You are on page 1of 11

This article was downloaded by: [177.9.188.

90]
On: 22 April 2014, At: 09:45
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Journal of Sports Sciences


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rjsp20

A comparison of time-motion and technical–tactical


variables between age groups of female judo matches
a a a a a
Bianca Miarka , Rubiana Cury , Ricardo Julianetti , Rafael Battazza , Ursula Ferreira Julio ,
b ac
Michel Calmet & Emerson Franchini
a
Martial Arts and Combat Sports Research Group – School of Physical Education and Sport,
University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
b
Faculty of Sports Sciences, University of Montpellier, São Paulo, Brazil
c
Sport Department, University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
Published online: 10 Apr 2014.

To cite this article: Bianca Miarka, Rubiana Cury, Ricardo Julianetti, Rafael Battazza, Ursula Ferreira Julio, Michel Calmet &
Emerson Franchini (2014): A comparison of time-motion and technical–tactical variables between age groups of female judo
matches, Journal of Sports Sciences, DOI: 10.1080/02640414.2014.903335

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2014.903335

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the
Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and
are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and
should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for
any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever
or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of
the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
Journal of Sports Sciences, 2014
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2014.903335

A comparison of time-motion and technical–tactical variables between


age groups of female judo matches

BIANCA MIARKA1, RUBIANA CURY1, RICARDO JULIANETTI1, RAFAEL BATTAZZA1,


URSULA FERREIRA JULIO1, MICHEL CALMET2 & EMERSON FRANCHINI1,3
1
Martial Arts and Combat Sports Research Group – School of Physical Education and Sport, University of São Paulo, São
Paulo, Brazil, 2Faculty of Sports Sciences, University of Montpellier, São Paulo, Brazil and 3Sport Department, University of
São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil

(Accepted 5 March 2014)


Downloaded by [177.9.188.90] at 09:45 22 April 2014

Abstract
The aim of the study was to verify differences between age groups of female judo matches in time-motion and technical–
tactical analysis. The sample was composed of pre-cadet (13–14 years, n = 148), cadet (15–16 years, n = 228), junior (17–19
years, n = 104) and senior (>20 years, n = 237) groups. The time-motion indicators consisted of total combat time, standing
combat time, displacement without contact, gripping time, total time of techniques, groundwork combat time and pause
time, per match and by each combat/pause cycle. Technical and tactical variables were also collected. The one-way analysis
of variance and a post hoc test were conducted, P ≤ 0.05. Cadets, with a median of 7 (2, 12), had a number of combat/pause
cycles different from junior, with 3 (1, 8.5). Regarding time-motion per match and per cycle, senior had longer total combat
time, standing combat time and gripping time than other groups. Senior presented lower frequency of leg techniques than
pre-cadet, cadet and junior. Time-motion and technical–tactical variables effects in female judo athletes emphasise the
difference between seniors and other groups.

Keywords: time and motion studies, task performance and analysis, judo, martial arts and women’s groups

