You are on page 1of 6

Meat Science 93 (2013) 449–454

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Meat Science
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/meatsci

Consumers' expected quality and intention to purchase high quality pork meat
P. Papanagiotou a,⁎, I. Tzimitra-Kalogianni a, K. Melfou b
a
Department of Agricultural Economics, School of Agriculture, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, P.O. Box 232, 54124, Thessaloniki, Greece
b
Department of Agricultural Products Marketing and Quality Control, TEI of Western Macedonia — Florina branch, Terma Kondopoulou, 53100, Florina, Greece

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Expected quality is believed to be one of the most important factors that influence consumers' intention to
Received 21 April 2012 purchase food. The present study seeks to explore the concept of pork meat expected quality and compare
Received in revised form 6 November 2012 it with self-stated consumer intention to purchase pork meat. The aim is attempted by means of a field re-
Accepted 10 November 2012
search conducted in Greece, following a conjoint analytic procedure. Results show that quality expectations
comply with intention to buy pork, in many aspects. However, several differences have been identified.
Keywords:
Expected quality
More specifically, country of origin and marbling appear to be more important for respondents' purchase de-
Intention to purchase cisions than they are for their quality evaluations, while the opposite appears to be true for price. Finally,
Pork socio-demographic factors such as gender, level of education, place of purchase and consumption habits
Greece seem to influence perceptions.
© 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction of relevant definitions and theoretical models have been proposed.


However, there is a consensus regarding their basic concepts. Thus,
Food quality has been in the forefront for many decades. However, the perceived quality approach explores product quality from the
food scandals and scares, public efforts to guarantee food quality, as consumer's perspective by making quality a subjective evaluation de-
well as consumers' becoming more demanding and well-informed pendent on the perceptions, needs and goals of the consumer
about quality, have made its study more intensive during the past de- (Northen, 2000; Steenkamp, 1990).
cade. Quality is a concept with many aspects, and its definition de- Quality evaluation takes place at two stages, prior and during pur-
pends on the background of the person that uses this term (Becker, chase and after purchase and during consumption. After purchase,
2000). Nevertheless, it is often acknowledged that it is the consumer's consumers evaluate food quality (experienced quality or quality per-
definition of food quality that drives the economy of the global food formance) on the basis of the quality attributes they believe that the
industry (Cardello, 1995), while this definition is at the same time product possesses. According to Steenkamp (1989), quality attributes
very complex and dynamic (Troy & Kerry, 2010). Despite the fact are the functional and psychosocial benefits provided by the product,
that quality is often believed to influence consumers' buying decisions and they represent what the product is perceived as doing or provid-
(Fandos & Flavián, 2006; Grunert, 1997; Steenkamp, 1986), there is ing to the consumer. Furthermore, quality attributes are distinguished
evidence that consumers do not always purchase quality food products between experience and credence attributes, with the former being
and an example of that is “junk food” (Moskowitz, 1995). ascertained by consumers on the basis of actual experience with the
The present study has attempted to investigate expected quality and product (e.g. taste), and the latter being difficult to be ascertained
intention to purchase pork meat, by employing the perceived quality even after frequent consumption (e.g. healthiness).
theoretical approach, without, though, incorporating purchase motives. Nevertheless, quality attributes are not known before consump-
It, actually, attempted to identify similarities and differences between tion, and consequently consumers have to rely on some indicators,
these two concepts. More specifically, the purpose of this research known as quality cues, to predict food quality (expected quality or
was to apply a conjoint analytic procedure to pork meat in Greece and quality expectations). Quality cues are defined as any informational
to identify consumer segments with similar perceptions and intentions. stimulus that can be ascertained through the senses prior to consump-
tion (Steenkamp & Van Trijp, 1996), and are either intrinsic or extrinsic.
2. Theoretical framework In particular, intrinsic cues cannot be manipulated without changing
the nature of the product (e.g. marbling), while extrinsic are related to
The perceived quality theoretical framework has drawn much the product but are not physically part of it (e.g. price) (Bernués,
attention over the past three decades, and consequently, a number Olaizola, & Corcoran, 2003).
Most of the theoretical models proposed (Acebrón & Dopico, 2000;
⁎ Corresponding author. Tel.: +30 2310998821; fax: +30 2310998809. Becker, 2000; Hoffmann, 2000; Poulsen, Juhl, Kristensen, Bech, &
E-mail address: ppapanag@agro.auth.gr (P. Papanagiotou). Engelund, 1996; Steenkamp, 1989; Steenkamp & Van Trijp, 1996; Van

