You are on page 1of 9

Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain © 2016 American Psychological Association

2016, Vol. 26, No. 3, 211–219 0275-3987/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pmu0000144

Age-Related Changes Affecting the Learning of Music Performance Skills


for Older Adults
James L. Reifinger Jr.
Southern Illinois University Carbondale

This paper surveys research relevant to the learning of music performance skills in mid to late adulthood
with the aim of identifying how the process of teaching and learning these skills may differ at this age
as compared with youth. First, research related to the acquisition of musical expertise is presented in
order to establish what is known about the development of music performance skills. Next, theories and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

research findings related to characteristics of adult learning and musical skills are presented. Finally,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

basic research studies are presented that have examined age-related differences in learning and perform-
ing fine motor tasks such as those used when performing music. These studies indicate that learning
occurs at all ages, although at slower rates with older adults due to changes in cognitive and psychomotor
functioning. The findings are considered in relation to skills used when performing music and suggest
that music learning and performance may be affected by age-related differences in music reading
responses, practice effects, motor memory development, and aural capacities. Research findings also
prompt suggestions for adaptations with older adults that may improve teaching and learning.

Keywords: music performance, older adult, learning, skills, age

In everyday situations people tend to be surprised when adults Pioneers in the adult music education movement, Ernst and
talk about their instrumental music lessons. Why might this be the Emmons (1992) founded their instrumental music program in 1991
case? Learning to play an instrument is often viewed as an en- for adults 50 years of age and older. Called the New Horizons
deavor that commences in childhood and continues through earlier Band, this program was designed to provide an avenue for older
adulthood for those who persevere, but musical development can adults who wanted to be actively involved in music making.
occur at any time during the life span. Furthermore, once acquired, Presently over 200 New Horizons organizations are listed on the
musical skills can be maintained and improved during old age. New Horizons International Music Association website and many
Opportunities to learn or improve music performance skills are of these groups now open their membership to adults of any age
pursued by adults of all ages and ability levels, from beginners to (“New Horizons Groups,” 2016). Although it may not be possible
advanced performers. In a survey of 275 audience members at- to develop the technical skills of an élite performer later in life,
tending a musical performance, Bowles (1991) found that 67% older adults can learn to perform music at a level that results in a
stated they would consider taking a music course, and the highest tremendous personal pleasure and a sense of accomplishment
level of interest was for performance courses. Requested levels of (Ernst & Emmons, 1992). A study by Gembris (as cited in Gem-
study indicated that 72% were at the beginner level, 22% at the bris, 2012) found that an amateur musician’s self-reported personal
intermediate level, and 6% at the advanced level. Certain life best performance on an instrument can occur anytime throughout
events such as retirement, divorce, or the death of a spouse or one’s life. Among the senior citizens (Mage ⫽ 71 years) inter-
relative have been cited as the catalyst for a decision to initiate or viewed in his study, 30% indicated that they currently reached
resume active music study, based on self-reports of older amateur 90% to 100% of their previous best instrumental performance
pianists ages 53 to 87 years (Taylor, 2011). Piano was the most level.
commonly played instrument in a small sample of older adults Though the pursuit of musical learning is common at all age
(Hanna-Pladdy & MacKay, 2011). Frequently cited reasons for levels, much of the research that examines the developmental
taking piano lessons in adulthood in Cooper’s (2001) sample were process in music learning is generally limited to the period of
development of personal skills (55%) and personal pleasure (45%), infancy through early adulthood (Prickett, 2003). Likewise, music
with 78% reporting that they enjoyed practicing. teacher preparation programs focus almost exclusively on how to
teach music to children and young adults. Coffman (2002) pointed
out that the body of research on music education with adults is
only beginning to emerge. Psychomusicology directed attention to
This article was published Online First May 16, 2016. the topic in 2002 with an entire special volume on the Psychology
JAMES L. REIFINGER, Jr. is an assistant professor of music at South-
of Music and Aging (Volume 18). While advanced aging is some-
ern Illinois University Carbondale. His research interests include music
literacy, assessment, and music cognition.
times associated with overt changes that can hinder learning, such
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to James L. as diminished acuity in hearing and vision, and increased ortho-
Reifinger, Jr., School of Music, Southern Illinois University Carbondale, pedic challenges, music performance skills include covert pro-
1000 South Normal Avenue, Carbondale, IL 62901. E-mail: reifinger@ cesses that may also change with age and affect learning. With
alumni.iu.edu practice, improvements in performance skills result from specific

211
212 REIFINGER

adaptations that are physiological, psychomotor, and cognitive in between each aural stimulus and the tap), but early trained musi-
nature (Lehmann & Ericsson, 1997). Age-related changes in per- cians were significantly more accurate in a measured of intertap
ceptual, cognitive, and motor functioning that are known to occur deviation (the deviation of time interval between each pair of taps
may have an effect on the process of learning music performance from the actual time interval of each pair of notes in the stimulus),
skills in adulthood, although, as Halpern and Bartlett (2010) indicating that early trained musicians had enhanced auditory
pointed out, little research has examined these changes in relation rhythm synchronization abilities as compared with late-trained
to music learning. musicians. Although it might be auspicious to beginning music
Musical skills and progress in acquiring them manifests with study at an early age, the attainment of an élite performance level
great variability across individuals at any age, making comparisons is not the goal for most adults who are striving to develop and
among age levels difficult. Furthermore, very few studies have improve their music performance skills.
examined age-related differences in skill development and perfor- Ericsson, Krampe, and Tesch-Römer (1993) suggested that the
mance with actual music performance tasks. Studies examining ideal preconditions for initiating the musical study are perceived
age-related differences in the cognitive and psychomotor aspects talent and enjoyment of the activities, not age. They concluded that
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

of the learning process, however, are providing a growing body of different types of musical engagement vary in terms of their effect
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