Introduction judo athletes (Calmet et al., 2006; Franchini et al.,


2008). The role of feedback is essential in the per-
According to the rules established by the
formance improvement process (Gutiérrez-Santiago,
International Judo Federation (IJF), there are four
Prieto, Ayán, & Cancela, 2013) and to prevent inju-
ways to score and, consequently, to win judo com-
ries, which could occur during the combat (Koshida,
bats: (1) by using throwing techniques (nage-waza),
Deguchi, Miyashita, Iwai, & Urabe, 2010), but little
which are classified as leg techniques (ashi-waza),
information has been presented about these vari-
arm techniques (te-waza), hip techniques (koshi-
ables, especially for the actions of female judo ath-
waza) and sacrifice techniques (sutemi-waza); (2)
letes (Sterkowicz, Sacripanti, & Sterkowicz-
through immobilisation techniques (osae-waza); (3)
Przybycień, 2013).
due to submission of the opponent via strangulation
The history of competitive female judo begins in
(shime-waza) or via joint-lock techniques (kansetsu-
the late 1960s and 1970s, when women started to
waza) and (4) due to penalties received by the oppo-
participate in official competitions around the world
nent (IJF, 2012).
(Miarka, Marques, & Franchini, 2011) . Nowadays,
The success in technical and tactical variables in
the total time for female combats is similar to men’s,
tournaments is associated with unpredictability, such
which consists of 3 min for pre-cadet (13–14 years),
as, for instance, gripping variability (Calmet, Miarka,
4 min for cadet (15–17 years) and 5 min for junior
& Franchini, 2010), different types of attack
(18–20 years) and senior (>20 years) (IJF, 2012),
(Franchini, Sterkowicz, Meira, Gomes, & Tani,
although recently the IJF proposed a 4-min period
2008) and its orientations (Calmet & Ahmaidi,
for senior women starting in 2014 (IJF, 2013). This
2004; Calmet, Trezel, & Ahmaidi, 2006; Franchini
period can be reduced if one athlete obtains the
et al., 2008). The connection of these variables
maximum score (ippon). Until 2012, in case of a
represents the main aspects of attacking systems of

Correspondence: Emerson Franchini, Martial Arts and Combat Sports Research Group – School of Physical Education and Sport, University of São Paulo,
São Paulo, Brazil. E-mail: emersonfranchini@hotmail.com

© 2014 Taylor & Francis


2 B. Miarka et al.

tie, it could be complemented by an extra time of 3 sports, determinant arrangements are dependent on
min for senior and junior athletes and 2 min for the match progression, since every action is predis-
cadet and pre-cadet or until one of the athletes posed by the previous one and influences the following
achieved any score in this period, the golden score ones (Del Vecchio, Hirata, & Franchini, 2011; Santos,
(IJF, 2012). However, in 2013, the IJF started test- Franchini, & Lima-Silva, 2011).
ing new rules and, currently, extra time is unlimited, Recently, a comparison of time-motion indicators
i.e. the match is ended only when one of the athletes during judo matches conducted an analysis that
score (IJF, 2013). Despite several changes in judo included the total combat, standing combat, displace-
rules over the years, similar and consistent results of ment without contact, gripping, groundwork combat
effort and pause time for senior male judo athletes and pause times, following the match progression
have been reported over the past 25 years (Miarka et al., 2012). The authors compared these
(Castarlenas & Planas, 1997; Franchini, Artioli, & indicators in 1,811 performances from four age groups
Brito, 2013; Sikorski, Mickiewicz, Majle, & Laksa, (pre-cadet, 13–14 years, n = 522; cadet, 15–16 years,
1987). Therefore, this study offers a diachronic tem- n = 353; junior, 17–19 years, n = 349, and; senior, >20
poral analysis of the same type of data for senior years, n = 587). Results indicated differences between
female judo athletes. This can be useful for compar- groups and suggested that each age category presented
ison with further findings, since the studies compar- particularities in their actions, which have important
ing data for male athletes have not shown implications for the development of training (Miarka
significative differences over the years. et al., 2012). However, the sample was limited to male
Downloaded by [177.9.188.90] at 09:45 22 April 2014

