You are on page 1of 10

Adv Physiol Educ 34: 25-34, 2010;

doi:10.1152/advan.00002.2010. Using Classic Papers To Teach Physiology

Thermoreception and nociception of the skin: a classic paper of Bessou and


Perl and analyses of thermal sensitivity during a student laboratory exercise
Johann P. Kuhtz-Buschbeck,1 Wiebke Andresen,1 Stephan Göbel,1 René Gilster,1 and Carsten Stick2
1
Physiologisches Institut and 2Medizinische Klimatologie, Universität Kiel, Kiel, Germany
Submitted 6 January 2010; accepted in final form 8 March 2010

Kuhtz-Buschbeck JP, Andresen W, Göbel S, Gilster R, Stick potentials were recorded while the skin was stimulated using
C. Thermoreception and nociception of the skin: a classic paper of various methods: graded pressure (von Frey hairs), light touch,
Bessou and Perl and analyses of thermal sensitivity during a puncture, application of acid, cooling, and heating. In this way,
student laboratory exercise. Adv Physiol Educ 34: 25–34, 2010; the receptive characteristics of 131 unmyelinated fibers were
doi:10.1152/advan.00002.2010.—About four decades ago, Perl
examined. Thirty percent of these fibers belonged to high-
and collaborators were the first ones who unambiguously identified
specifically nociceptive neurons in the periphery. In their classic threshold receptors, which were activated by intense mechan-
work, they recorded action potentials from single C-fibers of a ical stimuli, irritant chemicals, and heat, with thresholds be-
cutaneous nerve in cats while applying carefully graded stimuli to tween 42 and 56°C. Since noxious stimulation was the common
the skin (Bessou P, Perl ER. Response of cutaneous sensory units denominator, they were designated “polymodal nociceptors.”
with unmyelinated fibers to noxious stimuli. J Neurophysiol 32: Two original graphs from the classic paper (2) show their re-
1025–1043, 1969). They discovered polymodal nociceptors, which sponses to local heating of the skin with a thermode (Fig. 1). Some
responded to mechanical, thermal, and chemical stimuli in the of these nociceptors were also weakly excited by very low
noxious range, and differentiated them from low-threshold ther- temperatures (ice placed on the skin). Further high-threshold
moreceptors. Their classic findings form the basis of the present unmyelinated afferent units were classified as mechanoreceptors
method that undergraduate medical students experience during and subcutaneous receptors. Low-threshold sensory units com-
laboratory exercises of sensory physiology, namely, quantitative
testing of the thermal detection and pain thresholds. This diagnos-
prised other mechanoreceptors and thermoreceptors. The latter
tic method examines the function of thin afferent nerve fibers. We had some background activity at normal ambient skin temperature
collected data from nearly 300 students that showed that 1) women (30 –34°C) and were highly responsive to slight thermal changes
are more sensitive to thermal detection and thermal pain at the (⬍2°C). Lightly myelinated afferent fibers (A␦-fibers) responding
thenar than men, 2) habituation shifts thermal pain thresholds to noxious mechanical stimuli had been described in an earlier
during repetititve testing, 3) the cold pain threshold is rather publication from the same research group (4).
variable and lower when tested after heat pain than in the reverse Later experiments with human volunteers used microneu-
case (order effect), and 4) ratings of pain intensity on a visual rography to record action potentials from single C-fibers in-
analog scale are correlated with the threshold temperature for heat nervating the hairy skin of the leg and foot while heat stimuli
pain but not for cold pain. Median group results could be repro- were applied to the receptive fields of these fibers (45). The
duced in a retest. Quantitative sensory testing of thermal thresholds
magnitude of the evoked pain sensation, as rated by the
is feasible and instructive in the setting of a laboratory exercise and
is appreciated by the students as a relevant and interesting tech- volunteers, was closely related to the discharge rate of poly-
nique. modal mechano-heat-sensitive C-fibers. Moreover, C-fibers
innervating the glabrous and hairy skin of the hand and forearm
thermal detection thresholds; thermal pain thresholds; pain ratings; were selectively excited by intraneural microstimulation in
reproducibility; quantitative sensory testing awake human subjects (32). This evoked sensations of dull or
burning pain that were accurately projected to the receptive
THE QUESTION OF what the role of slowly conducting afferents is fields of the respective fibers. These findings indicate that
was answered four decades ago by Perl and colleagues, who polymodal nociceptors of the skin, as originally described in
proved, for the first time, that a distinctive set of primary cats in the aforementioned classic paper (2), have similar
afferent units codes for painful stimuli in the periphery. Today, characteristics in humans as well.
it is basic physiological knowledge that A␦-fiber nociceptors, Nowadays, the function of thermosensitive cutaneous A␦-
excited by strong mechanical stimuli, and polymodal C-fiber and C-fibers can be examined in humans by measuring the
nociceptors are the principal afferents signaling cutaneous pain thermal pain thresholds and detection thresholds for tempera-
(30). The discovery of these nociceptors is described in the ture change with a computer-controlled thermode (40, 41).
classic paper of Bessou and Perl (2). In anesthetized cats, they Elevated thresholds indicate a loss or dysfunction of thin nerve
exposed the posterior femoral nerve and prepared very thin fibers, e.g., in patients with neuropathic pain (18). Undergrad-
filaments by dissection under the microscope. Afferent fibers uate medical students (first year) encounter this diagnostic
with a conduction velocity under 2.2 m/s (C-fibers) were method in our laboratory exercise in physiology. The right
identified by electrical nerve stimulation. The cutaneous recep- thenar eminence serves as a test region that is convenient to
tive fields of single afferent units were identified and action examine during a teaching exercise. The thermal detection and
pain thresholds are determined, and the students rate the
perceived intensity of pain on a visual analog scale (VAS).
Address for reprint requests and other correspondence: J. P. Kuhtz-Busch-
Data from 287 medical students have been collected since
beck, Institute of Physiology, Christian-Albrechts-Universität Kiel, Olshausen- 2005. The results are summarized in the present article and
strasse 40, Kiel D 24098, Germany (e-mail: kuhtz@physiologie.uni-kiel.de). may serve as a database for teaching purposes. We evaluated
1043-4046/10 Copyright © 2010 The American Physiological Society 25
Downloaded from journals.physiology.org/journal/advances (183.082.157.055) on December 31, 2020.
Using Classic Papers To Teach Physiology
26 THERMORECEPTION AND NOCICEPTION

Fig. 1. Original data of the classic paper by


Bessou and Perl (2), adapted from their Figs. 2 and
5. A: responses recorded from an afferent fiber
(conduction velocity: 1 m/s) of a polymodal noci-
ceptor of a cat. First heating of the skin was above
40°C. B: 10 s after A; now heating from 45 to
50°C. C: instantaneous discharge frequency (F) of
a polymodal nociceptor during skin heating over a
period of 150 s, with a parallel record of the
corresponding skin temperature (bottom curve).

