You are on page 1of 9

The purpose of this study was to determine the reli-

Loewenstein ability and validity of the Loewenstein Occupa-


tional Therapy Cognitive Assessment (LOTCA) bat-
Occupational T erapy tery. The battery prOVides an initial profile of the
cognitive abilities of the brain-injured patient that
Cognitive Assessment can be used as a starting point for occupational
therapy intervention and as a screening test for fur-
(LOTCA) Battery for ther assessment. The LOTCA consists of 20 subtests
and is divided into four areas: orientation, visual
Brain-Injured Patients: and spatial perception, visuomotor organization,
and thinking operations. The battery takes 30 to 45
Reliability and Validity minutes to administer. Subjects in the study con-
sisted of two patient groups (20 traumatic head in-
jury patients and 28 cerebrovascular accident pa-
tients) and one control group (55 non-brain-
Noomi Katz, MaIka Itzkovich, injured adults). Results showed interrater reliability
coeffiCients of. 82 to .97 for the various subtests and
Sara Averbuch, Betty Elazar an alpha coefficient of. 85 and above for the inter-
nal consistency of the areas ofperception, visuomo-
tor organization, and thinking operations. The Wil-
Key Words: cerebrovascular disorder • coxon two-sample test showed that all subtests differ-
cognition • head injury, occupational entiated at the. 0001 level of Significance be-
tween the patient groups and the control group.
therapy. tests, by title, Loewenstein This supported the LOTCA 's validity. Furthermore,
Occupational Therapy Cognitive factor analysis provided initial construct validation
Assessment (LOTCA) for three areas of the battery: perception, visuomo-
tor organization, and thinking operations.

T
he purpose of this study was to determine the
reliability and validity of the Loewenstein Oc-
cupational Therapy Cognitive Assessment
(LOTCA) battery for brain-injured patients. The bat-
tery provides (a) a cognitive performance assessment
on which to base occupational therapy intervention
and (b) an objective way to examine clinical change.
The subtests of the LOTCA are specifically related to
rehabilitation purposes. "The tests are expected to
proVide a picture of the patient's abilities and defi-
ciencies with a view towards his capacity to cope with
everyday and occupational tasks" (Askenasy & Rah-
mani, 1988, p. 316).
Noami Katz, PhD, OTR, is Assistant Professor, School of Oc· Traumatic head injuries are a main cause of death
cupational Therapy, Hebrew University, Mount Scopus, for young persons in the United States and elsewhere,
PO Box 24026, Jerusalem 91240, Israel. and cause major and persisting disabilities that re-
Maika Itzkovich, OTR, is Research Coordinator, Occupa- quire long periods of treatment and rehabilitation
tional Therapy Department, Loewenstein Rehabilitation (Adamovich, Henderson, & Auerbach, 1985). The
Hospital, Raanana, Israel. high incidence and severity of head injuries warrant
the attention of health professionals such as occupa-
Sara Averbuch, OTR, is Senior Occupational Therapist, Loe-
wenstein Rehabilitation Hospital, and faculty member, Oc- tional therapists (Zoltan & Ryckman Meeder, 1985).
cupational Therapy Department, Tel Aviv University, Adults who have had cerebrovascular accidents
Ramat Aviv, Israel. (CVAs) make up one of the largest hospitalized popu-
lations, and they require long rehabilitation periods.
Betty Elazar, OTR, is Director, Occupational Therapy De-
This population represents the largest incidence of a
partment, Loewenstein Rehabilitation Hospital, Raanana,
single diagnosis evaluated and treated by occupa-
Israel.
tional therapists (Allen, 1985; Ottenbacher, 1980;
This article was accepted for publication October 27, 1988. Siev, Freishtat, & Zoltan, 1986; Trombly, 1983).

184 March 1989, Volume 43, Number 3

Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 02/18/2021 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms


In both of these populations, perceptual cogni- mation processing theories, and acqUisitional frame
tive deficits are the major determinants of confusion of reference. They identified six areas worthy of eval-
and a lack of rehabilitation progress even in patients uation: orientation, attention, visual processing,
whose motor skills have returned (Siev et aI., 1986). motor planning, cognition, and occupational behav-
Cerebral dysfunctions caused by brain lesions pro- ior. These areas were assumed to be in hierarchical
duce intellectual and/or behavioral changes that af- order according to the capacity to process information
fect the person's capacity to carry out daily tasks (Na- and, therefore, to gUide the assessment sequence.
jenson, Rahmani, Elaser, & Averbuch, 1984; Rahmani, Conditions that facilitated or deteriorated perfor-
1982). Therefore, the effort to restore functional mance, as well as strategies used by the patient, were
capacity must include an assessment of cognitive considered.
abilities. Abreu (1987) designed a manual with theoretical
Neuropsychological test batteries have been de- and practical gUidelines for the rehabilitation of per-
veloped for various purposes in an attempt to relate ceptual-cognitive dysfunction based on four ap-
behavioral defiCits to underlying brain dysfunction. proaches: cognitive retraining, neurodevelopmental
According to Goldstein and Ruthven (1983), the his- principles, biomechanical principles, and motor
tory of psychological assessment is characterized by learning. The manual includes suggestions for variOUS
controversy and rapid change, and has gone full circle assessment instruments and techniques for process
from clinical examination and interview, through re- testing each area looking at qualitative and quantita-
fined quantitative and qualitative evaluation, back to tive performance. However, as emphasized by the au-
more clinical methods that focus on processes as well thor, the techniques have not been tested for validity
as end results (Lezak, 1983). This pattern is exempli- and reliability.
fied by Luria's 1973 neuropsychological tests, which An important contribution to occupational ther-
were integrated into a set of tests by Christensen apy literature is Siev et al.'s 1986 book, which pro-
(1975) and further developed into the Luria-Nebraska vides information on standardized and nonstandard-
Neuropsychological Battery (LNNB) (Golden, 1984; ized procedures for assessing the perceptual and cog-
Golden, Hammerse, & Purisch, 1980). nitive abilities of adult stroke patients. As the authors
Burnell (1985) and Barret (1986) suggested that stated, however, most evaluations are nonstandar-
the LNNB is an appropriate assessment tool in occupa- dized, and research is only in its early stages. Some of
tional therapy. However, the full battery is too exten- the book's authors also contributed to the Perceptual
sive and too detailed to be practical for an initial eval- Motor Evaluation battery, which was developed for
uation in occupational therapy, which typically fo- head-injured and other neurologically impaired
cuses on basic deficits related to dysfunction in the adults (Jabri, Ryckman, Panikoff, & Zoltan, 1987).
performance of daily tasks (Najenson et aI., 1984) This evaluation was designed to comprehensively
Moreover, the LNNB focuses mainly on diagnostic cat- screen perceptual motor skills, but standardization
egories and the localization of lesions (Askenasy & has not been completed and only interrater reliability
Rahmani, 1988). The goal of the short screening ver- coefficients have been proVided. Except for three
sion of the LNNB is only to predict whether giving the subtest scores from the groSS/Visual skill area, which
full battery is indicated (Golden, 1987). Therefore, are between .60 and .70, all are above .82. To date,
the LNNB screening test should not be used for occu- occupational therapy research studies related to the
pational therapy treatment planning related to activi- brain-injured population have concentrated primarily
ties of daily living. on stroke victims; only a few have addressed the
problems of people with traumatic head injuries
Literature Review (Baum & Hall, 1981; Giles, 1988; Meeder, 1982;
Despite the prevalence of brain-injured patients in Schwartz, Shipkin, & Cermak, 1979). Most studies
occupational therapy and the major role perceptual have attempted to describe relationships between
cognitive abilities play in their dysfunctions, current various cognitive disorders and functional perfor-
occupational therapy theories or frames of reference mance (e.g., between visual perception and activities
have not been developed. Allen's 1985 cognitive dis- of daily living) (Carter, Howard, & O'Neil, 1983;
ability theory was developed primarily for use with Carter, Oliveira, Duponte, & Lynch, 1988; Kaplan &
the mentally ill. It was assumed that the theory ap- Hier, 1982; Kowalski Lundi & Mitcham, 1984;
plied to all patients with central nervous system defi- Mitcham, 1982). Others have combined factors of
cits, but attempts to apply it to the head trauma popu- constructional apraXia and body scheme. For exam-
lation have been made only recently (Katz, 1988) ple, MacDonald (1960) and Warren (1981) found that
Abreu and Toglia (1987) presented a model of body scheme was a better predictor of dressing per-
cognitive rehabilitation for brain-injured patients formance than praxis function as examined by a copy-
based on Luria's neuropsychological approach, infor- ing test, and Bradley (1982) found a difference in the

The Americanjoumal a/Occupational Therapy 185

Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 02/18/2021 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms


effectiveness of three-dimensional praxis tests (as patients' daily functioning and their ability to cope
compared to two-dimensional) in predicting upper with occupational tasks. EmphasiS is placed on cogni-
extremity dressing skills. tive training through the performance of purposeful
Other researchers have employed more compre- tasks in daily activities.
hensive batteries consisting of subtests assessing vi- The LOTCA battery is based on clinical experi-
sual perception, praxis, and body scheme (Meeder, ence as well as on Luria's neuropsychological and
1982; Taylor, 1968; Van Deusen Fox & Harlowe, Piaget's developmental theories and evaluation pro-
1984). Of these, only Taylor used sub tests for con- cedures (Golden, 1984; Inhelder & Piaget, 1964). The
ceptual and operational thinking, although these battery is composed of 20 subtests including the Riska
areas represent major problems for the stroke popula- Object Classification (ROC) (Williams Riska & Allen,
tion. Moreover, few studies included non-brain-in- 1985), which was added to enhance the evaluation of
jured subjects (Concha, 1986; Kaplan & Hier, 1982; categorization abilities with attributes such as shape,
Schwartz et aI., 1979; Taylor, 1968). color, or number, in addition to tangible object cate-
This review revealed that occupational therapists gorization. This made it analogous to the sequence
use an array of instruments and methods to evaluate operation of the battery, which has both a tangible
perceptual and cognitive abilities, and that only a few pictorial subtest and a geometrical subtest. At the time
tests were developed in occupational therapy and the study was conducted, however, the battery con-
studied for their measurement properties. The most sisted of 19 subtests. (Orientation initially had only
systematic attempt to determine instrument validity one subtest.)
has been the studies of the St. Marys CVA Evaluation Children's performances on the LOTCA were as-
(Hariowe & Van Deusen, 1984; Van Deusen & Har- sessed recently to determine the age norms of the
lowe, 1986, 1987). This evaluation was designed for various subtests and to verify the hierarchical order of
use in an acute care setting and includes self-care acquisition of the various cognitive competencies in-
activities, motor strength, arm and hand strength, bi- cluded in the battery. Results suggested a progression
lateral awareness, and perception (including stereog- with age from 6 to 12 years in both level and speed of
nosis and body scheme). The studies of the St. Marys performance (Averbuch, 1988).
battery focused on the construct validity of the battery The LOTCA is divided into four areas: orienta-
as a whole and of each of its parts. However, no con- tion, perception, visuomotor organization, and think-
trol group was compared with the acute CVA group, ing operations. Procedures for assessing aphasic pa-
and no reliability studies were reported. tients have been incorporated into the tests. The bat-
More recently, Boys, Fisher, Holzberg, & Reid, tery takes a total of 30 to 45 minutes, and can be
(1988) reported that the Ontario Society of Occupa- administered in 2 or 3 short sessions if necessary. A 4-
tional Therapy (OSOT) Perceptual Evaluation (which or 5-point rating scale is used for scoring each subtest.
comprises six areas: sensory function, scanning func- A profile of the battery is given in Figure 1.
tion, apraxia, body awareness, spatial relations, and The purpose of this study was to investigate the
visual agnosia) has high internal correlations and dif- reliability and validity of the LOTCA's measurement
ferentiates between neurologically impaired and properties.
non-neurologically impaired people. These findings
indicate the battery's reliability and validity. However, Method
patients with aphasia or traumatic head injury were
Subjects
excluded from the study for unexplained reasons, and
as in previous batteries, thinking operations and The sample consisted of three groups. Two groups
problem solving were not evaluated. comprised brain-injured adults: 20 patients diagnosed
The LOTCA was developed at Loewenstein Reha- with craniocerebral injury (CCl) and 28 patients
bilitation Hospital (LRH) in Israel to assess the basic diagnosed with CVA. A third group, the control, con-
cognitive abilities of brain-injured patients. The term sisted of 55 non-brain-injured adults who were se-
basic cognitive abilities is defined as those "intellec- lected according to age (between 20 and 70 years),
tual functions thought to be prerequisite for manag- sex, and social position class. Social position is a
ing everyday encounters with the environment" (Na- measure determined by a person's years of education
jenson et aI., 1984, p. 315). Cognition is conceived of and type of occupation. For each of the five social
as a general term that covers attention, perception, position classes, the scale lists the type of occupations
thinking, and memory. included and the range of years of education. An
Information from the battery and from a func- equation that combines both variables is used to de-
tional evaluation of the patient's activities of daily liv- termine a person's social position class (Hollingshead
ing is used to plan the occupational therapy treat- & Redlich, 1958; Williams Riska & Allen, 1985, p. 329,
ment. Treatment at LRH is aimed at improving the proVides the scale).

186 March 1989, Volume 43, Number 3

Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 02/18/2021 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms


Instructions: For each item, circle the appropriate number.

SCore
Subtests low high Comments

Orientation
Time 1 2 3 4
Place 1 2 3 4
Perception
Object identification 1 2 3 4
Shape identification 1 2 3 4
Overlapping figures 1 2 3 4
Object constancy 1 2 3 4
Spatial perception 1 2 3 4
Praxis 1 2 3 4

Visuomotor Organization
Copying geometric forms 1 2 3 4
Reproducing a two-dimensional 1 2 3 4
model
Constructing a pegboard design 1 2 3 4
Constructing a colored block design 1 2 3 4
Constructing a plain block design 1 2 3 4
Reproducing a puzzle 1 2 3 4
Drawing a clock 1 2 3 4

Thinking Operations
Categorization 1 2 3 4 5
ROC: Unstructured 1 2 3 4 5
Structured 1 2 3 4 5
Pictorial sequence 1 2 3 4
Geometrical sequence 1 2 3 4

Indicate length of time given in one session or more


Based on observation during test performance, circle the ap propriate number:
Attention and concentration 1 2 3 4