experimental evidence that may explain differences in extent and on skill improvement in music performance. The type of musical
rate of music skill acquisition across the life span. engagement found to have a strong relationship with performance
This paper reviews research examining facets of cognitive and skill improvement is identified as “deliberate practice”—activities
psychomotor performance relevant to music learning in mid to late that address specific goals and strategies. A series of articles offer
adulthood to elucidate how the process of teaching and learning a theoretical framework of deliberate practice and describe specific
music performance skills may differ at this age as compared with ways to gain the maximum benefit from practice time (Ericsson &
youth. Because music performance is a multifaceted task involving Lehmann, 1996; Krampe & Ericsson, 1996; Lehmann & Ericsson,
the integration of many different skills, the scope of this review 1997). With deliberate practice, specific practice goals are formu-
aims to include the range of topics that might affect learning of lated and students continually monitor achievement toward those
music performance skills, precluding an exhaustive review of each goals during practice. Students must be fully attentive to their
topic. The review includes representative studies on each topic that playing in order to notice areas of potential improvement and to
employed musical tasks or tasks that most closely resemble the avoid errors. Inadequate strategies will limit progress, even with
process of learning and performing music, as very little research the most motivated individuals and extended practice. Sloboda,
has been conducted with actual musical instruments. Additionally, Davidson, Howe, and Moore (1996) found that high achievers
the studies cited are limited to individuals in good to excellent tended to maintain more consistent practice schedules and tended
health—relevant to those who want to pursue active music making to focus on technical skills in the morning. Ericsson et al. (1993)
opportunities in midlife and later adulthood. An introductory sec- stressed that it is more important to identify and engage in activ-
tion provides a brief overview of music skill development and ities that are most likely to result in desired achievements rather
theories related to learning differences across age. A concluding than trying to maximize the number of hours of practice. Higher
section summarizes findings for adult learners and uses the cited performance levels have been shown to correlate with the amount
research literature to provide recommendations for tailoring music of deliberate practice, even when age differences are controlled
training to address the unique capacities of learners who are in mid (Ericsson et al., 1993); furthermore, performance levels attained
to late adulthood. with years of accumulated experience alone are much lower than
the levels attained by individuals who adhere to regimens of
deliberate practice (Ericsson & Lehmann, 1996).
Developing Music Performance Skill
Ericsson et al. (1993) listed several important prerequisites for
Many studies examining the development of music skills have effective practice, including access to training facilities, appropri-
focused on achievement of an élite performance level. The élite ate training exercises, and proper sequencing of instruction. Ac-
performers in a large sample interviewed by Manturzewska (1990) cess to appropriate instruction is also an important factor contrib-
all began musical study no later than the age of nine years, uting to performance gains and achievement of higher levels of
suggesting that the early developmental stages of childhood may performance in shorter amounts of time (Lehmann & Ericsson,
be a critical period to initiate musical study in order to achieve an 1998). In order to sustain the regular activity of practice for months
élite skill level. Rather than referring to a critical period with and years, duration should be limited to 2 to 4 hours a day due to
rigidly defined age boundaries, recent studies are suggesting the the considerable effort that is required. A practice schedule that is
possibility of a sensitive period for music training in childhood, a too lengthy or intense can lead to exhaustion, burn out, and even
period during which musical experiences may have a greater injury. Studies of training have suggested no benefit from practice
influence on development (Bailey & Penhune, 2010). Bailey and durations exceeding 4 hours per day and reduced benefits from
Penhune (2010), for example, found some differences in rhythmic practice exceeding 2 hours (Ericsson et al., 1993). Most impor-
abilities of young adult musicians (Mage ⫽ 26 years) who were tantly, the data from Ericsson’s et al. (1993) study showed that
early trained (initiated music training on or before age 7) versus improvement occurred whether training began at an early age or a
late-trained (started after age 7), but were statistically equal in total later age. Although individuals who started early in life and main-
years of musical experience and performance on cognitive tests. tain intense practice schedules had a higher level of performance
When tapping along with aurally presented rhythm phrases, the throughout development, improvement did occur for those who
two groups did not differ in percent correct (tapped within half of started later in life, and was more dependent on the level of
the time interval between notes) or asynchrony (length of deviation deliberate practice rather than the starting age of training.
AGE-RELATED CHANGES AFFECTING LEARNING 213

More recently, some researchers argued that the focus on delib- Addressing age-related differences in skill learning and perfor-
erate practice has been too limiting, emphasizing environmental mance, Baltes (1997) pointed out that biology-based plasticity that
influences without taking into account other factors that may affect accompanies the process of physical maturation plays a large role
skill development (Ruthsatz, Detterman, Griscom, & Cirullo, in cognitive development in the early years. If biology-based
2008). Detterman and Ruthsatz (1999) theorized that attained plasticity decreases with maturity, opportunities to interact with
levels of achievement are influenced by a combination of three culture become more essential to maintain growth and reach higher
factors: general intelligence, domain specific skills, and practice. levels of cognitive development. Baltes (1997) offered the selec-
Meinz and Hambrick (2010) found that, while deliberate practice tive optimization with compensation model (SOC), which he devel-
accounted for 45% of the variance in sight-reading performance of oped along with colleagues as a general framework for developmental
pianists, working memory capacity accounted for an additional 7% theory. SOC explains how individuals adapt to age-related changes in
of the variance beyond that accounted for by deliberate practice. In cognitive and motor processes that affect performance by utilizing a
a meta-analysis of eight previously published studies examining combination of three types of strategies. Selection involves choices of
deliberate practice in music, Hambrick et al. (2014) reported that goals and outcomes that are in alignment with current capacity, such
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

the mean correlation of deliberate practice with music performance as selecting appropriate repertoire or choosing what to focus on when
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

