You are on page 1of 6

Psychology of Sport and Exercise 12 (2011) 153e158

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Psychology of Sport and Exercise


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psychsport

The effects of instructional and motivational self-talk on students’


motor task performance in physical educationq
Athanasios Kolovelonis a, *, Marios Goudas a, Irini Dermitzaki b
a
Department of Physical Education and Sport Science, University of Thessaly, 42 100 Karies, Trikala, Greece
b
Department of Special Education, University of Thessaly, Argonafton & Fillelinon, 38221 Volos, Greece

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Objective: The aim of the study was to examine the effects of instructional and motivational self-talk on
Received 4 March 2010 students’ motor task performance in a chest pass and a modified push-ups test in physical education.
Received in revised form Design: The design involved one between-groups factor, the group with three levels (instructional,
8 September 2010
motivational, no self-talk), and two within-groups factors, the task (chest pass, modified push-ups) and
Accepted 10 September 2010
Available online 18 September 2010
the time (pre-test, post-test).
Method: Participants were 54 fifth and sixth grade students who were randomly assigned to two
experimental groups (instructional self-talk, motivational self-talk) and one control group. Students were
Keywords:
Instructional self-talk
pre-tested in a chest pass and in a modified push-ups test, were instructed to use the respective self-talk
Motivational self-talk type and were post-tested in the two tests.
Physical education Results: Results showed that both self-talk groups surpassed control group in both tasks. Instructional
Motor task performance and motivational self-talk were equally effective in the chest pass test, but motivational self-talk was
more effective compared to instructional self-talk in the modified push-ups test.
Conclusions: Self-talk was an effective technique for motor task performance enhancement in physical
education. These results were discussed with reference to the task-demand-oriented matching hypothesis.
Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Research supports that the use of psychological techniques can Additionally, according to Hardy (2006, p. 84) “self-talk should be
enhance performance in sports (Hardy, Jones, & Gould, 1996; Krane defined as: a) verbalizations or statements addressed to the self, b)
& Williams, 2006) and physical education (Anderson, 1997). Over- multidimensional in nature, c) having interpretive elements asso-
all, mental training has been found to be effective in enhancing the ciated with the content of statements employed, d) somewhat
performance success and the mental skills in athletes (Vealey, dynamic, and e) serving at least two functions: instructional and
2007). Furthermore, the use of mental skills (e.g., imagery, goal motivational, for the athlete”. That is, self-talk helps athletes,
setting, and relaxation) has been proposed as a mean of perfor- through the use of appropriate cue-words, to control and organize
mance enhancement in elementary physical education (Anderson, their thoughts, to focus on basic skill components, or to motivate
1997) as it is supported that children are highly capable of learning themselves to exert more effort during practice (Zinsser, Bunker, &
and applying a variety of such mental techniques (Orlick & Williams, 2006). Therefore, many coaches incorporate self-talk into
McCaffrey, 1991). A common mental technique which is used in their programs and theorists consider it as an integral part of
sport settings is self-talk (Hardy, Oliver, & Tod, 2009). The present psychological skill training programs (Hardy et al., 1996).
study examined the effects of self-talk on students’ motor task Hardy et al. (2009) have presented possible mechanisms that
performance in primary physical education. might help to explain the self-talk/performance relationship,
Self-talk refers to “those automatic statements reflective of, and including cognitive, motivational, behavioural, and affective
deliberate techniques (e.g., thought-stopping) athletes use to processes. Cognitive mechanisms, which reflect processes such as
direct, sports-related thinking” (Hardy et al., 2009, p. 38). information processing, concentration, and attention control, have
received most attention from self-talk researchers. Landin (1994) has
proposed that cue-words can be used to increase focus on task-re-
levant stimuli. Drawing from Nideffer’s (1993) approach to atten-
q This research was supported by a scholarship to the first author from the Greek
tional style, Hardy (2006) has also proposed that the use of cue-
State Scholarship Foundation.
* Corresponding author. words may help switching attentional focus but also maintaining
E-mail address: sakisanta@hotmail.com (A. Kolovelonis). appropriate focus for specific tasks. Furthermore, Hardy (2006) has

