You are on page 1of 12

This article was downloaded by: [University of Toronto Libraries]

On: 07 December 2014, At: 12:00


Publisher: Taylor & Francis
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Assistive Technology: The Official Journal of RESNA


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/uaty20

Use of Reading Pen Assistive Technology to


Accommodate Post-Secondary Students with Reading
Disabilities
a a a
Ara J. Schmitt PhD , Elizabeth McCallum PhD , Jenna Hennessey MSEd , Temple Lovelace
a b
PhD & Renee O. Hawkins PhD
a
School of Education , Duquesne University , Pittsburgh , Pennsylvania
b
School of Education , University of Cincinnati , Cincinatti , Ohio , USA
Accepted author version posted online: 08 Feb 2012.Published online: 27 Sep 2012.

To cite this article: Ara J. Schmitt PhD , Elizabeth McCallum PhD , Jenna Hennessey MSEd , Temple Lovelace PhD & Renee
O. Hawkins PhD (2012) Use of Reading Pen Assistive Technology to Accommodate Post-Secondary Students with Reading
Disabilities, Assistive Technology: The Official Journal of RESNA, 24:4, 229-239, DOI: 10.1080/10400435.2012.659956

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400435.2012.659956

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the
Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and
are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and
should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for
any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever
or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of
the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
Assistive Technology® , 24:229–239, 2012
Copyright © 2012 RESNA
ISSN: 1040-0435 print/1949-3614 online
DOI: 10.1080/10400435.2012.659956

Use of Reading Pen Assistive Technology


to Accommodate Post-Secondary Students
with Reading Disabilities
Ara J. Schmitt, PhD,1
Downloaded by [University of Toronto Libraries] at 12:00 07 December 2014

Elizabeth McCallum, PhD,1 ABSTRACT Reading pens are a form of assistive technology that may be
Jenna Hennessey, MSEd,1 used to bypass weak word decoding and vocabulary skills of students with
Temple Lovelace, PhD,1 and reading disabilities. Only two known studies have examined the effects of
Renee O. Hawkins, PhD2
1 reading pens on the comprehension of school-aged students, and no known
School of Education,
Duquesne University,
studies have been published regarding post-secondary students. The present
Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania study investigated the effects of reading pen assistive technology on the
2
School of Education, University comprehension accuracy and rate of three post-secondary students with uni-
of Cincinnati, Cincinatti, Ohio versity-recognized reading disabilities. An alternating treatments design was
implemented to compare the effects of (1) a reading pen decoding accommo-
dation, (2) concurrent reading pen decoding and vocabulary accommodations,
and (3) a no-accommodation control condition on the comprehension of
the three participants when provided college level difficulty reading passages.
Results indicate that use of a reading pen did not uniformly improve the
comprehension of the post-secondary students. However, the student with the
poorest reading skills benefitted the most. Discussion focuses on explanations
for the results, practical implications for post-secondary students with reading
disabilities, and future directions for study.

KEYWORDS reading accommodation, reading assistive technology, reading pen

INTRODUCTION
The number of students with disabilities attending colleges and universities
has dramatically increased across recent decades. Henderson (2001) reported
that the percentage of full-time post-secondary students who self-identify as
having any type of disability rose from 2.3% in 1978 to 9% in 2000. Although
statistics vary regarding the types of disabilities represented by post-secondary
students, there is some consensus that more than a third of these students have
Address correspondence to Ara J. learning disabilities (Harbour, 2004; Henderson, 2001). Of those students with
Schmitt, Duquesne University, School disabilities that attend college, the majority have been formally diagnosed with
Education, G3A Canevin Hall,
600 Forbes Ave., Pittsburgh, PA 15282.
a reading disability as it is the most common learning disability across grades
E-mail: schmitta2106@duq.edu and ages (Hudson, High, & Al Otaiba, 2007; Shaywitz, 2003).

229
Post-secondary education is an important transi- or word lists. This LWR implementation procedure is
tional outcome for students with learning disabilities. taxing on human resources and is not likely realistic for
A college degree may serve as a protective factor against most college students who must be independent, self-
commonly experienced poor outcomes for students regulated learners. A means to self-regulate the LWR
with disabilities, like employment obstacles. Madaus accommodation for college students is through the
(2006) reported that students with disabilities who use of assistive technology devices (Lee & Templeton,
graduate from college do not differ from their non- 2008; Raskind & Higgins, 1998).
disabled peers in terms of employment rates, benefits, Assistive technology devices include any piece
and annual salaries. Accordingly, these graduates expe- of equipment used to improve or maintain the
rience more favorable outcomes across the lifespan functioning of individuals with disabilities (Assistive
than individuals with disabilities who do not earn Technology Act, 1998; Wepner & Bowes, 2004). These
college degrees (Shaw, Madaus, & Banerjee, 2009). run the gamut from low-tech tools, such as stopwatches
Section 504 of the 1973 Rehabilitation Act is a and metronomes, to high-tech devices, such as comput-
federal law designed to protect the rights of indi- erized accommodations (McCallum & Schmitt, 2011;
viduals with disabilities engaged in any federally Reed, 2007). An alternative to LWR using live read-
Downloaded by [University of Toronto Libraries] at 12:00 07 December 2014