achievement was r ⫽ .52 and concluded that future study should practicing. Optimization involves increasing efficacy and functioning
consider the influence of other variables that may account for to compensate for changes, including strategies such as practicing
individual differences in music performance achievement. In ad- more or taking lessons to gain new ideas. Compensation involves
dition to considering deliberate practice, they suggested that future making adjustments in some manner to counteract changes or losses,
research continue to examine factors such as starting age, general such as playing a piece at a different tempo, using different fingerings
intelligence, working memory, heredity, and personality traits such or bowings, or taking breaks more frequently when practicing.
as persistence and passion. Platz, Kopiez, Lehmann, and Wolf Another framework by which age-related changes in learning
(2014) argued that other variables have explained only a smaller can be considered is the theory of fluid and crystallized intelli-
proportion of the variance in achievement. Their meta-analysis of gence (Horn & Cattell, 1967). Fluid intelligence includes abilities
13 studies found a correlation between deliberate practice and used in mental manipulation such as reasoning, abstract thinking,
music achievement of r ⫽ .61, and they suggested that this and concept formation. Crystallized intelligence refers to accumu-
correlation may be a theoretical lower bound of the true effect of lated knowledge, the body of information acquired through prior
deliberate practice because estimates of practice durations can interaction of fluid ability with the environment. Because fluid
include suboptimal practice activities in addition to deliberate intelligence involves mental mechanics, it would be subject to
practice. Though researchers continue to try to identify additional decline with age. Conversely, because crystallized intelligence is
relevant factors and attributes and their relative importance in the gained through experience, it would tend to increase with age.
development and improvement in performance skills, deliberate Horn and Cattell (1967) found that, on average, fluid intelligence
was higher for younger versus older adults, and crystalized intel-
practice has consistently been identified as the single, most im-
ligence was higher for older versus younger adults.
portant factor, by far, influencing improvement in performance
With increases in domain-specific crystalized intelligence, indi-
skills.
viduals acquire more extensive cognitive resources that may be
used to facilitate higher levels of performance. Seminal research
Characteristics of Adult Learners by Salthouse (1984) with typists between the ages of 20 and 72
found that older expert typists compensated for age-related motor
Although many facets of the educational process are consistent slowing (as evidenced by performance on other motor tasks) by
regardless of age, certain characteristics of adult learners may have using the cognitive strategy of reading ahead when typing, thereby
an effect on learning. Older students have a wealth of life experi- using advanced cues to maintain fast typing speeds. Because of the
ence that provides a basis for evaluating musical performances in benefits achieved with the use of more effective cognitive strate-
terms of tuning, precision, expression, and so forth. One of the gies developed through practice, Ericsson et al. (1993) concluded
functions that music teachers working with older beginners may that individual differences in simple reaction time (RT) in general
need to assume, according to Bruhn (2002), is to mediate these may be unrelated to the attainment of expert performance. They
students’ personal views of their performance so that they can pointed out that untrained adults can acquire new cognitive skills
enjoy the music despite an awareness of their meager playing. It consisting of qualitatively different processes that allow the limits
would seem that adults could be more self-directed as learners and imposed by speed and memory capacities to be overcome. Simi-
could more readily use metacognitive or self-regulating behaviors larly, Palmer and Meyer (2000) pointed out that conceptual knowl-
than young students. However, based on interviews with 29 piano edge about pitch relationships is distinct and disassociated from
students, ages 21 to 77 years, Chen (1996) concluded that music the movement knowledge acquired when learning to perform the
instruction with adult learners at beginning or intermediate levels pitches. They found that as skill level increases, knowledge about
should encourage the students’ self-directed learning. Interview conceptual dimensions rather than knowledge about movement
responses indicated that many students needed to develop better dimensions had a greater effect on facilitating performance on
thinking and problem-solving skills; 40% stated that they could not transfer tasks. Acquiring and using information about conceptual
decide whether they were playing well until they received their relationships in music may be particularly beneficial in improving
teacher’s positive feedback, and 72% stated that the only solution older adults’ performance.
they could think of when practicing a problematic spot was to In later adulthood, learning may be affected by a greater sus-
practice it over and over. ceptibility to distraction by irrelevant stimuli. Mentally screening
214 REIFINGER

out irrelevant contextual stimulation involves inhibitory processes. (dotted quarter-eighth-quarter - quarter-eighth-dotted quarter). Al-
Hasher and Zacks (1988) found that inhibitory processes decline though the two age groups performed similarly with the simple
with age, leading to heightened distractibility and proneness to rhythm, older adults performed significantly more trials with errors
interference from irrelevant processing during memory encoding with the complex rhythm. Older adults also experienced signifi-
and retrieval. Additionally, age-related deficits in the performance cantly greater decreases in accuracy than younger adults at increas-
of relevant motor and cognitive skills may have an effect on the ingly faster tempos. A second experiment compared performance
rate of acquisition or improvement of performance skills for adult with rhythms composed of subdivisions that were either symmet-
learners. Krampe (1997) listed components of skills related to rical (eight-quarter-dotted quarter) or asymmetrical (eighth-dotted
musical performance that are susceptible to age-related changes. quarter-quarter). Although no age differences were found in pro-
Those relevant across different types of instruments include simple ducing the symmetrical rhythm, older adults produced the asym-
motor speed, bimanual coordination, timing capacity, sequencing, metrical rhythm with significantly greater inaccuracy than younger
and dynamic variation in expressive performance. adults. The older adults showed a significantly greater tendency to
distort rhythms in the direction of simpler ratios, while preserving
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

the boundaries of beats and subdivisions.