1469-0292/$ e see front matter Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2010.09.002
154 A. Kolovelonis et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 12 (2011) 153e158

proposed Bandura’s (1997) self-efficacy theory as a framework for instructional and motivational self-talk, compared to control group,
explaining the motivational function of self-talk. Verbal persuasion increased adult male rugby union players’ hip displacement and
through positive self-talk might increase self-efficacy and subse- velocity in a vertical jump task, but only motivational self-talk had
quently effort, persistence, and performance. a positive effect on performance in this task (Edwards, Tod, &
Empirical research has provided evidence for the effectiveness McGuigan, 2008). In a similar study, both instructional and moti-
of self-talk in sport settings. In particular, self-talk had a positive vational self-talk led to greater center-of-mass displacement, greater
effect on young basketball players’ dribbling and passing skills impulse, and quicker angular rotation about the knee compared to
(Perkos, Theodorakis, & Chroni, 2002), elite female soccer players’ neutral self-talk (Tod, Thatcher, McGuigan, & Thatcher, 2009).
shooting performance (Johnson, Hrycaiko, Johnson, & Halas, 2004), The aforementioned studies provided evidence in favor of the
university physical education students’ basketball shooting skill task-demand-oriented matching hypothesis. Nevertheless, they have
(Theodorakis, Chroni, Laparidis, Bebetsos, & Douma, 2001), colle- also revealed some contradictory results that warrant further exam-
giate tennis players’ volleying skill (Landin & Hebert, 1999), and it ination (Hardy et al., 2009). For example, both instructional and
was beneficial for injury rehabilitation (Theodorakis, Beneca, motivational self-talk were effective in tasks with different demands,
Goudas, Antoniou, & Malliou, 1998). such as precision in a water polo task (Hatzigeorgiadis et al., 2004), hip
Two major types of self-talk have been identified, namely displacement and angular rotation about the knee in a vertical jump
instructional and motivational self-talk (Hardy, 2006). Instructional (Edwards et al., 2008; Tod et al., 2009), and power in the shot put
self-talk refers to statements designed to enhance performance by (Goudas et al., 2006) and knee extension (Theodorakis et al., 2000).
stimulating desired actions through attentional focus on the tech- These inconsistent results might have resulted from the different
nical aspects of a skill. Motivational self-talk refers to statements functions that the selected cue-words served. In particular, some cue-
designed to facilitate performance by building confidence, words may play both instructional and motivational role. Therefore,
increasing effort and energy expenditure and creating a positive in research testing the task-demand-oriented matching hypothesis,
mood (Hardy et al., 1996; Theodorakis, Weinberg, Natsis, Douma, & motivational and instructional self-talk should be clearly differenti-
Kazakas, 2000; Weinberg & Gould, 2007). ated (Theodorakis et al., 2000). Thus, their unique effects could be
Instructional self-talk has been investigated experimentally empirically tested. Following this suggestion, we selected cue-words
more extensively compared to motivational self-talk (Hardy et al., (see procedure section) using the guidelines proposed by Landin
2009). Indeed, it has been found that instructional self-talk had (1994) who has argued that cue-words should be: (a) brief and
a positive effect on experienced golfers’ performance (Harvey, Van phonetically simple, (b) logically associated with the referent element
Raalte, & Brewer, 2002), tennis players’ skills (Cutton & Landin, of the skill, and (c) compatible with the sequential timing of the task.
2007; Landin & Hebert, 1999), basketball players’ skills (Perkos Previous self-talk research has focused on athletes, either
et al., 2002; Theodorakis et al., 2001), and figure skaters’ skills novices or experienced, from various sports. For example, previous
(Ming & Martin, 1996). These results showed that instructional self- studies have employed young basketball players (Perkos et al.,
talk enhanced performance in various sports and tasks with 2002), amateur track and field athletes (Goudas et al., 2006),
