gifted by the not shared by those with dual exceptionalities. It is the job of
PERCEPTIONS OF TWICE-EXCEPTIONAL STUDENTS 119

educators to take these social risks into consideration when Dole, S. (2000). The implications of the risk and resilience literature for
developing optimal programming for twice-exceptional stu- gifted students with learning disabilities. Roeper Review, 23, 91–96.
dents, just as it is the job of researchers to continue this Erikson, E. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis. New York, NY: Norton.
Gagné, F. (2004). Transforming gifts into talents: The DMGT as a develop-
line of study further with more diverse populations of mental theory. High Ability Studies, 15, 119–147.
students. Griffin, G. L. (2001). Parenting gifted adolescents. Gifted Child Today,
24(2), 54–58.
Haager, D., & Vaughn, S. (1995). Parent, teacher, peer, and self-reports of
AUTHOR NOTE the social competence of students with learning disabilities. Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 28, 205–215.
Halpern, C. T., Joyner, K., Udry, J. R., & Suchindran, C. (2000). Smart
This research uses data from AddHealth, a program project teens don’t have sex (or kiss much either). Journal of Adolescent Health,
designed by J. Richard Udry, Peter S. Bearman, and Kathleen 26, 213–225.
Mullan Harris and funded by a grant P01-HD31921 from the Harris, K. M. (2008). The National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent
Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health Health, Waves I & II, 1994–1996; Wave III, 2001–2002 [Machine-
readable data file and documentation]. Chapel Hill, NC: University of
and Human Development, with cooperative funding from 17
Downloaded by [The University of Manchester Library] at 07:15 31 October 2014

North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Carolina Population Center.


other agencies. Special acknowledgment is due Ronald R. Hertzog, N. B. (2003). Impact of gifted programs from the students’
Rindfuss and Barbara Entwisle for assistance in the origi- perspectives. Gifted Child Quarterly, 47, 131–143.
nal design. Persons interested in obtaining data files from Hishinuma, E. S. (1993). Counseling gifted/at risk and gifted/dyslexic
AddHealth should contact AddHealth, Carolina Population youngsters. Gifted Child Today, 16(1), 30–33.
Hogan, A., McLellan, L., & Bauman, A. (2000). Health promotion needs
Center, 123 W. Franklin Street, Chapel Hill, NC 27516-2524 of young people with disabilities: A population study. Disability and
(addhealth@unc.edu). No direct support was received from Rehabilitation, 22, 129–135.
grant P01-HD31921 for this analysis. Hoogeveen, L., van Hell, J. G., & Verhoeven, L. (2009). Self-concept and
The authors also thank the UMKC AddHealth research social status of accelerated and nonaccelerated students in the first 2
team for their feedback in forming the current analysis years of secondary school in The Netherlands. Gifted Child Quarterly, 53,
50–67.
and the University of Maryland Department of Human Kavale, K. A., & Forness, S. R. (1996). Social skill deficits and learn-
Development Graduate Student Research Grants program ing disabilities: A meta-analysis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29,
for providing funding to purchase pilot public-use data for 226–237.
this analysis from the Sociometrics Corporation. A previ- King, E. W. (2005). Addressing the social and emotional needs of twice-
ous version of this article was presented at the American exceptional students. Teaching Exceptional Children, 38, 16–20.
Lovett, B. J., & Lewandowski, L. J. (2006). Gifted students with learning
Psychological Association Annual Conference in Boston, disabilities: Who are they? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39, 515–527.
Massachusetts. McCoach, D. B., Kehle, T. J., Bray, M. A., & Siegle, D. (2001). Best prac-
tices in the identification of gifted students with learning disabilities.
Psychology in the Schools, 38, 405–411.
REFERENCES McDougal, J., Dewit, D. J., King, G., Miller, L. T., & Killip, S. (2004).
High school-aged youths’ attitudes toward their peers with disabilities:
Adams-Byers, J., Whitsell, S. S., & Moon, S. M. (2004). Gifted students’ The role of school and student interpersonal factors. International Journal
perceptions of the academic and social/emotional effects of homogenous of Disability, Development and Education, 51, 287–313.
and heterogeneous grouping. Gifted Child Quarterly, 48, 7–20. Mueller, C. E. (2009). Protective factors as barriers to depression in gifted
Assouline, S., Nicpon, M. F., & Whiteman, C. (2007, November). Gifted and nongifted adolescents. Gifted Child Quarterly, 53, 3–14.
students with written language disabilties. Paper presented at the Neihart, M. (2008). Identifying and providing services to twice-exceptional
54th National Association for Gifted Children Annual Conference, children. In S. I. Pfeiffer (Ed.), Handbook of giftedness in children:
Minneapolis, MN. Psychoeducational theory, research, and best practices (pp. 115–138).
Barber, C., & Torney-Purta, J. (2008). The relation of high-achieving ado- New York, NY: Springer.
lescents’ social perceptions and motivation to teachers’ nominations for Nielsen, M. (2002). Gifted students with learning disabilities:
advanced programs. Journal of Advanced Academics, 19, 412–433. Recommendations for identification and programming. Exceptionality,
Blum, R. D., Kelly, A., & Ireland, M. (2001). Health-risk behaviors and pro- 10, 93–111.
tective factors among adolescents with mobility impairments and learning Olsewski-Kubilius, P. (2008). The role of the family in talent devel-
and emotional disabilities. Journal of Adolescent Health, 28, 481–490. opment. In S. I. Pfeiffer (Ed.), Handbook of giftedness in children:
Brody, L. E., & Mills, C. J. (1997). Gifted children with learning disabilities: Psychoeducational theory, research, and best practices (pp. 53–70). New
A review of the issues. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 30, 282–296. York, NY: Springer.
Carolina Population Center. (1999). National Longitudinal Study of Reis, S. M., & Renzulli, J. S. (2004). Current research on the social and
Adolescent Health: Parental questionnaire codebook. Chapel Hill, NC: emotional development of gifted and talented students: Good news and
Author. future possibilities. Psychology in the Schools, 41(1), 119–129.
Coleman, M. R. (2001). Surviving or thriving? Gifted Child Today, 24(3), Rimm, S. (2002). Peer pressures and social acceptance of gifted students.
56–64. In M. Neihart, S. M. Reis, N. M. Robinson, & S. M. Moon (Eds.), The
Crosnoe, R., Riegle-Crumb, C., & Mueller, C. (2007). Gender, self- social and emotional development of gifted children: What do we know?
perception, and academic problems in high school. Social Problems, 54, (pp. 13–18). Waco, TX: Prufrock Press.
118–138. Silverman, L. K. (2002). Asynchronous development. In M. Neihart, S. M.
Cross, T. L., Coleman, L. J., & Stewart, R. A. (1993). The school-based Reis, N. M. Robinson, & S. M. Moon (Eds.), The social and emotional
social cognition of gifted adolescents: An exploration of the stigma of development of gifted children: What do we know? (pp. 31–37). Waco,
giftedness paradigm. Roeper Review, 16, 37–40. TX: Prufrock Press.
120 C. BARBER AND C. T. MUELLER