Figure 1. LOTCA Battery Scoring Sheet. ROC = Riska Object Classification_

As can be seen in Table 1, which summarizes the group. This is typical for the type of injUry. Mean years
demographic characteristics of the subjects, the dis- of education were similar in each group, but CVA
tribution of social position classes was not equaL The patients were generally lower in social position.
table shows also that the CCl group contained more
male subjects and younger subjects than the CVA Procedures
Every patient diagnosed with CVA or CCl who was
Table 1 admitted to the LRH during a 3-month period was
Subjects' Demographic Characteristics assessed with the LOTCA battery upon referral to oc-
CCI Group CVA Group Control Group cupational therapy, and again after 2 months of treat-
Variable (n = 20) (n = 28) (n = 55) ment. The second test was intended to proVide a per-
Sex formance profile after initial recovery, which would
Male 15 14 26 suggest more long-lasting problem areas.
Female 5 14 29
Age i (SD) 254 (93) 57.5 (73) 426 (126) Six occupational therapists trained to administer
Years of the evaluation tested the subjects. lnterrater reliability
education 96 (30) 92 (4.0) 13.3 (44) was determined prior to data collection in two ways.
Social
position First, three pairs of raters tested 10 subjects, and each
class I 0 2 10 separately scored the subjects' performance on the 19
II 0 0 15 subtests. Spearman's rank correlation coefficient be-
III 2 3 13
IV 4 5 9
tween the raters showed interrater reliability ranging
V 4 18 8 from .82 to .97 for the various subtests. Second, a
Soldiers 10 video recording of the assessment of one patient was
Note. CCI = craniocerebral injUry. CVA = cerebrovascular accident made. Each of the six therapists indiVidually viewed

The American journal a/Occupational Therapy 187

Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 02/18/2021 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms


the recording and scored it. The therapists reached 19 subtests at the .0001 level, and social position was
100% agreement for 14 subtests, 86% for 4 subtests, significant only for the areas of visuomotor organiza-
and 72% for 1 subtest This last subtest was changed, tion and thinking operations at the .02 level. This in-
and thereafter the therapists reached 86% agreement. dicated that, among the normal subjects, years of edu-
These levels of agreement and the correlation coeffi- cation was the main variable related to perceptual
cients above .80 were considered acceptable, and en- cognitive performance. In the patient samples, none
abled systematic data collection. of the variables was significantly related to perfor-
mance, which indicated the predominance of the
Data Analysis brain damage over other preexisting conditions.
Descriptive statistics and non parametric statistical
procedures were used because the data were mea- Descrzption ofPerformance
sured on an ordinal scale, the sample sizes were mod- Table 2 shows means and standard deviations for all
erate, and no assumption of an underlying normal subtests of the two patient groups at each of the two
distribution could be made for a group of brain-in- testing times, and for the control group. The control
jured patients. group performed almost perfectly on all subtests ex-
cept categorization, geometrical sequence, and classi-
Results fication. It seemed that, although the differences were
small, variability increased with the complexity of the
Demographic Variables
tests.
The chi-square analysis of the relationships between In the CCI group, the scores on the first assess-
demographic variables and performance on the bat- ment ranged from a low mean of 2.1 (range 1 to 5) on
tery revealed that, in the control group, age was non- categorization to a high of 3.6 on visual identification
significant, years of education was significant for all of objects. In the CVA group, scores ranged from a low

Table 2
Means and Standard Deviations of the LOTCA Subtests for Three Populations
CCI Group Assessments CVA Group Assessments
Control Group
Subtests 2 2 Assessments
ORIENTATION

Time and place 24 (14) 33 (12) 33 (11) 36 (10) 4.0


PERCEPTION

Object identification 36 (89) 4.0 39 (42) 39 (.26) 4.0


Shape identification 31 (79) 35 (60) 29 (97) 33 (.72) 39 (0.3)
Overlapping figures 30 (12) 38 (44) 29 (11) 34 (79) 39 (0.3)
Object constancy 29 (14) 35 (10) 28 (14) 32 (11) 39 (0.3)
Spatial perception 29 (14) 35 (95) 35 (I.I) 36 (95) 39 (0. I)
Praxis 29 (14) 38 (64) 34 (87) 37 (60) 39 (0.1)
VISliOMOTOR ORGANIZATION

Copying geometric forms 2.5 (13) 32 (95) 2.8 (10) 30 (96) 39 (0.3)
Reproducing a two-
dimensional model 27 (14) 34 (88) 2.6 (12) 31 (97) 39 (0.4)
Constructing a pegboard
design 24 (13) 33 (11) 24 (I I) 28 (11) 38 (0.5)
Constructing a colored
block design 2.6 (14) 33 (11) 25 (1.3) 27 (14) 38 (0.5)
Constructing a plain
block design 24 (12) 30 (1.3) 2.0 (1.3) 23 (12) 37 (06)
Reproducing a puzzle 24 (13) 30 (12) 20(1.1) 24 (13) 38 (0.6)
DraWing a clock 23 (13) 30 (12) 25 (1.0) 28(1.1) 38 (0.5)
THINKING OPERATIONS

Categorization (I -5) 2.1 (14) 29 (17) 21 (14) 28 (14) 4.2 (10)


ROC: Unstructured (1-5) 23 (1.5) 29 (13) 20 (12) 24 (15) 39 (10)
Structured (1-5) 2.7 (17) 31 (1.7) 18 (13) 23 (16) 4.2 (10)
Pictorial sequence 2.6 (14) 36 (12) 27 (14) 31 (13) 39 (03)
Geometrical sequence 2.2 (12) 24 (1.5) 16 (87) 2.1 (12) 36 (0.8)
NOle. Wilcoxon test differentiated at .0001 level of significance between controls and both patients groups on all subtests (except object
identification) during Assessment 1, and above the .02 level of significance during Assessment 2 CCI = craniocerebral injury. CVA = cerebro-
vascular accident.