Furthermore, new findings have suggested differ- athletes and time-motion in women judo athletes
ences between age groups in male fighters when could have particularities, due to different physical
comparing the variation of combat time and the fitness (Franchini, Del Vecchio, Matsushigue, &
combination of different technical–tactical actions Artioli, 2011), gender construction (Guerandel &
(Miarka et al., 2012). Mennesson, 2007), social representation (Ferreira
Although women athletes have been competing Julio, Panissa, Miarka, Takito, & Franchini, 2013)
for more than four decades, studies analysing tech- and support (Miarka, Marques, et al., 2011) reported
nical and tactical aspects of female judo matches are between male and female judo athletes.
scarce (Calmet et al., 2006; Franchini et al., 2008; Socially and historically, judo practised by men and
Gutiérrez-Santiago, Prieto, Camerino, & Anguera, by women has been clearly distinguished since its
2011). Calmet et al. (2006) presented data from 18 start, and only in the last decades better opportunities
men and 10 women, during 185 matches over the and recognition for women has become apparent
competition season of 2001–2002, grouped, while a (Miarka, Marques, et al., 2011). Therefore, based
recent research reported the time-motion analysis of on the limitation of knowledge of female judo com-
Paralympic female athletes (Gutiérrez-Santiago bats and regarding the differences observed between
et al., 2011). Regarding international champion- age groups with large samples (Miarka et al., 2012),
ships, researchers demonstrated differences between the purpose of the current study was to: (1) quantify
male and female super elite athletes, who were the different phases in women judo matches based on
best athletes in each weight category, and elite, who time-motion analysis; (2) quantify number of penal-
were medal winners, but not champions and who did ties, techniques, technical orientations and grip varia-
not win more than three medals between 1995 and tions in women judo matches based on previous
2001 (Franchini et al., 2008). Authors reported that indicators presented and (3) determine the extent to
females received less penalties compared to males, which differences occur between age groups (pre-
but no difference was found concerning total num- cadet, cadet, junior and senior) in women judo
ber of attacks, number of attacks in standing or matches of different championship levels.
groundwork position, points and ippon scored per
match and direction of attacks in standing position
(Franchini et al., 2008). Also, a relation between Methods
self-confidence and the number of attacks used dur-
Participants
ing competitions of women judo athletes was
observed (Matsumoto, Takeuchi, Nakajima, & Iida, The sample was composed by 717 performances of
2000). However, information about time-motion athletes, comprised of four groups according to age
analysis in female judo athletes is lacking. groups: pre-cadet (13–14 years, n = 148), cadet
Such a method of analysis can be used in the crea- (15–16 years, n = 228), junior (17–19 years, n = 104)
tion of attack systems, as well as to quantify physical and senior (>20 years, n = 237). The age group divi-
and technical patterns, providing important guidelines sions were based on the rule available at the year of data
for training in a chosen sport (Atkinson & Nevill, 2001; collection. All participants were informed about the
Drust, 2010; Hughes & Franks, 2004). In combat procedures used in the record, and provided their
Female judo matches analysis 3

informed consent. In addition, the procedures in this Ahmaidi, 2004). Strangulation and joint-lock tech-
study were approved by the local Ethics Committee. niques were not allowed for pre-cadet athletes and
thus not considered for this group (IJF, 2013).
Regarding technical orientations, the present
Setting
research followed the protocol used in Franchini
In order to guarantee an adequate number of obser- et al. (2008) and in Calmet and Ahmaidi (2004),
vations, allowing the study to reflect time-motion and which defined the observation of technical orienta-
technical–tactical indicators by women judo athletes tions through the cardinal and ordinal directions.
in different age groups, we used the four most similar Concerning gripping, it was defined by the time
championships with the largest number of women between the accomplishment of a grip type (kumi-
fighters in the country. Therefore, three regional kata) and the lack of contact on the opponent judogi,
championships and one state championship of all as designed by preceding study (Calmet et al.,
weight categories in pre-cadet, cadet, junior and 2010). The frequency of gripping variations was
senior age groups for females were taped using four detected and observed through the modification of
Sony® cameras model DCR-DVD 508 (Tokyo, the grip type in the judogi segments (collar, sleeve
Japan) in 2008. In order to guarantee ecological valid- and back) and of their combinations (sleeve and
ity, each camera was responsible for one match area, sleeve, collar and collar, collar and sleeve, back and
such as coaches and match analysts have done in judo sleeve), separate by the left and right side, following
competitions (Miarka, Hayashida, Julio, Calmet, & prior research (Calmet et al., 2010). All variables
Downloaded by [177.9.188.90] at 09:45 22 April 2014