sex-related differences, effects of habituation during repeated a change of the temperature (“Press the response button immediately
testing of the pain thresholds, correlations between the thresh- when you perceive a change of the thermode temperature, i.e., warmer
old temperatures and corresponding ratings of pain intensity, or colder.”). In each subject, CDT and WDT were each averaged from
and effects of the sequence of stimuli. Retest reliability was five consecutive trials delivered at 7- to 10-s intervals. Thereafter,
CPT and HPT were determined with ramp stimuli (1.5°C/s). Students
examined in a subgroup of volunteers. The results of the were now explicitly instructed to press the button as soon as the heat
student collective are discussed in the context of the aforemen- or cold sensation became painful (“Press the stop button at the first
tioned classic paper (2). painful sensation! The temperature increase/decrease will then stop
immediately.”). We emphasized that it was not the goal to examine
METHODS pain tolerance. The lower temperature limit of the thermode was 0°C
The thermal sensitivity of the right thenar was examined in 287 due to technical limitations, and the upper limit was set to 53°C for
medical students (170 women and 117 men) during a laboratory safety reasons. Again, five consecutive trials were performed per pain
exercise in physiology. We always assessed the cold and warm threshold, and mean values of the CPT and HPT were calculated. This
detection thresholds (WDT and CDT, respectively) first and then the approach and the rates of the thermal ramps were chosen in line with
cold and heat pain thresholds (CPT and HPT, respectively). Students previously published test protocols (1, 41). Immediately after the
were divided into three groups: groups A, B, and C. Group A consisted respective fifth trial, students rated the perceived intensity of pain on
of 141 subjects (90 women and 51 men, age: 22.9 ⫾ 4.7 yr, mean ⫾ a 101-point VAS. The VAS was a ruler with a slide, which could be
SD), whose thermal thresholds were examined in the following adjusted between “no pain” (0) and the “worst imaginable pain” (100).
sequence: CDT-WDT-CPT-HPT. Group B consisted of 124 subjects To minimize distraction, students wore ear muffs during the entire
(69 women and 55 men, age: 23.1 ⫾ 3.5 yr), who were tested in the testing session, which lasted ⬃5 min. They could not watch the
following sequence: WDT-CDT-HPT-CPT, i.e., warm stimuli pre- computer screen that indicated the current results. Experienced staff
ceded cold stimuli in this group. Group C consisted of 22 subjects (11 members (S. Göbel, W. Andresen, and R. Gilster) operated the
women and 11 men, age: 21.8 ⫾ 2.6 yr), who were examined with thermal sensory analyzer (TSA 2001, Medoc) and gave the instruc-
both test sequences at an interval of ⬃30 min. tions.
Classes of ⬃20 students took part in the laboratory exercise, which Nonparametric statistical methods were applied because the detec-
took place in one room during afternoon hours. They were informed tion thresholds and CPT were not distributed normally. We deter-
that data of normal thermal sensitivity were to be gathered from mined median values (50th percentile), interquartile ranges (25–75th
healthy subjects. About five students per class volunteered to undergo percentile), and normal ranges (95% reference interval), which was
the noninvasive tests; the others performed different tasks (e.g., defined as the interval between the 2.5th and 97.5th empirical per-
vibration thresholds and tactile acuity). Volunteers gave their in- centiles (52). Data from men and women were contrasted with
formed consent before the start of the experiments, which were Mann-Whitney U-tests. The dynamics of the five consecutive CPT
approved by the Ethical Committee of the Medical Faculty of Kiel and HPT measurements were evaluated with nonparametric ANOVA
University (Kiel, Germany). for repeated measures (Friedman ANOVA) followed by Wilcoxon
The student was seated at a table in a corner of the room, with the signed-rank tests. Relations between different measures were de-
right forearm comfortably resting on a padded splint. Before the test scribed with Spearman’s rank correlation coefficients (␳). Possible
started, the initial skin surface temperature of the right thenar was effects of the test sequence (cold-warm vs. warm-cold) were evaluated
measured with a thermistor (Testo 720 Pt 100, Lenzkirch, Germany) by comparing the results of groups A and B with Mann-Whitney
in group A. With the hand in a prone position, the right thenar was U-tests. Sequence effects in group C (same subjects examined with
then placed on a computer-controlled Peltier-type thermode (size of both test sequences) were evaluated with Wilcoxon tests. To assess
contact surface: 3 ⫻ 3 cm2) of a thermal sensory analyzer (TSA 2001, retest reliability, 33 students from group A (20 women and 13 men)
Medoc). Testing started after an adaptation period of ⬃1 min. A were reexamined one by one in a climatic chamber (22°C, relative
device with a response button was held in the left hand. humidity: 50%) ⬃1 wk after the laboratory exercise. The test and
All tests started from a “thermal-neutral” (indifferent) baseline retest results were compared with Wilcoxon tests, and correlation
thermode temperature of 32°C. Series of thermal stimuli that in- coefficients were calculated. The software package SPSS (version 15)
creased or decreased from baseline were applied. The response button was used for statistical computations.
was pressed with the left hand when the respective thermal sensation RESULTS
was perceived, i.e., with the Method of Limits (54). This stopped the
heating up/cooling down. The respective temperature was recorded, Thermal Detection and Pain Thresholds
and the thermode was returned to baseline (32°C). CDT and WDT
were examined first, using ramp stimuli with a velocity of 1°C/s. The results from group A are shown in Table 1, and the
Students were instructed to press the button as soon as they detected corresponding data of women and men are contrasted in Fig. 2.

Advances in Physiology Education • VOL 34 • JUNE 2010


Downloaded from journals.physiology.org/journal/advances (183.082.157.055) on December 31, 2020.
Using Classic Papers To Teach Physiology
THERMORECEPTION AND NOCICEPTION 27

Table 1. Thermal detection and pain thresholds in group A difference between the first and last trial was ⬃4°C for the
HPT and ⬃3°C for the CPT.
Percentile

