You are on page 1of 20

CCM International Journal of

Cross Cultural
2009 Vol 9(2): 217–236 Management

Emotional Intelligence
Factorial Structure and Construct Validity
across Cultures

Sudeep Sharma Ramakrishna Biswal


London School of Economics and Political Science, Delhi University, India
UK, and Olin Business School, Washington
University, St. Louis, USA
Manas K. Mandal
Indian Institute of Technology-Kharagpur, India
Jürgen Deller
Leuphana University of Lüneburg, Germany, and
Lucas Graduate School of Business, San José State
University, USA

abstract   This study empirically examines the construct of emotional Intelligence (EI)
cross-culturally. Participants (N = 200) from Germany (N = 100) and India (N = 100)
completed the Emotional Intelligence Scale, NEO-Five Factor Personality Inventory
(Form-S) and Hofstede’s Value Survey (Module 94). The major objectives of the study were
to examine cross-culturally (a) the factor structure of EI, and (b) the construct of EI as
distinct from personality. Results revealed different factor structures for these two cultures,
and the construct of EI failed to correlate with the dimensions of personality. Implications
of the findings for leadership in a specific culture as well as across different cultures are
discussed.
key words • Big Five • construct validity • emotional intelligence • factorial structure •
Germany • India • personality
as distinct from intelligence (Bar-On, 1997;
1. Introduction
Day and Carroll, 2004; Mayer et al., 1999;
Recent studies conducted on ‘emotional Van Rooy and Viswesvaran, 2004) and per-
intelligence’ (EI) illustrate the relevance of sonality (Ackerman and Heggestad, 1997;
this construct for psychological research. Davies et al., 1998; Dwada and Hart, 2000;
The major question analyzed in these stud- Newsome et al., 2000; Saklofske et al., 2003).
ies relates to the construct validity of EI, Even though this question has raised diverse
© The Author(s), 2009. Reprints and Permissions: http://www.sagepub.co.uk/JournalsPermissions.nav 
DOI: 10.1177/1470595809335725

Downloaded from ccm.sagepub.com at University of New England on June 6, 2015


218   International Journal of Cross Cultural Management 9(2)

opinions amongst researchers, few attempts Many researchers also recognize two differ-
have been made to examine the cross-­cultural ent models of emotional intelligence (e.g.,
validity of the construct. This validity, how- Hedlund and Sternberg, 2000; Mayer et
ever, is a prerequisite for international man- al., 1999). One of these is referred to as the
agers’ success, as it will allow them to use the ‘ability model’ and the other as the ‘mixed
construct in their daily routines in different model’. The ability model defines EI as a type
cultures in the same way. of intelligence, whereas the mixed model is
Emotional intelligence is perhaps one more dispositional or trait-based in nature
of the most widely researched psychological (Petrides and Furnham, 2000), with less of a
constructs in the 21st century (e.g. Ashkanasy, cognitive emphasis. According to Mayer and
2003; Bar-On, 2006; Boyatzis et al., 2000; Salovey, who propounded the ability model
Drusket and Wolff, 2001). Many researchers in 1997, the MEIS was developed using the
have debated the concept of emotional intel- notion of emotional intelligence as a model
ligence and the validity of emotional intel- of general intelligence. Salovey and Mayer
ligence measures (e.g. Ciarrochi et al., 2000; (1990, p. 189) proposed a formal definition
Law et al., 2004; McCrae, 2000; Perez et of emotional intelligence as being, ‘the abil-
al., 2005; Petrides and Furnham, 2000; Van ity to monitor one’s own and others’ feel-
Rooy and Viswesvaran, 2004). In an effort to ings, to discriminate among them, and to
clarify different concepts of emotional intel- use this information to guide one’s thinking
ligence, the Encyclopedia of Applied Psychology and action’. Later this definition was refined
(Spielberger, 2004) recently suggested that and broken down into four proposed fac-
there are currently three major conceptual tors that are distinct yet related: perceiving,
models in this field: using, understanding, and managing emotions
(Mayer and Salovey, 1997).
(a) The Salovey-Mayer model (Mayer
In contrast, Bar-On deems emotional
and Salovey, 1997), which defines this
intelligence to comprise an array of non-
construct as the ability to perceive,
cognitive capabilities, competencies, and
understand, manage and use emotions
skills, which all influence one’s ability to suc-
to facilitate thinking, measured by an
ceed in coping with environmental demands
ability-based measure: either the Mayer
and pressures (Bar-On, 1997). The factor
Emotional Intelligence Scale – MEIS
structure of emotional intelligence includes
(Mayer and Salovey, 1997) or its latest ver-
Intrapersonal (emotional self-awareness,
sion, MESCEIT v 2.0 (Mayer et al., 2002).
assertiveness, self regard, self actualization,
(b) The Goleman model (1998), which views
independence), Interpersonal (empathy, inter­
this construct as comprising a wide array
personal relationships, social responsibility),
of competencies and skills, which drive
Stress Management (problem solving, reality
managerial performance, measured
testing, flexibility), and Adaptability (stress
by a multi-rater assessment named the
tolerance, impulse control) (Bar-On, 1997).
Emotional Competencies Inventory
These theoretical models described the
– ECI (Boyatzis et al., 2000).
construct of emotional intelligence without
(c) The Bar-On model (1997b, 2000), which
much attention to any cross-cultural evidence
describes a cross-section of interrelated
which supported their theoretical positions.
emotional and social competencies, skills
Because it is believed that emotions are socially
and facilitators that impact intelligent
constructed to a great extent, the construct of
behavior, measured by self-report in the
emotional intelligence needs a cross-cultural
Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory
database to validate its theoretical position.
– EQ-I (1997a, 1997b).
In the present paper, an attempt has been

Downloaded from ccm.sagepub.com at University of New England on June 6, 2015


Sharma et al.: Emotional Intelligence across Cultures 219

made to create such a database. Further to naires ­versus facial recognition measures, for
this, the cultural invariance, whether proven example), substantive country-level variables
or not, will have great relevance in the field of such as mode of subsistence, political vari-
cross-cultural management. This is because ables, stability of a country, and aggregated
the input drawn from it should assist global psycho­logical variables (e.g. individualism,
managers in any decisions which they make religious values) explained a substantial part
based on cultural peculiarities or similarities of the cross-cultural differences in emo-
in a global market. tions that have been reported. It is possible
One of the most pertinent issues domi- that some facets of EI contain pan-cultural
nating current research in psychology is the characteristics while other facets are more
cross-cultural study of emotion. Culture ­culture-­specific. These aspects have rarely
plays an important role in the understand- been studied across cultures with reference to
ing and expression of emotions (Ekman, the construct of emotional intelligence.
1972, 1993; Friesen, 1972; Mandal et al., Generalizability across cultures and lan-
1996; Matsumoto, 1989, 2002; Mesquita et guages is crucial for the evaluation of an
al., 1997). Cross-cultural studies have shown emotional intelligence taxonomy or structure.
that social behaviour varies across cultures When new instruments have to be used for
(Amir and Sharon, 1987), and that emo- educational purposes or for job selection, test
tional responses within a social network are developers need to consider the multicultural
also evaluated based on core cultural prac- dimensions of our societies. Methodological
tices (Markus and Kitayama, 1994; Shweder aspects then become an issue; when an EI
and Much, 1987; see also Mesquita et al., questionnaire developed in one cultural con-
1997). Markus and Kitayama (1991) further text is to be applied to another, invariance of
explained that cultures have a set of specific its psychometric properties cannot be merely
consequences for cognition, emotion and assumed (Parker et al., 2005). Therefore, an
motivation. Elfenbein and Ambady (2002a, investigation into the cross-cultural validity
2002b, 2003) argued that the rate of emotion of the EI construct could make a valuable
recognition is fairly stable across cultures, but contribution to the present literature.
that people tend to recognize the expression Because the origin of the EI construct
of basic emotions more easily during interac- lies primarily in Western literature (Bar-On,
tions with people from their own culture than 2000; Cooper and Ayman, 1997; Goleman,
with people from other cultures. 1995; Mayer et al., 2004), there has been a
Recently Matsumuto et al. (2008) found general tendency to conceive the construct as
significant differences in the understanding being universal without realizing that ‘emo-
and expression of emotion across cultures. tion’ is also a cross-culturally embedded con-
They found that members of individualistic struct (Sibia et al., 2003). Pant and Prakash
cultures endorse more emotion expression, (2004) critically evaluated one of the origi-
whereas members of collectivistic cultures nal measures of emotional intelligence – the
endorse less. Their findings further illustrate Mayer Emotional Intelligence Scale (MEIS)
that the expression of negative emotions – in an Indian context, and concluded that
with in-group members is more common in most of the reliabilities of the MEIS subtests
individualistic societies. In an extensive meta- were weak for the Indian sample. They sug-
analysis of emotion expression and recogni- gested, however, that the MEIS might work
tion studies across cultures, Van Hemert et in the Indian sample if cultural context were
al. (2007) found that besides statistical arte- taken into account during the validation and
facts directly related to sample sizes, and testing process.
method-related factors (self-report question- Hofstede (2001), and more recently the