funded programming, including post-secondary ing models involves text-to-speech assistive technology
schools. Although Section 504 entitles elementary that converts written material to spoken language.
and secondary students with disabilities to a free and These devices have been demonstrated to increase the
appropriate public education (FAPE), post-secondary comprehension of primary and secondary students
schools are not held to this same standard. Instead, with reading disabilities (Elkind, Cohen, & Murray,
Section 504 mandates that post-secondary schools 1993; Izzo, Yurick, & McArrell, 2009; Montali &
must provide appropriate academic adjustments to Lewandowski, 1996; Moorman, Boon, Keller-Bell,
qualified students based upon type of disability and Stagliano, & Jeffs, 2010; Schmitt, McCallum, Hale,
individual needs (Madaus & Shaw, 2004). These Obeldobel, & Dingus, 2009).
adjustments may include modifications to academic Few studies have investigated the use of text-
requirements and, germane to this study, auxiliary to-speech assistive technology with college students.
aids and services that may be applied as learning Existing data suggest that for many students, the com-
accommodations. prehension of material is improved when passages are
Accommodations are not meant to create a compet- read by the technology compared to a no-treatment
itive advantage, but “level the playing field” between control (Elkind, Black, & Murray, 1996; Higgins &
students with and without disabilities by removing Raskind, 1997). An interesting finding of these college
disability-related impediments. Formally defined, an studies is that comprehension gains are not uniformly
accommodation is any method that removes construct- present and when participant data are aggregated,
irrelevant barriers to accessing material (Ketterlin- changes in comprehension are not statistically present.
Geller, Yovanoff, & Tindal, 2007). In the case of a col- That being said, data suggest that comprehension with-
lege student with a basic reading disability (dyslexia), out use of the technology is inversely related to the
the inability to fluently decode words impedes the stu- amount of comprehension gains that come from use of
dent’s ability to comprehend material in textbooks and the assistive technology accommodation. Those with
written course materials (Gillon, 2007; Morris et al., the greatest reading difficulties benefited the most from
1998; National Reading Panel, 2000; Shaywitz, 2003; the text-to-speech technology. Similar to findings of
Wolf & Bowers, 1999). studies conducted with school-aged students, however,
The listening-while-reading (LWR) accommodation comprehension of text was not raised to the average
allows students to follow along while listening to text in range. In sum, given the time and expense associated
order to improve comprehension. Several studies have with text-to-speech software that reads entire passages,
demonstrated that LWR may be used to improve the reading pens may be a more cost effective means for
performance of school-aged students with reading dif- providing LWR support.
ficulties (e.g., Hale et al., 2005; Rose, 1984; Rose & The reading pen is a handheld optical character
Beattie, 1986). However, these LWR studies required recognition (OCR) device that allows students to scan
live models (teachers or peers) to read aloud passages printed text either a word or a line at a time. The

A. J. Schmitt et al. 230


scanned text appears on a built-in LCD screen and can effectiveness of this technology with college students
be converted to speech through digital voice. Potential could not be found after searching the peer-reviewed
benefits of the reading pen as an accommodation literature in PsycInfo and ERIC databases. If found to
for college students with reading disabilities include be effective, reading pens may be particularly benefi-
its portability, relatively low cost compared to OCR cial for college students who must read college-level
systems requiring the use of a computer, and ability texts, even with Section 504 accommodations in place.
to scan individual words instead of entire pages or Furthermore, the portability and self-regulated use of
chapters. In addition to decoding, and like other text- the reading pen may be particularly conducive to the
to-speech tools, reading pens allow students to access college environment.
definitions of unknown words.
To date, two empirical studies have been conducted
evaluating the effectiveness of the reading pen technol- METHOD
ogy as an accommodation for students with reading
disabilities. Higgins and Raskind (2005) found that
Participants
school-aged students with significant reading disabili- Participants included three undergraduate students
Downloaded by [University of Toronto Libraries] at 12:00 07 December 2014

ties were able to comprehend more difficult material enrolled in a Mid-Atlantic United States private uni-
on a reading inventory using reading pens than they versity who were recognized by the school’s disability
could on their own (comprehension standard score of concerns office as being students with reading disabil-
85 versus 80). This study did not test the hypothe- ities. The university’s criteria for learning ability status
sis that reading pens could effectively accommodate was a significant discrepancy between a student’s abil-
readers when provided grade-level material, however. ity as measured by an individually administered IQ
Schmitt, McCallum, Rubinic, and Hawkins (2011) test, and reading achievement as measured by an indi-
evaluated the use of reading pens on the reading com- vidual achievement test. Each participant received ser-
prehension accuracy and rates of high school students vices and accommodations through a Section 504 plan.
with reading disabilities when provided grade-level pas- In order to recruit these participants, an electronic invi-
sages. Results indicated that reading pen use actually tation to participate in a study was emailed by the
impaired the comprehension of some students. Not disability concerns office to all students recognized as
only did the students’ comprehension not improve, having a reading learning disability. Using this proce-
by the nature of using a reading pen, it took the stu- dure, the students with reading disabilities remained
dents longer to finish individual passages. The authors anonymous to the investigators. All three undergrad-
suggested that the use of the reading pens may have uate students who contacted the primary investigator
disrupted the students’ reading fluency, which in turn, were included as participants in the study. Review of a
impeded their comprehension of the text. recent psychological report by the investigators verified
Interestingly, these results are in contrast with the the disability status of each participant. All participants
results of the Higgins and Raskind (2005) reading pen earned IQ scores within the Average range. Because
study. A possible explanation for the differences in individual achievement testing was completed earlier in
findings between the two published reading pen stud- each student’s university career, the researchers admin-
ies involves the assessment procedures and specifically, istered three reading curriculum-based measurement
the levels of reading material provided. Higgins and (CBM) probes to each participant in order to establish
Raskind found reading pens to aid students in compre- the most current oral reading fluency levels. As college
hending reduced grade-level material whereas Schmitt level CBM norms are not available, the performance of
et al. found reading pens to impede students’ compre- each was compared to a 12th grade student in the spring
hension of on grade-level material. Despite these differ- using AIMSweb (widely used CBM system) norms
ences, neither study found reading pen usage increased (AIMSweb, 2008).
reading performance to a functionally acceptable level. Joseph was a 20-year-old, Caucasian freshman whose
Although reading pens are currently marketed for major involved a helping profession. Most recent eval-
use by college students, and have been suggested to uation results revealed the presence of a reading disabil-
be appropriate for use in the post-secondary setting ity. His median oral reading fluency performance was
(Raskind & Higgins, 1998), research demonstrating the 131 words correct per minute (below 10th percentile).