athletes of different ages and expertise. However, the content of rugby players (Edwards et al., 2008), university physical education
self-talk seems to play a role in its effectiveness. For example, students (Hatzigeorgiadis et al., 2004), and experienced male and
Theodorakis et al. (2001) found that physical education students female golfers (Harvey et al., 2002). On the other hand, self-talk
who used the cue-word “relax” improved their performance in a 3- research in school physical education is limited. In a respective
min basketball shooting task more than those who used the cue- study, Anderson, Vogel, and Albrecht (1999) found that instruc-
word “fast” and the control group students. Motivational self-talk tional self-talk had a positive effect on third grade students’ over-
has also been found to have a positive effect on motor task hand throw performance. Therefore, more research is needed to
performance in dart-throwing (Van Raalte et al., 1995) and tennis examine the effectiveness of self-talk in school physical education.
(Hatzigeorgiadis, Zourbanos, Goltsios, & Theodorakis, 2008; Furthermore, self-talk research in young children is limited.
Hatzigeorgiadis, Zourbanos, Mpoumpaki, & Theodorakis, 2009). Previous studies employed mainly university physical education
A recent line of research has focused on comparing instructional students (Cutton & Landin, 2007; Goudas et al., 2006, study 2;
and motivational self-talk, testing the task-demand-oriented Hatzigeorgiadis et al., 2004; Landin & Hebert, 1999) and adult
matching hypothesis (Hardy et al., 2009; Theodorakis et al., 2000). athletes (Edwards et al., 2008; Goudas et al., 2006, study 1; Harvey
According to this hypothesis, instructional self-talk is more bene- et al., 2002). Exceptions are Perkos et al. (2002) who used young
ficial to tasks requiring skill, timing or precision, whereas motiva- basketball players aged 12 years old and Hatzigeorgiadis et al.
tional self-talk is more effective in tasks requiring strength or (2008, 2009) who used young tennis players aged 13e14 years
endurance (Hardy et al., 2009). Theodorakis et al. (2000) reported old. Therefore, we focused on young elementary students to
that instructional self-talk was more effective compared to moti- examine the effectiveness of self-talk in earlier ages.
vational self-talk in a soccer passing task and in a badminton serve Finally, no study to our knowledge has tested the task-demand-
task, but in a sit-up task and in a knee extension task both types of oriented matching hypothesis in physical education as Anderson
self-talk were effective. That is, when the task required fine motor et al. (1999) used only instructional self-talk in their study.
movements, instructional self-talk was more effective, whereas However, this hypothesis has important practical implications for
when it required strength and endurance both motivational and self-talk use in physical education. In particular, physical educators
instructional self-talk were effective. should know which type of self-talk, instructional, motivational or
Hatzigeorgiadis, Theodorakis, and Zourbanos (2004) compared both match better with which tasks and which cue-words are the
the effects of instructional and motivational self-talk on a precision most appropriate for the various tasks used in physical education.
and a power water polo task and found that both instructional and Thus, self-talk can be used as a technique for enhancing learning and
motivational self-talk were effective in the precision task, whereas skill development in physical education (Hardy et al., 2009; Zinsser
only motivational self-talk improved performance in the power task. et al., 2006). Therefore, expanding previous self-talk research in
Recently, Goudas, Hatzidimitriou, and Kikidi (2006) found that sports, we tested empirically the task-demand-oriented matching
instructional, motivational and kinaesthetic self-talk helped amateur hypothesis in primary physical education.
adult athletes to improve their shot put performance whereas none The aim of the study was to examine the effects of instructional
self-talk type was effective in a standing long-jump task. Finally, both and motivational self-talk on students’ chest pass and modified
A. Kolovelonis et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 12 (2011) 153e158 155