Downloaded from ccm.sagepub.com at University of New England on June 6, 2015


220   International Journal of Cross Cultural Management 9(2)

GLOBE study (House et al., 2004), explained making to achieve appropriate outcomes.
that cultural differences could affect psycho- EI helps them communicate visions to, and
logical constructs of human behavior. So far, form constructive relationships with, others
all factor structures of emotional intelligence (cf. Ashkenasy and Daus, 2002). If emotional
have evolved only on the basis of studies intelligence contributes to effective domestic
done in Western (predominantly individualis- leadership, it may also contribute to effective
tic) cultures (Bar-On, 2000; Goleman, 1995; cross-­cultural leadership. Expatriates who are
Mayer et al., 2000). Few studies have assessed exposed to EI stimuli in a different culture
the factor structure of emotional intelligence would be able to read these just as they can
in Eastern (primarily collectivistic) cultures. at home, without further learning. However,
This becomes all the more important if we this will only be the case if EI is the same
assume that the basic constituents are only construct across cultures.
emotion and intelligence. Because both are To understand the differences and simi-
psychological processes, and all psychological larities between factor structures of the scale
processes are somewhat culturally embedded, in Western and Eastern cultures, Hofstede’s
it is presumed that emotional intelligence (1983) distinction between collectivistic and
must also be culturally constructed. individualistic cultures can be used as a frame-
The need for a cross-cultural ­ validation work. According to Hofstede (1983), individ-
of EI (Bar-On, 2006; Conte, 2005) and its ualism and collectivism represent a conglom-
implications for management (Gabel et al., eration of values concerning the relationship
2005) have been argued by earlier ­researchers. between an individual and his or her collec-
However, research to date has yet to system- tivity in society. Triandis (1989, 1995) men-
atically examine the pan-­culturality of this tioned that collectivistic societies emphasize
construct across diverse cultural groups. a number of characteristics, including giving
Parker et al. (2005) have investigated the priority to group goals as a means for attain-
generalizability of the concept of EI to the ing individual goals, concern for how one’s
North American culture, but not many stud- actions will impact on in-group members,
ies have tried to evaluate the replicability of and a tendency to share resources with other
factor structures of the EI construct beyond in-group members. The other important dif-
this culture, for example, in Eastern cultures. ference between individualism and collectiv-
A cross-cultural application of emotional ism concerns the pursuit of personal versus
intelligence may have implications for leader­ group goals and interests (Triandis, 1989).
ship, communication, and human relations Individualists operate according to self-inter-
skills in organizations. Emotional intelligence, est, whereas collectivists operate according
that is, sensing others’ emotional states, is a to group interest. The key word here, ‘self ’,
key element of interpersonal communicative is inseparable from emotional intelligence
competence. As such, it may help leaders to (Gangopadhyay and Mandal, 2008).
build up trusting and empathic relations with The notion that emotional intelligence is
direct reports and superiors. Leaders who more than simply emotion and intelligence
can both recognize and manage their own is substantiated by the concept of ‘self ’ hav-
and others’ emotions will be able to more ing developed in culture-specific settings.
successfully manage emotionally challenging Such an idea suggests that ‘self ’ has the
situations and provide support and model- potential to influence emotional intelligence
ling to direct reports. Emotionally intelligent (Gangopadhyay et al., 2008). Therefore,
­leaders are proficient to regulate their own dimensions like understanding the emotions
emotions and those of others. They can of self and others could have a different
also use emotional information in decision- meaning for collectivistic societies. People

Downloaded from ccm.sagepub.com at University of New England on June 6, 2015


Sharma et al.: Emotional Intelligence across Cultures 221

in these societies prefer social relationships developers (Austin et al., 2004; Gignac et al.,
to individual profit during social interac- 2005; Petrides and Furnham, 2000; Saklofske
tions, for example in negotiation (Salacuse, et al., 2003), as yet no studies have tried to
1998). Therefore, they may control their evaluate the factor structure of this scale
own feelings in order to preserve harmoni- cross-­culturally. Two of Hofstede’s dimen­
ous relationships in groups. For example, if sions, namely individualism/­collectivism and
we look closely at Indian literature, we find power distance, are being used to measure
some ­traces in the text of references to emo- the impact of cultural differences on the EI
tional intelligence. In our view one of the construct. We have previously mentioned that
most famous scriptures of Hindu literature, individualism/collectivism could influence
The Bhagavad Gita (origin dated around 1500 the EI construct. Power distance is a dimen-
B.C.), also speaks about managing one’s emo- sion that is embedded within the individual-
tions. It contains a beautiful description of the ism/collectivism dimension. According to
discourse between Arjun and his charioteer, Hofstede (1991), individualism is associated
Lord Krishna. Their discourse, which took with lower power distance, while collectiv-
place just before the onset of a war, is one of ism is associated with higher power distance.
the greatest philosophical and religious dia- Based on an empirical comparison of data
logues known to man (Prabhupada, 1986). If from three different data samples (Bond,
we look at some of the shloks (some examples 1987; Hofstede, 1980; Schwartz, 1994), Bond
given in Appendix 2), out of the eighteen (1996) identified a single factor that included
chapters of The Bhagavad Gita, we find that both individualism (.86) and power distance
they speak about how man is caught in emo- (–.76), and this led him to suggest that col-
tional crises and how one should understand lectivism is associated with hierarchy. In the
and manage his emotions in order to fulfill his spirit of this recommendation, the likely inter-
duty. There, for the sake of the group, Arjun action between collectivism and high power
manages his own emotions and sacrifices his distance as dimensions of national cultures
self-feelings, for the good of society. In one deserves consideration (Schermerhorn and
sense, the Indian notion of emotional intel- Bond, 1997). Therefore, in order to under-
ligence can also be defined as using emotions stand the differences in the factor structures
to do what is right and fulfill one’s karma in of EI across individualistic and collectivistic
life. We can conclude from this that in many societies, the influence of power distance on
situations people in a collectivistic culture the EI construct cannot be ignored.
like India tend to manage their emotions for Thus, the first objective of this study is
the sake of their society or group, where that to examine the similarities and differences
may not be the case so much in individualis- in the factor structures of EI between an
tic cultures. Indian and a German sample. According
Given this background, the broad objec- to GLOBE (House et al., 2004) as well as
tive of the present study was to validate the Hofstede’s (1983) cultural dimensions, these
construct of EI cross-culturally. For this, we countries represent two different culture
have used one of the most widely used EI clusters. India is a society which is character-
measures: Schutte Emotional Intelligence ized by high power distance and collectivism,
Scale (Schutte et al., 1998). The rationale whereas Germany has lower power distance
behind using this scale was that although and a higher degree of individualism.
many researchers in the past have investi­
gated the psychometric properties of this
scale and compared the resulting factor struc-
tures with the factor structure given by the