231 Reading Pen Assistive Technology


The second participant, Tim, was a 21-year-old Readingpen also allows the dictionary function to be
Caucasian student who was identified as having a locked from use. Earbuds can be worn to avoid dis-
reading disability, as well as receptive and expressive tracting others and volume may be adjusted to a
language disorders. He was sophomore who majored comfortable level. This study locked the vocabulary
in a hard science. Tim’s median oral reading fluency function and allowed access to the decoding func-
performance was 144 words correct per minute (below tion for the RP-D condition, and permitted use of
10th percentile). the decoding and vocabulary functions for the RP-DV
Liz was a 21-year-old, Caucasian student majoring condition.
in a social science. A reading disability was docu- Prior to beginning the experimental sessions, a par-
mented in her most recent psychological evaluation. ticipant Readingpen competency checklist was com-
Her median oral reading fluency performance was pleted. An intervention integrity checklist was used to
81 words correct per minute (below 10th percentile). collect treatment integrity data and stopwatches were
used to record the amount of time students spent
reading the passages. Participants completed an inter-
Materials vention acceptability rating form at the conclusion of
Downloaded by [University of Toronto Libraries] at 12:00 07 December 2014

The reading passages and comprehension questions the study.


used in this study were taken from Timed Readings in
Literature (Spargo, 1989), a series used in previous inter-
vention studies involving school-aged students (e.g.,
General Experimental Procedures
Hale et al., 2005; Schmitt, Hale, McCallum, & Mauck, Data collection was scheduled for 10 sessions across
2011; Schmitt et al., 2011). This series contains a 13th consecutive days of student availability. Session one
(college) grade-level book comprised of 50, 400 word was comprised of a 45 min training protocol designed
passages. The difficulty of passages within each grade by the first author. The protocol consisted of an
level book was controlled by using Fry’s readability explanation of Readingpen functions, research assistant
index (1968). This index calculates readability by con- modeling of functions, guided student practice, and a
sidering the number of syllables in words and the num- checklist to document participant ability to fluidly use
ber of sentences present in each passage. Each passage the technology. Specifically, it was explained that the
is approximately one 50th of a grade-level more diffi- pen could be used to scan unknown printed words that
cult than the preceding story. Following each passage would then appear on the LCD screen. At that point,
are 10 multiple choice comprehension questions (5 fac- buttons (arrows) on the pen could be used to select
tual and 5 inferential). Only the first 30 passages and the function that would be of assistance. In the case of
sets of comprehension questions were photocopied for this study, the functions were access to audible pronun-
use by the participants. Passages were counterbalanced ciation of words and definitions of words, depending
across conditions and sessions to control for passage on treatment condition. The participant was then pro-
difficulty. No participant was exposed to a specific vided a reading passage and prompted to use the
reading passage twice across the study’s procedures. reading pen functions until the participant felt com-
During the decoding accommodation (RP-D) con- fortable using the technology. The 45 minute training
dition and decoding plus vocabulary accommodations session culminated with the completion of a perfor-
(RP-DV) condition, students were provided with a mance checklist to verify each participant was able
Readingpen® Advanced Edition (2006) as developed to effectively use functions of the Readingpen. The
by Wizcom Technologies. The handheld Readingpen checklist included components such as fluidly scanned
is placed against the page before the word(s) of inter- unknown words, efficiently retrieved the audible pro-
est and then swept across the text in a fluid motion. nunciation of a scanned word, efficiently retrieved the
The scanned content then appears in black letters on definition of a scanned word, efficiently retrieved the
the Readingpen’s LCD screen. Buttons (arrows) on audible presentation of the definition, and so forth.
the Readingpen are then used to select word pro- Researchers also administered the three CBM probes
nunciation, syllabication, or definition. The student used to determine current reading fluency levels dur-
also may elect to have this information, including a ing the first session. The experimental procedures were
word’s definition, read aloud by a digital voice. The then applied across the remaining nine sessions. All