0309-1740/$ – see front matter © 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.meatsci.2012.11.024
450 P. Papanagiotou et al. / Meat Science 93 (2013) 449–454

den Heuvel, Van Trijp, Van Woerkum, Renes, & Gremmen, 2007) adopt Table 1
the aforementioned key concepts. However, the first model to incorpo- Factors and their levels.

rate intention to purchase in the perceived quality process was the L⁎ a⁎ b⁎


Total Food Quality Model, TFQM (Grunert, Hartvig Larsen, Madsen, & Factors Levels of factors
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
Baadsgaard, 1996), followed by the Model of Bernués et al. (2003).
Colour Light 61.23 0.48 19.53 0.20 20.10 0.53
According to the TFQM, expected quality and expected fulfilment of
Medium 48.10 2.13 34.03 0.92 26.00 0.67
purchase motives are the positive outcomes consumers expect from Dark 37.31 0.29 37.81 0.17 24.03 1.23
buying a food product, and are offset against the negative consequences Marbling 0%
in the form of various costs, while this trade-off determines intention 3%
to buy (Grunert, Bredahl, & Brunsø, 2004, p. 261). More specifically, 6%
Price 3.00 €/kg
expected quality is believed to be a mediator between cue perception 5.45 €/kg
and expected purchase motive fulfilment, which in turn mediates be- 7.90 €/kg
tween expected quality and intention to buy. However, there is evi- Origin Greek
dence that expected quality is not the only mediator between quality Imported
cues and purchase motives and intention to purchase (Grunert, 1997). L⁎ is a factor of brightness (Lightness, black-white axis), a⁎ defines the content of red
Conjoint Analysis refers to a set of procedures that explore con- or green (magenta-green spectrum), and b⁎ indicates the content of yellow or blue
sumers' responses to combinations (profiles) of independent factors (yellow-blue spectrum), using the CIE Lab colour space (Du & Sun, 2004; Van Oeckel,
Warnants, & Boucqué, 1999).
or attributes in an attempt to understand the contributions of these
factors to the evaluation of the profiles (Moskowitz & Silcher, 2006).
More specifically, Conjoint Analysis is based on the assumption that Finally, an additive mixed model (Green & Srinivasan, 1978, 1990)
consumers assess the value of a product by combining the separate was adopted, assuming that marbling had an ideal point function, and
amounts of value provided by each level of each attribute or factor price and origin a linear function, with utility increasing with the in-
(Hair, Black, Babin, & Anderson, 2010). As a result, respondents are crease of price and when pork chops were labelled as Greek. In addi-
asked to give an overall rating for each profile, and the researcher is tion, colour was assumed to have a discrete function because it was
able to estimate the relative importance of the factors and the part- not safe to make an assumption about the relationship between factor
worth utility of each factor level, without having to ask directly levels and data. More details about the methodology followed are
the consumer about his or her opinion of either of them (Cardello, given in Papanagiotou, Tzimitra-Kalogianni, and Melfou (2012).
Schutz, & Lesher, 2007). The fact that both relative importance and utility are estimated at
the individual level in the context of Conjoint Analysis facilitates the
3. Materials and methods segmentation of respondents into groups with similar preferences
(Deliza, MacFie, & Hedderley, 2003; DeSarbo, Ramaswamy, & Cohen,
The factors (attributes) investigated in the present study were 1995; Green & Krieger, 1991). In this case, respondents were classi-
quality cues that were selected after an extensive review of the per- fied according to the relative importance they attached to each factor