Franchini, 2011; Miarka et al., 2012). After record- presented were collected in each match following
ing, the data were analysed by one expert. previous recommendations (Calmet et al., 2010;
Although the data collection involved only one Franchini et al., 2008; Miarka et al., 2012).
expert to conduct the combat analyses, it was impor- The present cross-sectional study design is pre-
tant to use a tool that could be replicable in future sented in Figure 1.
studies. Therefore, the present study selected a soft-
ware and a protocol which had reliability measures
Reliability testing
previously reported through intra-observer and inter-
observer testing procedures (Miarka, Hayashida, The reliability measures were assessed in previously
et al., 2011; Miarka et al., 2012). published studies through intra-observer and inter-
observer testing procedures (Miarka, Hayashida,
et al., 2011; Miarka et al., 2012). According to
Time-motion and performance indicators measures
Miarka et al. (2012), the Cohen’s kappa compared
Time-motion measures were composed of total two experts, with more than 10 years of judo practice
combat time, standing combat time, displacement and graduated in physical education. The first expert
without contact, gripping time, technique time, analysed 20 performances of athletes, and the second
groundwork combat time and pause time. The fre- analysed the same 20 athletes. After this procedure,
quency of each time-motion indicator was collected the last expert performed the intra-observer agree-
per match and expressed by second of playing time, ment, with the selection and analysis of 10 combats
following preceding recommendations (Miarka, (20 athletes) in the first time in a randomised form,
Hayashida, et al., 2011; Miarka et al., 2012). Each performed it one more time. The inter- and intra-
combat phase is formed by specific technical–tactical observer agreements were assessed using the method
actions, which could influence the success in the advocated by Bland and Altman (2007), with all data
combat. They are called performance indicators in found to be within acceptable limits (i.e. <5% error,
the present research, and these variables were pre- like demonstrated by Lago (2009)). The significance
viously designed by other studies (Calmet et al., level of P < 0.05 was used. The protocol and the
2010; Franchini et al., 2008; Miarka et al., 2012). FRAMI® software demonstrated agreements classi-
Performance indicators measures were defined fied as “Almost Perfect” for kappa values, with a
and analysed through previous protocols, which range of agreement of all analysed variables between
included the frequency of penalties, techniques 0.84 and 1.00 – both assessed through intra-observer
applied, technical orientations, grip type and their and inter-observer tests (Miarka et al., 2012).
variations. The penalty (shido) frequencies were
noted over the referee sinalisation in each combat,
Statistical analysis
following the protocol of Miarka, Hayashida, et al.
(2011). In addition, all techniques were classified Descriptive data was presented as mean ± SD or med-
and accounted following the Kodokan technical clas- ian (first quartile, third quartile). For the non-para-
sification and the resultant score sinalised by the metric data, Kruskal–Wallis and Mann–Whitney tests
referee, as suggested by previous studies (Calmet & were conducted in order to compare groups.
4 B. Miarka et al.

Figure 1. Study design of time-motion and performance indicators measured during female judo matches.
Downloaded by [177.9.188.90] at 09:45 22 April 2014

Table I. Total time of each match showed by mean ± SD for each time-motion indicator by each age group, in seconds.

Total combat Standing combat Displacement without Gripping Groundwork Pause Technique
Age groups time time contact time combat time time time

Pre-cadet n = 148 147 ± 98 105 ± 76a 42 ± 40 57 ± 43d 41 ± 31 48 ± 49 4 ± 5


Cadet n = 228 128 ± 81 91 ± 62 40 ± 29 45 ± 35 35 ± 26 54 ± 58 5 ± 5
Junior n = 104 119 ± 83 76 ± 57 31 ± 26 38 ± 30 41 ± 35 47 ± 77 4 ± 5
Senior n = 237 179 ± 135d 141 ± 119d 52 ± 48b 73 ± 57d 36 ± 31 55 ± 58 7 ± 8c

Note: aAge group significantly different from junior and senior groups (P < 0.05); bAge group significantly different from cadet and junior
groups (P < 0.05); cAge group significantly different from pre-cadet and junior groups (P < 0.05); dAge group significantly different from
other groups (P < 0.05).