A. J. Schmitt et al. 232


procedures were individually conducted in a distrac- After answering any questions, the researcher said,
tion free room within the university’s reading clinic. “Begin,” and started the stopwatch. The number of
Each intervention session, which lasted approximately times the Readingpen was used in the session was
20 min, was comprised of 3 counterbalanced condi- tracked to confirm that the participant accessed the
tions (i.e., RP-D, RP-DV, and C). A script was used available accommodation. When the participant fin-
to ensure uniform and thorough instructions for all ished reading the passage, the stopwatch was stopped
participants. Scheduled sessions were completed by all and the time spent reading the passage was recorded
participants. At the end of the study, rating forms were in seconds. The associated comprehension questions
completed by each student to assess acceptability of were provided on a sheet of paper and participants were
Readingpens as an accommodation. To acknowledge instructed to carefully read each question and circle the
student participation, each was provided with a gift best answer. Unlimited time to complete the questions
card at the conclusion of the study. was available. The participants were allowed to use the
decoding function of the Readingpen on the passage
and comprehension questions.
Intervention Procedures
Downloaded by [University of Toronto Libraries] at 12:00 07 December 2014

No-accommodation control condition Decoding and vocabulary accommodations


During the control condition, participants read pas- condition
sages and answered questions without any Readingpen At the start of the RP-DV condition, participants
assistance. Participants were given the following were given a Readingpen accompanied by earbuds with
instructions prior to the start of this condition: “You a volume control. Participants adjusted the volume to a
are going to read a short story on your own. Read it comfortable level. Prior to the start of the RP-DV con-
very carefully to yourself because when you are fin- dition, the researcher ensured the vocabulary function
ished you are going to answer some questions about was unlocked so that participants could access both the
what you read. Are you ready?” After providing any decoding and vocabulary functions. The participants
necessary clarification, the researcher said, “Begin,” were read the following directions:
and started the stopwatch. When the student finished
reading the passage, the researcher recorded the time “You are going to read a short story using a Readingpen.
spent reading the passage in seconds. The participant As you read the passage, you may use the Readingpen to have
words read aloud to you and get the definitions of unknown
then was provided the corresponding 10 comprehen- words. If you would like for the Readingpen to read the defi-
sion questions on a sheet of paper. Participants were nitions to you, that is allowed too. Read very carefully because
instructed to carefully read each question and circle the when you are finished you are going to answer some questions
about what you read. Are you ready?”
best answer. Unlimited time was available to complete
the comprehension questions.
After providing any necessary clarification, the stop-
watch was started. Each instance the participant used
the Readingpen was recorded to document that the
Decoding accommodation condition
accommodations were being used. When the partici-
At the start of the RP-D condition, participants were pant finished the passage, the stopwatch was stopped
given a Readingpen accompanied by earbuds with a and the time spent reading the passage was recorded in
volume control. Participants were allowed to adjust the seconds. The corresponding 10 comprehension ques-
volume as needed. The vocabulary function was locked tions were provided with the instructions to carefully
for use so that only the decoding function could be consider each question and circle the best answer.
accessed. The participants were provided the following Like the other conditions, unlimited time to com-
instructions: plete the questions was available. The participants were
allowed to use the decoding and vocabulary functions
“You are going to read a short story using a Readingpen. of the Readingpen on the passage and comprehension
As you read the passage, you may use the Readingpen to have
questions as needed. Comprehension questions were
words read aloud to you. Read very carefully because when you
are finished you are going to answer some questions about what reviewed to ensure that the vocabulary function would
you read. Are you ready?” not directly provide the correct answer.

233 Reading Pen Assistive Technology


Experimental Design and reading pen was closely monitored and recorded by
Dependent Variables an experimenter. A second researcher also reviewed 24
(30%) of the comprehension assessments for scoring
Similar to previous reading accommodation stud- accuracy.
ies (e.g., Hale et al., 2005; Higgins & Raskind, 1997;
Schmitt, McCallum, Hale, Obeldobel, & Dingus,
2009), an alternating-treatments design was used to RESULTS
compare the effects of the two accommodation condi- In order to assess the impact of reading pen accom-
tions (RP-D and RP-DV) and the no-accommodation modations on post-secondary students, the reading
control condition on reading comprehension. The comprehension accuracy and rates of three undergrad-
conditions were counterbalanced across sessions to uate college students was obtained across two reading
control for order effects. This type of design allows for pen accommodation conditions (RP-V and RP-DV)
the three conditions to be applied to equivalent tasks and a no-accommodation control condition. Results
within an experimental session that also accounts for for comprehension accuracy are presented first, fol-
history and spillover effects (Poncy, Skinner, & Jaspers, lowed by the results for comprehension rate. With
Downloaded by [University of Toronto Libraries] at 12:00 07 December 2014