Parameter Median 2.5% 25% 75% 97.5% Minimum Maximum Ratings of Pain Intensity on the VAS
CDT, °C 30.8 28.9 30.4 31.1 31.5 27.0 31.5 The ratings of heat pain intensity increased with the temper-
WDT, °C 33.5 32.9 33.2 33.9 35.3 32.7 36.2 ature of the HPT (Fig. 5A). No such correlation was found for
CPT, °C 9.0 0.8 4.9 15.2 22.7 0.0 25.4
HPT, °C 46.4 37.8 43.5 48.1 52.3 37.0 52.9 the ratings of cold pain intensity, which were rather variable
VAS and independent from the temperature of the CPT (Fig. 5B).
CPT 27.0 5.0 20.0 38.0 53.0 0.0 80.0 However, the ratings of cold and heat pain intensity were
HPT 37.0 10.0 27.0 50 77.0 0.0 80.0 positively correlated (␳ ⫽ 0.68, P ⬍ 0.01), so that students who
Skin temperature, °C 32.2 29.3 31.3 33.0 34.4 28.0 34.3
indicated the intensity of heat pain to be high tended to give
n ⫽ 141 students in group A. Shown are median values, percentiles, and high ratings of cold pain intensity as well.
minimum and maximum values. Results are combined data of women and
men. VAS, pain rating on visual analog scale (where no pain ⫽ 0 and the Effects of the Test Sequence
“worst imaginable pain” ⫽ 100). CDT, cold detection threshold; WDT, warm
detection threshold; CPT, cold pain threshold; HPT, heat pain threshold. The A comparison of the results of group A (Table 1) and group
order of testing was CDT-WDT-CPT-HPT in group A.
B (Table 2) suggested that the order of stimuli influences the
perception of cold pain. Students in group B, who were tested
Women detected temperature changes more sensitively than in the sequence WDT-CDT-HPT-CPT, had a significantly
men, as reflected by significant differences (P ⬍ 0.01) of the lower CPT (P ⬍ 0.01) than the members of group A (sequence
WDT and CDT. In addition, the HPT was lower in female CDT-WDT-CPT-HPT). Moreover, heat pain was rated to be
students than in male students (P ⬍ 0.05), whereas the CPT more intense (P ⬍ 0.05) by group B than by group A. The
and ratings of pain intensity did not significantly differ. Both results of group C confirmed the effect of the order of stimuli
women and men rated the intensity of heat pain stronger (P ⬍ on the CPT because the median CPT of 9.5°C (test sequence
0.01) than cold pain intensity. The HPT was less variable than HPT-CPT) was significantly lower than the median value
the CPT, whose normal range covered a span of ⬃20°C. (10.3°C) that was obtained with the reverse sequence (CPT-
A significant correlation (P ⬍ 0.01) existed between the HPT) in the same subjects (P ⬍ 0.05 by Wilcoxon test). The
HPT and CPT (Fig. 3), i.e., volunteers who did not perceive thermal detection thresholds and HPT were not influenced by
cold pain until the thermode reached very low temperatures the order of stimuli. Sex-related differences in group B were
had high (insensitive) thresholds of heat pain as well. Simi- similar to those described for group A (see Fig. 2).
larly, CDT and WDT were correlated. However, we found no
significant correlation between the thermal detection and pain Retest Reliability
thresholds. In addition, the initial skin temperature (measured The setting of a laboratory exercise includes the presence of
before testing) was not significantly correlated with any of the classmates, social interactions, and noise in a room where other
thresholds. tasks are performed at the same time. Distraction may be a
Short-Term Changes of Thermal Pain Thresholds relevant factor, which conceivably can affect data that are
obtained with a reaction time-dependent method. Furthermore,
Both thermal pain thresholds shifted gradually over the five the ambient room temperature and humidity could not be kept
consecutive trials (P ⬍ 0.05 by ANOVA), i.e., the response perfectly constant. To assess reproducibility, 33 students from
button that signaled the first painful sensation was pressed later group A were retested one by one in a quiet climatic chamber
from trial to trial (Fig. 4). Significant differences between (22°C, relative humidity: 50%) by the same examiner. Their
successive trials were found up to the fifth measurement of the thermal detection and pain thresholds did not differ signifi-
HPT and up to the fourth measurement of the CPT. The median cantly from the previous laboratory exercise (not significant by

Fig. 2. Warm and cold detection thresholds


(WDT and CDT, respectively) and heat and cold
pain thresholds (HPT and CPT, respectively) as
well as subjective ratings of pain intensity on a
visual analog scale (VAS). Box plots show me-
dians, quartiles, and minimum and maximum
values. Significant differences between women
and men are shown (*P ⬍ 0.05 and **P ⬍ 0.01
by Mann-Whitney U-tests). Data of the labora-
tory exercise (group A, n ⫽ 141 students) were
measured at the right thenar.

Advances in Physiology Education • VOL 34 • JUNE 2010


Downloaded from journals.physiology.org/journal/advances (183.082.157.055) on December 31, 2020.
Using Classic Papers To Teach Physiology
28 THERMORECEPTION AND NOCICEPTION

Fig. 3. Correlations. HPT and CPT were cor-


related, as were WDT and CDT. Spearman’s
rank correlation coefficients (␳) were significant
(P ⬍ 0.01). Each symbol indicates the results of
one student. The solid lines are regression lines;
the dashed lines indicate normal ranges (2.5th
and 97.5th percentiles). Data are from group A
(laboratory exercise).

Wilcoxon tests), and correlation coefficients between test and resting on the thermode is comfortable, and no straps are
retest results ranged between 0.4 and 0.7 (Table 3). necessary to fix the thermode. Regardless of the body region,
the detection and evaluation of a physical stimulus (e.g.,
DISCUSSION warmth) is a subjective experience. Especially, the perception
of pain varies from person to person and must be distinguished
The classic study of Bessou and Perl (2) characterized from the objective neural consequences of a noxious stimulus
polymodal nociceptors in cats by measuring the discharge rates as analyzed by Bessou and Perl, such as receptor potentials and
of single C-fibers (see Fig. 1) in response to thermal, mechan- discharge rates of afferent fibers. Psychophysical sensory
ical, and chemical stimulation of the skin. It is important for the thresholds are not fixed but can be influenced by practice,
students to understand that diagnostic determination of the
thermal detection and pain thresholds in humans (discovery
learning) not only involves peripheral receptors but the entire
afferent pathway: nociceptors and low-threshold thermorecep-
tors, their afferent C- and A␦-fibers, the synaptic relay in the
dorsal horn of grey matter of the spinal cord, the spinothalamic
pathway, the thalamus, and cortical regions engaged in the
processing and evaluation of thermal sensations and pain
(somatosensory, cingulate, and insular cortex).
As a further difference, Bessou and Perl (2) studied the hairy
skin of the cat hindlimb, whereas the glabrous skin of the
thenar was examined during the laboratory exercise. Potential
differences between hairy and glabrous skin with regard to
thermal sensitivity and nociception are an issue of current
research. Harrison and Davis (14) found that CDT and cold-
induced pain thresholds were lower (i.e., occurred at higher
absolute temperatures) for the hairy skin of the dorsal hand
compared with the thenar. Other researchers have reported that
the human palm has a higher HPT than the hairy skin of the
dorsal hand (9). A very recent study (7) varied the size of the
stimulated area and concluded that spatial summation depends
on skin type (more summation in glabrous skin) and on skin
sensitivity. However, Iannetti and colleagues (17) found that
laser heat stimulation of the dorsum as well as palm of the hand
elicited remarkably similar EEG brain responses (laser-evoked
potentials) with very similar psychophysical ratings of pain
intensity. They inferred that nociceptive afferents with compa-
rable properties mediate the first pain to heat stimulation of the
glabrous and hairy skin in humans. Besides the type of skin, the
body region seems to be relevant. Hagander et al. (13) found
significantly lower CDT and WDT at the dorsum of the hand
Fig. 4. Short-term habituation of thermal pain thresholds. Changes from the first to the
than at the dorsum of the foot. fifth measurement for the CPT and HPT are shown. Significant differences between
For a laboratory exercise, the thenar is a very convenient test successive measurements are shown (*P ⬍ 0.05 and **P ⬍ 0.01 by Wilcoxon tests
region. Students do not have to undress, the posture of the hand after a Bonferroni correction). Box plots are as in Fig. 2. ns, Not significant.