Afterwards, the effect size measure for non-parametric P < 0.001, η2 = 0.075), where senior displayed
analysis was calculated, defined as r = Z/√N, where r longer values than other groups (P < 0.001 for all
represents the effect size, Z is derived from the conver- comparisons) and pre-cadet presented longer values
sion of Mann–Whitney test and N is the total number of than junior (P < 0.001).
observations. This analysis considers r-values as: small Significant differences were found in gripping time
effect size (r = 0.10), medium effect size (r = 0.30) or (F3,713 = 21.65, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.083), with senior
large effect size (r = 0.50) (Field, 2005). Time-motion presenting longer periods in this action when com-
data were presented as mean ± SD. One-way ANOVA pared to other ages (pre-cadet, P = 0.038; cadet and
was conducted to compare measured variables across junior, P < 0.001). Pre-cadet had longer gripping
age groups. When differences were detected, the Tukey periods than cadet (P = 0.038) and junior
test was used as post hoc to identify specific differences (P = 0.005).
between age groups. Eta-squared (η2) was calculated. Concerning differences in displacement without
The significance level of P ≤ 0.05 was used. contact (F3,713 = 7.78, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.032), senior
presented longer periods than cadet (P = 0.006) and
junior (P = 0.001). Total time of techniques differed
Results among groups (F3,713 = 5.38, P = 0.010,
Total times accumulated per match for each time- η2 = 0.022): senior displayed longer time than pre-
motion indicator in pre-cadet, cadet, junior and cadet (P = 0.005) and junior (P = 0.006).
senior groups are presented in Table I. Table II presents the frequency for each time-
The ANOVA revealed differences between ages in motion indicator of the match separated by groups.
total combat time (F3,713 = 12.54, P < 0.001, Statistical analysis showed differences in pause
η2 = 0.05), in which seniors displayed longer values frequencies per combat (K3,713 = 12.97,
than other groups (for pre-cadet, P = 0.019 and for P = 0.005), where the junior age group demon-
cadet and junior, P < 0.001). Similar effects were strated lower values than pre-cadets (r = 0.08,
observed in standing combat time (F3,713 = 19.20, P = 0.025), cadets (r = 0.14, P < 0.001) and senior
Female judo matches analysis 5

Table II. Frequency of time-motion indicators in each combat/pause cycle showed by median (first; third quartile) by age group, in seconds.

Age groups Standing combat Displacement without contact Gripping Technique Groundwork combat Pause

Pre-cadet n = 148 7(3;11) 7(3;11) 6(3;11) 4(2;7) 4(2;8)c 6(2;10)


Cadet n = 228 7(3;12) 7(3;12) 7(3;11) 4(2;8)b 4(2;8)d 6(2;11)
Junior n = 104 4(2;10.5) 4(2;10.5) 4(2;10)a 3(1;6) 3(1;7) 3(1;9.5)*
Senior n = 237 6(3;11) 6(3;11) 6(3;11) 4(2;7) 4(1;7) 5(2;10)

Note: *Age group significantly different from all other age groups; aAge group significantly different from cadet (P = 0.016) and senior
(P = 0.003); bdifferent from pre-cadet (P = 0.011) and junior (P < 0.001); cdifferent from senior (P = 0.012) and junior (P = 0.035).
d
Different from junior (P < 0.001).

Table III. Total time of each match phase showed by mean ± SD for each time-motion indicator by each age group, in seconds.