push-ups performance in primary physical education. We a) group 1: instructional self-talk, b) group 2: motivational self-talk
hypothesized that students who used self-talk, regardless of its and c) group 3: control group. Students of these groups were pre- and
type, would improve their performance more than control group post-tested on the chest pass and the modified push-ups test. The
students in both tasks (Hypothesis 1), while instructional self-talk dependent variables were the students’ scores in two tests.
would have a more positive effect on the chest pass task
(Hypothesis 2) and motivational self-talk on the modified push-ups Procedure
task (Hypothesis 3).
Permission to conduct the study was obtained from the Greek
Method Ministry of National Education and Religious Affairs and the school
principals. The study was conducted in the school gym with the
Participants presence of the experimenter. Students of each group came to the
gym in subgroups of 3e4 students. All instructions were provided
Participants were 54 students (24 boys and 30 girls, at the subgroup level. However, during the testing procedure, to
Mage ¼ 11.26, SDage ¼ 0.76) who attended two fifth grade (12 boys prevent potential bias due to social comparisons and observational
and 18 girls, Mage ¼ 10.69, SDage ¼ 0.33) and two sixth grade learning effects, students were not allowed to make eye contact and
(12 boys and 12 girls, Mage ¼ 11.97, SDage ¼ 0.52) physical education to interact with the student who was tested.
classes from two elementary schools located in a medium-sized Initially, students were informed about the procedure of the
city in central Greece. Students participated in the study voluntary. study and they warmed up for 5 min. Then, they were provided
No student refused to participate. Participants were randomly instructions regarding two tests, observed the experimenter to
assigned to three groups using the proportional stratified sampling demonstrate the two tests and were allowed to become familiar
method so the same number of boys and girls from each grade with each test for ten seconds. After, each student was pre-tested in
could be included in each group. the chest pass first and then in the modified push-ups test. After
pre-tests, students stretched for 3 min.
Measures The 5-min instruction phase followed. Group 1 students were
provided initial general self-talk instructions regarding self-talk
Basketball chest pass test definition, its functions and effectiveness and examples of famous
A chest pass accuracy test was used (Bös, 1988). Students, from athletes who use self-talk to improve their performance. Then, they
a 3 m distance, tried to hit consecutively two 30  30 cm squares as were provided specific instructions regarding the use of the
many times as they could in 30 s. The squares were painted on instructional self-talk during the two tests. For the chest pass test
a wall in a 1.2 m height and 1.8 m width. The number of the the cue-word “fingers-target” was selected (Perkos et al., 2002).
successful passes was each student’s score. High indices of objec- Students were asked to repeat the word “fingers” to remind
tivity (r: .99) and test-retest reliability (r: .91) have been reported themselves to catch the ball with their fingers and then the word
for this test (Bös, 1988). “target” to focus on the direction of the target. For the modified
push-ups test, the cue-word “bend-stretch” was used. Students
Modified push-ups test were asked to repeat the word “bend” when they were going to
Students’ strength and endurance were evaluated with a modi- bend their elbows and the word “stretch” when they were ready to
fied push-ups test (Safrit, 1995). Students put their knees on the push and stretch their elbows to come back to starting position.
floor, their feet up with ankles raised and crossed, and their hands These two words aimed at helping students to acquire and main-
parallel to their shoulders and a bit wider than shoulder width. tain a sense of rhythm during push-ups execution (Landin, 1994).
From this starting position, students slowly lowered their body Group 2 students, after receiving the same with group 1 initial self-
close to the floor, bending their elbows and keeping their back talk instructions, were instructed to use motivational self-talk
straight. When the chest was about to touch the floor, they pushed during the two tests. For the chest pass test, students were asked to
to come back to starting position. The number of the correctly repeat the cue-word “I can” to enhance their confidence in hitting
performed modified push-ups, regardless of the time, was each the target. For the modified push-ups test, students were asked to
student’s score. Johnson and Nelson (as cited in Miller, 1998) repeat the cue-word “strongly” when they were ready to push and
referred content validity and high reliability (.93) for this test. stretch their elbows to come back to starting position in order to
motivate themselves to put the necessary power to execute the
Self-talk manipulation check motion (Landin, 1994).
After each post-test, students were asked the following ques- After the oral instructions, the experimenter demonstrated the
tion: “Did you say anything to yourself during the chest pass/ use of both self-talk types in two tests. Group 1 and 2 students were
modified push-ups test?” responding on a “yes or no” format. In the prompted to use the respective cue-words loudly and continually
cases of positive response, they were asked: “If yes, what exactly during the two tests trying to “do their best”. Control group
did you say to yourself?” Students’ responses were coded into four students, for reasons of equal experimental treatment, were
categories: no, assigned cue-word (e.g., “fingers-target” for the provided healthy eating instructions after pre-test, observed an
instructional self-talk in the chest pass test), general self-talk (e.g., additional demonstration of the tests, and they were told to “do
“I must go on”) and other (e.g., “an English song”). Two coders their best”. All students were post-tested in both tests and
categorized the results independently. Kappa analysis revealed answered the respective self-talk manipulation check question.
a perfect intercoder agreement (Landis & Koch, 1977).
Statistical analysis
Design
Data were analyzed with a 3 (group)  2 (task)  2 (time)
Design included one between-groups factor, the group with three repeated measures ANOVA. Next, a 3 (group)  2 (time) repeated
levels (instructional, motivational, no self-talk), and two within- measures ANOVA were conducted for each task, followed by pre- to
groups factors, the task (chest pass, modified push-ups) and the time post-test comparisons within each group and the interpretation of
(pre-test, post-test). Three groups were formed based on this design: the respective plots (Thomas & Nelson, 2001). In the cases of
156 A. Kolovelonis et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 12 (2011) 153e158