Advances in Physiology Education • VOL 34 • JUNE 2010


Downloaded from journals.physiology.org/journal/advances (183.082.157.055) on December 31, 2020.
Using Classic Papers To Teach Physiology
THERMORECEPTION AND NOCICEPTION 29

Fig. 5. Correlation between pain intensity and thermal pain thresholds. A: relation between ratings for heat pain intensity (vertical axis) and temperature of the
HPT. B: relation between ratings for cold pain intensity and temperature of the CPT. Pain intensity was rated on a VAS. Each symbol shows the results from
one student. Data are from group A (laboratory exercise).

fatigue, and reaction time. Diurnal and situational variations of ences in endogenous pain modulation (22, 37, 43, 51). Differ-
the pain threshold have an influence (23); descending pain ences in the central processing of noxious input have been
control systems may, e.g., reduce sensitivity during competi- reported in a recent neuroimaging study (15), where cutaneous
tive sports. pain evoked stronger activity of the midcingulate cortex, dor-
solateral prefrontal cortex, and hippocampus in women than
Teaching Points men. A meta-analysis of published articles (39) showed that
The present results, which were collected over several years, sex differences in response to noxious experimental stimuli are
lead to the following five questions that can be discussed with relatively small so that large sample sizes (n ⬎ 40) are
the participating students after the laboratory exercise. necessary to detect them. The skin temperature at the thenar,
Question 1. What can explain differences in thermal sensi- which had been measured before the hand was placed on the
tivity between women and men (Fig. 2)? Female students thermode, was lower in women than in men (Table 1). Al-
detected cold and warm stimuli more sensitively than male though a lower temperature reduces the nerve conduction
students and had a lower HPT. Nine previous studies, con- velocity (33), we found no significant influence of the skin
ducted in research laboratories, have reported similar differ- temperature on the thermal detection and pain thresholds,
ences (see Table 4). Women have a thinner epidermis at the probably because the effects were too small (48). In addition,
palm and fingers than men (27), which may result in better Hagander and collaborators (13) reported that WDT and CDT
peripheral temperature conductance and, therefore, a stronger are independent of the local skin temperature.
afferent input at the peripheral level. In the present study, Question 2. What can explain the correlations of the WDT
higher alertness of female students may have influenced the and CDT and of HPT and CPT (Fig. 3)? The detection of small
results of the reaction time-dependent method as well. The changes in skin temperature depends crucially on the function
increased heat pain sensitivity (lower HPT) of the women of low-threshold thermoreceptors. Bessou and Perl (2) de-
could furthermore be due a higher readiness of women to scribed sensory units that responded to mild cooling of the skin
report pain, gender role expectations, and sex-related differ- and had the greatest tonic discharge between 23 and 32°C and
warm-sensitive receptors that responded to slight increases
(⬍2°C) of the skin temperature, with maximal tonic activity
Table 2. Thermal detection and pain thresholds in group B

Percentile
Table 3. Test and retest results of 33 students
Parameter Median 2.5% 25% 75% 97.5% Minimum Maximum
Laboratory Exercise Climatic Chamber
CDT, °C 30.9 29.3 30.5 31.1 31.5 28.0 31.7 Parameter (Test Results) (Retest Results) ␳
WDT, °C 33.5 32.8 33.1 33.9 34.8 32.6 35.4
CPT, °C† 5.2 0.0 1.6 11.3 20.4 0.0 25.5 CDT, °C 30.9 30.9 0.50
HPT, °C 46.8 39.9 44.7 48.7 51.7 37.4 52.3 WDT, °C 33.6 33.6 0.56
VAS CPT, °C 10.6 14.1 0.72
CPT 30.0 0.1 20.8 46.5 68.0 0.0 78.0 HPT, °C 46.1 46.1 0.59
HPT* 50.0 20.2 36.8 60.0 73.9 18.0 87.0 VAS
CPT 20.0 15.0 0.38
n ⫽ 124 students in group B. Shown are median values, percentiles, and HPT 29.0 22.0 0.59
minimum and maximum values. Results are combined data of women and
men. The order of testing was WDT-CDT-HPT-CPT in group B. Significant The 33 volunteers were first tested during the laboratory exercise and then
differences between group A (see Table 1) and group B are shown (ⴱP ⬍ 0.05 reexamined one by one ⬃1 wk later under standardized conditions in a climatic
and †P ⬍ 0.01 by Mann-Whitney U-test). The initial skin temperature was not chamber. Shown are median values of the group and Spearman’s correlation
measured in group B. coefficients (␳).

Advances in Physiology Education • VOL 34 • JUNE 2010


Downloaded from journals.physiology.org/journal/advances (183.082.157.055) on December 31, 2020.
Table 4. Summary of published normal thermal thresholds at the thenar and hand 30

Number of
Repeated Sex-Related
Measurements Differences
Number Temperature Underlying the (more sensitive Pain
Tested of WDT, change CDT, change Change Rate, Averaged Thermode gender in Sequence of Rating
Year Reference Region Subjects Age, yr in °C in °C HPT, °C CPT, °C Baseline, °C °C/s Results Size, cm2 parentheses) Tests (VAS)