Downloaded from ccm.sagepub.com at University of New England on June 6, 2015


222   International Journal of Cross Cultural Management 9(2)

1.1 Emotional Intelligence and Germany and India. The German sample
Personality comprised 51 men and 49 women (mean
In recent years, a growing number of empiri­ age = 33.82 years, S.D. = 9.16), whereas
cal studies have also aimed to validate the the Indian sample consisted of 80 men
construct of emotional intelligence (Brackett and 20 women (mean age = 28.85 years,
and Mayer, 2003; Ciarrochi et al., 2002; Perez S.D. = 3.50). Middle ­ managers from lead-
et al., 2005; Petrides and Furnham, 2000; ing IT companies which the authors could
Saklofske et al., 2003). Personality research- access, and graduate management ­ students,
ers, in particular, have found a high correla- in nearly equal proportions, participated in
tion between EI measures and personality the study. All participants volunteered for the
dimensions. They claim that emotional intel- study, and none reported having any mood
ligence is not a unique construct; instead, they disturbance at the time of testing.
see it as a part of the personality model (e.g.
Ackerman and Heggestad, 1997; Davies et al., 2.2. Scales
1998; Dwada and Hart, 2000; Newsome et al., 2.2.1. Measure of Emotional Intelli­
2000; Saklofske et al., 2003). In contrast, EI gence  The 33-item self-report Emotional
researchers have claimed that emotional intel- Intelligence Scale (SEIS) (Schutte et al.,
ligence is a construct which is independent of 1998) represents all portions of the concep-
personality (e.g. Bar-On, 1997; Brackett and tual model of Salovey and Mayer (1990).
Mayer, 2003; Lopes et al., 2003). This is a self-report measurement of emo-
McCrae (2000), in a review of the Big tional intelligence. The internal consistency
Five personality dimensions, suggested that (Cronbach’s alpha) of the scale is α = .87,
EI should be most strongly related to the and the test-retest reliability is .78. As there
‘openness to experience’ dimension, but this was no German version of this scale availa-
claim lacks strong empirical evidence (see ble, it was translated and back translated into
Van Rooy and Viswesvaran, 2004). Dawda the German language by bilingual experts
and Hart (2000), using the self-report EQ- (see Appendix 1). The internal consistency
i (Bar-On, 1997), found particularly high of the German version was satisfactory, with
correlations between EI and personality, Cronbach’s alpha ranging from α = .71 to
as measured with the NEO-FFI personal- .76 for all four factors. For the Indian sam-
ity dimensions. Bar-On (1997) found low to ple, the original (English) version of the scale
moderate correlation between EI dimensions was utilized. The Cronbach’s alpha for the
based on his model and 16-PF personality present India sample was reported as α =
dimensions. Such a contradiction in findings .84. The four factorial structure of the SEIS
may be resolved by examining the functional given by Ciarrochi et al. (2002) was consid-
independence of EI from personality across ered as a standard factor solution for all the
cultures. Therefore, the second objective of major statistical analysis of the present stud-
our study is to examine the proposition that ies. The factors are:
EI is distinct from personality dimensions
(a) perception of emotions,
across cultures.
(b) managing own emotions,
(c) managing others’ emotions, and
(d) utilization of emotions.
2. Method
2.1. Participants 2.2.2 Measure of Personality  The NEO-
The sample for the present study consist- FFI: Form S (NEO-FFI; Costa and McCrae,
ed of 100 adult participants from each of 1988) is a 60-item questionnaire measuring

Downloaded from ccm.sagepub.com at University of New England on June 6, 2015


Sharma et al.: Emotional Intelligence across Cultures 223

the personality dimensions of Neuroticism, the Emotional Intelligence Scale (Schutte et


Extraversion, Openness, Agreeableness and al., 1998), and the VSM94 Scale (Hofstede,
Conscientiousness; there are 12 questions for 1991, 2001).
each dimension. The five NEO-FFI factors,
as a brief version of the NEO Personality
3. Results
Inventory NEO-PI-R) correlate at between
r = .75 and r = .89 with the respective In order to study the cross-cultural generaliz-
dimensions. Internal consistency indices of abiltiy of the EI construct, we examined the
the NEO-FFI scales range from α = .74 to Schutte Emotional Intelligence Scale’s factor
.89, which is satisfactory to good. The reli- structure in the Indian and German samples
ability coefficients for the German NEO-FFI using exploratory (EFA) and confirmatory
(Borkenau and Ostendorf, 1993) range from factor analyses (CFA).
α = .70 to .85. The reliability coefficients for
the NEO-FFI were α = .73 for the German 3.1. Exploratory Factor
sample and α = .69 for the Indian sample, Analyses of Emotional
respectively. Intelligence Construct
Based on the outcome measure (SEIS score),
2.2.3 Measures of Cultural Dimensions a 33 x 33 inter-correlation matrix was cre-
  The Hofstede VSM 94 questionnaires ated and submitted to principal component
(Hofstede, 1994) are one of the major analysis with a varimax-rotation (Kaiser
descriptors of cultural differences. VSM94 Normalization) method for the German
consists of 20 items, and measures five cul- and Indian data. We used scree plot as the
tural dimensions: power distance, individual- ­criterion, and found that the four-factor solu-
ism, masculinity, uncertainty avoidance, and tion was the most suitable in both the Indian
long-term orientation (Hofstede, 1991). The and German samples. The four-factor solu-
internal consistency of Hofstede’s 20 item tion accounted for 40.4% and 54.3% of the
VSM 94 for both cultures is satisfactory (α = variance for Germany and India respectively.
.77 for German sample; α = .84 for Indian Similarly to previous findings (Ciarrochi et
sample). The German version of Hofstede’s al., 2002; Petrides and Furnham, 2000), our
VSM 94 (Hofstede, 1994) was used for the factor analysis for two samples resulted in
German sample. four-factor solutions. We followed Ciarrochi’s
(2002) four-factor solution as standard, and
2.3. Procedure found nine common items shared by both
Permission for data collection was obtained cultures (see bold items in Tables 1 and 2).
well in advance from the respective organi-
zations (IT firms and management schools) 3.2 Confirmatory Factor
in Germany and India. Participants from dif- Analyses of Emotional
ferent management schools completed the Intelligence Construct
questionnaire during class time (average 1.5 The four-factor structure of the SEIS was
hours). Managers in workplaces were given tested in both the Indian and the German
the questionnaires during office hours, along sample using confirmatory factor analy-
with the permission of their superiors to com- sis with R statistic- version 2.6 (R-Project,
plete them. The researchers then explained 1994). Examining the replicability of fac-
the purpose and procedure of filling out the tors by means of CFA can be achieved by
questionnaires. The participants completed two different strategies – single group models
a short demographic form, the NEO-FFI and multiple group models. We followed the
personality test (Costa and McCrae, 1988), single-group model of CFA, in which the fit

Downloaded from ccm.sagepub.com at University of New England on June 6, 2015


224   International Journal of Cross Cultural Management 9(2)

Table 1  Factor loadings of SEIS based on German sample. (N = 100)a

Factors
1 2 3 4

EI14 .746
EI13 .689
EI26 .644
EI11 .616
EI16 .616
EI12 .592
EI23 .591
EI5 .524
EI20 .478
EI15 .424
EI1 .328
EI33 .267
EI32 .821
EI29 .705
EI18 .596
EI2 .567
EI22 .470
EI8 .642
EI21 –.592
EI17 .547
EI7 .509
EI9 .459
EI6 .453
EI4 .423
EI24 .336
EI31 .288
EI27 –.637
EI3 .526
EI30 –.521
EI10 .515
EI25 .489
EI19 .473
EI28 .370
a
Please refer to Schutte et al. (1998) for item details. Bold items indicate common items between the Indian and
German factor structures.

Downloaded from ccm.sagepub.com at University of New England on June 6, 2015


Sharma et al.: Emotional Intelligence across Cultures 225

Table 2  Factor loadings of SEIS based on Indian sample (N = 100)a

Factors
1 2 3 4

EI6 .723
EI22 .667
EI33 –.637
EI5 –.612
EI3 .611
EI7 .563
EI15 –.543
EI8 .500
EI27 .479
EI23 .446
EI10 .432
EI17 .813
EI18 .756
EI19 .689
EI20 .653
EI4 .572
EI2 .549
EI9 .461
EI30 .815
EI31 .768
EI1 .594
EI21 .583
EI32 .544
EI26 .502
EI16 .495
EI28 –.711
EI13 .687
EI14 .633
EI29 .625
EI25 .602
EI12 .586
EI24 .531
EI11 .503
a
Please refer to Schutte et al. (1998) for item details. Bold items indicate common items between the Indian and
German factor structures.