Stanovich, P. J., Jordan, A., & Perot, J. (1998). Relative differences in Vespi, L, & Yewchuk, C. (1992). A phenomenological study of the
academic self-concept and peer acceptance among students in inclusive social/emotional characteristics of gifted learning disabled children.
classrooms. Remedial and Special Education, 19, 120–126. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 16, 55–72.
Swiatek, M. A. (1995). An empirical investigation of social coping Waldron, K. A., Saphire, D. G., & Rosenblum, S. A. (1987). Learning dis-
strategies used by gifted adolescents. Gifted Child Quarterly, 39, abilities and giftedness: Identification based on self-concept, behavior,
154–161. and academic patterns. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 20, 422–427.
Swiatek, M. A. (2001). Social coping among gifted high-school students Wright, D. (1983). Effectiveness of the PPVT-R for screening gifted
and its relationship to self-concept. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, students. Psychology in the Schools, 20(1), 25–26.
30, 19–39. Ziemann, S. (2009). Merely bright—or brilliant? Finding out whether
Trail, B. (2008, October/November). Twice-exceptional learners: What they your child is gifted is the first step in getting her the challenge she
needed in order to thrive. Paper presented at the 55th annual National craves. Retrieved from http://articles.chicagotribune.com/1999-09-19/
Association for Gifted Children Annual Conference, Tampa, FL. features/9909190194_1_parents-of-gifted-kids-giftedness-bright-child

AUTHOR BIOS

Carolyn Barber, PhD, is an assistant professor in the Division of Counseling and Educational Psychology at the
Downloaded by [The University of Manchester Library] at 07:15 31 October 2014

University of Missouri, Kansas City. Her research interests focus on the social aspects of secondary schooling,
particularly as related to civic development and the education of gifted adolescents. She has special interest in
the use of large-scale national and international databases to address research questions in these fields. E-mail:
barberce@umkc.edu

Conrad T. Mueller is a doctoral student in the Counseling Psychology program at the University of Missouri,
Kansas City. His interests focus on the use of quantitative research methodologies to understand social and moral
development in diverse contexts. E-mail: ctmwc2@mail.umkc.edu

You might also like