Results from the thenar


1986 Kenshalo (21) Thenar 27 19–31 0.3* ⫺0.2* 44.6 (1.6) ND 40 0.3 20 7.1 No differences Randomized No
1991 Lautenbacher Thenar 64 38 (12) 1.8 (0.9) ⫺0.9 (0.3) 45.5 (2.5) ND 32/40 0.7 5–7 5.8 WDT (women) WDT-CDT- No
and Strian but not HPT HPT
(24)†
1992 Verdugo and Thenar 61 17–72 1.5 (1.2) ⫺1.4 (0.4) 44.6 (1.9) 12.3 (4.4) 32 2–4 3–6 12.5 Not evaluated CDT-WDT- No
Ochoa (46) CPT-HPT
1994 Meh and Thenar 150 36 (17) ND ND 38.9 (4.5) 23.6 (4.8) 30 1 Unknown 12.5 HPT (women) CPT-HPT No
Denišlič but not CPT
(31)
1994 Yarnitsky and Thenar 46 20–39 1.6 ⫺1.6 ND ND 32 1 4 12.5 No differences Not reported No
Sprecher
(54)
1995 Yarnitsky et Thenar 46 20–39 ND ND 45.1 ND 32 2 3 13.8 No difference HPT only No
Using Classic Papers To Teach Physiology

al. (55)
1999 Liou et al. Thenar 23 22–30 2.4 (1.1) ⫺0.8 (0.6) 49.6 (2.2) 8.2 (2.3) 30 1 3–6 12.5 WDT (women) WDT-CDT- Yes
(29) and CDT HPT-CPT
(men)
1999 Hilz et al. Thenar 157 31 (9) 1.4 (0.6) ⫺1.9 (0.8) ND ND 32 1 5 3.75 WDT (women) Random No
(16)
2000 Hagander et Thenar 46 33 (10) 0.5* ⫺0.5* 43.4 9.8 32 1 6 13.3 Not evaluated Not reported No
al. (13)‡
2005 Lin et al. (28) Thenar 122 30 (6) 1.7 (0.7) ⫺1.4 (0.6) ND ND 32 1 4 9.0 WDT (women) Random No
2005 Kelly et al. Thenar 50 19–59 0.8* ⫺0.7* 45.6 6.7 32 1.5 4 6.25 No differences WDT-CDT- Yes
(19) HPT-CPT
2005 Palmer and Thenar 24 21 (3) 1.2 (0.4) ⫺1.1 (0.6) 40.7 (4.4) 21.6 (5.9) 32 0.5 3 9.0 Only female Randomized No
Martin subjects
(34)†
2008 Li et al. (27) Thenar 25 26 (4) 4.1 ⫺2.9 ND ND 32 2 3 2.0 WDT (women) Not reported No
THERMORECEPTION AND NOCICEPTION

and CDT

Advances in Physiology Education • VOL 34 • JUNE 2010


(women)
2009 Agostinho et Thenar 39 42 (15) 1.6 ⫺1.6 45.9 (3.1) 8.5 (6.4) 32 1–1.5 3–8 9.0 Not evaluated CDT-WDT- No
al. (1) CPT-HPT
2010 Present Thenar 141 23 (5) 1.7 (0.6) ⫺1.4 (0.7) 45.7 (3.6) 10.2 (6.4) 32 1–1.5 5 9.0 WDT (women) CDT-WDT- Yes
study§ and HPT CPT-HPT
(women)
Results from adjacent sites
1995 Ruffell and Third 10 26 (4) 6.5 (2.9) ⫺3.6 (1.6) ND ND 32 1 5 Unknown Only male Random No

Downloaded from journals.physiology.org/journal/advances (183.082.157.055) on December 31, 2020.


Griffin (42) finger subjects
1999 Fillingim et Volar 37 21 (1) ND ND 45.9 (4.0) ND 32 1 4 9.0 HPT (women) HPT only No
al. (8)† forearm
2002 Defrin et al. Dorsum of 18 36 (7) 4.0 (2.0) ⫺4.0 (2.0) 42.5 (2.5) 19.0 (6.5) 32/35 2 4 15.4 Not evaluated WDT-CDT- No
(6)† the hand HPT-CPT
2005 Lautenbacher Volar 20 27 (4) 3.3 (2.0) ⫺1.8 (1.0) 45.2 (2.1) ND 32/35 0.5–2 5 6.0–9.0 WDT (women) WDT-CDT- No
et al. (25)† forearm but not HPT HPT
2005 Watson et al. Volar 20 34 (5) 2.5 (1.1) ⫺2.2 (1.1) 45.2 (3.5) 11.9 (5.4) 32 1 4 9 Only male CDT-WDT- No
(50) forearm subjects CPT-HPT

Continued
Using Classic Papers To Teach Physiology
THERMORECEPTION AND NOCICEPTION 31

Data are means (SD) where available; otherwise, data are ranges or exact values. Published values are of thermal thresholds in normal subjects arranged in chronological order for the thenar and adjacent

(SD) or as range depending on the available data. If a study included several age groups, data of the young subjects were selected, in accordance with the ages of the student groups in the present study. ND,
sites. Some studies report separate data of women and men but no combined results. In this case, the respective data have been averaged post hoc. The Method of Limits (41, 54) was always used to examine
pain thresholds and also for most detection thresholds. However, three studies (13, 19, 21) determined detection thresholds with the Method of Levels (indicated by ⴱ). Ages of subjects are given as means

no data available. Baseline, starting temperature of the thermode; v. forearm, volar forearm; dors. hand, dorsum of the hand. †Numerical values had to be read from figures (there were no tables available).
between 35 and 40°C. A change of temperature will, therefore,

Rating
(VAS)
Pain

Yes
No

No
increase the activity of one receptor population above back-
ground level and decrease the activity of the other receptors

Sequence of

CPT-HPT

CPT-HPT

CPT-HPT
below background level at the same time. The difference in

CDT-WDT-

CDT-WDT-

CDT-WDT-
Tests
activity between warm and cold receptors encodes the amount
of change. Since the CDT and WDT are both determined by
the combined information of the two receptor populations, the
correlation of both thresholds is plausible.
(more sensitive

No differences
parentheses)
Sex-Related

CPT (women)
Differences

Not evaluated
gender in

and HPT The correlation of the thermal pain thresholds may simply
(women) reflect that some people react more sensitively to noxious
stimuli than others, irrespective of the stimulus type (heat/
cold). Individual characteristics such as anxiety, attitude to-
ward pain, distraction, and pain coping strategies may be
Thermode
Size, cm2

‡Hagander et al. (13) and Wasner and Brock (49) reported median values, not mean values. §Data from the present study are results 关means (SD)兴 from group A.
9.0–12.5

7.8

9.0

underlying factors. As a possible peripheral mechanism, it has


been shown in humans that some high-threshold cold-sensitive
C-fibers are also heat sensitive and mechanosensitive (5). Since
Underlying the
Measurements

these afferent fibers will discharge at both the HPT and CPT,
Number of

Averaged
Repeated

Results

their information may contribute to the aforementioned


3

correlation.
Common factors affecting all thresholds are alertness and
attention, integration of sensory information from the stimu-
Change Rate,
Temperature

lated skin area (spatial summation), and skin thickness (7).


°C/s

Interestingly, however, we found no significant relationship


1

between the thermal detection and pain thresholds, most likely


due to the dissimilar nature of the tasks (detection of change vs.
Baseline, °C

perception of pain) and the fact that they involve different


receptor populations (low-threshold thermoreceptors vs. high-
32

32

32

threshold polymodal nociceptors).