ceived quality of meat literature. The number of cues was restricted with regard to perceived quality. More specifically, k-means Cluster
to four so that the number of profiles would be low enough to be eas- Analysis was employed to group participants, while both the number
ily handled by respondents. Thus, two intrinsic (colour and marbling/ of clusters and the centres of each cluster were decided using a hier-
intramuscular fat) and two extrinsic (price and country of origin) archical method (Ward's method, squared Euclidean distance).
quality cues (Acebrón & Dopico, 2000; Banović, Grunert, Barreira, & Subsequently, non parametric tests (Kruskal–Wallis and Mann–
Fontes, 2009, 2010; Becker, Benner, & Glitsch, 2000; Davidson, Whitney), as well as cross tabulation (Pearson's chi-square, χ2), were
Schröder, & Bower, 2003; Glitsch, 2000; Grunert, 1997; Issanchou, used to uncover statistically significant differences among the clusters
1996; Krystallis & Arvanitoyannis, 2006; Krystallis, Chryssochoidis, and describe clusters in terms of socio-demographic variables and pork
& Scholderer, 2007; Mannion, Cowan, & Gannon, 2000; Verbeke, purchase and consumption habits.
Demey, Bosmans, & Viaene, 2005) were included in the study. The field research was conducted in the second largest urban area
The full-profile method was utilised as the means of presentation of Greece, Thessaloniki, from May until November 2010. The sample,
to respondents and pictures of pork loin chops were computer ma- included 626 pork consumers and was selected by means of a non
nipulated, using Adobe Photoshop CS, to attain several levels of the probability procedure, quota sampling. The population under research
aforementioned factors. This method has been applied for the first consisted of all adult pork consumer inhabitants of the 13 municipal-
time in Greece to assess the quality perception of meat, for, to the ities in the wider urban area of Thessaloniki. Furthermore a screening
best of our knowledge, the only other study using computer manipu- question was used to identify the sample.
lated pictures of meat focused on consumer preference (Fortomaris et
al., 2006), and was a part of a research project performed in 23 coun- Table 2
tries (Dransfield, Martin, Miramont, & Ngapo, 2001; Ngapo, Martin, & Final profiles rated by respondents.
Dransfield, 2007a, 2007b). Factors' Level
More specifically, levels for colour and marbling were deter- Profile Number Profile status
Colour Marbling Price Origin
mined based on the National Pork Producer Council (NPPC) standards
(Aberle, Forrest, Gerrard, & Mills, 2001). Furthermore, pork chops were 1 Light 3% 5.45 €/kg Imported Design
2 Medium 0% 7.90 €/kg Imported Design
labelled as either Greek or imported, while price labels were also added
3 Medium 6% 5.45 €/kg Greek Design
(Table 1). 4 Medium 3% 3.00 €/kg Greek Design
The combination of all the levels of all the factors resulted in 54 5 Dark 6% 3.00 €/kg Imported Design
profiles. The orthogonal design procedure in PASW Statistics 18 was 6 Dark 0% 5.45 €/kg Greek Design
7 Light 0% 3.00 €/kg Greek Design
employed to reduce the number of profiles to 13, 4 of which were
8 Light 6% 7.90 €/kg Greek Design
holdout profiles (rated by respondents only to assess validity and 9 Dark 3% 7.90 €/kg Greek Design
reliability of the original estimates). Respondents were asked to eval- 10 Light 0% 7.90 €/kg Greek Holdout
uate the final group of profiles (Table 2) with regard to quality and 11 Light 3% 7.90 €/kg Greek Holdout
intention to purchase, using two 11-point semantic differential scale 12 Medium 0% 7.90 €/kg Greek Holdout
13 Light 0% 3.00 €/kg Imported Holdout
questions.
P. Papanagiotou et al. / Meat Science 93 (2013) 449–454 451