Downloaded from ccm.sagepub.com at University of New England on June 6, 2015


226   International Journal of Cross Cultural Management 9(2)

Table 3  Overall Goodness-of-Fit Indices for Confirmatory Factor Analysis Single-Group Models

Absolute indices Relative indices


Structural Model χ2 df χ 2/df GFI RMS TLI NFI CFI

1.  Indian Sample


German Varimax 2678.8 516 5.19 .406 .206 .165 .158 .184
2.  German Sample
Indian Varimax 1433.6 516 2.78 .541 .134 .151 .124 .171

Note. N = 100. GFI = goodness-of-fit index; RMS = root mean square; TLI = Tucker-Lewis Index; NFI = normed
fit index; CFI = normed noncentrality fit index.

of a normative target structure matrix can Table 3 shows the significance tests and
be tested on a new sample by fixing all the goodness-of-fit indices for each model. Based
variable loadings to the values in that matrix on their values, our analysis indicates that
(the target structure matrix could have been the Indian varimax model does not provide
derived from previous EFAs on a normative a good fit to the data when applied to the
sample). German sample. Also the German model is
CFA offers a variety of statistical tests and not a good fit for the Indian sample. Thus,
indices designed to assess the goodness-of-fit on the basis of conventional CFA fit criteria,
of data to a proposed model. Considering the Indian and German models can not be
the multidimensionality of goodness-of-fit judged as being cross-culturally valid ­models.
(Tanaka, 1993) in all models, we evaluated A x2/df value of 2 or less is generally viewed
the goodness-of-fit of the German and Indian as a good fit (Church and Burke, 1994). The
model in both samples by means of the fol- GFI provides an index of the proportion of
lowing indices: variance accounted for by the model (Joreskog
and Sorbom, 1989), and values of .90 or
(a) the chi-square (χ2) test statistic,
higher are generally considered to indicate a
(b) the Goodness of Fit index (GFI)
good fit; the root mean square index gives the
(Joreskog and Sorbom, 1989),
average correlational discrepancy between
(c) the Root Mean Square Error of
the sample and model-produced correlation
Approximation (RMSEA) (Browne and
matrices relating to the observed measures
Cudek, 1993),
(Joreskog and Sorbom, 1989). Browne and
(d) the Non Normed Fit Index (NFI)
Cudeck (1993) argued that a model shows
(Bentler and Bonett, 1980),
a close fit if the root mean square error of
(e) the Tucker Fit Index (TFI) (Tucker and
approximation (RMSEA) is less than .05 and
Lewis, 1973), and
that ‘values up to .08 represent reasonable
(f) the Comparative Fit Index (CFI)
errors of approximation in the population’.
(Bentler, 1989, see Table 3).
RMSEA, NFI, and CFI are particularly 3.3. EI and NEO-FFI Personality
important in our case because they have been Dimensions
designed with the goal of evaluating the fit Table 4 shows the relationship between
of covariance structure models (Caprara et EI and NEO-FFI personality dimensions.
al., 2000). For the German sample, agreeableness was

Downloaded from ccm.sagepub.com at University of New England on June 6, 2015


Sharma et al.: Emotional Intelligence across Cultures 227

Table 4  Intercorrelations (Pearson’s ‘r’) NEO-FFI Personality Dimensions and SEIS for German
sample (N = 100) and Indian sample (N = 100)

Big-Five Personality Dimensions →


Emotional Intelligence dimensions ↓ N E O A C

Perception of emotion –.033(G) –.023(G) –.070(G) –.297**(G) –.094(G)


.069(I) .600**(I) .123(I) –.047(I) .459**(I)
Managing own emotions –.123(G) .090(G) –.112(G) –.109(G) .038(G)
.070(I) .585**(I) .031(I) –.031(I) .463**(I)
Managing others emotions .190(G) .131(G) .127(G) .261**(G) –.058(G)
.172(I) .743**(I) –.089(I) –.086(I) .313**(I)
Utilization of emotions .240*(G) .167(G) .220(G)* .250*(G) –.008(G)
.216*(I) .244*(I) –.024(I) –.293**(I) .221*(I)

Note. N = Neuroticism, E = Extraversion, O = Openness to Experience, A = Agreeableness, C =


Conscientiousness, G=German sample, I= Indian sample.
* significant at the 0.05 level
** significant at the 0.01 level

Table 5  Summary of ANCOVA indicating the difference (F) between Germany and India on
SEIS dimensions with individualism (Fi) and power distance (Fpd) as covariates

SEIS dimensions F Fi Fpd

Perception of emotion 6.22 ** 5.25** 6.08**


Manage own emotion 14.33** 13.28** 14.67**
Managing others’ emotion 22.26** 21.29** 21.52**
Utilization of emotion 0.55 0.84 0.53

** significant at the 0.01 level; degrees of freedom = 1

found to have a low correlation with ‘percep- tions’ (r = .45) and ‘managing own emotions’
tion of emotions’ (r = –.29) and ‘managing (r = .46).
emotions of others’ (r = .26). All other rela-
tionships between EI and personality dimen- 3.4. Individualism and Power
sions were negligible. For the Indian sample Distance
‘extraversion’ and ‘conscientiousness’ were The differential factor structure, as well as
significantly correlated with most dimensions the relationship between EI and personality
of emotional intelligence. Table 3 shows that dimensions, suggested we should analyze the
extraversion was highly correlated with ‘man- effect of culture (denoted here by individual-
aging other’s emotions’ (r = .74), ‘perception ism and power distance). The mean scores on
of emotions’ (r = .60) and ‘managing own individualism were 56 for India and 89 for
emotions’ (r = .58). Conscientiousness had Germany, and on power distance, they were
a high correlation with ‘perception of emo- 63 for India and 21 for Germany. The differ-

Downloaded from ccm.sagepub.com at University of New England on June 6, 2015


228   International Journal of Cross Cultural Management 9(2)

ences between these dimensions were found of Saklofske et al. (2003) and Petrides and
to be significant for both the Indian and Furnham (2000), and 31 items between the
German samples (p < .05 for each dimen- studies of Petrides and Furnham (2000) and
sion). These scores justify our claim that the Ciarrochi et al. (2002) had similar items clus-
two samples differ on these dimensions. We tered within the four factorial structures of
hypothesized that the indices of individual- SEIS. These findings are more indicative of
ism and power distance, if partialled out, the universality of EI as a construct than they
could provide an explanation for the cultural are of culture specificities.
differences ­ identified between the German However, all of these studies exclusively
and Indian samples. Our results showed a used samples from GLOBE’s Anglo cluster,
­significant difference between these two cul- and thus these data only replicated findings
tures along most dimensions of SEIS, even from the same culture cluster. The results of
after controlling for the effects of individu- the present study differ from the Anglo ones.
alism and power distance. The difference Our findings noted only nine items that were
between Germany and India was non- shared between Germany and India within
­significant for the SEIS dimension of ‘utili- the given factor structures of SEIS proposed
zation of emotions’ (see Table 5). by Ciarrochi et al. (2002). For example, both
cultures had items like ‘By looking at their facial
expressions’, and ‘I recognize the emotions people are
4. Discussion
experiencing’ clustered under the factor ‘per-
The present study attempted to evaluate a ception of emotions’. These items shared
trait-based measure of EI cross-culturally. identical meanings for Germany and India.
Assuming EI to be a culturally embedded Earlier studies suggested that the perception
construct, we expected to find a difference of emotions is universal, while expression
in factor structures between samples from of such emotions may be culture-specific
Germany and India. Both EFA and CFA (Mandal et al., 1996; Mandal et al., 2001).
findings partially confirmed our notion. We There were nine items common to the
also examined the item clusters in terms factor structures of both countries of this
of commonalities and specificities between study, and among those nine items, only three
both cultures. The factor structures indicated fell into the same factor of the factor struc-
similarities and differences. The overall CFA ture found by other researchers (Ciarrochi
results also provided empirical support for et al., 2002; Petrides and Furnham, 2000).
cross-cultural specificities in a four-­factor Items such as ‘I have control over my emotions’, ‘I
structure of the EI construct in German use good moods to help myself keep trying in the face
and Indian cultures. The aim of the present of obstacles’ and ‘By looking at their facial expres-
study was not to identify the factor struc- sions, I recognize the emotions people are experienc-
tures that evolved in each culture, but rather ing’ showed some common features across all
to see whether the structures have evolved cultures. Therefore, these items show a com-
differ­ently in each culture. Therefore, in the ponent of emotional intelligence commonly
present study, we did not carry out any factor shared by three cultures.
labeling for the respective cultures. The differences between German and
Earlier studies which used British (Petrides Indian cultures may be explained in terms of
and Furnham, 2000), Australian (Gignac et individualism and power distance. Whereas
al., 2005), and Canadian (Saklofske et al., Western cultures (for example, Germany,
2003) samples have elicited nearly identical UK, Canada) are high in individualism and
factor structures of SEIS. Twenty-five (out low in power distance, Eastern cultures (for
of 33) items of SEIS between the studies example, India, Japan) are high in collectiv-