2007; Sindelar, Rosenberg, & Wilson, 1985; Skinner & respect to treatment integrity, results showed that the
Shapiro, 1989). primary researcher implemented the experimental pro-
The dependent variables of this study included cedures with 100% accuracy. With the exception of a
comprehension accuracy and comprehension rate. single RP-D passage (Joseph), each of the three par-
Comprehension accuracy per student, condition, and ticipants used the assistive technology at least one
session was determined by the number of correctly time across each of the 18 passages on which use of
answered multiple-choice comprehension questions the Readingpen was permitted. Joseph’s median use
(out of 10). Comprehension rate was also computed of the Readingpen was one time per accommoda-
per student, condition, and session. Comprehension tion condition. Tim’s median use was two times per
rate was calculated by dividing the number of cor- accommodation condition. Liz’s median use of the
rectly answered questions by the number of seconds Readingpen functions was four times per accommoda-
spent engaged with the passage and then multiplying tion condition. One hundred percent agreement was
by 60 (Skinner, 2008). Effect sizes regarding the depen- present regarding scoring accuracy of the comprehen-
dent variables were computed by subtracting the mean sion assessments.
performance of two conditions and then dividing by
the pooled standard deviation of the two conditions
(Weiner, Sheridan, & Jenson, 1998). Data Analysis of Comprehension
Accuracy
Consistent across all three students, comprehen-
Treatment Integrity and Interscorer
sion accuracy did not incrementally increase from the
Agreement control condition, to the RP-D condition, and then
A treatment integrity protocol was constructed for to the RP-DV condition. Figure 1 displays students’
each condition. This checklist determined whether comprehension accuracy as a function of condition
proper procedures were followed for each condition. and Table 1 includes the means and standard devia-
For example, the checklist for the RP-D condition tions of correctly answered comprehension questions.
included the following: locked the vocabulary func- For Joseph, comprehension accuracy was best in the
tion, provided correct passage, read decoding condi- RP-D condition, followed by the control condition,
tion script, started timer, stopped timer when student and then the RP-DV condition. For Tim, the RP-D
finished reading and recorded elapsed time, provided condition resulted in the greatest comprehension accu-
comprehension questions and read comprehension racy, trailed by identical performances in the control
script, and recorded number of times the Readingpen and RP-DV conditions. Liz’s comprehension accuracy
was used. One session per participant was observed by was highest in the RP-D and RP-DV conditions, fol-
a second researcher to gather treatment integrity data. lowed by the control condition. Also common across
As another estimate of treatment integrity, use of the participants, RP-D resulted in greater comprehension

A. J. Schmitt et al. 234


accuracy compared to RP-C. Visual analysis of Figure 1
reveals that an assistive technology accommodation
condition resulted in greater comprehension accuracy
in 6 of 9 sessions for Joseph (4 RP-D, 2 RP-DV), 7 of
9 sessions for Tim (4 RP-D, 3 RP-DV) and all 9 sessions
for Liz (4 RP-D, 5 RP-DV).
Effect size data reported in Table 2 reflect effect sizes
ranging from negligible (.10 for Joseph and .11 for Tim)
to essentially large (.79 for Liz). Effect sizes are inter-
preted consistent with the guidelines of Cohen (1992).
There were no appreciable differences in performance
between the control and RP-DV conditions across par-
ticipants, for Joseph and Tim. In contrast, an effect size
of 1.15 indicates that Liz’s comprehension accuracy
was meaningfully higher in the RP-DV condition com-
Downloaded by [University of Toronto Libraries] at 12:00 07 December 2014

pared to the control condition. The RP-D condition


resulted in greater comprehension accuracy compared
to the RP-DV condition for Joseph and Tim (effect
sizes of .01 and .09, respectively). No difference was
present for Liz.

Data Analysis of
Comprehension Rate
As stated earlier, comprehension rate is an index
of the number of correctly answered comprehension
questions given time spent engaged with the pas-
sage. Figure 2 displays students’ comprehension rate
as a function of condition and Table 1 includes the
means and standard deviations for comprehension
rate. For Tim and Joseph, the greatest comprehension
rate resulted from the control condition, followed by
the RP-D condition, and then the RP-DV condition.
FIGURE 1 Comprehension accuracy across participants, con- For Liz, comprehension rate was equally high in the
ditions, and sessions.
control and RP-D conditions, followed by the RP-
DV condition. Comprehension rate was lowest in the
RP-DV condition for all three participants.

TABLE 1 Mean and standard deviation of correctly answered comprehension questions across participants
and conditions

Mean number correct (SD) Mean comprehension rate (SD)

C RP-D RP-DV C RP-D RP-DV

Joseph 5.22 (2.17) 5.44 (2.35) 5.11 (1.90) 1.07 (0.53) 0.93 (0.36) 0.82 (0.39)
Tim 6.78 (1.79) 7.00 (2.34) 6.78 (2.44) 1.61 (0.56) 1.13 (0.47) 0.92 (0.44)
Liz 4.89 (1.45) 6.44 (2.46) 6.44 (1.24) 0.46 (0.14) 0.46 (0.20) 0.40 (0.09)

Note. C = Control condition. RP-D = Decoding accommodation condition. RP-DV = Decoding and vocabulary
accommodations condition. Maximum mean number correct per condition is 10.

235 Reading Pen Assistive Technology


TABLE 2 Comprehension accuracy and rate effect size across participants and between conditions

Effect size
Dependent
variable C vs. RP-D C vs. RP-DV RP-D vs. RP-DV

Joseph Accuracy ES = 0.10 D > C ES = 0.05 C > DV ES = 0.15 D > DV


Rate ES = 0.31 C > D ES = 0.54 C > DV ES = 0.29 D > DV
Tim Accuracy ES = 0.11 D > C ES = 0 C = DV ES = 0.09 D > DV
Rate ES = 0.92 C > D ES = 1.38 C > DV ES = 0.46 DV > D
Liz Accuracy ES = 0.79 D > C ES = 1.15 DV > C ES = 0 D = DV
Rate ES = 0 C = D ES = 0.50 C > DV ES = 0.40 D > DV

Note. C = Control condition. RP-D = Decoding accommodation condition. RP-DV = Decoding and vocabulary
accommodations condition.