Beat, Tempo, and Rhythm
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

With musicians, Krampe, Engbert, and Kliegl (2001) assessed


Maintaining a steady beat can occur without attentional or rhythm performance skill of younger (Mage ⫽ 24 years) and older
working memory processes, therefore it does not undergo age- (Mage ⫽ 71 years) accomplished pianists (deemed to be accom-
related decline. For healthy nonmusicians, ability to maintain a plished by fluently playing Bach’s Prelude No. 1 in C major). The
steady beat along with an external stimulus remains stable through first of two rhythms, performed as an ostinato, was a four-beat
old age (for a review, see Krampe, Doumas, Lavrysen, & Rapp, syncopated rhythm (left hand taps all beats and right hand taps
2010). Turgeon and Wing (2012) found that beat tapping motor beats one and four and all the half beats between). No differences
accuracy begins declining only above the age of 85 years, and by age were found in rhythmic accuracy or maintenance of tempo.
self-selected tapping tempo slows with age. Tempo preference also The second rhythm was a four-beat polyrhythm (left hand taps the
slows; a study by Jennen and Gembris (as cited in Gembris, 2012) beat and right hand taps 5 against those 4). Rhythmic accuracy and
in which individuals were asked to choose among recordings of the maintenance of tempo differed significantly with age. Rhythmic
same piece at different tempos found a curvilinear relationship mistakes were made on 3% of the trials by the younger group and
between age and performance tempo, with individuals above 60 19% of the trials by the older group; tempo deviations were made
years of age preferring slower tempos. on 15% of the trials by the younger group and 24% of the trials by
When keeping a beat in the absence of a pacing signal or at slow the older group. Greater differences between the two groups in
tempos in which beats may need to be mentally subdivided, tempo maintenance occurred at slower tempos. The researchers
attention and working memory are required to a greater extent pointed out that the deviations in performance were examined at
(Krampe et al., 2010). Subdivisions need not be explicitly counted, the level of accuracy necessary to consider psychological theories
but still require attention and working memory. The cost of addi- of motor processing differences. They clarified, however, that in a
tional demands on working memory is more pronounced in older real-life context, the majority of the less than perfect trials would
age when attentional and working memory resources may already correspond to acceptable musical performances.
be diminished (Blanchet, Belleville, & Peretz, 2006). Krampe et al.
(2010) found that beat tapping ability differed significantly be-
Reading Music While Performing
tween the younger (Mage ⫽ 24 years) and older (Mage ⫽ 67 years)
adults under conditions where additional cognitive tasks were Basic research studies in psychology have examined age differ-
performed (verbalizing a visually presented digit or mentally ences in learning and executing fine motor skills used in tasks
tracking the number of odd to even switches of visually presented similar to playing a musical instrument. To the extent that music is
digits). When simultaneously performing the additional memory not memorized, music performance includes notation reading that
task, participants in both age groups significantly shortened the requires perception of and response to visual stimuli. Slower
length of time between beats at a slow tempo (MM ⫽ 30), but only response times to stimuli by older adults versus younger adults is
older adults significantly shortened the length of time between perhaps the most frequently replicated finding in experimental
beats at a fast tempo (MM ⫽ 110). While the competing memory psychology of aging (Kausler, 1994). From early to late adulthood,
task demands impaired performance for both age groups at a slow the correlation of age and RT is positive and is even stronger with
tempo, older adults also experienced performance costs at the fast choice-response tasks—those in which the individual must make a
tempo. This suggests that while younger students may not be choice about which movement to use based on the stimuli pre-
affected by certain distractions or additional cognitive demands if sented. A recent study found that choice RT increases significantly
a music performance tasks is easy, older adults’ performance may by age, but only by 1.9 to 2.8 ms/year between the ages of 18 and
be affected under the same circumstances. 65; when comparing the oldest and youngest participant groups,
Investigating age differences in rhythm performance skills with the mean latency increased by 120 ms (Woods, Wyma, Yund,
nonmusicians, Krampe, Mayr, and Kliegl (2005) examined Herron, & Reed, 2015).
younger (Mage ⫽ 19 years) and older (Mage ⫽ 70 years) adults’ Light and Spirduso (1990) examined age differences in choice-
ability to tap a simple and complex rhythm pattern at various response time when performing simple fine motor movements of
tempos. The simple rhythm consisted of three beats with two the fingers, similar to movements used when playing an instru-
different durations (quarter-eighth-eighth-quarter), and the com- ment. Four types of finger movement responses were timed: right
plex rhythm consisted of six beats with three different durations index flexion, right pinch, bilateral index flexion, and bilateral
AGE-RELATED CHANGES AFFECTING LEARNING 215

pinch. Participants represented three age groups: young (Mage ⫽ time decreased significantly more than the other groups, thereby
22 years), middle-aged (Mage ⫽ 43 years), and older adults making older adults’ response time notably similar to that of the
(Mage ⫽ 63 years). Reaction time to randomly ordered stimuli middle-aged adults. Fraser, Li, and Penhune (2009), discussed
prompting responses was measured in milliseconds. Responses below, found a similar significant benefit from a rest period for
were significantly slower for the bilateral tasks than for the uni- older adults. This may indicate that rest periods are particularly
lateral tasks for all age groups. There was also a significant beneficial in the learning process when working with older adults.
interaction between age and movement type. The RT of the young Future research might investigate length and frequency of rest
adults was significantly quicker than the other two groups on all periods in relationship to specific task demands and age for opti-
tasks except the right index flexion, which was performed with mal learning with older adults.
similar speed by middle-aged adults, but not the older adults. For
the middle-aged adults, RT slowed significantly for right pinching
Motor Memory
movement as compared with the right flexion. For the older adults,
the RT for all four movement tasks differed significantly. As with Speed of responding can be an indication of how well the skill
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

the other groups, the unilateral movements required less RT, is integrated into memory as a motor routine. Performing on an
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