Table 1 Group 1 Group 2 Group 3


Means and standard deviations for each group and task in pre- and post-test.
10
Chest pass Modified push-ups
9,5
Pre-test Post-test Pre-test Post-test

Number of s uc c es s ful pas s es


M SD M SD M SD M SD 9
Group 1 7.89 1.78 9.50a 2.41 12.28 5.66 13.72a 5.73
Group 2 7.00 1.46 8.50a 1.82 11.83 5.19 14.83a 6.06 8,5
Group 3 7.78 3.06 8.11 2.40 12.67 5.69 12.89 5.18
8
Note: a: Significant mean difference (p < .001) with pre-test in the respective test.
Group 1: Instructional self-talk, Group 2: Motivational self-talk, Group 3: Control
group. 7,5

7
significant differences, the effect sizes of partial h2 and Cohen’s
d (Cohen, 1988) were calculated. 6,5

6
Results 1 2
Time
Means and standard deviations for all dependent variables,
separately for each group and task, are presented in Table 1. Fig. 1. Group and time interaction in the chest pass test.

KolmogoroveSmirnov and ShapiroeWilk tests showed that the


assumption of normality was met within each group. Frequency For the modified push-ups, the 3 (group)  2 (time) repeated
of students’ responses to self-talk manipulation check questions measures ANOVA showed a significant Group  Time interaction, F
is presented in Table 2. Data were analyzed based on both the (2, 51) ¼ 11.59, p < .001, partial h2 ¼ 0.31. Pre- to post-test
original groups as well as the groups after excluding students comparisons within each group showed a significant improvement
who reported that they did not use the assigned cue-word. Both in students’ scores in the modified push-ups test for the instruc-
analyses yielded the same results. Next we report the main tional self-talk group, p < .001, d ¼ 0.25, and the motivational self-
analyses with the original groups and then the analyses where talk group, p < .001, d ¼ 0.53, but not for the control group, p ¼ 0.59.
the students who reported that they did not use the assigned The interpretation of the plot (Thomas & Nelson, 2001) showed that
cue-word were excluded. students who used motivational self-talk improved their scores in
the modified push-ups test from pre- to post-test more than
Main analyses instructional self-talk group students, who in turn surpassed
control group students (Fig. 2).
The one-way ANOVAs showed a nonsignificant difference
between-groups in pre-test measure in the chest pass test, F(2, Manipulation check
51) ¼ .87, p ¼ .43, and in the modified push-ups test, F(2, 51) ¼ .10,
p ¼ .90. The 3 (group)  2 (task)  2 (time) repeated measures Over of 70% of students of the self-talk groups responded that
ANOVA showed a significant Group  Task  Time interaction, F(2, they used the assigned cue-word during the two tests. We reran the
51) ¼ 3.25, p ¼ .047, partial h2 ¼ 0.11. To analyze this significant analyses excluding students who reported that they did not use the
interaction, two separate for each task 3 (group)  2 (time) assigned cue-word and the results remained the same for the chest
repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted. pass test, F(2, 45) ¼ 23.58, p < .001, partial h2 ¼ 0.34, and for the
For the chest pass, the 3 (group)  2 (time) repeated measures modified push-ups test, F(2, 44) ¼ 39.70, p < .001, partial h2 ¼ 0.47.
ANOVA showed a significant Group  Time interaction, F(2, 51) ¼ Furthermore, eight control group students reported the use of some
3.18, p ¼ .050, partial h2 ¼ 0.11. Pre- to post-test comparisons within kind of self-talk during the two tests. This self-talk was mainly
each group showed a significant improvement in students’ scores general phrases motivational in nature, like “to be concentrated”,
in the chest pass test for the instructional self-talk group, p < .001, “to improve myself”, or “I must go on”. To examine the potential
d ¼ 0.76, and the motivational self-talk group, p < .001, d ¼ 0.91, but effects of this spontaneous use of general self-talk on control group
not for the control group, p ¼ 0.41. The interpretation of the plot students’ performance, we compared students who referred the
(Thomas & Nelson, 2001) showed that students who used use of general self-talk with those who referred non use of self-talk
instructional and motivational self-talk improved their scores in during the two tests and we found nonsignificant differences
the chest pass test from pre- to post-test more than control group between these two control subgroups in the chest pass test,
students, but no difference between two self-talk groups was found F(1, 16) ¼ 0.68, p ¼ .42, and in the push-ups test, F(1, 16) ¼ 0.27,
(Fig. 1). p ¼ .61.

Table 2
Frequency of students’ responses to manipulation self-talk questions separately for each group and task.