Effect size data reported in Table 2 support the


visual analysis. For Joseph and Tim, and in favor of
Downloaded by [University of Toronto Libraries] at 12:00 07 December 2014

the control condition, effect sizes comparing the con-


trol condition to RP-D were small for Joseph (.31) and
large for Tim (.92). No difference in comprehension
rate was indicated for Liz. With respect to the control
condition compared to the RP-D condition, compre-
hension rate was higher in the control condition for
Joseph, Tim, and Liz with effect sizes of .54 (moder-
ate), 1.38 (large), and .50 (moderate), respectively. For
Joseph and Liz, the RP-D condition resulted in greater
comprehension rate than the RP-DV condition with
effect sizes of .29 (small) and .40 (moderate). The RP-
DV produced higher comprehension rate compared to
RP-D with a small effect size of .46.

Accommodation Acceptability
All participants were administered a brief treatment
acceptability rating form. Two of the participants,
Joseph and Tim, found the Readingpen accommoda-
tions enjoyable while Liz indicated that she some-
what enjoyed using the Readingpen technology. All
three students reported the Readingpen was helpful in
decoding unknown words and looking up definitions
of unknown words. Two of the participants reported
that they would use the Readingpen technology again;
however, Liz indicated that she would possibly use this
technology again. Unexpected was that Liz, for whom
the Readingpen provided the most benefit, rated the
technology somewhat lower that the other two partic-
ipants. Plausible is that Liz’s cognitive resources were
FIGURE 2 Comprehension rate across participants, condi- so devoted to the task of word decoding that the ben-
tions, and sessions. efit to her comprehension was not fully recognized.
On the other hand, Joseph and Tim indicated they

A. J. Schmitt et al. 236


would use the Readingpen again, but unknown to some students, the act of disengaging from the passage
them, it did not provide as much a benefit compared and then manipulating the reading pen appears to dis-
to Liz. This finding is consistent with previous stud- rupt fluency to the extent that any benefit provided
ies involving text-to-speech assistive technology (e.g., by the accommodation is lost. One way to quan-
Schmitt et al., 2011). For some individuals it appears tify this phenomenon is by examining comprehension
that attention is drawn more to the technological capa- rates.
bilities of the Readingpen than whether or not any Joseph’s and Tim’s learning rates were negatively
changes in reading comprehension occurred. impacted by use of the reading pen accommoda-
tions. That is, not only did their comprehension not
appreciably improve (or improve at all), the students
DISCUSSION spent more time working through the passages by the
Prior to this study, only one known inquiry inves- nature of the necessity to operate the reading pens.
tigated the effectiveness of reading pen text-to-speech To illustrate, Tim’s comprehension rate was signifi-
assistive technology on the comprehension of stu- cantly greater in the control condition than the RP-DV
dents. In their sample of high school students with condition with an effect size of 1.38. This issue is
Downloaded by [University of Toronto Libraries] at 12:00 07 December 2014

reading disabilities, reading pen accommodations were particularly poignant for college students whose time
found to increase student performance on a standard- is often limited given class, extracurricular, and work
ized measure of reading comprehension (Higgins & schedules. For these two students, use of a reading pen
Raskind, 2005). The present study sought to exam- may not make sense as any benefit was minimal and it
ine the effects of reading pen accommodations on took up more of their time to use the technology.
the comprehension of college students when provided On the other hand, Liz did appear to benefit from
college level difficulty reading passages. This investi- using the reading pens. Her comprehension accuracy
gation also sought to contribute to the literature by improved when using the reading pen accommoda-
examining the effects of reading pen accommodations tions as reflected by generally large effect sizes (e.g.,
on college student comprehension rates, or compre- .79 for RP-D and 1.15 for RP-DV compared to the con-
hension accuracy given time spent engaged with the trol condition). Although continuous text-to-speech
passage. Reading pen assistive technology has been assistive technology was used to read entire passages,
suggested to be a tool useful for college students to this result is consistent with previous research that
increase their access to content in courses; however, found those with the greatest reading disabilities bene-
results of the current study indicate that the effective- fitted the most from the technological accommodation
ness of the technology may depend on the individual (Elkind et al., 1996; Higgins & Raskind, 1997). Each
student. of the participants’ oral reading fluency was measured
For two of the students, access to and use of reading prior to the start of the experiment. Of note, Liz’s oral
pen accommodations did not meaningfully improve reading fluency was markedly lower than Joseph’s and
their comprehension of college-level passages. On the Tim’s (81, 131, and 144, respectively). It is hypoth-
surface it would seem that additional decoding and esized that Liz’s reading was already so labored that
vocabulary assistance would improve the comprehen- the disruption to fluency caused by use of the read-
sion of the two college students. These results are ing pen was of no consequence. As Table 2 reveals,
similar to Schmitt et al. (2011) who found reading Liz’s comprehension rates did decrease with increased
pen accommodations did not improve the comprehen- access to the technology’s functions, and particularly
sion of high school students with reading disabilities. when access to the decoding and vocabulary functions
In fact, the researchers found that the use of the tech- were enabled. Again, this indicates that despite gains
nological accommodations hindered comprehension. in comprehension accuracy, it took her much longer
Studies have consistently demonstrated that reading to progress through the passages using the technol-
fluency is essential for comprehension (e.g., Deno, ogy. For a college student like Liz, one must consider
Marston, Shinn, & Tindal, 1983; Fuchs, Fuchs, & the benefit of the increased comprehension in light
Maxwell, 1988; Meisenger, Bloom, & Hynd, 2010; of the time it takes to complete passages using a
National Reading Panel, 2000; Rasinski, 1990). For reading pen.