4. Results and discussion Table 4


Conjoint analysis' results for quality and intention to buy.

4.1. Conjoint Analysis' results Factor Level Quality Intention to buy

Utility Importance Utility Importance


A description of the sample with regard to consumers' socio-
Colour Light −0.072 26.37% −0.123 26.78%
demographic characteristics and pork purchase and consumption
Medium 0.161 0.183
habits is presented in Table 3, while Conjoint Analysis' results are Dark −0.089 −0.061
presented in Table 4. The statistics that were estimated (Pearson's R Marbling 0% 0.872 30.01% 1.488 30.11%
and Kendall's tau) indicate that the models fit the data well for both 3% 0.886 1.794
perceived quality and intention to purchase pork meat. According 6% 0.043 0.917
Price 3.00 €/kg 0.656 27.35% 0.603 25.00%
to respondents' evaluations, a pork chop of medium colour, covered 5.45 €/kg 1.312 1.206
with 3% marbling, costing 7.90 €/kg and being of Greek origin would 7.90 €/kg 1.968 1.808
be considered of higher quality and would be bought by them. Origin Imported 0.625 16.27% 0.911 18.11%
Marbling appears to be the most important factor for consumers Greek 1.250 1.822
Constant 2.842 1.141
regardless of whether they evaluate quality or intention to purchase. Pearson's R 0.990 0.983
However, it should be mentioned that although marbling has the Kendall's tau 0.833 0.778
same relative importance for both quality and intention to buy, the Kendall's tau for holdouts 1.000 1.000
part-worth utilities for intention to purchase are much larger. This
fact indicates that respondents probably want an assurance that
the pork meat they choose possesses the desired sensory attributes
consumers rely on an extrinsic cue (price) to assess it. Moreover, price
(taste, tenderness, juiciness) that are influenced by marbling.
is a more objective quality cue, especially when compared to colour
It would be expected that price is more important for intention to
and especially when consumers are less experienced or less confident
buy than for perceived quality. Nevertheless, results reveal that the op-
in evaluating pork quality.
posite is true, which is surprising. An explanation for this phenomenon
In both quality and intention to purchase, origin appears to be the
could be that quality is a difficult concept to define, and consequently
factor that affects perceptions less. However, it should be mentioned
that the small percentage of origin's importance is probably partly
Table 3 due to the fact that it had a smaller number of levels compared to
Sample's socio-demographic characteristics and pork purchase and consumption the other factors in Conjoint Analysis, for relative importance of
habits. factors tends to increase with the increase in the number of levels
Variable Average Frequency Percentage (Verlegh, Schifferstein, & Wittink, 2002).
Origin seems to have greater influence on intention to buy in com-
Gender Male 262 41.9%
Female 364 58.1% parison with quality, for not only does it concentrate a higher percent-
Average age 43.8 age of relative importance but also has the highest utility compared to
Age 18–24 67 10.7% all the levels of all the factors studied. The fact that origin is important
25–34 145 23.2% for intention to purchase but not so important for quality, indicates
35–44 125 20.0%
45–54 102 16.3%
that consumers probably prefer Greek pork meat for other reasons
55–64 110 17.6% (ethnocentric behaviour or to support local pork producers), and not
>64 77 12.3% because they actually believe it to be of superior quality.
Educational level No formal education 17 2.7%
Primary school 85 13.6%
Junior high school 49 7.8% 4.2. Cluster Analysis' results
High school 204 32.6%
Institute of Vocational 36 5.8%
A Cluster Analysis was applied to classify respondents according to
Training (IVT)
Technological 58 9.3% the importance they attach to quality cues. A number of differences
Educational Institute among consumer segments have been highlighted, and these differ-
University 138 22.0% ences can be explained with regard to socio-demographic character-
Postgraduate studies 39 6.2% istics of respondents and their consumption and purchase habits.
Marital Status Single 210 33.5%
Married 377 60.2%
Conjoint Analysis' results for each Cluster are presented in Table 5,
Divorced 18 2.9% and socio-demographic characteristics and purchase and consump-
Widowed 21 3.4% tion habits by Cluster are presented in Table 6.
Average frequency of pork purchase 3.5 The solution given by Cluster Analysis appears to be very clear, for
(times per month)
consumers' preference for both quality and intention to buy is obvi-
Average frequency of pork consumption 5.4
(times per month) ous, and despite the fact that when it comes to intention to buy the
Average household consumption of pork 5.8 relative importance of the preferred cue is less than that of perceived
meat (kg/month) quality, they still coincide, with the exception of Cluster 3. It should
Average household consumption of pork 2.1 also be mentioned that utilities for Greek origin are higher for inten-
meat (kg/person/month)
tion to buy than for quality for all Clusters apart from 4, as the general
Average household expenditure on pork 35.1
(€/month) Conjoint Analysis' results suggest.
Preferred place Local butcher's shop 331 52.9% Despite the fact that a significant relationship has been found
of purchase Butcher's shop in a 78 12.5% between Cluster membership and place of pork purchase, butcher's
village
shop is the preferred place of purchase for participants in every Clus-
Remote butcher's shop 43 6.9%
Super market 110 17.6% ter. This fact signifies the role that traditional' shops play for Greek
Hyper market 9 1.4% consumers, and the personal relationship, and consequently trust,
Meat market 23 3.7% that has been developed between them.
Directly from the farmer 22 3.5% Cluster 1 consists of respondents that attach more importance
Other 9 1.6%
to intramuscular fat. However, contrary to the overall results, these
452 P. Papanagiotou et al. / Meat Science 93 (2013) 449–454

Table 5
Conjoint Analysis results' by cluster.

Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Cluster 4 Cluster 5 e


Significance
Factors and levels 139 respondents 102 respondents 187 respondents 87 respondents 111 respondents

Quality Intention Quality Intention Quality Intention Quality Intention Quality Intention Quality Intention
to buy to buy to buy to buy to buy to buy

Colour
Light −0.145 −0.145 −0.084 −0.303 −0.133 −0.200 −0.232 −0.291 0.258 0.275 ** **
Medium 0.130 0.088 0.053 0.178 0.140 0.160 0.231 0.276 0.276 0.332 n.s. n.s.
Dark 0.015 0.057 0.031 0.125 −0.008 0.040 0.001 0.015 −0.535 −0.608 ** **
Relative importance 20.94%a 23.15% 15.70%b 20.21% 23.84%c 25.59% 22.22%ac 22.61% 50.48% d 42.66% ** **

Marbling
0% 0.962 1.659 0.065 0.386 1.176 1.462 1.080 2.245 0.823 1.739 ** **
3% 0.496 1.588 −0.029 0.454 1.419 1.840 1.261 2.893 1.024 2.345 ** **
6% −1.396 −0.216 −0.284 0.206 0.727 1.134 0.540 1.943 0.604 1.820 ** **
Relative importance 54.06%a 44.63% 15.29%b 19.37% 28.50%c 29.23% 21.43%d 25.95% 22.70%d 26.53% ** **