Question 3. Why do the thermal pain thresholds shift grad-
ually when repeatedly tested (Fig. 4)? Habituation, the de-
14.3 (7.9)

7.7 (7.8)
CPT, °C

17.6

crease in a behavioral response to a repeated harmless stimu-


lus, is one likely explanation. A well-known example is the
decrease of the startle reaction in response to recurring irrele-
43.4 (3.0)

45.4 (3.6)
HPT, °C

vant noise (44). Volunteers stopped the increase/decrease of


42.9

the thermode temperature via the response button before heat


or cold could injure the skin, so that the categorization as a
harmless stimulus is justified. Increasing confidence certainly
CDT, change

⫺0.7 (0.3)

plays a role, as the students were often more uneasy when the
in °C

⫺1.1

ND

pain threshold was determined for the first time than during the
last trial. We never encountered adverse side effects such as
burns, blisters, or frostbite. Habituation has also been reported
WDT, change

by Agostinho and colleagues (1), who used eight repetitive


1.9 (0.8)
in °C

ND
1.6

measurements of thermal pain thresholds. As a peripheral


process, fatigue of cutaneous nociceptors, which refers to the
decrement in response to repeated stimulation, might be rele-
Age, yr

38 (14)

37 (15)

vant here. Fatigue to heat stimuli in C-fiber nociceptors prob-


17–40

ably results from transduction-spike initiation mechanisms


(35). Since heat pain perception and heat-evoked brain poten-
Subjects
Number

tials in humans show signs of more rapid habituation when the


90

18

10
of

site of stimulation is fixed compared with stimulation at vari-


able locations, Greffrath and colleagues (12) inferred that
the hand

the hand

the hand

fatigue of nociceptors is the underlying mechanism.


Dorsum of

Dorsum of

Dorsum of
Region
Tested

The opposite mechanism, namely, sensitization, was re-


ported by Bessou and Perl (2). Repeated heating (over ⬃10 s)
Table 1.—Continued

of the skin of a cat to 50°C lowered the threshold and increased


the discharge rate of polymodal nociceptors (Figs. 3–5 in Ref.
Reference

Wasner and
Rolke et al.

Rolke et al.

2). Sensitization was also observed in awake humans after a


Brock
(49)‡
(40)

(41)

mild experimental heat injury of the skin (thermode with 50°C


over 100 s), which caused hyperalgesia (45). Compared with
the uninjured skin, suprathreshold stimulation of the hyperal-
Year

2006

2006

2008

gesic skin was associated with elevated discharge rates of the

Advances in Physiology Education • VOL 34 • JUNE 2010


Downloaded from journals.physiology.org/journal/advances (183.082.157.055) on December 31, 2020.
Using Classic Papers To Teach Physiology
32 THERMORECEPTION AND NOCICEPTION

C-fiber nociceptors, as measured with microneurography, and In our study, cold pain at the threshold was rated less painful
with higher ratings of pain intensity. As a teaching example, than heat pain, conforming with two previous studies (19, 49).
most students will have experienced a similar hyperalgesia This points to the fact that beyond the tested range, noxious
after a sunburn. However, the heat and cold stimuli applied heat stimuli (of, e.g., ⫹60°C) would damage the skin faster
during our laboratory exercise were not strong enough to cause than painful cold stimuli (of, e.g., ⫺10°C), so that the warning
sensitization. function of pain is more important when heat is applied.
Question 4. Why are the ratings of pain intensity at the HPT A survey of previously published data shows inconsistent
and CPT not invariable (Fig. 5)? Pain intensity at the thresh- values of the “normal” CPT (see Table 4). The span between
olds was rated on a VAS between “no pain” (0) and the “worst the lowest [⬃7°C (19)] and highest mean CPT [⬃24°C (31)]
imaginable pain” (100). One may theoretically expect low and amounted to nearly 17°C. The respective instructions (often
invariable scores (e.g., around a value of 10) for the thermal not published) may have differed from study to study, either
stimuli that just surpass the threshold. Surprisingly, the ratings reinforcing or discouraging the subjects to classify uncom-
were higher (⬃30 –50; see Tables 1 and 2). This is in line with fortable cold as painful. Neurophysiologically, cold pain
three previous studies (19, 29, 49) of normal volunteers that involves the interaction of different afferent inputs because
used a VAS (see Table 4) and found that pain intensity at the signals from cold-specific A␦-fibers (low-threshold thermo-
HPT and CPT was rated considerably higher than the lowest receptors) inhibit the input from C-fiber nociceptors at the
perceptible pain. Therefore, it is unlikely that the explanations cortical level and can thereby suppress the sensation of pain
concerning the VAS given to the students during the laboratory (47, 48). The effectiveness of this central inhibition is
exercise were misleading. Rather, three other reasons are probably not constant but may depend on receptiveness and
plausible. First, it is possible that some volunteers hesitated a selective attention to the painful components of the cold
few seconds until they became clearly aware of the pain and stimulus. A variable inhibition will add to a high interindi-
only then pressed the response button. The threshold depends vidual variability of the CPT.
on the rate of temperature rise and reaction time (53). Second, Interestingly, the CPT was shifted slightly, but signifi-
ratings of pain intensity are influenced by psychological factors cantly, toward lower temperatures when it was tested after
such as expectations, anxiety, and pain coping strategies, the HPT compared with the reverse order. The series of the
which vary from individual to individual (19, 38). Third, some preceding five heat stimuli may have evoked some transient
late afferent signals of slow-conducting C-fibers reached the hypoalgesia. Painful conditioning stimuli are known to
brain even after the increase (or decrease) in the thermode activate central endogenous pain modulation systems and
temperature had been stopped by the button press so that can thereby evoke pain-evoked hypoalgesia (26, 37). This
delayed pain may have influenced the ratings. As a teaching will reduce the painfulness of subsequent noxious stimuli,
point, it is important to realize that these results are based on even when delivered at different body sites (36).
the subject’s “answers” and not on objective neurophysiolog- Methodological factors that can influence the thermal
ical data (such as, e.g., receptor potentials and discharge rates detection and pain thresholds are the rates of the tempera-
of afferent fibers). ture ramps and the thermode size (small thermode ⫽ little
Still, there is evidence that pain ratings of human subjects spatial summation). These factors are shown in Table 4 for
largely match the response profile of C-fiber nociceptors (45). previously published data and the present study. Due to the
Correspondingly, we found that higher temperatures of the phasic response characteristics of the thermoreceptors, rapid
HPT, which are associated with stronger activity of the afferent changes of the temperature are usually detected earlier than
units, were associated with higher ratings of pain intensity (Fig. slow changes (20). However, very rapid rates may introduce
5A). No such relation was found for the less-consistent CPT a bias, because the thermode temperature will continue to
(Fig. 5B). Yet, the strong positive correlation between the increase/decrease during the reaction time until the response
ratings of cold and heat pain intensity indicates a general button is pressed. This will, e.g., shift the HPT toward
tendency of the students to give either high or low ratings of higher temperatures and the CPT toward lower temperatures
pain intensity irrespective of the type of noxious stimulus. (53). We chose the rates of the thermal ramps and thermode
Green and Akirav (10) reported a high correlation between size in accordance with previous related publications (see
separate ratings of the intensity of different thermal sensations Table 4). These methodical aspects were discussed with the
(warmth and cold) and hypothesized that warm-sensitive and students.
cold-sensitive spinothalamic pathways converge so that the
modalities undergo common processing in the central nervous Conclusions
system.
Question 5. Why is the threshold for cold pain more variable Four decades after the pioneering studies of Perl and
than the threshold for heat pain (Fig. 2 and Tables 1 and 2)? collaborators, the role of C-fibers and A␦-fibers in nocicep-
Cold is a weaker stimulus than heat, at least within the limited tion and thermosensation is well established. Pain has been
range of the thermode temperatures that we could apply (0 – recognized as a discrete sensory modality. Quantitative
53°C). In addition, Bessou and Perl (2) reported that the sensory testing of thermal detection and pain thresholds in
polymodal nociceptors of the skin in cats were distinctly the setting of a student laboratory course proved to be an
activated by noxious heat, intense mechanical stimuli, and instructive method that yielded repeatable results. The re-
irritant chemicals, but only weakly (if at all) excited by low sults of nearly 300 students showed that women are more
temperatures. Cooling the skin down to 10°C had no effect sensitive to thermal detection and thermal pain than men,
unless the nociceptors had been sensitized beforehand by other thus adding to other sex-based differences in physiology (3).
stimuli (e.g., heat and irritant chemicals; see Table 1 in Ref. 2). Habituation shifted the thermal pain thresholds up to the