Responses Chest pass Modified push-ups

Group 1 Group 2 Group 3 Group 1 Group 2 Group 3


(“Fingers-target”) (“I can”) (“Bend-stretch”) (“Strongly”)
No 3 0 9 4 0 5
Assigned cue-word 13 16 e 13 14 þ 3a e
General self-talk 2 2 8 1 1 11
Other 0 0 1 0 0 2

Note: Group 1: Instructional self-talk, Group 2: Motivational self-talk, Group 3: Control group.
a
Number of students who use the motivational cue-word of the chest pass test (“I can”).
A. Kolovelonis et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 12 (2011) 153e158 157

Group 1 Group 2 Group 3 more trials during the test. Unfortunately, no data regarding
15,5 students’ trials were collected during test procedure to verify this
interpretation.
15 This result partially supports the task-demand-oriented
14,5 matching hypothesis. Students who used instructional self-talk
improved their motor task performance and outperformed control
Number of pus h- ups

14 group students, but did not surpass students who used motiva-
13,5 tional self-talk. It seems that for precision tasks, especially when
time constraints are present, motivational and instructional self-
13 talk can be equally effective. The present and a previous study
12,5
(Hatzigeorgiadis et al., 2004) provide evidence for this argument.
Future research should further explore this issue using various
12 precision tasks and cue-words.
Hypothesis 3 was confirmed. Students who used motivational
11,5
self-talk outperformed in the modified push-ups test their peers
11 who used instructional self-talk, who in turn surpassed control
1 2
group students. These results fully support the task-demand-
Time
oriented matching hypothesis and are in line with previous findings
Fig. 2. Group and time interaction in the modified push-ups test. in sport settings which have shown that motivational self-talk is
more effective in power and endurance tasks (Edwards et al., 2008;
Hatzigeorgiadis et al., 2004). Motivational self-talk can help
Discussion students to regulate their effort, to enhance their confidence, and to
increase their concentration on the task (Goudas et al., 2006).
The aim of the study was to examine the effects of instructional However, consistent with previous research (Theodorakis et al.,
and motivational self-talk on elementary students’ motor task 2000), instructional self-talk had also a positive effect on the
performance in two tasks with different demands, namely a chest power-endurance task, but it was less effective compared to moti-
pass and a modified push-ups task. Generally, the results showed vational self-talk.
that self-talk was effective in enhancing students’ motor task The present findings provide evidence for the effectiveness of
performance in physical education and provided partial support for self-talk in physical education, showing that students who used this
the task-demand-oriented matching hypothesis (Hardy et al., 2009; technique, regardless of its type, improved their motor task
Theodorakis et al., 2000). performance. Interestingly, self-talk in general had a stronger effect
The results fully supported hypothesis 1. Students who used on students’ performance in the chest pass test (d ¼ .76 & .91)
self-talk, regardless of its type, improved their post-test perfor- compared to the push-ups test (d ¼ .25 & .53). These results imply
mance in both tests compared to pre-test, whereas control group that the use of self-talk can have a powerful positive effect on young
students did not improve. These improvements varied from small students’ learning and skill development in physical education,
to large (d ¼ .25e.91). These results expand previous research in supporting previous views that children are highly capable of
sport settings, which has shown that self-talk can enhance sport learning and applying a variety of such mental techniques (Orlick &
performance (Hatzigeorgiadis et al., 2009; Johnson et al., 2004; McCaffrey, 1991). The present study, expanding previous research
Perkos et al., 2002; Theodorakis et al., 2001). Furthermore, in line with undergraduate or collegiate students and adult athletes,
with a previous study in physical education (Anderson et al., 1999) employed young elementary students, increasing our under-
which found a positive effect of instructional self-talk on third standing regarding self-talk effectiveness in earlier ages. Further-
grade students’ overhand throw performance, the present study more, results provide additional evidence regarding the task-
showed that elementary students can use self-talk to enhance their demand-oriented matching hypothesis. Motivational self-talk was
motor task performance in physical education. more effective in the power-endurance task, while both instruc-
The results did not support hypothesis 2. Students who used tional and motivational self-talk were effective in the precision
instructional self-talk did not outperform those who used motiva- task. That is, self-talk content is critical for its effectiveness and
tional self-talk in chest pass. In fact, the size of the motivational should be appropriately matched with the demands of the task to
self-talk effect on students’ chest pass performance was larger be performed (Hardy et al., 2009).
compared to instructional self-talk (d ¼ .91 and .76, respectively). Following the suggestion of including a manipulation check in
This result is in contrast with previous research (Theodorakis et al., self-talk research (Hardy et al., 2009), we asked students what they
2000) which showed that instructional self-talk was more effective were thinking during the two tests. Over of 70% of students of the
in soccer and badminton precision tasks compared to motivational self-talk groups responded that they used the assigned cue-word
self-talk. However, this result is consistent with a previous similar during two tests. This percentage should be considered acceptable
study in sport settings (Hatzigeorgiadis et al., 2004) which found (Edwards et al., 2008; Tod et al., 2009). Nevertheless, we reran the
that both instructional and motivational self-talk had a positive analyses excluding students who reported that they did not use the
effect on a precision water polo task. It has been proposed that self- assigned cue-word and the results remained the same. Further-
talk can positively affect performance via increasing attention to more, consistent with previous research (Hardy, Hall, Gibbs, &
the task. Theodorakis, Hatzigeorgiadis, and Chroni (2008) and Greenslade, 2005; Hardy et al., 2009), many control group
Hatzigeorgiadis, Zourbanos, and Theodorakis (2007) have provided students reported the use of some kind of self-talk during two tests
evidence regarding this attentional function of self-talk. In the (e.g., general phrases motivational in nature). However, these forms
present study the chest pass test required precision and increased of internal dialogue were not beneficial for control group students,
attention. Instructional and motivational self-talk helped students as no significant difference was found between control group
to increase their attention during the 30-s chest pass test and thus students who reported that they have used some kind of self-talk
to improve their performance. Additionally, students, who used and those who did not, in both tests. This result provides additional
motivational self-talk, probably exerted more effort and executed evidence that the selection and use of appropriate and specific for
158 A. Kolovelonis et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 12 (2011) 153e158