Price
3.00 €/kg 0.084 −0.036 1.623 1.614 0.931 0.923 0.534 0.299 0.116 0.171 ** **
5.45 €/kg 0.168 −0.072 3.245 3.229 1.863 1.847 1.069 0.598 0.231 0.342 ** **
7.90 €/kg 0.252 −0.108 4.868 4.843 2.794 2.770 1.603 0.897 0.347 0.514 ** **
Relative importance 13.07%a 17.39% 56.27%b 45.65% 33.57%c 28.04% 19.23%d 17.15% 14.54%a 16.59% ** **

Origin
Imported 0.344 0.717 0.415 0.685 0.442 0.755 2.107 2.293 0.315 0.542 ** **
Greek 0.688 1.434 0.830 1.369 0.884 1.510 4.215 4.586 0.631 1.084 ** **
Relative importance 11.93%a 14.83% 12.74%a 14.77% 14.09%b 17.14% 37.12%c 34.29% 12.28%a 14.22% ** **
abcd
Different superscripts indicate statistically significant differences of clusters' means (Mann–Whitney U test).
e
Statistically significant differences across groups were tested using Kruskal–Wallis H test (* indicates statistically significant relationships for p b 0.05, ** for p b 0.01 and n.s. stands
for non significant relationships).

consumers prefer lean pork meat. It is also worth noting that respon- familiarity with meat, and that is probably the reason they are able
dents of Cluster 1 are the only ones to admit that they would rather to evaluate pork on the basis of intrinsic cues and principally colour,
buy cheaper meat although they perceive it as being of inferior qual- which requires experience and self-confidence. Finally, there is evi-
ity, and that seems to be the reason they have the lowest expenditure dence that more educated consumers tend to employ intrinsic quality
on pork meat among respondents of all Clusters. Nevertheless, both cues more than extrinsic when evaluating pork meat, taking into con-
utilities of price levels and price's importance are very low to be sideration that participants grouped in this Cluster and in Cluster 1
very important. seem to be the most educated among all Clusters, while they attach
Respondents of Cluster 2 are mostly price-oriented and with the more importance on colour and marbling.
lowest familiarity with pork meat among all Clusters. This result is
consistent with past research, which suggests that consumers with 5. Conclusions
low familiarity with a product, and consequent uncertainty and per-
ceived difficulty in quality evaluation, rely principally on extrinsic The aim of this study was to explore consumer perceptions of pork
cues to judge its quality (Bredahl, 2004). Furthermore, this Clusters' meat quality, and to uncover possible differences and similarities
participants are the only ones that consider lean meat as being of su- between perceived quality and intention to purchase pork meat.
perior quality but would prefer to buy pork with a small quantity of According to the results, there is evidence of a strong relationship be-
marbling, which obviously results from their interest in the sensory tween expected quality and intention to buy pork. Nevertheless, there
attributes of meat. Taking into consideration that this Cluster consists appear to be some differences between them, and these differences do
mostly of men, it could be concluded that males prefer pork with not seem to result from perceived monetary costs, for respondents, in
some marbling, for they are oriented by the satisfaction they derive both cases, prefer the highest price. It should be mentioned, though,
from the consumption of meat. that intention to purchase often deviates from actual purchase, and
The third Cluster is the largest one, and consumers do not appear this means that in a real life situation, consumers might not always
to be particularly influenced by any of the cues studied. Furthermore, buy the most expensive meat. Furthermore, the usage of the cue
although they use price to evaluate quality, marbling is the cue that ‘price’ (extrinsic) for the formation of quality judgements is an indica-
they attach more importance to when they purchase pork meat. tion of uncertainty and perceived difficulty in quality evaluation, a
Cluster 4 consists of consumers that consider country of origin conclusion further supported by the decrease of price's relative impor-
as the most important quality cue. Given that this Cluster is the one tance regarding intention to purchase.
with the highest average age of respondents and the lowest level of It is possible that differences revealed in the present study could be
education, and combined with the results of Cluster 1, which consists partly explained by consumers' expected purchase motive fulfilment;
of younger and more educated consumers who attach the least im- unless there are other factors, requiring further research, that cause
portance to origin among Clusters, it seems that origin's importance them. First of all, country of origin seems to have a more important
increases with the increase of age while the reverse appears to be impact on consumers' purchase decisions than quality evaluations.
true for education. Furthermore, it seems that experience and high fa- Thus, respondents acknowledge that they prefer Greek pork not only
miliarity with pork meat has taught these consumers that a small per- because they consider it to be of superior quality but also for other rea-
centage of marbling is necessary for meat in order for it to be more sons. Such reasons could be social acceptance, ethnocentric behaviour
tasty, tender and juicy. or just the intention to support local meat producers.
Respondents of Cluster 5, who are mostly women, regard colour as Marbling has proven to be the most important criterion for both
the most important cue. In addition, it is the Cluster with the highest evaluating quality and purchasing pork meat. However, respondents
P. Papanagiotou et al. / Meat Science 93 (2013) 449–454 453

Table 6
Description of clusters and sample.

Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Cluster 4 Cluster 5 Sample Significancea

Gender *
Men 41.0% 52.9% 36.9% 48.3% 36.0% 41.9%
Women 59.0% 47.1% 63.1% 51.7% 64.0% 58.1%

Age n.s.
Average 41.6 45.2 43.6 47.6 42.6 43.8
18–24 12.2% 8.8% 12.3% 5.7% 11.7% 10.7%
25–34 25.9% 22.5% 21.4% 19.5% 26.1% 23.2%
35–44 23.0% 20.6% 20.9% 18.4% 15.3% 20.0%
45–54 13.7% 13.7% 16.0% 18.4% 20.7% 16.3%
55–64 17.3% 19.6% 16.0% 19.5% 17.1% 17.6%
65–74 7.9% 13.7% 10.7% 10.3% 9.0% 10.2%
≥75 0.0% 1.0% 2.7% 8.0% 0.0% 2.1%

Level of education **
No formal education 2.2% 2.0% 3.2% 6.9% 0.0% 2.7%
Primary school 4.3% 20.6% 10.7% 28.7% 11.7% 13.6%
Junior high school 2.9% 10.8% 7.5% 9.2% 10.8% 7.8%
High school 32.4% 30.4% 36.9% 32.2% 27.9% 32.6%
Institute of Vocational Training (IVT) 6.5% 7.8% 7.0% 2.3% 3.6% 5.8%
Technological Educational Institute 10.1% 8.8% 10.2% 4.6% 10.8% 9.3%
University 31.7% 18.6% 18.2% 14.9% 25.2% 22.0%
Postgraduate studies 10.1% 1.0% 6.4% 1.1% 9.9% 6.2%

Marital status n.s.


Single 38.1% 34.3% 34.8% 20.7% 35.1% 33.5%
Married 56.8% 59.8% 57.8% 73.6% 58.6% 60.2%
Divorced 3.6% 3.9% 2.1% 1.1% 3.6% 2.9%
Widowed 1.4% 2.0% 5.3% 4.6% 2.7% 3.4%

Preferred place of purchase *


Local butcher's shop 56.1% 46.1% 50.8% 58.6% 54.1% 52.9%
Remote butcher's shop 4.3% 11.8% 7.5% 5.7% 5.4% 6.9%
Butcher's shop in a village 16.5% 12.7% 10.7% 13.8% 9.0% 12.5%
Super market 16.5% 13.7% 20.9% 13.8% 19.8% 17.6%
Hyper market 2.2% 2.0% 1.6% 0.0% 0.9% 1.4%
Meat market 2.9% 9.8% 3.7% 1.1% 0.9% 3.7%
Directly from the farmer 1.4% 3.9% 2.7% 4.6% 6.3% 3.5%
Other 0.0% 0.0% 2.1% 2.3% 3.6% 1.6%
Average frequency of pork purchase (times per month) 3.5 3.1 3.2 3.7 4.0 3.5 n.s.
Average frequency of pork consumption (times per month) 5.3 5.0 5.4 5.5 5.8 5.4 n.s.
Average household consumption of pork meat (kg/month) 5.2 4.6 6.2 6.5 6.2 5.8 **
Average consumption of pork meat (kg/person/month) 1.8 1.8 2.0 2.4 2.6 2.1 *
Average household expenditure on pork (€/month) 31.9 32.0 36.6 38.7 36.8 35.1 *
a
Statistically significant differences across groups were tested using Pearson's χ2 and Kruskal–Wallis H test (* indicates statistically significant relationships for p b 0.05, ** for
p b 0.01 and n.s. stands for non significant relationships).