suggesting that bilateral movements are more complex. This con- instrument often involves the execution of motor patterns that are
clusion corroborates music research by Krampe and Ericsson overlearned through practice so as to be highly automatized and
(1996), who found that the maximum tempo of single finger rendered with little or no associated cognitive effort (Palmer,
tapping was the same for younger and older adult amateur pianists, 2006). A widely accepted model of motor learning is the Fitts-
but the maximum tempo of alternating taps was slower for older Posner three-stage theory of motor skill acquisition (Fitts & Pos-
adults. In sum, older adults are slower than younger adults on ner, 1967). In the cognition state the learner has to think about and
choice-response fine motor tasks and slowing as a function of task consciously control the movements. In music, knowledge of what
difficulty is disproportionately greater for older adults. to perform may be represented in symbolic form and must undergo
The locus of motor slowing has been investigated in numerous cognitive transformation into a motor action for responses to be
studies. Researchers have isolated the premotor and the motor produced, requiring considerable attentional capacity. With prac-
components of a movement response by using electromyographic tice the integration stage is attained at which the learner still needs
recordings of muscle-action potentials (see Kausler, 1994 for a to think about the movement, but parts of the movement are
review.) The premotor component is the time that occurs between executed more automatically and can be performed with less
the presentation of the stimulus and the production of increased attention. When performing music, the symbols guiding perfor-
muscle firing (rate of nerve impulses). The motor component is the mance may still need to be processed, but some implicit knowl-
time between this increase in action potential and the initiation of edge has been acquired about the familiar order of the stimuli,
the actual movement response. Most of the response latency has which results in faster transformation. Finally, at the automation
been found to occur in the premotor period, the time during which stage movement can be performed with little or no attention and
a cognitive choice of response type must be made. As a result, the movement is fluid, accurate, and seemingly effortless. Chunks
response selection has been suggested as the main locus of dis- of motor responses have been learned and can be produced without
proportionate slowing on choice-response tasks. processing individual stimuli, thus reducing cognitive demands
and facilitating advanced preparation.
The effect of age on learning and the retention of motor se-
Effects of Practice
quences performed on a music keyboard was examined by Fraser
The decision-making process required on choice-response tasks et al. (2009). Participants were not currently practicing a musical
can be partially attenuated with routines in which a pattern of instrument, had less than three years of musical experience, and
motor responses is learned through repeated practice. The effect of represented two age groups: younger (Mage ⫽ 24 years), and older
practice on reducing choice-response RT at different age ranges adults (Mage ⫽ 65 years). The task involved using four fingers on
was investigated by Gaillard, Destrebocqz, Michiels, and Cleere- the right hand to play 10-item sequences on the keyboard in
mans (2009). Participants represented three age groups: young response to visual prompts. Blocks of 14 trials were performed,
(Mage ⫽ 23 years), middle-aged (Mage ⫽ 46 years), and older with one repeated sequence occurring for 10 of the trials inter-
adults (Mage ⫽ 71 years). The task involved viewing four dots spersed with random sequences. Three blocks were completed on
arranged left to right, each representing a designated QWERTY Day 1 and two blocks were completed on Day 2. Sequence
keyboard key. When a small black circle appeared below a dot, the learning was assessed in terms of accuracy (percent correct) and
participant pressed the appropriate key as quickly as possible. The RT (length of time to produce correct key presses following the
same sequence of 96 key presses was practiced for either 15 or 30 onset of the visual stimulus) and was compared between the two
trials, allowing the assessment of speed improvement resulting age groups both within-days (learning) and across-days (retention).
from practice. Additionally, a different sequence of stimuli was Learning was evident by significantly faster speed of responding
used in the second last block, allowing the assessment of slowing when performing the repeated sequence versus the random se-
on a choice-response task with unpredictable stimuli. quences. For younger adults, speed was significantly faster when
Learning was evident by a significant reduction of RT with performing the repeated sequence versus the random sequences for
practice occurring for all age levels. However, response time all three practice blocks on Day 1. For the older adults, perfor-
differed and increased significantly with age. When practice rou- mance speed differed by sequence type on Blocks 2 and 3, but not
tines extended for 30 trial blocks, immediately following a rest on Block 1, indicating that the older adults needed more practice
period within the practice schedule, the older group’s response than the younger adults to learn the repeated sequence. Though
216 REIFINGER

learning was evident for both age groups, both within- and across- of sound significantly interacted with pianists’ aural imagery abil-
days, the younger adults were significantly faster overall in re- ities such that those with high aural ability scores were less
sponding than older adults. affected by the lack of sound during practice than those with low
Verwey, Abrahamse, Ruitenberg, Jiménez, and Kleine (2011) aural ability scores (Highben & Palmer, 2004). This study assessed
examined whether sequence length or segmentation of longer aural ability using an adaptation of the Wing test in which partic-
sequences had an effect on fine-motor learning and if effects ipants indicated whether the 7–10 note melody that was heard
differed by age. Participants were young (Mage ⫽ 22 years) and matched the notation.
middle-aged adults (Mage ⫽ 58 years). The task involved learning Many studies examining age differences in aural memory of
to perform 3- and 6-item key press sequences on a QWERTY music have used discrimination or recognition tasks, but tasks
keyboard using DFG and JKL with the index, middle, and ring consisting of short-term same-different discriminations are be-
fingers of both hands. Visual stimuli on a screen showed six boxes lieved to underestimate the extent of long-term encoding of me-
corresponding to the finger position on the keys with a space in the lodic information (Halpern & Bartlett, 2010). Greater encoding
middle representing the H key. A color change in a box indicated and later recognition of music presented aurally was found to
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

which key to press. One 3-item and one 6-item sequence were each occur for music associated with a pleasant (consonant) verses
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

practiced 24 times in a randomly alternating order for one practice unpleasant (dissonant) affect, and the music affect variable had a
block. Six blocks were completed, with breaks between blocks, so significantly greater effect on recognition memory for older adults
that each sequence was practiced for a total 144 of repetitions. (Mage ⫽ 74 years) verses younger (Mage ⫽ 22 years) or middle-
Additionally, the 6-item sequence was presented as a single unit to aged adults (Mage ⫽ 49 years; Parks & Clancy Dollinger, 2014).
half the participants (the unstructured group), and with a pause in Further research is needed to examine how long-term encoding
the middle to the other half (the prestructured group). Performance and retrieval of auditory images with repetition may affect the
was evaluated in terms of decreased response time and increased learning of music performance skills, and if these processes differ
accuracy with practice. with age. Studies also need to investigate if age differences in
Results indicated that both age groups improved significantly, central auditory processing abilities might affect music learning.
increasing response speed with practice on both the 3-item and the For example, declines in auditory cognition for older adults, even
6-item sequence. The speed increase was significantly greater for those with normal hearing, have been associated with increased
the young than the middle-aged adults. Average improvement for difficulty in understanding speech in presence of competing
each age group was compared after controlling for initial response sounds (for a review see Alain, Zendel, Hutka, & Bidelman, 2014),
speed differences between the two groups. The improvement and such deficits have also been associated with increased thresh-
achieved across practice blocks by the middle-aged adults was olds for detecting mistuning of harmonics (Alain, McDonald,
only 75% of the improvement that the young adults were able to Ostroff, & Schneider, 2001).
achieve with practice on the 3-item sequence and only 62% of the
improvement that the young adults were able to achieve on the
Implications and Conclusions
6-item sequence. However, the two age groups did not differ
significantly in their rate of improvement on the prestructured Although basic studies examining fine motor learning and per-
6-item sequence. The researchers suggested that, to facilitate learn- formance help to explain age-related differences in general capac-
ing, middle-aged adults might be instructed to avoid reading stim- ities that may affect music learning, future studies need to examine
uli as individual notes as soon as possible in the learning process. learning and performance with a greater range of tasks, particularly
They further suggested that older adults may benefit from practic- those that more closely approximate actual musical tasks. Even in
ing movement patterns in short series, preparing and executing basic studies employing tasks performed on a music keyboard, the
these short series as a single integrated unit, and being aware of the end goal may not be the production of music (e.g., Fraser et al.,
individual movements that make up the pattern. 2009). The production of musical sounds is the motivation and
purpose for musical study at all ages and ability levels and may be
one of the most important dimensions compelling learners to
Auditory Memory
produce their best possible motor and cognitive efforts. When
In addition to using visual stimuli and motor memory, auditory learning to play a piece of music, striving to produce, and then
memory can be accessed when playing a musical instrument to immediately hearing, the expected sounds can result in a gratifying
guide the performer and to provide information to facilitate ad- emotional response. Parks and Clancy Dollinger (2014) pointed
vanced preparation. Finney and Palmer (2003) examined the effect out that our knowledge about the relation between emotion and
of the presence or absence of sound while pianists (Mage ⫽ 22 cognition might expand by a better understanding of the underly-
years) learned piano pieces, each with 23 notes. Speed and accu- ing nature of music processing, especially in adulthood and aging.
racy did not differ significantly between sound and no-sound Music performance is a very complex process, integrating facets in
learning conditions, indicating the absence of sound had no effect the cognitive, psychomotor, and affective domains and including
on learning when using notation. However, auditory feedback multimodal sensory input and imagery (aural, visual, tactile, and
during practice does appear to contribute to the formation of motor). The interaction of these various facets when learning and
mental representations that may be used when performing from performing music at various ages should also continue to be
memory. Pianists’ memorized performances were significantly considered. Additionally, the technical skills required when per-
more accurate when sound was present during practice trials than forming vary from one instrument to the next; therefore, an indi-
when motor movements were practiced without sound (Finney & vidual may experience difficulty trying to learn one instrument, but
Palmer, 2003; Highben & Palmer, 2004). The presence or absence then experience success with another instrument.
AGE-RELATED CHANGES AFFECTING LEARNING 217