Downloaded from ccm.sagepub.com at University of New England on June 6, 2015


Sharma et al.: Emotional Intelligence across Cultures 229

ism as well as in power distance (Hofstede, tions’ and ‘utilization of emotions’. Western
1991). Matsumoto (1993) observed cultural researchers also found moderate to high cor-
differences for emotional regulation in terms relations between EI and the ‘agreeableness’
of individualism versus collectivism. He dimension of Big-Five personality tests (e.g.
found that people from collectivist societies Brackett and Mayer, 2003; Saklofske et al.,
like India and other Asian cultures avoid 2003). The personality dimension of extra-
­negative reflection upon the group, and they version had a moderate relationship with
are thus less likely to display negative affect most EI dimensions in the Indian sample.
publicly. In contrast, people from individual- The personality dimension of conscientious-
istic societies (like the U.S. and Canada) do ness was moderately correlated with the
not suppress these communication cues, and EI dimensions of ‘perception of emotions’
are more likely to display their negative emo- and ‘managing own emotions’. These find-
tions without masking. ings suggest that the constructs of EI and
In the EI scale, items like ‘I know when to per­sonality do not overlap except for some
speak about my personal problems to others’ and ‘I dimensions which depend upon the social
present myself in a way that makes a good impres- construction in a given culture. However,
sion on others’ seem to have some cultural some other variables might potentially have
s­ pecificity. While good impressions can form influenced the present research; there may,
an important aspect of social relationships for example, have been a response bias in
in India as an example of a collectivistic either or both samples.
culture, this is not so common in German However, the relatively high reliability
culture. Additionally, in German culture, as coefficients of NEO-FFI and SEIS in both
an example of an individualistic culture, it is cultures show that poor reliability cannot be
important that a person should know when the cause of different factor structures and
one should or should not speak about the the relationship between personality dimen-
problems he or she is facing. However, in col- sions and EI dimensions. Also, we are aware
lectivistic cultures like India, it is common- that factor analysis results are always ­sample
place for one to express personal problems dependent. Different sample compositions can
with others more often than those in individ- lead to different factor solutions. Therefore,
ualistic cultures would do. In a collectivistic replications of our findings with different sam-
society, others also feel open to hearing one’s ples from the same cultural backgrounds will
problems, since people in a collectivistic cul- be necessary. Another reason for our results
ture want to spend more time in social groups could be the poor factor structure of SEIS, the
and follow their norms by hearing the prob- importance of which has been noted by pre-
lems of others, although they may not try to vious researchers (see Petrides and Furnham,
solve their problems. 2000; Mayer et al., 2000). Thus, there is a
The second objective of the study was to need for further research in this direction.
examine whether EI is distinct from person- Nevertheless, the cultural differences between
ality dimensions across cultures. Correlations India and Germany are definitely one possi-
were done between EI and personality ble explanation for the results.
dimensions for each culture. The analyses For example, in collective societies like
yielded mixed findings. For the German India, individual responsibility has less rel-
sample, the relationship between these two evance, as people like to work in groups,
constructs was nearly independent, except and follow group ideals. Because of an indi-
for a moderately low correlation between the vidual’s preference for responsibility shar-
personality dimension of agreeableness and ing, it may be preferable to maintain strong
the EI dimensions of ‘managing other’s emo- interpersonal relationships through impulse

Downloaded from ccm.sagepub.com at University of New England on June 6, 2015


230   International Journal of Cross Cultural Management 9(2)

control or conscientiousness. In a group, sets. In a global market, such input will help
however, those who take initiative and con- improve decision-making for managers, by
nect people for societal causes are perceived producing evidence on cultural peculiarities
as being extraverted, conscientious and emo- and similarities in the understanding and
tionally intelligent. expression of emotion.
Unlike Indians, Germans live in an indi- Our results can be interpreted in sup-
vidualistic society, where individuals take port of Thomas’ (2006) notion that cultural
responsibility for their own actions, and the specific norms may exist for experiencing
present findings suggest that they tend to emotions. The construct of emotional intel-
perceive emotions in somewhat similar ways ligence (which in our study was measured
within groups. The relationship between EI by the SEIS scale), therefore, can be seen as
and personality dimensions was orthogonal a product of, and limited to, the culture in
for the German subjects, indicating that the which it was developed. Thomas cautions
two constructs are independent for individu- that emotional intelligence may be mean-
alistically oriented societies. Confirmation of ingful only within the culture in which that
this has also been found in previous studies particular construct was developed. Thus, it
(Bar-On, 1997; Brackett and Mayer., 2003; may not apply to another. Given our results,
Lopes et al., 2003). the construct of emotional intelligence can
To examine the effect of cultural predis- indeed be understood to be culture-specific
positions on EI, we controlled statistically rather than culture-general.
for two culturally embedded dimensions: However, given the limited psychometric
individualism and power distance (Hofstede, qualities of the SEIS scale, as well as the rela-
1983, 1991). There were cultural differ- tively small sample sizes, it may be premature
ences between these dimensions, confirm- to come to this conclusion without applying
ing Hofstede’s (1991) notion that German scales with better psychometric properties
society is more individualistic and has less to larger, possibly more representative sam-
power distance, while Indian society is more ples. Therefore, a conclusion should not be
collectivist and has more power distance. based on results from the SEIS scale alone.
These differences in cultural dimensions, The results of this study hint to the neces-
however, did not fully explain the difference sity for leaders to acquire culture-specific
in EI scores between the two countries. Even EI-knowledge. If they then apply this knowl-
after partialling out the effects of individual- edge appropriately, they will enjoy the effects
ism and power distance, EI group differences on leadership, communication, and human
remained significant for all dimensions except ­relations, in such organizations as those men-
‘how people utilize emotions’ as the outcome tioned earlier in this paper. Hence in terms
measure (see Table 5). of the selection of leaders, it could be wise to
Perhaps the most glaring weakness still select those who have already acquired these
evident in this research is one that has limited competencies for a specific different culture,
the study of factor analysis for decades: small or those with a high learning ability and
samples, which constrain the power of statis­ motivation.
tical analyses. Future studies investigating In summary, we propose EI to be a
the cross-cultural validation of EI measures ­culturally-shaped construct, consisting of
should incorporate large samples from mul- both culture-specific and culture-general ele-
tiple countries representing different cultural ments. Such a conclusion, however, triggers
clusters, as well as utilizing other prominent more questions than it answers, and therefore,
EI measures (e.g. MSCEIT, Bar-On EQ-I) demands further cross-cultural research.
for greater variability in the resulting data