seem to be very interested in intramuscular fat, especially with regard boost consumer confidence in quality evaluation and increase their
to intention to purchase. This fact constitutes actual evidence that knowledge by disseminating accurate information about meat quali-
Greek consumers' choices are influenced by expected satisfaction de- ty. Finally, despite the fact this research has been conducted in Greece
rived by pork consumption, for marbling affects experience attributes it is believed that results may be indicative of consumer perceptions
like taste, tenderness and juiciness. There is, however, a segment of re- in other Mediterranean countries as well, for Southern European con-
spondents that prefer lean meat, which is probably a consequence of sumers share similar consumption habits, e.g. Mediterranean diet.
modern lifestyle (e.g. weight control) and consequent eating habits.
It could be concluded, though, that no matter whether consumers pre-
References
fer lean meat or meat with some marbling, it is probably purchase mo-
tive fulfilment that drives their preference for intramuscular fat. Aberle, E. D., Forrest, J. C., Gerrard, D. E., & Mills, E. W. (2001). Principles of meat science.
In addition, gender, educational level, and preferred place of pur- Duduque, Iowa: Kendall/Hunt.
Acebrón, L. B., & Dopico, D. C. (2000). The importance of intrinsic and extrinsic cues to
chase, as well as average consumption of pork meat, have been identi- expected and experienced quality: An empirical application for beef. Food Quality
fied as the variables that differ significantly among Clusters. However, and Preference, 11, 229–238.
it appears that age also influences consumers' quality perception pro- Banović, M., Grunert, K. G., Barreira, M. M., & Fontes, M. A. (2009). Beef quality percep-
tion at the point of purchase: A study from Portugal. Food Quality and Preference,
cess and intention to buy, despite the fact that a statistically significant
20, 335–342.
relationship was not identified. Banović, M., Grunert, K. G., Barreira, M. M., & Fontes, M. A. (2010). Consumers' quality per-
It could be concluded that although expected quality and inten- ception of national branded, national store branded, and imported store branded beef.
Meat Science, 84, 54–65.
tion to buy pork meat are similar in many aspects, in a way, con-
Becker, T. (2000). Consumer perception of fresh meat quality: A framework for analy-
sumers appear to admit that they do not always want to purchase sis. British Food Journal, 102, 158–176.
meat of superior quality. Furthermore, there is evidence that this re- Becker, T., Benner, E., & Glitsch, K. (2000). Consumer perception of fresh meat quality
sult is not only a matter of personal consumer preferences but is in Germany. British Food Journal, 102, 246–266.
Bernués, A., Olaizola, A., & Corcoran, K. (2003). Extrinsic attributes of red meat as indi-
also a consequence of consumers' lack of confidence and proper infor- cators of quality in Europe: An application for market segmentation. Food Quality
mation about meat quality. Therefore, public efforts are required to and Preference, 14, 265–276.
454 P. Papanagiotou et al. / Meat Science 93 (2013) 449–454