Based on research findings cited above, the following summary improve significantly following a rest period (Fraser et al.,
and recommendations for praxis with adult learners are offered: 2009; Gaillard et al., 2009).
• Adult learners need teacher feedback and guidance appro- Perhaps the most important finding is that music performance
priate to their skill level similar to that provided to younger skills can be developed and maintained even in later adulthood.
learners (Chen, 1996), but, in addition, teachers may also Musical study is often pursued as a source of recreation and
need to mediate an adult’s critical self-evaluation (Bruhn, enjoyment in the later years of life. Though it is beyond the scope
2002). of this paper, studies have shown that involvement with music can
• To maintain achieved performance levels into later adult- be a factor contributing to healthy aging (Akbaraly et al., 2009;
hood, older adults may mitigate age-related declines in Bugos, Perlstein, McCrae, Brophy, & Bedenbaugh, 2007; Sein-
motor or cognitive abilities by using techniques involving feld, Figueroa, Ortiz-Gil, & Sanchez-Vives, 2013; Verghese et al.,
selection, optimization, and compensation (Baltes, 1997). 2003; Wan & Schlaug, 2010). Teachers and researchers of music
Selection involves choosing appropriate repertoire. Optimi- learning should continue to identify and adapt to the unique needs
zation of efficacy and functioning may be accomplished of adult learners, thereby leading to more successful learning
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

outcomes and continued engagement in satisfying musical expe-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

with strategies such as using conceptual knowledge to


riences.
increase reading and processing speed. Compensation may
involve procedures such as making adjustments in tempos
and bolstering advanced preparation. References
• Older adult learners may experience a decreased ability to Akbaraly, T. N., Portet, F., Fustinoni, S., Dartigues, J. F., Artero, S.,
avoid distractions from irrelevant stimuli due to declines Rouaud, O., . . . Berr, C. (2009). Leisure activities and the risk of
with inhibitory process (Hasher & Zacks, 1988). Changes dementia in the elderly results from the three-city study. Neurology, 73,
in the auditory system with age may also result in decreased 854 – 861. http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181b7849b
efficiency to hear sounds within a context including com- Alain, C., McDonald, K. L., Ostroff, J. M., & Schneider, B. (2001).
Age-related changes in detecting a mistuned harmonic. Journal of the
peting sounds. Additionally, older adults may experience
Acoustical Society of America, 109, 2211–2216. http://dx.doi.org/10
disproportionately greater difficulty than younger adults in .1121/1.1367243
situations requiring divided attention (Blanchet et al., 2006; Alain, C., Zendel, B. R., Hutka, S., & Bidelman, G. M. (2014). Turning
Krampe et al., 2010). Minimizing distractions and limiting down the noise: The benefit of musical training on the aging auditory
the range of focus may be beneficial for older adult learn- brain. Hearing Research, 308, 162–173. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
ers. .heares.2013.06.008
• Beat-keeping ability is not affected by age (Krampe et al., Bailey, J. A., & Penhune, V. B. (2010). Rhythm synchronization perfor-
2010), though older adults may prefer a slower tempo mance and auditory working memory in early- and late-trained musi-
cians. Experimental Brain Research, 204, 91–101. http://dx.doi.org/10
(Jennen & Gembris, as cited in Gembris, 2012). When
.1007/s00221-010-2299-y
performing simple rhythms, performance accuracy is sim- Baltes, P. B. (1997). On the incomplete architecture of human ontogeny.
ilar across ages. When performing complex rhythms, older Selection, optimization, and compensation as foundation of develop-
adults who are less skilled tend to simplify rhythms while mental theory. American Psychologist, 52, 366 –380. http://dx.doi.org/
preserving the metric accents (Krampe et al., 2005). 10.1037/0003-066X.52.4.366
• Motor response time on choice-response tasks slows with Blanchet, S., Belleville, S., & Peretz, I. (2006). Episodic encoding in
age (Kausler, 1994). Reading notation while performing a normal aging: Attentional resources hypothesis extended to musical
material. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition: Section B
sequence of notes may constitute a choice-response to the
Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 13, 490 –502. http://dx.doi.org/
extent that movement sequences are not encoded into motor 10.1080/13825580500428492
memory. Slowing has been attributed to an increase in time Bowles, C. L. (1991). Self-expressed adult music education interests and
needed for selection of the correct movement response music experiences. Journal of Research in Music Education, 39, 191–
(Kausler, 1994). Motor responses become disproportion- 205. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3344719
ately slower as compared with younger adults when tasks Bruhn, H. (2002). Musical development of elderly people. Psychomusicol-
become more difficult (Light & Spirduso, 1990). ogy: A Journal of Research in Music Cognition, 18, 59 –75. http://dx
• When learning a sequence of fine-motor movements, older .doi.org/10.1037/h0094051
Bugos, J. A., Perlstein, W. M., McCrae, C. S., Brophy, T. S., & Beden-
adults need more repetition (practice) than younger adults
baugh, P. H. (2007). Individualized piano instruction enhances executive
to develop similar motor memory. Though learners should functioning and working memory in older adults. Aging and Mental
be aware of the individual items that make up a group of Health, 11, 464 – 471. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607860601086504
notes, the focus should shift to the unit rather than individ- Chen, H. (1996). An investigation of self-directed learning among non-
ual notes. The movement used to perform the notes should music major adult piano learners in one-to-one piano instruction (Un-
be learned, prepared, and executed as a single unit, rather published doctoral dissertation). Teachers College, Columbia Univer-
than resting on individual movements. Segmenting longer sity, New York, NY.
Coffman, D. (2002). Adult education. In R. Colwell & C. Richardson
units into shorter units may be beneficial (Fraser et al.,
(Eds.), The new handbook of research on music teaching and learning
2009; Gaillard et al., 2009; Verwey et al., 2011). (pp. 199 –209). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
• Periodic rest periods during practice or rehearsal may be Cooper, T. L. (2001). Adults’ perceptions of piano study: Achievements
particularly beneficial to the learning process for older and experiences. Journal of Research in Music Education, 49, 156 –168.
adults. Motor performance of older adults has been found to http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3345867
218 REIFINGER