each task cue-words helps students to improve their motor task Hardy, L., Jones, J. G., & Gould, D. (1996). Understanding psychological preparation for
sport: Theory and practice of elite performers. Chichester: John Wiley.
performance.
Harvey, D. T., Van Raalte, J., & Brewer, B. W. (2002). Relationship between self-talk
From an applied perspective, physical educators should teach and golf performance. International Sport Journal, 6, 84e91.
students to use self-talk technique to enhance their motor task Hatzigeorgiadis, A., Zourbanos, N., Goltsios, C., & Theodorakis, Y. (2008). Investi-
performance in primary physical education. Both instructional and gating the functions of self-talk: the effects of motivational self-talk on self-
efficacy and performance in young tennis players. The Sport Psychologist, 22,
motivational self-talk are effective. However, consistent with the 458e471.
task-demand-oriented matching hypothesis, physical educators Hatzigeorgiadis, A., Zourbanos, N., Mpoumpaki, S., & Theodorakis, Y. (2009).
should discern the basic features of each task and select the Mechanisms underlying the self-talk-performance relationship: the effects of
motivational self-talk on self-confidence and anxiety. Psychology of Sport and
appropriate, for each task, cue-words. The criteria provided by Exercise, 10, 186e192.
Landin (1994) could be a useful guide in this process. Hatzigeorgiadis, A., Zourbanos, N., & Theodorakis, Y. (2007). The moderating effects
A possible limitation of the present study may be that the results of self-talk content on self-talk functions. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 19,
240e251.
were based exclusively on quantitative evaluations of students’ Hatzigeorgiadis, A., Theodorakis, Y., & Zourbanos, N. (2004). Self-talk in the
motor task performance, using only one post-test trial. Technical swimming pool: the effects of self-talk on thought content and performance on
aspects of the skills’ execution should also be evaluated in future water-polo tasks. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 16, 138e150.
Johnson, J., Hrycaiko, D. W., Johnson, G. V., & Halas, J. M. (2004). Self-talk and female
research. Furthermore, the examination of the effects of the youth soccer performance. The Sport Psychologist, 18, 44e59.
different types of self-talk on learning sports skills could be an Krane, V., & Williams, J. M. (2006). Psychological characteristics of peak perfor-
interesting area for future research. In these cases, extended prac- mance. In J. M. Williams (Ed.), Applied sport psychology: Personal growth to peak
performance (5th ed.). (pp. 207e227) New York: McGraw Hill.
tice sessions and retention measures should be included. Another
Landin, D. (1994). The role of verbal cues in skill learning. Quest, 46, 299e313.
possible limitation may be that not all students of self-talk groups Landin, D., & Hebert, E. P. (1999). The influence of self-talk on the performance
used the assigned cue-words. Nevertheless, results remained the of skilled female tennis players. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 11,
same after excluding students who reported that did not use the 263e282.
Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for
assigned cue-word. Future research should employ largest samples categorical data. Biometrics, 33, 159e174.
to form distinct experimental groups discarding participants who Miller, D. K. (1998). Measurement by the physical educator. Why and how (3rd ed.).
would not use the assigned cue-words (Hardy et al., 2009). Champaign, IL: McGraw Hill.
Ming, S., & Martin, L. G. (1996). Single-subject evaluation of self-talk package for
Furthermore, future research should use various tasks, alternatives improving figure skating performance. The Sport Psychologist, 10, 227e238.
cue-words and other types of self-talk (e.g., kinaesthetic; Goudas Nideffer, R. N. (1993). Attention control training. In R. N. Singer, M. Murphey, &
et al., 2006) to explore further the nature of the task-demand- L. K. Tennant (Eds.), Handbook of research on sport psychology (pp. 127e170).
New York: Macmillan.
oriented matching hypothesis in physical education. Finally, the Orlick, T., & McCaffrey, N. (1991). Mental training with children for sport and life.