237 Reading Pen Assistive Technology


Practice Implications effects of reading pens on the comprehension of actual
college textbooks since reading for memorization, such
The practical implications of this investigation par-
as for a test, may influence how a student engages a pas-
allel previous studies in the text-to-speech assistive
sage (Linderholm, 2006). Much research is still needed
technology literature. First, individual college students
on use of reading pen text-to-speech assistive tech-
with reading disabilities should not assume that read-
nology across populations. Continued research should
ing pens will necessarily improve their comprehension
focus using reading pen accommodations in other
of text. Although variability in response was present
educational contexts, such as understanding instructor-
among the participants, consistent was that the tech-
provided notes, test preparation, and reading scholarly
nology did not normalize their reading comprehen-
manuscripts.
sion. Therefore, the technology did not appear to
facilitate absolute access to curricular content across
sessions. Other interventions like repeated reading; sur-
REFERENCES
vey, questions, read, recite, and restate (SQ3R); and
peer tutoring should also be attempted (see Daly, AIMSweb. (2008). AIMSweb growth table: Reading curriculum based
measurement multi-year aggregate. Retrieved from http://aimsweb.
Chafouleas, & Skinner, 2005 for reviews of these inter-
Downloaded by [University of Toronto Libraries] at 12:00 07 December 2014

com/
ventions). Still, of the three participants, one student’s Assistive Technology Act (1998). Pub L. No. 105–394.
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159.
comprehension did appear to improve compared to Daly, E., Chafouleas, S., & Skinner, C. H. (2005). Interventions for reading
the control condition. That student demonstrated the problems: Designing and evaluating effective strategies. New York,
poorest oral reading fluency of the three participants. NY: Guilford Press.
Deno, S. L., Marston, D., Shinn, M. R., & Tindal, G. (1983). Oral read-
This is consistent with previous text-to-speech litera- ing fluency: A simple datum for scaling reading disability. Topics in
ture which suggests that those with the greatest reading Learning and Learning Disabilities, 2, 53−59.
Elkind, J., Black, M. S., & Murray, C. (1996). Computer-based compensa-
disability may benefit the most from text-to-speech tion of adult reading disabilities. Annals of Dyslexia, 46, 159–186.
assistive technology. Another practical implication of Elkind, J., Cohen, K., & Murray, C. (1993). Using computer-based readers
this study is that use of reading pens takes additional to improve reading comprehension of students with dyslexia. Annals
of Dyslexia, 43, 238–259.
time compared to silent reading, and no benefit may Fry, E. (1968). A readability formula that saves time. Journal of Reading,
be evident. Like school-aged students (McGivern & 11, 513–516.
Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., & Maxwell, L. (1988). The validity of informal
McKevitt, 2002), college students are strongly encour- reading comprehension measures. Remedial and Special Education,
aged to use the assistive technology on a trial basis to 9, 20−28.
determine if it improves their comprehension and if Gillon, G. T. (2007). Phonological Awareness: From Research to Practice.
Challenges in Language and Literacy. New York, NY: The Guilford
the time it takes to read with the reading pen remains Press.
reasonable. Hale, A. D., Skinner, C. H., Winn, B. D., Oliver, R., Allin, J. D., &
Molloy, C.C.M (2005). An investigation of listening and listening-
while-reading accommodations on reading comprehension levels and
Limitations and Future Research rates in students with emotional disorders. Psychology in the Schools,
42, 39–51.
Harbour, W. S. (2004). Final report: The 2004 AHEAD survey of higher
Although very appropriate for the implemented
education disability services providers. Waltham, MA: The Association
experimental design, this study included only three on Higher Education and Disability.
participants and in depth analyses across participants Henderson, C. (2001). College freshmen with disabilities, 2001: A
Biennial statistical profile. Washington, DC: American Council on
was not possible. Future studies should strive to Education
include a greater number of participants in order to Higgins, E. L., & Raskind, M. H. (1997). The compensatory effective-
ness of optical character recognition/speech synthesis on the reading
tease apart for whom reading pens may meaningfully
comprehension of postsecondary students with learning disabilities.
improve comprehension of text. Like Elkind et al. Learning Disabilities: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 8, 75–87.
(1996) and Higgins and Raskind (1997), a larger sample Higgins, E. L., & Raskind, M. H. (2005). The compensatory effectiveness
of the Quicktionary Reading Pen II on the reading comprehension
would permit the analysis of treatment gains in light of of students with learning disabilities. Journal of Special Education
reading disability severity. A strength of the research Technology, 20, 31–40.
Hudson, R. F., High, L., & Al Otaiba, S. (2007). Dyslexia and the
design was that the readability of the passages were
brain: What does current research tell us? Reading Teacher, 60,
tightly controlled and the participants were exposed to 506–515.
numerous unfamiliar passages. This required the use Izzo, M., Yurick, A., & McArrell, B. (2009). Supported eText: Effects
of text-to-speech on access and achievement for high school stu-
of a literature series commonly used in intervention dents with disabilities. Journal of Special Education Technology, 24,
studies. Future studies should consider studying the 9–20.