188 March 1989, Volume 43, Number 3

Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 02/18/2021 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms


of 1.6 on geometrical sequence to a high of 3.9 on process of performance and evaluate the speed factor,
identification of objects. In the first assessment, both which was not measured on the LOTCA, the standard
CCI and CVA patients showed impairment on almost procedure of the Block Design subtest was varied, and
all subtests, with greater variability among them. By patients were given an unlimited amount of time to
the second assessment, 2 months later, some im- complete the designs, although the patients were un-
provement on the average was seen, more so in the aware of this (Lezak, 1983). Thus, two scores were
CCI group than in the CVA group. given, one for performance in the standard time and a
second if the patient's score increased in the unlim-
Internal Consistency Reliability ited time situation. This procedure resulted in a sec-
ond score for some of the patients. In these cases, a
Alpha coefficients were calculated for three areas in-
stronger correlation was found (r = .77). Interest-
cluded in the battery. An alpha coefficient of .87 was
ingly, when the same procedure was used with a
found for perception, which consisted of 5 subtests
group of 20 chronic schizophrenic adult inpatients
(object identification, shape identification, overlap-
(r = .69 and r = .78, respectively), almost identical
ping figures, object constancy, and spatial percep-
results were found (Katz, 1988). This procedure,
tion). An alpha coefficient of .95 was found for visuo-
which was first employed by Katz (1985) with a sam-
motor organization, which consisted of 7 subtests
ple of depressive patients to study the effects of slow-
(copying geometric forms; reproducing a 2-dimen-
ness on performance, appears useful for patient
sional model, constructing a pegboard design, con-
evaluation.
structing a colored block design, constructing a plain
block design, reproducing a puzzle, and draWing a
clock). The third area, thinking operations, which Construct Validity
consisted of 5 subtests (categorization, ROC unstruc-
Principal component factor analysis with orthogonal
tured and ROC structured, pictorial sequence, and
varimax rotation was performed to determine the con-
geometrical sequence), had an alpha coefficient
struct validity of the battery. This validation procedure
of .85.
determined whether the subtests measure what they
These high reliability coefficients support the
were intended to measure and cluster into the three
structure of the battery. On the other hand, correla-
assumed underlying areas of perception, visuomotor
tion coefficients ranging from 40 to .80 among the
organization, and thinking operations. The analysis
subtests suggest that they are not all equivalent, and
included 18 subtests (orientation was excluded be-
therefore, that all parts of the battery should be
cause it had only 1 subtest) and was performed sepa-
retained.
rately for the patient groups and for the control group.
For the patient groups, the results of both assessments
Validity were included (n = 96). Table 3 presents the results
To evaluate the battery's ability to differentiate be- of the analyses for the patient and control groups on
tween known groups, the Wilcoxon two-sample test three factors.
was used to compare each patient group with the Factor 1 in the patient groups, the Visuomotor
control group. Results showed that all subtests except Organization and Sequence subtests, explained 44%
identification of objects differentiated at the .000 1 of the variance. All 7 of the Visuomotor Organization
level of significance between the control group and subtests loaded above .63. Praxis loaded on Factor 2,
each of the patient groups at the first assessment (z together with spatial perception, object identification,
scores ranged from 4.0 to 6.2), and above the .02 level and overlapping figures. Shape identification loaded
for the second assessment (z scores ranged from 25 on Factor 3 with categorization and classification.
to 4.5). The Kruskal-Wallis test was administered Object constancy was the only subtest that loaded
among the three groups and showed the same level of lower (53) and equally on both Factors 1 and 2.
significance at both assessment times. This finding These results within the patient groups may be
supported the battery's validity in assessing percep- explained by the fact that, in all of the Perception
tual cognitive impairment and in differentiating be- subtests as well as in the Praxis subtest, subjects were
tween known groups. reqUired to identify, comprehend, point to given ob·
Criterion validity was examined within the CCl jects, or imitate movements, but in the Visuomotor
group for the visuomotor organization area with the Organization and Thinking Operations subtests they
Block Design subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelli- were asked to perform a task, manipulate objects, or
gence Scale (WAIS) (Wechsler, 1981). A correlation copy and build things involVing motor, spatial, and
coefficient of r = .68 was found between the score on logical knowledge. These latter requirements are
the Block Design subtest and the mean score on the more complex, and integrate the higher mental pro-
Visuomotor Organization subtests. To examine the cesses of problem solving, planning, and motor orga-

The American journal a/Occupational Therapy 189

Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 02/18/2021 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms


Table 3
Factor Loadings From a Principal Component Factor Analysis With Orthogonal Varimax Rotation of the LOTCA Battery
Patients (n = 96) Control Subjects (n = 55)
SuI-nests Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3
PEHCEI'TION
ObjeCt identification .09 79" 05
Shape identification .02 55 .62- 18 .81 - 07
Overlapping figures 33 67- 38 .89" .20 29
Object constancy 53 53 35 96" .21 02
Spatial perception 33 68- 11 .96- .21 02
Praxis .48 69- 11 .96- .21 02
VIS110MOTOH ORGANIZATION
Copying geometric forms .73- .42 25 21 52 .70-
Producing a 2·
dimensional model .68- 33 31 .11 .54- - 34
Constructing a pegboard
design .80- 29 .27 .44 .48- - 38
Constructing a colored
block design .80- .28 .27 54 .07 .63-
Constructing a plain
block design .81- 19 28 25 .45 .74-
Reproducing a puzzle .86- 18 .24 .57- - .46 .47
Drawing a clock .n- .31 30 .51 .58-- .01
TIIINKING OI'EHATIONS
Categorization .59 19 .61* .17 .77- .22
ROC: Unstructured .46 19 .73- .21 .74- 43
Structured 47 13 .77- 19 .84- 15
Pictorial sequence .73- 40 .27 89- 20 29
Geometrical sequence .63- 05 58 .27 .71- .22
Percent variance 44 12 10 33 23 6
- Loadings above .60. - - Loadings around .50 highest for a subtest.

nizational abilities (Najenson et al., 1984). A very sim- ceiling effect). Differences among them may be bet-
ilar factor structure can be seen in the factor analysis ter explained by- basic perceptual differences or by
of items measuring perceptual functions that was per- the most elementary praxis and pictorial sequence
formed by Eriksson, Bernspang, and Fugl-Meyer, thinking test, in which the variability was very small,
(1988). In their analysis, visuomotor organization than by the somewhat more complex tests in which
items (termed high-order perception) loaded on Fac- the variability was greater (see Table 2). However,
tor 1, and visual and spatial perception items (termed because the number of control subjects was small for
low-order perception) loaded on Factor 2, which ex- a factor analysis, the results should be confirmed with
plained 49% and 16.3% of the variance, respectively, a larger group. Interestingly, in both groups, shape
for a group of 109 stroke patients. (Some of their identification loaded on the same factor as thinking
items were adapted from an earlier version of the operations. The reason for this appears to be that the
LOTCA). test included four familiar shapes, such as circle,
In the control group, the results were different. square, triangle, and rectangle, but also four more
Three Perception subtests loaded highly (above .89) difficult ones, such as rhomboid, trapezoid, hexagon,
on Factor 1, along with Praxis and pictorial sequence, and half-circle, so that it became more than a basic
which explained 33% of the variance. Factor 2 con- identification task. As scored in the study, shape
sisted of the Shape Identification and Thinking Oper- identification seemed to require higher formal knowl-
ations, subtests and had lower loadings (around .50) edge; therefore, it clustered together with the same
for reproducing a two-dimensional model, construct- factor as thinking operations.
ing a pegboard design, and draWing a clock. Factor 3 Comparison between the groups showed that,
was composed of 3 Visuomotor Organization sub- within the patient group, the area of visuomotor orga-
tests: block design, constructing a plain block design, nization explained the most variance in performance,
and copying geometric forms. and the other two areas (perception and thinking
The profile of the control group was different in operations) explained almost equal percentages of
the importance of the factors accounting for the vari- the variance. In contrast, within the control group,
ance, which seemed to be due to the fact that all perception explained the most variance, followed by
subjects performed high on almost all subtests (a thinking operations; visuomotor organization contrib-