effects of self-talk on motor task performance allowing the The Sport Psychologist, 5, 322e334.
participants to use self-selected self-talk cue-words from a list Perkos, S., Theodorakis, Y., & Chroni, S. (2002). Enhancing performance and skill
acquisition in novice basketball players with instructional self-talk. The Sport
provided should be examined (Harvey et al., 2002; Hatzigeorgiadis Psychologist, 16, 368e389.
et al., 2009). Safrit, M. J. (1995). Complete guide to youth fitness testing. Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.
Theodorakis, Y., Beneca, A., Goudas, M., Antoniou, P., & Malliou, P. (1998). The effect
of self-talk on injury rehabilitation. European Yearbook of Sport Psychology, 2,
References 124e135.
Theodorakis, Y., Chroni, A., Laparidis, C., Bebetsos, E., & Douma, E. (2001). Self-talk in
Anderson, A. (1997). Learning strategies in physical education: self-talk, imagery, a basketball shooting task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 92, 309e315.
and goal-setting. Journal of Physical Education, Recreation, and Dance, 68, 30e35. Theodorakis, Y., Hatzigeorgiadis, A., & Chroni, S. (2008). Self-talk: it works, but
Anderson, A., Vogel, P., & Albrecht, R. (1999). The effect of instructional self-talk on how? Development and preliminary validation of the functions of self-talk
the overhand throw. Physical Educator, 56, 215e221. questionnaire. Measurement in Physical Education and Exercise Science, 12,
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W.H. Freeman and 10e30.
Company. Theodorakis, Y., Weinberg, R., Natsis, P., Douma, E., & Kazakas, P. (2000). The effects
Bös, K. (1988). Der Heidelberger-basketball-test (HBT). Leistungssport, 18, 17e24. of motivational versus instructional self-talk on improving motor performance.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). The Sport Psychologist, 14, 253e272.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Thomas, J. R., & Nelson, J. K. (2001). Research methods in physical activity (4th ed.).
Cutton, D. M., & Landin, D. (2007). The effects of self-talk and augmented feedback Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
on learning the tennis forehand. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 19, Tod, D., Thatcher, R., McGuigan, M., & Thatcher, J. (2009). Effects of instructional and
288e303. motivational self-talk on the vertical jump. Journal of Strength and Conditioning
Edwards, C., Tod, D., & McGuigan, M. (2008). Self-talk influences vertical jump Research, 23, 196e202.
performance and kinematics in male rugby union players. Journal of Sports Vealey, R. S. (2007). Mental skill training in sport. In G. Tenenbaum, & R. C. Eklund
Sciences, 26, 1459e1465. (Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (3rd ed.). Hoboken, New Jersey: John
Goudas, M., Hatzidimitriou, V., & Kikidi, M. (2006). The effects of self-talk on Willey & Sons.
performance on throwing and jumping events. Hellenic Journal of Psychology, 3, Van Raalte, J. L., Brewer, B. W., Lewis, B. P., Linder, D. E., Wildman, G., & Kozimor, J.
105e116. (1995). Cork! The effects of positive and negative self-talk on dart performance.
Hardy, J. (2006). Speaking clearly: a critical review of the self-talk literature. Journal of Sport Behavior, 3, 50e57.
Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 7, 81e97. Weinberg, R., & Gould, D. (2007). Foundations of sport and exercise psychology (4th
Hardy, J., Hall, C. R., Gibbs, C., & Greenslade, C. (2005). Self-talk and gross motor skill ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
performance: an experimental approach? Athletic Insight, 7, 1e13. Zinsser, N., Bunker, L., & Williams, J. M. (2006). Cognitive techniques for building
Hardy, J., Oliver, E., & Tod, D. (2009). A framework for the study and application of confidence and enhancing performance. In J. M. Williams (Ed.), Applied sport
self-talk within sport. In S. Mellalieu, & S. Hanton (Eds.), Advances in applied psychology: Personal growth to peak performance (5th ed.). (pp. 349e381)
sport psychology. A review (pp. 37e74). New York: Routledge. New York: McGraw Hill.

You might also like