Detterman, D. K., & Ruthsatz, R. (1999). Toward a more comprehensive Krampe, R. T., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2001). Age-specific problems in
theory of exceptional abilities. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, rhythmic timing. Psychology and Aging, 16, 12–30. http://dx.doi.org/10
22, 148 –158. .1037/0882-7974.16.1.12
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of Krampe, R. T., & Ericsson, K. A. (1996). Maintaining excellence: Delib-
deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psycholog- erate practice and elite performance in young and older pianists. Journal
ical Review, 100, 363– 406. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.100.3 of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 331–359. http://dx.doi.org/
.363 10.1037/0096-3445.125.4.331
Ericsson, K. A., & Lehmann, A. C. (1996). Expert and exceptional per- Krampe, R. T., Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R. (2005). Timing, sequencing, and
formance: Evidence of maximal adaptation to task constraints. Annual executive control in repetitive movement production. Journal of Exper-
Review of Psychology, 47, 273–305. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev imental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 379 –397.
.psych.47.1.273 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.31.3.379
Ernst, R. E., & Emmons, S. (1992). New horizons for senior adults. Music Lehmann, A. C., & Ericsson, K. A. (1997). Research on expert perfor-
Educators Journal, 79, 30 –34. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3398527 mance and deliberate practice: Implications for the education of amateur
Finney, S. A., & Palmer, C. (2003). Auditory feedback and memory for musicians and music students. Psychomusicology: A Journal of Re-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

music performance: Sound evidence for an encoding effect. Memory and search in Music Cognition, 16, 40–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Cognition, 31, 51– 64. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03196082 h0094068