190 March 1989, Volume 43, Number 3


Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 02/18/2021 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms
uted less to the variance explained. However, in both tion-based score for attention and concentration.
groups the total amount explained is above 60%, Length of time and number of sessions are also re-
which is substantial. corded on the scoring sheet.
The next step in the validation process of the
Discussion LOTCA battery includes continued data collection at
The measurement properties of the LOTCA were the LRH and in other countries to enlarge the database
found to be reliable for all subtests in the agreement on the adult brain-injured population and on stroke
between raters and in the internal consistency of the patients. Regarding construct validation with the fac-
three major areas: perception, visuomotor organiza- tor analysis procedure, additional studies with larger
tion, and thinking operations. The LOTCA was also patient populations and control groups are necessary
found to be valid in differentiating between healthy to prOVide further support for the structure of the bat-
adult persons and brain-injured patients. This finding, tery based on the four underlying areas of orientation,
along with the results of a previous study in which the perception, visuomotor organization, and thinking
performance profiles of psychiatric patients were operations. The relationship between the LOTCA
shown to differ from those of CCI patients (Averbuch scores and functional evaluation and activities of daily
& Katz, 1988), suggests that the LOTCA differentiates living rating scales also need to be studied, as do
level of performance as well, as some localization changes in performance related to the rehabilitative
patterns related to brain lesions emerge. In addition, process.
the beginning work on the determination of construct
Acknowledgments
validity with factor analysis demonstrated a three-fac-
tor solution in accordance with the assumed underly- We thank Sara Grinbaum, Dorit Hefner, and Batia Bor, the
occupational therapists at the Loewenstein Rehabilitation
ing areas, even though the solution differed in the Hospital who participated in the study. We also thank Pro-
strength of its explanation of the variance in perfor- fessor Levi Rahmani, who consulted in the development of
mance between patients and controls. the battery. This study was supported by the Occupational
The LOTCA battery is used also as a measure of Therapy Central Office of Kupat Holim, the Health Insur-
the patient's status over time. In those cases in which ance Institute of the General Federation of Labour in Israel.
difficulties are present at initial assessment, the References
LOTCA can be employed as an objective measure for
Abreu, B. C. (1987). Rehabilitation ofperceptual-cog-
follOWing up on patient progress. It is recommended nitive dysfunction. Unpublished manuscript.
that the assessment be repeated after an interval of at Abreu, B. c., & Toglia J. P. (1987). Cognitive rehabili-
least 2 months to avoid simple memory carryover. tation: A model for occupational therapy. Americanjournal
However, learning as a possible explanation for of Occupational Therapy, 41, 439-448.
Adamovich, B. B., Henderson, J. A., & Auerbach, S.
higher scores must always be considered because (1985) Cognitive rehabilitation of closed head injured pa-
many similar tasks are practiced during treatment. tients: A dynamiC approach. Boston: Little, Brown & Co.
Nevertheless, it is precisely this learning, if general- Allen, C. K. (1985). Occupational theraPYforpsychiat-
ized, that is the purpose of treatment, so it should not ric diseases: Measurement and management of cognitive
be regarded as a threat to validity as defined in mea- disabilities. Boston: Little, Brown & Co.
surement theory. Askenasy,J.J. M., & Rahmani, L. (1988). Neuropsycho-
social rehabilitation of head injUry. American journal of
As a result of the data analysis, some changes Physical Medicine, 66, 315-327.
were made on the score sheet (see Figure 1). The Averbuch, S., & Katz, N. (1988). Assessment of percep-
Object Identification subtest seemed to be very easy tual cognitive performance of psychiatric and brain injured
for all three subject groups, and thus may be retained adult patients. Occupational Therapy in Mental Health,
only as a start-up task. The Shape Identification sub- 8(1),57-71.
Averbuch, S. (1988). Developmental norms for the
test, as discussed earlier, seems to contain two levels Loewenstein Occupational Therapy Cognitive Assessment
of difficulty (four easy items and four harder ones), in (LOTCA) Unpublished master's thesis, New York Univer-
which the second set of shapes probably requires Sity, New York, NY.
more formal knowledge. Therefore, a score of 4 will Barret, C. (1986) In search of brain-behavior relation-
be given if the subject identifies all shapes either by ship in dementia and the Luria-Nebraska Neuropsychologi-
cal Battery. Physical & Occupational Therapy in Geriatrics,
naming, understanding, or matching. Orientation was 4, 113-139.
divided into time and place, and each will be scored Baum, B., & Hall, K. M. (1981). Relationship between
separately. A scoring key for rating attention and con- constructional praxis and dressing in the head-injured adult.
centration was added to allow analysis of these areas. Americanjournal of Occupational Therapy, 35, 438-442.
Attention and concentration are not tested with a spe- Boys, M., Fisher, P., Holzberg, C., & Reid, D. W.
(1988). The OSOT Perceptual Evaluation; A research per-
cific subtest but should be evaluated by observing the spective. American journal of Occupational Therapy, 42,
patients during performance of the LOTCA. Thus, the 92-98.
final battery consists of 20 Jubtests and an observa- Bradley, K. P. (982). The effectiveness of construc-

The Americanjournal of Occupational Therapy 191

Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 02/18/2021 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms


tional praxis tests in predicting upper extremity dressing Lezak, M. D. (1983). Neuropsychological assessment.
abilities. Occupational Therapy journal of Research, 2, New York: Oxford University Press.
184-185. Luria, A R. (1973). The working brain. London: Pen-
Burnell, D. P. (1985 July). Neuropsychological ap- guin Books.
proach in psychiatric occupational therapy. Paper deliv- MacDonald, ]. C. (1960). An investigation of body
ered at the second European Congress of Occupational scheme in adults with cerebral vascular accidents. Ameri-
Therapy, London. can Journal of Occupational Therapy, 14, 75-79
Carter, L. T., Howard, B. E., & O'Neil, W. A. (1983). Meeder, D. L. (1982). Cognitive perceptual motor eval-
Effectiveness of cognitive skill remediation in acute stroke uation research findings for adult head injured. In L. E.
patients. American journal of Occupational Therapy, 37, Trexler (Ed.), Cognitive rehabilitation, conceptualization
320-326. and intervention (pp. 153-172). New York: Plenum Press.
Carter, L. T., Oliveira, D.O., Duponte,]., & Lynch, S. V. Mitcham, M. (1982). Visual perception and its rela-
(1988). The relationship of cognitive skills performance to tionship to an activity of daily living. Occupational Therapy
activities of daily living in stroke patients. American jour- journal of Research, 2, 245-246.
nal of Occupational Therapy, 42,449-455. Najenson, T., Rahmani, L., Elasar, B., & Averbuch, S.
Christensen, A. L. (1975). Luria's neuropsychological (1984). An elementary cognitive assessment and treatment
investigation. New York: Spectrum. of the craniocerebrally injured patient. In B. A. Edelstein &
Concha, M. E. (1986). Adult performance on the devel- E. T. Couture (Eds.), Behavioral assessment and rehabilita-
opmental test of visual motor integration. The S. A. journal tion of the traumatically brain-damaged (pp. 313-338).
of Occupational Therapy, May, 11-19. New York: Plenum Press.
Eriksson, S., Bernspang, B., & Fugl-Meyer, A. R. Otten bacher, K. (1980). Cerebral vascular accident:
(1988). Perceptual and motor impairment within 2 weeks Some characteristics of occupational therapy evaluation
after stroke: A multifactorial statistical approach. Occupa- forms. American journal of Occupational Therapy, 34,
tional Therapy journal ofResearch, 8, 114-125. 268-271.
Giles, G. M. (1988). Head injury rehabilitation. In Rahmani, L. (1982). The intellectual rehabilitation of
D. W. Scott & N. Katz (Eds.), Occupational therapy in brain damaged patients. Clinical Neuropsychology, 4(1),
mental health, principles in practice (pp. 169-180). Lon- 44-45
don: Taylor & Francis. Schwanz, R., Shipkin, D., & Cermak, L. S. (1979). Ver-
Golden, C.]. (1984). Rehabilitation and the Luria-Ne- bal and nonverbal memory abilities of adult brain damaged
braska Neuropsychological Battery. In B. A Edelstein & E. patients. American journal of Occupational Therapy, 33,
T. Conture (Eds.), Behavioral assessment and rehabilita- 79-83.
tion of the traumatically brain-damaged (pp. 83-120) Siev, E., Freishtat, B., & loltan, B. (1986). Perceptual
New York: Plenum Press. and cognitive dysfunction in the adult stroke patient (rev.
Golden, c.]. (1987). Screening test for the Luria-Ne- ed.) Thorofare, N]: Slack.
braska Neuropsychological Battery: Adult and children's Taylor, M. M. (1968). Analysis of dysfunction in left
forms. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. hemiplegia following stroke. Americanjournal ofOccupa-
Golden, C. J., Hammerse, T. A., & Purisch, A. D. tional Therapy, 22, 512-520.
(1980) The Luria-Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery. Trombly, C. A. (Ed) (1983). Occupational therapy for
Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. physical dysfunction (2nd ed.). Baltimore: Williams & Wil-
Goldstein, G., & Ruthven, L. (1983). Rehabilitation of kins
the brain damaged adult. New York: Plenum Press. Van Deusen, J _, & Harlowe, D. (1986). Continued con·
Harlowe, D., & Van Deusen,]. (1984). Construct vali- struct validation of the St. Marys CVA evaluation: Brunn-
dation of the St. Marys CVA evaluation: Perceptual mea- strom arm and hand stage ratings. American journal of Oc-
sures. American journal of Occupational Therapy, 38, cupational Therapy, 40,561-563.
184-186. Van Deusen,]., & Harlowe, D. (1987). Continued con-
Hollingshead, A B., & Redlich, F C. (1958). Social struct validation of the St. Marys CVA evaluation: Bilateral
class and mental illness. New York: John Wiley & Sons. awareness scale. American journal of Occupational Ther-
Inhelder, B., & Piaget,]. (1964) The early growth of apy, 41, 242-245.
logic in the child. New York: W. W. Morton. Van Deusen Fox,]., & Harlowe, D. (1984) Construct
Jabri, J., Ryckman, D. L., Panikoff, L. B., & loltan, B. validation of occupational therapy measures used in CVA
(1987). Perceptual motor evaluation for head injured and evaluation: A beginning. American journal of Occupa-
other neurologically impaired adults (rev. ed.). Santa Clara tional Therapy, 38, 101-106.
Valley Medical Center, Occupational Therapy Department. Warren, M. (1981). Relationship of constructional
Kaplan,]., & Hier, D. (1982). Visuospatial deficits after apraxia and body scheme disorders to dressing performance
right hemisphere stroke. American journal of Occupa- in adult CVA. Americanjournal of Occupational Therapy,
tional Therapy, 36, 314-321. 35,431-437.
Katz, N. (1985). Research on major depression. In C. K. Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence
Allen (Ed)., Occupational therapy for psychiatric diseases: Scale. New York: Psychological Corporation.
Measurement and management of cognitive disabilities Williams Riska, L., &Allen, C. K. (1985). Research with
(pp. 299-313). Boston: Little, Brown & Co. nondisabled population. In C. K. Allen (Ed.), Occupational
Katz, N. (1988, April). Research related to the cogni- therapy for psychiatric diseases: Measurement and man-
tive disability theory. Paper presented at the AOTA confer- agement of cognitive disabilities (pp. 315-338). Boston:
ence, Phoenix, Al. Little, Brown & Co.
Kowalski Lundi, M. H., & Mitcham, M. D. (1984). The loltan, B. B., & Ryckman Meeder, D. (1985). Head
relationship of constructional praxis to an upper extremity injury in adults. In L. W. Pedretti (Ed.), Occupational ther-
dressing task. Occupational Therapy journal of Research, apy practice skills for physical dysfunction (2nd ed.) (pp.
4,313-315. 436-461) St. Louis: C. V. Mosby.

192 March 1989, Volume 43, Number 3

Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 02/18/2021 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms

You might also like