Advances in Physiology Education • VOL 34 • JUNE 2010


Downloaded from journals.physiology.org/journal/advances (183.082.157.055) on December 31, 2020.
Using Classic Papers To Teach Physiology
THERMORECEPTION AND NOCICEPTION 33

fourth or fifth trial. Ratings of pain intensity were correlated 20. Kenshalo DR, Holmes CE, Wood PB. Warm and cold thresholds as a
with the HPT but not with the CPT. The CPT was lower function of rate of stimulus temperature change. Percept Psychophys 3:
51–54, 1968.
when tested after the HPT than in the reverse case. Quan- 21. Kenshalo DR. Somesthetic sensitivity in young and elderly humans. J
titative sensory testing was appreciated by the students as a Gerontol 41: 732–742, 1986.
relevant and interesting method, which complements other 22. Kim H, Neubert JK, San MA, Xu K, Krishnaraju RK, Iadarola MJ,
well-known tasks (like two-point discrimination and map- Goldman D, Dionne RA. Genetic influence on variability in human acute
ping of warm and cool spots) of a laboratory exercise in experimental pain sensitivity associated with gender, ethnicity and psy-
chological temperament. Pain 109: 488 – 496, 2004.
somesthesis (11). 23. Labrecque G, Vanier MC. Biological rhythms in pain and in analgesics.
In: Physiology and Pharmacology of Biological Rhythms, edited by
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Redfern PH, Lemmer B. Berlin, Germany: Springer, 1997.
24. Lautenbacher S, Strian F. Sex differences in pain and thermal sensitiv-
The help of R. Neumann in data collection and preparation of the manu- ity: the role of body size. Percept Psychophys 50: 179 –183, 1991.
script is gratefully acknowledged. 25. Lautenbacher S, Kunz M, Strate P, Nielsen J, Arendt-Nielsen L. Age
effects on pain thresholds, temporal summation and spatial summation of
DISCLOSURES heat and pressure pain. Pain 115: 410 – 418, 2005.
26. Le Bars D, Dickenson AH, Besson JM. Diffuse noxious inhibitory
No conflicts of interest are declared by the author(s). controls (DNIC). I. Effects on dorsal horn convergent neurones in the rat.
Pain 6: 283–304, 1979.
REFERENCES 27. Li X, Petrini L, Defrin R, Madeleine P, Arendt-Nielsen L. High
resolution topographicalmapping of warm and cold sensitivities. Clin
1. Agostinho CM, Scherens A, Richter H, Schaub C, Rolke R, Treede Neurophysiol 119: 2641–2646, 2008.
RD, Maier C. Habituation and short-term repeatability of thermal testing 28. Lin YH, Hsieh SC, Chao CC, Chang YC, Hsieh ST. Influence of aging
in healthy human subjects and patients with chronic non-neuropathic pain. on thermal and vibratory thresholds of quantitative sensory testing. J
Eur J Pain 13: 779 –785, 2009. Peripher Nerv Syst 10: 269 –281, 2005.
2. Bessou P, Perl ER. Response of cutaneous sensory units with unmyeli- 29. Liou JT, Lui PW, Lo YL, Liou L, Wang SS, Yuan HB, Chan KH, Lee
nated fibers to noxious stimuli. J Neurophysiol 32: 1025–1043, 1969. TY. Normative data of quantitative thermal and vibratory thresholds in
3. Blair ML. Sex-based differences in physiology: what should we teach in normal subjects in Taiwan: gender and age effect. Chin Med J (Taipai) 62:
the medical curriculum? Adv Physiol Educ 31: 23–25, 2007. 431– 437, 1999.
4. Burgess PR, Perl ER. Myelinated afferent fibres responding specifically 30. Mason P. Placing pain on the sensory map: classic papers by Ed Perl and
to noxious stimulation of the skin. J Physiol 190: 541–562, 1967. colleagues. J Neurophysiol 97: 1871–1873, 2007.
5. Campero M, Serra J, Ochoa JL. C-polymodal nociceptors activated by 31. Meh D, Denislic M. Quantitative assessment of thermal and pain sensi-
noxious low temperature in human skin. J Physiol 497: 565–572, 1996. tivity. J Neurol Sci 127: 164 –169, 1994.
6. Defrin R, Ohry A, Blumen N, Urca G. Sensory determinants of thermal 32. Ochoa J, Torebjörk HE. Sensations evoked by intraneural microstimu-
pain. Brain 125: 501–510, 2002. lation of C nociceptor fibres in human skin nerves. J Physiol 415:
7. Defrin R, Petrini L, Arendt-Nielsen L. Spatial summation of thermal 583–599, 1989.
sensations depends on skin type and skin sensitivity. Exp Brain Res 198: 33. Paintal AS. A comparison of the nerve impulses of mammalian non-
29 –36, 2009. medullated nerve fibres with those of the smallest diameter medullated
8. Fillingim RB, Maddux V, Shackelford JA. Sex differences in heat pain fibres. J Physiol 193: 523–533, 1967.
thresholds as a function of assessment method and rate of rise. Somatosens 34. Palmer ST, Martin DJ. Thermal perception thresholds recorded using
Mot Res 16: 57– 62, 1999. method of limits change over brief time intervals. Somatosens Mot Res 22:
9. Granovsky Y, Matre D, Sokolilk A, Lorenz J, Casey KL. Thermore- 327–334, 2005.
ceptive innervation of human glabrous and hairy skin: a contact heat
35. Peng YB, Ringkamp M, Meyer RA, Campbell JN. Fatigue and para-
evoked potential analysis. Pain 115: 238 –247, 2005.
doxical enhancement of heat response in C-fiber nociceptors from cross-
10. Green BG, Akirav C. Individual differences in temperature perception:
modal excitation. J Neurosci 23: 4766 – 4774, 2003.
evidence of common processing of sensation intensity of warmth and cold.
36. Price DD, McHaffie JG. Effects of heterotopic conditioning stimuli on
Somatosens Mot Res 24: 71– 84, 2007.
first and second pain: a psychophysical evaluation in humans. Pain 34:
11. Greenspan JD. A laboratory exercise in somesthesis that is expeditious,
245–252, 1988.
inexpensive, and suitable for large classes. Adv Physiol Educ 10: 2–9,
1993. 37. Quiton RL, Greenspan JD. Sex differences in endogenous pain modu-
12. Greffrath W, Baumgartner U, Treede RD. Peripheral and central lation by distracting and painful conditioning stimulation. Pain 132, Suppl
components of habituation of heat pain perception and evoked potentials 1: S134 –S149, 2007.
in humans. Pain 132: 301–311, 2007. 38. Quiton RL, Greenspan JD. Across- and within-session variability of
13. Hagander LG, Midani HA, Kuskowski MA, Parry GJ. Quantitative ratings of painful contact heat stimuli. Pain 137: 245–256, 2008.
sensory testing: effect of site and skin temperature on thermal thresholds. 39. Riley JL, Robinson ME, Wise EA, Myers CD, Fillingim RB. Sex
Clin Neurophysiol 111: 17–22, 2000. differences in the perception of noxious experimental stimuli: a meta-
14. Harrison JL, Davis KD. Cold-evoked pain varies with skin type and analysis. Pain 74: 181–187, 1998.
cooling rate: a psychophysical study in humans. Pain 83: 123–135, 1999. 40. Rolke R, Baron R, Maier C, Tolle TR, Treede RD, Beyer A, Binder A,
15. Henderson LA, Gandevia SC, Macefield VG. Gender differences in Birbaumer N, Birklein F, Botefur IC, Braune S, Flor H, Huge V, Klug
brain activity evoked by muscle and cutaneous pain: a retrospective study R, Landwehrmeyer GB, Magerl W, Maihofner C, Rolko C, Schaub C,
of single-trial fMRI data. Neuroimage 39: 1867–1876, 2008. Scherens A, Sprenger T, Valet M, Wasserka B. Quantitative sensory
16. Hilz MJ, Stemper B, Axelrod FB, Kolodny EH, Neundorfer B. Quan- testing in the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS):
titative thermal perception testing in adults. J Clin Neurophysiol 16: standardized protocol and reference values. Pain 123: 231–243, 2006.
462– 471, 1999. 41. Rolke R, Magerl W, Campbell KA, Schalber C, Caspari S, Birklein F,
17. Iannetti GD, Zambreanu L, Tracey I. Similar nociceptive afferents Treede RD. Quantitative sensory testing: a comprehensive protocol for
mediate psychophysical and electrophysiological responses to heat stim- clinical trials. Eur J Pain 10: 77– 88, 2006.
ulation of glabrous and hairy skin in humans. J Physiol 577: 235–248, 42. Ruffell CM, Griffin MJ. Effect of starting temperature on the repeatabil-
2006. ity of thermotactile thresholds. Cent Eur J Public Health 3, Suppl: 81– 84,
18. Jensen TS, Baron R. Translation of symptoms and signs into mechanisms 1995.
in neuropathic pain. Pain 102: 1– 8, 2003. 43. Sarlani E, Farooq N, Greenspan JD. Gender and laterality differences in
19. Kelly KG, Cook T, Backonja MM. Pain ratings at the thresholds are thermosensation throughout the perceptible range. Pain 106: 9 –18, 2003.
necessary for interpretation of quantitative sensory testing. Muscle Nerve 44. Silverthorn DU. Human Physiology: an Integrated Approach. San Fran-
32: 179 –184, 2005. cisco, CA: Pearson, 2007.