Total combat Standing combat Displacement without Gripping Groundwork Pause Technique
Age groups time time contact time combat time time time

Pre-cadet n = 148 22 ± 14 14 ± 7 6 ± 4 8 ± 7 9 ± 9 5 ± 3 0 ± 0
Cadet n = 228 16 ± 8 11 ± 4 5 ± 4a 5 ± 3 7 ± 8c 5 ± 3b 0 ± 1
Junior n = 104 22 ± 10 14 ± 6 7 ± 5 7 ± 5 10 ± 9 8 ± 14 0 ± 0
Senior n = 237 35 ± 63d 27 ± 49d 7 ± 6 13 ± 22d 8 ± 9 6 ± 7 2 ± 3d
Downloaded by [177.9.188.90] at 09:45 22 April 2014

Note: aAge group significantly different from senior group (P < 0.05); bage group significantly different from junior group (P < 0.05); cage
group significantly different from pre-cadet and junior groups (P < 0.05); dage group significantly different from other groups (P < 0.05).

(r = 0.08, P = 0.021). Similar results were found differences (F3,713 = 19.23, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.075),
when compared standing combat and displacement which demonstrated that senior displayed longer
without contact frequencies (K3,713 = 13.10 times than other age groups (P < 0.001 for all
P = 0.004); juniors demonstrated lower frequency comparisons).
than pre-cadets (r = 0.03, P = 0.025), cadets Regarding differences in displacement without
(r = 0.06, P < 0.001) and seniors (r = 0.08, combat (F3,713 = 4.32, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.018),
P = 0.021). cadet presented shorter time than senior
Effects of age groups were found in gripping fre- (P = 0.008). In total pause differences
quencies (K3,713 = 10.50, P = 0.015), where junior (F3,713 = 3.34, P = 0.05, η2 = 0.014), cadet pre-
demonstrated lower frequency than cadet (r = 0.12, sented a shorter time than junior (P = 0.038).
P = 0.016) and senior group (r = 0.08, P = 0.003). Differences of groundwork combat time
Regarding technique frequency, the analysis of var- (F3,713 = 16.34, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.064) revealed
iance showed differences between age groups that cadet had shorter values than pre-cadet
(K3,713 = 20.06, P < 0.001), where cadets obtained (P = 0.032) and junior (P = 0.002).
higher values than pre-cadets (r = 0.13, P = 0.011) Table IV shows a sequential parameter with the
and junior (r = 0.13, P < 0.001). Regarding ground- representative temporal structure of female judo
work sequences, significant differences were combat separated by age groups.
observed (K3,713 = 23.31, P < 0.001), where pre- No difference was found between groups in fre-
cadets was higher than senior (r = −0.03 P = 0.012) quency of throwing techniques, attacks, attacks
and junior (r = 0.08, P = 0.035). Moreover, cadets orientations, gripping variations and penalties
obtained higher groundwork frequency than junior (shido) (Table V).
(r = 0.14, P < 0.001). Table III presents total time of The frequency of koshi-waza, ashi-waza, te-waza,
each cycle of combat/pause for each time-motion sutemi-waza, osae-waza, shime-waza and kansetsu-
indicator of the match separated by groups. waza applied in each match of all groups is presented
When combat was separated in each combat/pause in Table VI.
sequential time, statistical differences were found in Regarding ashi-waza, non-parametric tests
total combat time (F3,713 = 10.10, P < 0.001, demonstrated differences between groups
η2 = 0.041). Senior presented longer times than (K3,713 = 49.99, P < 0.001, r = 0.072), with senior
other groups (P = 0.006 for pre-cadet, P < 0.001 presenting lower frequency of ashi-waza than pre-
for cadet and P = 0.027 for junior). Effects were cadet (P < 0.001), Cadet (P < 0.001) and junior
found in standing combat time (F3,713 = 14.02, (P = 0.032). In addition, there was an effect of age
P < 0.001, η2 = 0.056): senior group had longer groups on kansetsu-waza (K2,565 = 11.55, P = 0.009,
times than all groups (P < 0.001 for all compari- r = 0.016), with senior using more these techniques
sons). In gripping time, the comparisons presented than cadet (P < 0.001). Similar results were
6 B. Miarka et al.