Downloaded from ccm.sagepub.com at University of New England on June 6, 2015


Sharma et al.: Emotional Intelligence across Cultures 231

Acknowledgements Bentler, P.M. (1989) EQS: A structural equations


program manual. Los Angeles: BMDP Statistical
We are indebted to Nicolai Dyroff, Christian Software.
Müller, and A. M. Rao for collecting data in Bentler, P.M. and Bonett, D.G. (1980)
their organizations, and to Joyce Osland for her ‘Significance Tests and Goodness of Fit in
comments on an earlier version of this paper. the Analysis of Co-variance Structures’,
Data in both countries were collected for partial Psychological Bulletin 88(3): 588–606.
fulfilment of a Master’s dissertation submitted by Bond, M.H. (1987) ‘Chinese Culture Connection:
the first author. For research assistance, we thank Chinese Values and the Search for Culture-
our colleague Sebastian Laube at Leuphana free Dimensions of Culture’, Journal of Cross-
Cultural Psychology 18(2): 143–64.
University of Lüneburg and Illinois Institute of
Bond, M.H. (1996) ‘Chinese Values’, in M.H.
Technology, and Amit Gupta at Indian Institute Bond (ed.), The Handbook of Chinese Psychology.
of Science, Bangalore. Hong Kong: Oxford University Press.
Borkenau, P. and Ostendorf, F. (1993) NEO-
Fünf-Faktoren Inventar (NEO-FFI) nach Costa und
References McCrae. Göttingen: Hogrefe.
Boyatzis, R.E., Goleman, D. and Rhee, K.
Ackerman, P.L. and Heggestad, E.D. (1997) (2000) ‚Clustering Competence in Emotional
‘Intelligence, Personality and Interests: Intelligence: Insights from the Emotional
Evidence for Overlapping Traits’, Psychological Competence Inventory (ECI)’, in R. Bar-On
Bulletin 121(2): 219–245. and J. D. Parker, (eds). Handbook of Emotional
Amir, Y. and Sharon, I. (1987) ‘Are Social Intelligence, San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Psychological Laws Cross-culturally Valid?’ Brackett, M.A. and Mayer, J.D. (2003)
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 18(4): ‘Convergent, Discriminant, and Incremental
383–470. Validity of Competing Measures of EI’,
Ashkanasy, N.M. (2003) ‘Emotions in Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 29(9):
Organizations: A Multilevel Perspective’, 1147–1158.
in F.Dansereau, and F. J. Yammarino (eds), Browne, M.W. and Cudeck, R. (1993)
Research in Multi-level Issues: Multi-level Issues in ‘Alternative Ways of Assessing Model Fit’,
Organizational Behavior and Strategy, Vol. 2 pp. in K. A. Bollen and J. S. Long (eds), Testing
9–54. Oxford: Elsevier Science. structural equation models, pp. 136–162. Beverly
Ashkenasy, N.M. and Daus, C.S. (2002) ‘Emotion Hills, CA: Sage.
in the Workplace: The New Challenge for Caprara, G.V., Barbaranelli, C., Bermez,
Managers’, Academy of Management Executive J., Maslach, C. and Ruch, W. (2000)
16(1): 76–86. ‘Multivariate Methods for the Comparison
Austin, E.J., Saklofske, D.H., Huang, S.H.S., and of Factor Structures in Cross-cultural
McKenney, D. (2004) ‘Measurement of Trait Research: An Illustration With the Big Five
Emotional Intelligence: Testing and Cross- Questionnaire’, Journal of Cross-Cultural
validating a Modified Version of Schutte et Psychology 31(4): 437–464.
al.’s (1998) Measure’, Personality and Individual Church, A.T. and Burke, P.J. (1994) ‘Exploratory
Differences 36: 555–62. and Confirmatory Tests of the Big Five and
Bar-On, R. (1997a) The Emotional Quotient Inventory Tellegen’s Three- and Four-dimensional
(EQ-i): A Test of Emotional Intelligence. Toronto, Models’, Journal of Personality and Social
Canada: Multi-Health Systems, Inc. Psychology 66(1): 93–114.
Bar-On, R. (1997b) The Emotional Quotient Inventory Ciarrochi, J.V., Chan, A.Y.C. and Caputi, P.
(EQ-i): Technical Manual. Toronto, Canada: (2000) ‘A Critical Evaluation of the Emotional
Multi-Health Systems, Inc. Intelligence Construct’, Personality and Individual
Bar-On, R. (2000) ‘Emotional and Social Differences 28(3): 539–561.
Intelligence: Insights from the Emotional Ciarrochi, J., Deane, F.P. and Anderson, S. (2002)
Quotient Inventory’, in R. Bar-On and J.D.A. ‘EI Moderates the Relationship Between
Parker (eds), The Handbook of EI, pp. 363–388. Stress and Mental Health’, Personality and
San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Individual Differences 32(2): 197–209.
Bar-On, R. (2006) The Bar-On Model of Conte, J.M. (2005) ‘A Review and Critique of
Emotional-Social Intelligence (ESI), Psicothema EI Measures’, Journal of Organizational Behavior
18(supl.): 13–25. 26(4): 433–440.

Downloaded from ccm.sagepub.com at University of New England on June 6, 2015


232   International Journal of Cross Cultural Management 9(2)

Cooper, R. and Ayman, S. (1997) Executive ‘Emotional Intelligence: A Universal or


EQ: Emotional Intelligence in Leadership and a Culture-specific Construct?’, In R.J.
Organizations. London: Orion Books. Emmerling, V.K. Shanwal and M.K. Mandal
Costa, P.T. and McCrae, R.R. (1988) The NEO (eds), Emotional Intelligence: Theoretical and
PI/FFI Manual Supplement. Odessa, FL: Cultural Perspectives, New York: Nova Science
Psychological Assessment Resources . Publishers, Inc.
Davies, M., Stankov, L. and Roberts, R.D. (1998) Gignac, G.E., Palmer, B.R., Manocha, R. and
‘Emotional Intelligence: In Search of an Stough, C.(2005) ‘An Examination of the
Elusive Construct’, Journal of Personality and Factor Structure of the Schutte Self-report
Social Psychology 75(4): 989–1015. Emotional Intelligence (SSREI) Scale via
Day, A.L. and Carroll, S.A. (2004) ‘Using Confirmatory Factor Analysis’, Personality and
an Ability-based Measure of Emotional Individual Differences 39(6): 1029–1042.
Intelligence to Predict Individual Goleman, D. (1995) Emotional Intelligence. New
Performance, Group Performance, and York: Bantam Books.
Group Citizenship Behaviours’, Personality and Goleman, D. (1998) Working with Emotional
Individual Differences 36(6): 1443–58. Intelligence. New York: Bantam.
Drusket, V.U and Wolff, S.V. (2001) ‘Building the Hedlund, J. and Sternberg, R.J. (2000) ‘Too
Emotional Intelligence of Groups’, Harvard Many Intelligences? Integrating Social,
Business Review (March): 80–90. Emotional, and Practical Intelligence’, in
Dwada, D. and Hart, S.D. (2000) ‘Assessing R. Bar-On and J. D. A. Parker (eds), The
Emotional Intelligence: Reliability and Handbook of Emotional Intelligence, pp.136–167.
Validity of the Bar-On Emotional Quotient San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Inventory (EQ-i) in University Students’, Hofstede, G. (1980) Culture’s Consequences:
Personality and Individual Differences 28(4): International Differences in Work-related Values. Los
797–812. Angeles, CA: Sage.
Ekman, P. (1972) ‘Universals and Cultural Hofstede, G. (1983) ‘Dimensions of Natural
Differences in Facial Expressions of Cultures in Fifty Countries and Three
Emotion’, in J. Cole (ed.), Nebraska Symposium Regions’, in J.B. Deregowski, S. Dziurawiec
on Motivation, 1971, Vol. 19, pp. 207–282. and R.C. Annis (eds), Expiscations in Cross-
Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. cultural Psychology, , pp. 335–355. Lisse, NL:
Ekman, P. (1993) ‘Facial Expression and Swets and Zeitlinger.
Emotion’, American Psychologist 48(4): 384–392. Hofstede, G. (1991) Cultures and Organizations:
Elfenbein, H.A. and Ambady, N. (2002a) ‘Is Software of the Mind. London: McGraw-Hill.
There an In-group Advantage in Emotion?’, Hofstede, G. (1994) ‘Value Survey Module 94’,
Psychological Bulletin 128(2): 243–249. http://feweb.uvt.nl/center/hofstede/VSM.
Elfenbein, H.A. and Ambady, N. (2002b) ‘On html (accessed 9 February 2008).
the Universality and Cultural Specificity Hofstede, G. (2001) Culture’s Consequences (2nd
of Emotion Recognition: A Meta-analysis’, edn). Los Angeles, CA: Sage.
Psychological Bulletin 128(2): 203–235. House, R.J., Hanges, P.J., Javidan, M., Dorfman,
Elfenbein, H.A. and Ambady, N. (2003) ‘When P.W. and Gupta, V. (2004) Leadership, Culture,
Familiarity Breeds Accuracy: Cultural and Organizations: The GLOBE Study of 62
Exposure and Facial Emotion Recognition’, Societies. Los Angeles, CA: Sage.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 85(2): Joreskog, K.G. and Sorbom, D. (1989) LISREL
276–290. 7: User’s Reference Guide. Mooresville, IN:
Friesen, W.V. (1972). ‘Cultural Difference in Scientific Software.
Facial Expressions in a Social Situation: An Law, K.S., Wong, C.S. and Song, L.J. (2004)
Experimental Test of the Concept of Display ‘The Construct and Criterion Validity of
Rules’, Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Emotional Intelligence and its Potential
University of California, San Francisco. Utility for Management Studies’, Journal of
Gabel, R.S., Dolan, S.L. and Cerdin, J.L. (2005) Applied Psychology 89(3): 483–496.
‘Emotional Intelligence as a Predictor of Lopes, P.N., Salovey, P. and Strauss, R. (2003)
Cultural Adjustment for Success in Global ‘Emotional Intelligence, Personality and the
Assignments’, Career Development International Perceived Quality of Social Relationships’,
10(5): 359–374. Personality and Individual Differences 35(3):
Gangopadhyay, M. and Mandal, M.K. (2008) 641–658.