A. J. Schmitt et al. 238


Ketterlin-Geller, L. R., Yovanoff, P., & Tindal, G. (2007). Developing Readingpen® Advanced Edition [Apparatus]. (2006). Westford, MA:
a new paradigm for conducting research on accommodations in Wizcom Technologies.
mathematics testing. Exceptional Children, 73, 331–347. Reed, P. R. (2007). A Resource guide for teachers and administra-
Lee, H., & Templeton, R. (2008). Ensuring equal access to technology: tors about assistive technology. Oshkosh, WI: Wisconsin Assistive
Providing assistive technology for students with disabilities. Theory Technology Initiative.
Into Practice, 47, 212–219. Rose, T. L. (1984). The effects of two prepractice procedures on oral
Linderholm, T. (2006). Reading with purpose. Journal of College Reading reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 17, 544–548.
and Learning, 36, 70–80. Rose, T. L., & Beattie, J. R. (1986). Relative effects of teacher-directed
Madaus, J. W. (2006). Employment outcomes of university gradu- and taped previewing on oral reading. Learning Disability Quarterly,
ates with learning disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 29, 9, 193–199.
19–31. Schmitt, A. J., Hale, A. D., McCallum, E., & Mauck, B. (2011).
Madaus, J. W., & Shaw, S. F. (2004). Section 504: Differences in the reg- Accommodating remedial readers in the general education setting:
ulations for secondary and postsecondary education. Intervention in Is listening-while-reading sufficient to improve factual and inferential
School and Clinic, 40, 81–87. comprehension? Psychology in the Schools, 48, 37–45.
McCallum, E., & Schmitt, A. J. (2011). Using technology in schools to Schmitt, A. J., McCallum, E., Hale, A. D., Obeldobel, E., & Dingus, K.
enhance student performance. Journal of Evidence-Based Practices (2009). Can text-to-speech assistive technology improve the read-
For Schools, 12, 129–130. ing comprehension of students with severe reading and emotional
McGivern, J. E., & McKevitt, B. C. (2002). Best practices in working with disabilities? Journal of Evidence-Based Practices for Schools, 10,
students using assistive technology. In A. Thomas & J. Grimes (Eds.), 95–115.
Best practices in school psychology IV (pp. 1537–1553). Bethesda, Schmitt, A. J., McCallum, E., Rubinic, D., & Hawkins, R. (2011). Reading
MD: National Association of School pen decoding and vocabulary accommodations: Impact on student
Downloaded by [University of Toronto Libraries] at 12:00 07 December 2014

Meisinger, E. B., Bloom, J. S., & Hynd, G. W. (2010). Reading fluency: comprehension accuracy and rate. Journal of Evidence Based Practices
Implications for the assessment of children with reading disabilities. for Schools, 12, 129–130.
Annals of Dyslexia, 60, 1–17. Shaw, S. F., Madaus, J. W., & Banerjee, M. (2009). Enhance access to
Montali, J., & Lewandowski, L. (1996). Bimodal reading: Benefits of a talk- postsecondary education for students with disabilities. Intervention in
ing computer for average and less skilled readers. Journal of Learning School and Clinic, 44, 185–190.
Disabilities, 29, 271–279. Shaywitz, S. (2003). Overcoming dyslexia: A new and complete science-
Moorman, A., Boon, R. T., Keller-Bell, Y., Stagliano, C., & Jeffs, T. based program for reading problems at any level. New York, NY:
(2010). Effects of text-to-speech software on the reading rate and Alfred A. Knopf.
comprehension skills of high school students with specific learn- Sindelar, P. T., Rosenberg, M. S., & Wilson, R. J. (1985). An adapted alter-
ing disabilities. Learning Disabilities: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 16, nating treatments design for instructional research. Education and
41–49. Treatment of Children, 8, 67–76.
Morris, R. D., Stuebing, K. K., Fletcher, J. M., . . . Shaywitz, S. E., Lyon, G., Skinner, C. H. (2008). Theoretical and applied implications of precisely
Shankweiler, D. P., & Shaywitz, B. A. (1998). Subtypes of reading dis- measuring learning rates. School Psychology Review, 37, 309–314.
ability: Variability around a phonological core. Journal of Educational Skinner, C. H., & Shapiro, E. S. (1989). A comparison of taped-words
Psychology, 90, 347–373. and drill interventions on reading fluency in adolescents with behavior
National Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence- disorders. Education and Treatment of Children, 12, 123–133.
based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and Spargo, E. (1989). Timed readings in literature. Providence, RI:
its implications for reading instruction. Washington, DC: National Jamestown.
Institute for Child Health and Human Development. Weiner, R. K., Sheridan, S. M., & Jenson, W. R. (1998). The effects of
Poncy, B. C., Skinner, C. H., & Jaspers, K. E. (2007). Evaluating and com- conjoint behavioral consultation and a structured homework program
paring interventions designed to enhance math fact accuracy and on math completion and accuracy in junior high students. School
fluency: Cover, copy, and compare versus taped problems. Journal of Psychology Quarterly, 13, 281–309.
Behavioral Education, 16, 27–37. Wepner, S. B., & Bowes, K. A. (2004). Using assistive technology for liter-
Rasinski, T. V. (1990). Investigating measures of reading fluency. acy development. Reading & Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning
Educational Research Quarterly, 14, 37−44. Difficulties, 20, 219–223.
Raskind, M. H., & Higgins, E. L. (1998). Assistive technology for post- Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the
secondary students with learning disabilities: An Overview. Journal of developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91,
Learning Disabilities, 31, 27–40. 415–438.

239 Reading Pen Assistive Technology

You might also like