Table IV. Representative sequential temporal structure in female judo combat in different age groups.

Pre-cadet Cadet Junior Senior


Combat start Combat start Combat start Combat start

1st SCT (14 s) 1st SCT (11 s) 1st SCT (14 s) 1st SCT (27 s)
1st DWC (6 s) 1st DWC (5 s) 1st DWC (7 s) 1st DWC (7 s)
1st GT (8 s) 1st GT (5 s) 1st GT (7 s) 1st GT (13 s)
2nd DWC (6 s) 2nd DWC (5 s) 1st TT (<1 s) 2nd DWC (7 s)
1st TT (<1 s) 1st TT (<1 s) 1st PT (8 s) 1st TT (2 s)
1st PT (5 s) 1st PT (5 s) 2nd SCT (14 s) 1st PT (6 s)
2nd SCT (14 s) 2nd SCT (11 s) 3rd DWC (7 s) 2nd SCT (27 s)
3rd DWC (6 s) 3rd DWC (5 s) 2nd GT (7 s) 3rd DWC (7 s)
2nd GT (8 s) 2nd GT (5s) 2nd TT (<1 s) 2nd GT (13 s)
2nd TT (<1 s) 2nd TT (<1 s) 2nd PT (8 s) 4th DWC (7 s)
1st GCT (9 s) 1st GCT (7 s) 3rd SCT (14 s) 2nd TT (2 s)
2nd PT (5 s) 2nd PT (5 s) 4th DWC (7 s) 2nd PT (6 s)
3rd SCT (14 s) 3rd SCT (11 s) 3rd GT (7 s) 3rd SCT (27 s)
4th DWC (6 s) 4th DWC (5 s) 3rd TT (<1 s) 4th DWC (7 s)
3rd GT (8 s) 3rd GT (5 s) 1nd GCT (10 s) 3rd GT (13 s)
3rd TT (<1 s) 5th DWC (5 s) 3rd PT (8 s) 3rd TT (2 s)
2nd GCT (9 s) 3rd TT (<1 s) 4th SCT (14 s) 1st GCT (8 s)
3rd PT (5 s) 2nd GCT (7 s) 5th DWC (7 s) 3rd PT (6 s)
Downloaded by [177.9.188.90] at 09:45 22 April 2014

4th SCT (14 s) 3rd PT (5 s) 4th GT (7 s) 4th SCT (27 s)


5th DWC (6 s) 4th SCT (11 s) 4th TT (<1 s) 5th DWC (7 s)
4th GT (8 s) 5th DWC (5 s) 2nd GCT (10 s) 4th GT (13 s)
4th TT (<1 s) 4th GT (5 s) 4th PT (8 s) 4th TT (2 s)
3rd GCT (9 s) 4th TT (<1 s) 5th SCT (14 s) 2nd GCT (8 s)
4th PT (5 s) 3rd GCT (7 s) 6th DWC (7 s) 4th PT (6 s)
5th SCT (14 s) 4th PT (5 s) 5th GT (7 s) 5th SCT (27 s)
6th DWC (6 s) 5th SCT (11 s) 5th TT (<1 s) 6th DWC (7 s)
5th GT (8 s) 6th DWC (5 s) 3rd GCT (10 s) 5th GT (13 s)
5th TT (<1 s) 5th GT (5 s) 5th PT (8 s) 5th TT (2 s)
5th GCT (9 s) 5th TT (<1 s) 6th SCT (14 s) 3rd GCT (8 s)
5th PT (5 s) 5th GCT (7 s) 7th DWC (7 s) 5th PT (6 s)
6th SCT (14 s) 5th PT (5 s) 6th GT (7 s) 6th SCT (27 s)
7th DWC (6 s) 6th SCT (11 s) 6th TT (<1 s) 7th DWC (7 s)
6th GT (8 s) 7th DWC (5 s) 4th GCT (10 s) 6th GT (13 s)
6th TT (<1 s) 6th GT (5 s) 6th PT(8s) – End 6th TT (2 s)
6th GCT (9 s) 6th TT (<1 s) 6th PT (6 s)
7th PT (5s) – End 6th GCT (7 s) 7th SCT (27 s)
7th PT (5 s) 7th DWC (7 s)
7th SCT (11 s) 6th GT (13 s)
8th DWC (5 s) 6th TT (2 s)
7th GT (5 s) 4th GCT (8 s)
7th TT (<1 s)
7th GCT (7 s) 7th PT (6s) End
8th PT (5s) – End