Downloaded from ccm.sagepub.com at University of New England on June 6, 2015


Sharma et al.: Emotional Intelligence across Cultures 233

Mandal, M.K., Bryden, M.P. and Bulman- (MSCEIT), Version 2.0. Toronto, Canada:
Fleming, M.B. (1996) ‘Similarities and Multi-Health Systems.
Variations in Facial Expressions of Emotions: Mayer, J.D., Salovey, P. and Caruso, D. (2004)
Cross-cultural Evidence’, International Journal ‘Emotional Intelligence: Theory, Findings,
of Neuroscience 31(1): 49–58. and Implications’, Psychological Inquiry 15(3):
Mandal, M.K., Harizuka, S., Bhushan, B. and 197–215.
Mishra, R.C. (2001) ‘Cultural Variation McCrae, R.R. (2000) ‘EI from the Perspective of
in Hemi Facial Asymmetry of Emotional the Five-factor Model of Personality’, in R.
Expressions’, British Journal of Social Psychology Bar-On and J.D.A. Parker (eds), Handbook of
40(3): 385–398. EI, pp. 263–276. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-
Markus, H. and Kitayama, S. (1991) ‘Culture Bass.
and the Self: Implications for Cognition, Mesquita, B., Frijda, N.H. and Scherer,
Emotion, and Motivation’, Psychological Review K.R.(1997) ‘Culture and Emotion’, in J.E.
98(2): 224–253. Berry, P.B. Daisen, and T.S. Saraswathi (eds),
Markus, H.R. and Kitayama, S. (1994) ‘The Handbook of Cross-cultural Psychology: Vol. 2.
Cultural Construction of Self and Emotion: Basic Processes and Developmental Psychology, pp.
Implications for Social Behaviour’, in S. 255–297. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Kitayama, and H.R. Markus (eds), Emotion Newsome, S., Day, A.L. and Catano, V.M. (2000)
and Culture: Empirical Studies of Mutual Influence, ‘Assessing the Predictive Validity of Emotional
pp. 89–130. Washington, DC: American Intelligence’, Personality and Individual Differences
Psychological Association. 29(6): 1005–1016.
Matsumoto, D. (1989) ‘Cultural Influences on Pant, N. and Prakash, A. (2004) ‘Multi-factor
the Perception of Emotion’, Journal of Cross EI Scale (MEIS) in India: An Evaluation’,
Cultural Psychology 20(1): 92–105. Psychological Studies 49(1): 142–146.
Matsumoto, D. (1993) ‘Ethnic Differences in Parker, J.D.A., Saklofske, D.H., Shaughnessy,
Affect Intensity, Emotion Judgments, Display P.A., Huang, S.H.S., Wood, L.M. and
Rule Attitudes, and Self-reported Emotional Eastabrook, J.M. (2005) ‘Generalizability
Expression in an American Sample’, of the Emotional Intelligence Construct:
Motivation and Emotion 17(2): 107–123. A Cross-cultural Study of North American
Matsumoto, D. (2002) ‘Methodological Aboriginal Youth’, Personality and Individual
Requirements to Test a Possible In-Group Differences 39(1): 215–227.
Advantage in Judging Emotions Across Perez, J.C., Petrides, K.V. and Furnham, A.
Cultures: Comments on Elfenbein and (2005) ‘Measuring Trait EI’, in R. Schulze
Ambady and Evidence’, Psychological Bulletin and R.D. Roberts (eds), EI –An International
128(2): 236–242. Handbook, pp. 181– 201.Cambridge, MA:
Matsumoto, D. et al. (2008) ‘Mapping Expressive Hogrefe and Huber Publishers.
Differences Around the World: The Petrides, K.V. and Furnham, A. (2000) ‘On
Relationship Between Emotional Display the Dimensional Structure of Emotional
Rules and Individualism Versus Collectivism’, Intelligence’, Personality and Individual Differences
Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology 39(1): 55–74. 29(2): 313–320.
Mayer, J.D., Caruso, D.R. and Salovey, P. (1999) Prabhupada, A.C.B.S. (1986) Bhagavad Gita as it
‘Emotional Intelligence Meets Traditional is. Mumbai: The Bhaktivedanta Book Trust.
Standards for Emotional Intelligence’, R- Project (1994) ‘R Statistic- version 2.6’,
Intelligence 27(4): 267–298. http://www.r-project.org/ (accessed 24
Mayer, J.D. and Salovey, P. (1997) ‘What is December 2007).
Emotional Intelligence?’, in P. Salovey, and Saklofske, D.H., Austin, E.J. and Minski, P.S.
D. Sluyter (Eds.), Emotional Development and (2003) ‘Factor Structure and Validity of
Emotional Intelligence: Educational Implications, pp. a Trait Emotional Intelligence Measure’,
3–31. New York: Basic Books. Personality and Individual Differences 34(4):
Mayer, J.D., Salovey, P. and Caruso, D. (2000) 707–721.
‘Models of Emotional Intelligence’, in R.J. Salacuse, J. (1998) ‘Ten Ways That Culture
Sternberg (ed.), Handbook of Intelligence, pp. Affects Negotiating Style: Some Survey
396–420. New York: Cambridge University Results’, Negotiation Journal 14(3): 221–235.
Press. Salovey, P. and Mayer, J.D. (1990) ‘Emotional
Mayer, J.D., Salovey, P. and Caruso, D. (2002) Intelligence’, Imagination, Cognition and
Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test Personality 9(3): 185–211.