Bredahl, L. (2004). Cue utilisation and quality perception with regard to branded beef. Krystallis, A., & Arvanitoyannis, I. S. (2006). Investigating the concept of meat quality
Food Quality and Preference, 15, 65–75. from the consumers' perspective: The case of Greece. Meat Science, 72, 164–176.
Cardello, A. V. (1995). Food quality: Relativity, context and consumer expectations. Krystallis, A., Chryssochoidis, G., & Scholderer, J. (2007). Consumer-perceived quality
Food Quality and Preference, 6, 163–170. in ‘traditional’ food chains: The case of the Greek meat supply chain. Appetite, 48,
Cardello, A. V., Schutz, H. G., & Lesher, L. L. (2007). Consumer perceptions of foods 54–68.
processed by innovative and emerging technologies: A conjoint analytic study. Mannion, M. A., Cowan, C., & Gannon, M. (2000). Factors associated with perceived
Innovative Food Science and Emerging Technologies, 8, 73–83. quality influencing beef consumption behaviour in Ireland. British Food Journal,
Davidson, A., Schröder, M. J. A., & Bower, J. A. (2003). The importance of origin as a 102, 195–210.
quality attribute for beef: Results from a Scottish consumer survey. International Moskowitz, H. R. (1995). Food quality: Conceptual and sensory aspects. Food Quality
Journal of Consumer Studies, 27, 91–98. and Preference, 6, 157–162.
Deliza, R., MacFie, H., & Hedderley, D. (2003). Use of computer-generated images and Moskowitz, H. R., & Silcher, M. (2006). The applications of conjoint analysis and their
conjoint analysis to investigate sensory expectations. Journal of Sensory Studies, possible uses in Sensometrics. Food Quality and Preference, 17, 145–165.
18, 465–486. Ngapo, T. M., Martin, J. -F., & Dransfield, E. (2007a). International preferences for pork
DeSarbo, W. S., Ramaswamy, V., & Cohen, S. H. (1995). Marketing segmentation with appearance: I. Consumer choices. Food Quality and Preference, 18, 26–36.
choice-based Conjoint Analysis. Marketing Letters, 6, 137–147. Ngapo, T. M., Martin, J. -F., & Dransfield, E. (2007b). International preferences for pork
Dransfield, E., Martin, J. -F., Miramont, J., & Ngapo, T. M. (2001). Meat appearance: Pork appearance: I. Factors influencing consumer choice. Food Quality and Preference, 18,
chops. A tool for surveying consumer preferences. Paris: INRA. 139–151.
Du, C. -J., & Sun, D. -W. (2004). Recent developments in the applications of image pro- Northen, J. R. (2000). Quality attributes and quality cues: Effective communication in
cessing techniques for food quality evaluation. Trends in Food Science & Technology, the UK meat supply chain. British Food Journal, 102, 230–245.
15, 230–249. Papanagiotou, P., Tzimitra-Kalogianni, I., & Melfou, K. (2012). Pork quality in the eye of
Fandos, C., & Flavián, C. (2006). Intrinsic and extrinsic quality attributes, loyalty and the Greek consumer. British Food Journal, 114, 647–660.
buying intention: An analysis for a PDO product. British Food Journal, 108, 646–662. Poulsen, C. S., Juhl, H. J., Kristensen, K., Bech, A. C., & Engelund, E. (1996). Quality guid-
Fortomaris, P., Arsenos, G., Georgiadis, M., Banos, G., Stamataris, C., & Zygoyiannis, D. ance and quality formation. Food Quality and Preference, 7, 127–135.
(2006). Effect of meat appearance on consumer preferences for pork chops in Steenkamp, J. -B. E. M. (1986). Perceived quality of food products and its relationship to
Greece and Cyprus. Meat Science, 72, 688–696. consumer preferences: Theory and measurement. Journal of Food Quality, 9, 373–386.
Glitsch, K. (2000). Consumer perceptions of fresh meat quality: Cross-national compar- Steenkamp, J. -B. E. M. (1989). Product quality: An investigation into the concept and how
ison. British Food Journal, 102, 177–194. it is perceived by consumers. Assen: Van Gorcum.
Green, P. E., & Krieger, A. M. (1991). Segmenting markets with Conjoint Analysis. Jour- Steenkamp, J. -B. E. M. (1990). Conceptual model of the quality perception process.
nal of Marketing, 55, 20–31. Journal of Business Research, 21, 309–333.
Green, P. E., & Srinivasan, V. (1978). Conjoint Analysis in consumer research: Issues and Steenkamp, J. -B. E. M., & Van Trijp, H. C. M. (1996). Quality guidance: A consumer-
Outlook. Journal of Consumer Research, 5, 103–123. based approach to food quality improvement using partial least squares. European
Green, P. E., & Srinivasan, V. (1990). Conjoint Analysis in marketing: New develop- Review of Agricultural Economics, 23, 195–215.
ments with implications for research and practice. Journal of Marketing, 54, 3–19. Troy, D. J., & Kerry, J. P. (2010). Consumer perception and the role of science in the meat
Grunert, K. G. (1997). What's in a steak? A cross-cultural study on the quality percep- industry. Meat Science, 86, 214–226.
tion of beef. Food Quality and Preference, 8, 157–174. Van den Heuvel, T., Van Trijp, H., Van Woerkum, C., Renes, R. J., & Gremmen, B. (2007).
Grunert, K. G., Bredahl, L., & Brunsø, K. (2004). Consumer perception of meat quality Linking product offering to consumer needs; inclusion of credence attributes and
and implications for product development in the meat sector — A review. Meat the influences of product features. Food Quality and Preference, 18, 296–304.
Science, 66, 259–272. Van Oeckel, M., Warnants, N., & Boucqué, C. (1999). Measurement and prediction of
Grunert, K. G., Hartvig Larsen, H., Madsen, T. K., & Baadsgaard, A. (1996). Market orien- pork colour. Meat Science, 52, 347–354.
tation in food and agriculture (7th ed.). Boston: Kluwer Academic. Verbeke, W., Demey, V., Bosmans, W., & Viaene, J. (2005). Consumer versus producer
Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., & Anderson, R. E. (2010). Multivariate data analysis: A expectations and motivations related to “superior” quality meat: Qualitative re-
global perspective. New Jersey: Pearson. search findings. Journal of Food Products Marketing, 11(3), 27–41.
Hoffmann, R. (2000). Country of origin — A consumer perception perspective of fresh Verlegh, P. W. J., Schifferstein, H. N. J., & Wittink, D. R. (2002). Range and number-
meat. British Food Journal, 102, 211–229. of-levels effects in derived and stated measures of attribute importance. Marketing
Issanchou, S. (1996). Consumer expectations and perceptions of meat and meat product Letters, 13, 41–52.
quality. Meat Science, 43, S5–S19.

You might also like