Fitts, P. M., & Posner, M. I. (1967). Human performance. Belmont, CA: Lehmann, A. C., & Ericsson, K. A. (1998). The historical development of domains
Brooks/Cole Publishing Company. of expertise: Performance standards and innovations in music. In A. Steptoe
Fraser, S. A., Li, K. Z., & Penhune, V. B. (2009). A comparison of motor (Ed.), Genius and the mind: Studies of creativity and temperament in the
skill learning and retention in younger and older adults. Experimental historical record (pp. 67–94). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Brain Research, 195, 419 – 427. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009- http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198523734.003.0004
1806-5 Light, K. E., & Spirduso, W. W. (1990). Effects of adult aging on the
Gaillard, V., Destrebocqz, A., Michiels, S., & Cleeremans, A. (2009). movement complexity factor of response programming. Journals of
Effects of age and practice in sequence learning: A graded account of Gerontology Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 45,
ageing, learning, and control. European Journal of Cognitive Psychol- P107–P109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronj/45.3.P107
ogy, 21, 255–282. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09541440802257423 Manturzewska, M. (1990). A biographical study of the life-span develop-
Gembris, H. (2012). Music-making as a lifelong development and resource ment of professional musicians. Psychology of Music, 18, 112–139.
for health. In R. MacDonald, G. Kreutz, & L. Mitchell (Eds.), Music, http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0305735690182002
health, & wellbeing (pp. 367–382). New York, NY: Oxford University Meinz, E. J., & Hambrick, D. Z. (2010). Deliberate practice is necessary
Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586974.003.0025 but not sufficient to explain individual differences in piano sight-reading
Halpern, A. R., & Bartlett, J. C. (2010). Memory for melodies. In M. R. skill: The role of working memory capacity. Psychological Science, 21,
Jones, F. F. Fay, & A. N. Popper (Eds.), Music perception (Vol. 36, pp. 914 –919. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797610373933
233–258). New York, NY: Springer-Verlag. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/ New Horizons Groups. (2016, April 1). New horizons organizations by
978-1-4419-6114-3_8 region. Retrieved from http://newhorizonsmusic.org/new-horizons-groups/
Hambrick, D. Z., Altmann, E. M., Oswald, R. L., Meinz, E. J., Gobet, F., Palmer, C. (2006). The nature of memory for music performance skills. In
& Campitelli, G. (2014). Accounting for expert performance: The devil E. Altenmüller, J. Kesselring, & M. Wiesendanger (Eds.), Music, motor
is in the details. Intelligence, 45, 112–114. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j control and the brain (pp. 39 –53). New York, NY: Oxford University
.intell.2014.01.007 Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199298723.003.0003
Hanna-Pladdy, B., & MacKay, A. (2011). The relation between instrumen- Palmer, C., & Meyer, R. K. (2000). Conceptual and motor learning in
tal musical activity and cognitive aging. Neuropsychology, 25, 378 –386. music performance. Psychological Science, 11, 63– 68. http://dx.doi.org/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021895 10.1111/1467-9280.00216
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and Parks, S. L., & Clancy Dollinger, S. (2014). The positivity effect and
aging: A review and a new view. In G. H. Boser (Ed.), The psychology auditory recognition memory for musical excerpts in young, middle-
of learning and motivation (Vol. 22, pp. 193–225). San Diego, CA: aged, and older adults. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 24,
Academic Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60041-9 298 –308. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pmu0000079
Highben, Z., & Palmer, C. (2004). Effects of auditory and motor mental Platz, F., Kopiez, R., Lehmann, A. C., & Wolf, A. (2014). The influence
practice in memorized piano performance. Bulletin of the Council for of deliberate practice on musical achievement: A meta-analysis. Fron-
Research in Music Education, 159, 58 – 65. tiers in Psychology, 5, 646. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00646
Horn, J. L., & Cattell, R. B. (1967). Age differences in fluid and crystal- Prickett, C. A. (2003). Is there musical life after graduation? Mining the resources
lized intelligence. Acta Psychologica, 26, 107–129. http://dx.doi.org/10 of an understudied population. Research Studies in Music Education, 21, 58–
.1016/0001-6918(67)90011-X 71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1321103X030210010501
Kausler, D. H. (Ed.). (1994). Skill learning and procedural learning. In Ruthsatz, J., Detterman, D., Griscom, W. S., & Cirullo, B. A. (2008). Becoming an
Learning and memory in normal aging (pp. 26 – 65). San Diego, CA: expert in the musical domain: It takes more than just practice. Intelligence, 36,
Academic Press, Inc. 330–338. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2007.08.003
Krampe, R. T. (1997). Age-related changes in practice activities and their Salthouse, T. A. (1984). Effects of age and skill in typing. Journal of
relation to musical performance skills. In H. Jorgensen and A. C. Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 345–371. http://dx.doi.org/10
Lehmann (Eds.), Does practice make perfect? Current theory and re- .1037/0096-3445.113.3.345
search on instrumental music practice (pp. 165–178). Oslo, Norway: Seinfeld, S., Figueroa, H., Ortiz-Gil, J., & Sanchez-Vives, M. V. (2013).
Norges musikkhøgskole. Effects of music learning and piano practice on cognitive function, mood
Krampe, R. T., Doumas, M., Lavrysen, A., & Rapp, M. (2010). The costs and quality of life in older adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 810.
of taking it slowly: Fast and slow movement timing in older age. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00810
Psychology and Aging, 25, 980 –990. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ Sloboda, J. A., Davidson, J. W., Howe, M. J. A., & Moore, D. G. (1996).
a0020090 The role of practice in the development of performing musicians. British
AGE-RELATED CHANGES AFFECTING LEARNING 219

Journal of Psychology, 87, 287–309. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044- development of motor chunks and explicit sequence knowledge. Psy-
8295.1996.tb02591.x chological Research, 75, 406 – 422. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00426-
Taylor, A. (2011). Older amateur keyboard players learning for self- 011-0320-0
fulfilment. Psychology of Music, 39, 345–363. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ Wan, C. Y., & Schlaug, G. (2010). Music making as a tool for promoting
0305735610376262 brain plasticity across the life span. The Neuroscientist, 16, 566 –577.
Turgeon, M., & Wing, A. M. (2012). Late onset of age-related difference http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073858410377805
in unpaced tapping with no age-related difference in phase-shift error Woods, D. L., Wyma, J. M., Yund, E. W., Herron, T. J., & Reed, B. (2015).
detection and correction. Psychology and Aging, 27, 1152–1163. http:// Age-related slowing of response selection and production in a visual
dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0029925 choice reaction time task. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 193.
Verghese, J., Lipton, R. B., Katz, M. J., Hall, C. B., Derby, C. A.,
Kuslansky, G., . . . Buschke, H. (2003). Leisure activities and the risk of
dementia in the elderly. The New England Journal of Medicine, 348,
2508 –2516. http://dx.doi.org/10.1056/NEJMoa022252 Received June 17, 2015
Verwey, W. B., Abrahamse, E. L., Ruitenberg, M. F. L., Jiménez, L., & de Revision received April 1, 2016
Accepted April 5, 2016 䡲
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Kleine, E. (2011). Motor skill learning in the middle-aged: Limited


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Members of Underrepresented Groups:


Reviewers for Journal Manuscripts Wanted
If you are interested in reviewing manuscripts for APA journals, the APA Publications and
Communications Board would like to invite your participation. Manuscript reviewers are vital to the
publications process. As a reviewer, you would gain valuable experience in publishing. The P&C
Board is particularly interested in encouraging members of underrepresented groups to participate
more in this process.

If you are interested in reviewing manuscripts, please write APA Journals at Reviewers@apa.org.
Please note the following important points:

• To be selected as a reviewer, you must have published articles in peer-reviewed journals. The
experience of publishing provides a reviewer with the basis for preparing a thorough, objective
review.

• To be selected, it is critical to be a regular reader of the five to six empirical journals that are most
central to the area or journal for which you would like to review. Current knowledge of recently
published research provides a reviewer with the knowledge base to evaluate a new submission
within the context of existing research.

• To select the appropriate reviewers for each manuscript, the editor needs detailed information.
Please include with your letter your vita. In the letter, please identify which APA journal(s) you
are interested in, and describe your area of expertise. Be as specific as possible. For example,
“social psychology” is not sufficient—you would need to specify “social cognition” or “attitude
change” as well.

• Reviewing a manuscript takes time (1– 4 hours per manuscript reviewed). If you are selected to
review a manuscript, be prepared to invest the necessary time to evaluate the manuscript
thoroughly.

APA now has an online video course that provides guidance in reviewing manuscripts. To learn
more about the course and to access the video, visit http://www.apa.org/pubs/authors/review-
manuscript-ce-video.aspx.

You might also like