Downloaded from ccm.sagepub.com at University of New England on June 6, 2015


234   International Journal of Cross Cultural Management 9(2)

Schermerhorn, J.R. and Bond, M.H. (1997) SUDEEP SHARMA is in the Department
‘Cross-cultural Leadership Dynamics in of Organizational Behavior, Olin Business
Collectivism and High Power Distance School, Washington University in St. Louis,
Settings’, Leadership and Organizational One Brookings Drive, St. Louis, Missouri,
Development Journal 18(4): 187–202. United States, 63130. He recently moved from
Schutte, N.S., Malouff, J.M., Hall, L.E., Department of Employment Relations and
Haggerty, D.J., Cooper, J.T., Golden, C.J. Organizational Behavior, the London School
and Dornheim, L. (1998) ‘Development of Economics and Politicial Scienes, Houghton
and Validation of a Measure of Emotional Street, London, WC2A 2AE, United Kingdom.
Intelligence’, Personality and Individual Differences [email:sudeep.sharma@alumni.lse.ac.uk]
25(2): 167–177.
Schwartz, S.H. (1994) ‘Cultural Dimensions JÜRGEN DELLER is founder of the Institute
of Values: Toward an Understanding of for Strategic HR Management Research and
National Differences’, in U. Kam, H.C. Development (SMARD), Leuphana University of
Triandis, C. Kagitcibasi, S.C. Choi, and Lüneburg, Wilschenbrucher Weg 84a, D-21335,
G. Yoon (eds), Individualism and Collectivism: Lüneburg, Germany. He is also visiting professor
Theory, Method, and Application, pp. 85–119. Los in Lucas Graduate School of Business, San José
Angeles, CA: Sage. State University, USA.
Sibia, A., Srivastava, A.K. and Misra G. (2003) [email:deller@leuphana.de]
‘Emotional Intelligence: Western and Indian
perspectives, Indian Psychological Abstracts and RAMAKRISHNA BISWAL is in the
Reviews 10(1): 3–41. Department of Psychology, Delhi University,
Shweder, R. and Much, N. (1987) North Campus, Near Mall Road, Delhi, India,
‘Determinations of Meaning: Discourse and 110007. [email:rkbpsych@gmail.com]
Moral Socialization’, in W. Kurtines, and J.
Gewirtz (eds), Moral Development Through Social Manas K. Mandal is Director in the
Interaction, pp.197–244. New York: Wiley. Defence Institute of Psychological Research,
Spielberger, C. (ed.) (2004) Encyclopedia of Applied Defence Research and Development Organiza­
Psychology. Academic Press. tion, Lucknow Road, Delhi-110054, India.
Tanaka, J.S. (1993) ‘Multifaceted Conceptions of [email address: mkmdipr@yahoo.co.in]
Fit in Structural Equation Models’, in K.A.
Bollen and J.S. Long (eds), Testing Structural Please address correspondence to Manas K.
Equation Models, pp. 10–39. Los Angeles, CA: Mandal.
Sage.
Thomas, D.C. (2006) ‘Domain and Development
of Cultural Intelligence: The Importance of Appendix 1
Mindfulness’, Group Organization Management
31(1): 78–99. The 33 item German version of Schutte
Triandis, H.C. (1989) ‘The Self and Social Emotional Intelligence Scale.
Behaviour in Differing Cultural Contexts’,
Psychological Review 96(3): 506–520.   1. Ich weiß, wann ich mit anderen über
Triandis, H.C. (1995) Individualism and Collectivism. meine persönlichen Probleme sprechen
Boulder, CO: Westview Press. kann.
Tucker, L.R. and Lewis, C. (1973) ‘A Reliability   2. Wenn ich in schwierigen Situationen
Coefficient for Maximum Likelihood Factor
Analysis’, Psychometrika 38(1): 1–10. bin, erinnere ich mich an ähnliche
Van Hemert, D.A., Poortinga, Y.H. and Van de Situationen und daran, wie ich sie
Vijver, F.J.R. (2007) ‘Emotion and Culture: bewältigt habe.
A Meta-analysis’, Cognition and Emotion 21(5):   3. Ich erwarte, daß ich die meisten Dinge,
913–943. die ich versuche, gut mache.
Van Rooy, D.L. and Viswesvaran, C. (2004)
‘Emotional Intelligence: A Meta-Analytic   4. Andere Personen finden es leicht, mir zu
Investigation of Predictive Validity and vertrauen.
Nomological Net’, Journal of Vocational   5. Ich finde es schwierig, die non-verbalen
Behaviour 65(1): 71–95. Botschaften anderer Personen zu
verstehen.

Downloaded from ccm.sagepub.com at University of New England on June 6, 2015


Sharma et al.: Emotional Intelligence across Cultures 235

  6. Einige bedeutsame Ereignisse in meinem spüre, neige ich zur Entwicklung neuer
Leben haben mich dazu geführt, neu zu Ideen.
bewerten, was wichtig oder unwichtig ist. 28. Wenn ich vor einer Herausforderung
  7. Wenn sich meine Stimmung ändert, sehe stehe, gebe ich auf, weil ich glaube, daß
ich neue Möglichkeiten. ich scheitern werde.
  8. Gefühle gehören zu den Dingen, die 29. Ich weiß, was andere Personen fühlen,
mein Leben lebenswert machen. wenn ich sie nur ansehe.
  9. Ich bin mir meiner Gefühle bewusst, 30. Ich helfe anderen Personen, sich besser
wenn ich sie erlebe. zu fühlen, wenn sie niedergeschlagen
10. Ich erwarte, daß gute Dinge passieren. sind.
11. Ich teile meine Gefühle gerne mit 31. Ich nutze gute Stimmung, um
anderen. mich angesichts von Hindernissen
12. Wenn ich ein positives Gefühl erlebe, anzuspornen.
weiß ich, was ich tun muss, damit es 32. Ich kann sagen, wie Personen sich
andauert. fühlen, wenn ich den Tonfall ihrer
13. Ich plane Ereignisse, die anderen Freude Stimme höre.
bereiten. 33. Es ist schwierig für mich zu verstehen,
14. Ich finde Aktivitäten heraus, die mich warum Personen so fühlen wie sie fühlen.
glücklich machen.
15. Ich bin mir der non-verbalen Botschaften
bewusst, die ich anderen sende. Appendix 2
16. Ich präsentiere mich auf eine Art, Shlok 1
die bei anderen einen guten Eindruck duhkhesv anudvigna-manah
hinterlässt. sukhesu vigata-sprhah
17. Wenn ich in guter Stimmung bin, fällt es vita-raga-bhaya-krodhah
mir leicht, Probleme zu lösen. sthita-dhir munir ucyate
18. Ich erkenne die Gefühle anderer an (The Bhagavad Gita, 2; 56)
ihren Gesichtsausdrücken.
19. Ich weiß, weshalb sich meine Gefühle Translation – One who is not disturbed in
ändern. mind even amidst the threefold miseries or
20. Wenn ich in guter Stimmung bin, kann elated when there is happiness, and who is
ich neue Ideen entwickeln. free from attachment, fear and anger, is called
21. Ich habe meine Gefühle unter Kontrolle. a sage of steady mind (Swami Prabhupada,
22. Ich erkenne meine Gefühle leicht, wenn 1986).
ich sie erlebe.
Shlok 2
23. Ich motiviere mich dadurch, daß ich
krodhad bhavati sammohah
mir ein positives Ergebnis der Aufgaben sammohat smrti-vibhramah
vorstelle, die ich angehe. smrti-bhramsad buddhi-naso
24. Ich lobe andere, wenn sie etwas gut buddhi-nasat pranasyati
gemacht haben. (The Bhagavad Gita, 2; 63)
25. Ich bin mir der non-verbalen
Botschaften anderer Personen bewusst. Translation – From anger, complete delusion
26. Wenn mir eine andere Person von einem arises, and from delusion bewilderment of
wichtigen Ereignis in ihrem Leben memory. When memory is bewildered, intel-
berichtet, fühle ich mich fast, als hätte ligence is lost, and intelligence is lost one falls
ich es selbst erlebt. down again into the material pool (Swami
27. Wenn ich eine emotionale Veränderung Prabhupada, 1986).

Downloaded from ccm.sagepub.com at University of New England on June 6, 2015


236   International Journal of Cross Cultural Management 9(2)

Résumé
L’intelligence émotionnelle : structure factorielle et validité dans diverses
cultures (Sudeep Sharma, Juergen Deller, Ramakrishna Biswal et Manas K.
Mandal)
Cette étude est un examen transculturel de l’intelligence émotionnelle (IE). Les participants
(N = 200) d’Allemagne (N = 100) et d’Inde (N = 100) ont complété l’échelle d’intelligence
émotionnelle, NEO- Five Factor Personality Inventory et le module de valeur de la surveillance
de 1994 (Hofstede’s Value Survey – Module 94). L’étude avait pour objectifs majeurs
d’examiner de façon transculturelle (i) la structure des facteurs de l’IE et (ii) le construct de
l’IE comme étant distinct de la personnalité. Les résultats ont révélé différentes structures
factorielles pour ces deux cultures, tandis que le construct de l’IE n’a pas pu être corrélé aux
dimensions de la personnalité. Les implications de ces résultats pour le leadership dans une
culture spécifique et pour des cultures différentes sont examinées.

Sudeep Sharma, Juergen Deller, Ramakrishna Biswal and Manas K. Mandal

Downloaded from ccm.sagepub.com at University of New England on June 6, 2015

You might also like