Advances in Physiology Education • VOL 34 • JUNE 2010


Downloaded from journals.physiology.org/journal/advances (183.082.157.055) on December 31, 2020.
Using Classic Papers To Teach Physiology
34 THERMORECEPTION AND NOCICEPTION

45. Torebjörk HE, LaMotte RH, Robinson CJ. Peripheral neural correlates 50. Watson PJ, Latif RK, Rowbotham DJ. Ethnic differences in thermal
of magnitude of cutaneous pain and hyperalgesia. Simultaneous record- pain responses: a comparison of South Asian and White British healthy
ings in humans of sensory judgments of pain and evoked responses in males. Pain 118: 194 –200, 2005.
nociceptors with C-fibers. J Neurophysiol 51: 325–339, 1984. 51. Wise EA, Price DD, Myers CD, Heft MW, Robinson ME. Gender role
46. Verdugo R, Ochoa JL. Quantitative somatosensory thermotest. A key expectations of pain: relationship to experimental pain perception. Pain
method for functional evaluation of small calibre afferent channels. Brain 96: 335–342, 2002.
115: 893–913, 1992. 52. Wright EM, Royston P. Calculating reference intervals for laboratory
47. Wahren LK, Torebjörk E, Jorum E. Central suppression of cold- measurements. Stat Methods Med Res 8: 93–112, 1999.
induced C fibre pain by myelinated fibre input. Pain 38: 313–319, 1989. 53. Yarnitsky D, Ochoa JL. Studies of heat pain sensation in man: perception
48. Wasner G, Schattschneider J, Binder A, Baron R. Topical menthol–a thresholds, rate of stimulus rise and reaction time. Pain 40: 85–91, 1990.
human model for cold pain by activation and sensitization of C nocicep- 54. Yarnitsky D, Sprecher E. Thermal testing: normative data and repeat-
tors. Brain 127: 1159 –1171, 2004. ability for various test algorithms. J Neurol Sci 125: 39 – 45, 1994.
49. Wasner GL, Brock JA. Determinants of thermal pain thresholds in 55. Yarnitsky D, Sprecher E, Zaslansky R, Hemli JA. Heat pain thresholds:
normal subjects. Clin Neurophysiol 119: 2389 –2395, 2008. normative data and repeatability. Pain 60: 329 –332, 1995.

Advances in Physiology Education • VOL 34 • JUNE 2010


Downloaded from journals.physiology.org/journal/advances (183.082.157.055) on December 31, 2020.

You might also like