Note: SCT = standing combat time; DWC = displacement without contact; GT = gripping time; technique time; GCT = groundwork
combat time, and; PT = pause time.

Table V. Frequency (mean ± SD) of gripping types, different attacks, projections, attacks orientations and penalties, in each match
separated by age groups.

Age groups Gripping types Different attacks Projections Attack orientations Penalties

Pre-cadet n = 148 3.2 ± 1.7 1.8 ± 2.4 0.84 ± 0.93 2.2 ± 1.3 0.21 ± 0.52
Cadet n = 228 2.7 ± 1.3 2.2 ± 2.8 0.78 ± 0.95 2.5 ± 1.6 0.14 ± 0.43
Junior n = 104 3.0 ± 1.8 1.6 ± 2.2 0.75 ± 0.98 2.2 ± 1.3 0.06 ± 0.23
Senior n = 237 3.0 ± 1.7 1.5 ± 1.9 0.75 ± 0.92 2.4 ± 1.4 0.31 ± 0.67
Female judo matches analysis 7

Table VI. Frequency (mean ± SD) of koshi-waza, ashi-waza, te-waza, sutemi-waza, osae-waza, shime-waza and kansetsu-waza applied in each
combat separated by age groups.

Age groups ashi-waza koshi-waza te-waza sutemi-waza osae-waza shime-waza kansetsu-waza

Pre-cadet n = 148 2.1 ± 2.6 0.4 ± 0.9 1.8 ± 2.4 0.5 ± 0.9 0.2 ± 0.4 NA NA
Cadet n = 228 3.0 ± 3.2 0.5 ± 1.0 2.2 ± 2.8 0.3 ± 0.9 0.15 ± 0.4 0.00 ± 0.00 0.00 ± 0.00
Junior n = 104 1.8 ± 2.2 0.5 ± 1.0 1.6 ± 2.2 0.4 ± 0.7 0.1 ± 0.4 0.02 ± 0.13 0.02 ± 0.13
Senior n = 237 1.3 ± 1.8* 0.4 ± 0.8 1.5 ± 2.0 0.9 ± 1.3 0.08 ± 0.3 0.07 ± 0.25 0.05 ± 0.21

Note: *Age group significantly different from other groups (P < 0.05); NA = techniques not allowed for this age category; ashi = leg;
koshi = hip; te = arm; osae = immobilisation; shime = strangle; kansetsu = joint lock; waza = technique.

observed in comparison with shime-waza displacement without contact (32 ± 28 s) and grip-
(K2,565 = 19.78, P < 0.001, r = 0.02), as senior had ping time (89 ± 63 s) in senior athletes when com-
higher utilisation frequency than cadet (P < 0.001). paring to other ages (Miarka et al., 2012).
No significant differences were found for koshi-waza, Following protocols used in Miarka et al. (2012),
te-waza, sutemi-waza and osae-waza execution. the present study observed a large sample of combats
distributed in the seven official weight categories,
which allowed a reliable characterisation of the age
Discussion
Downloaded by [177.9.188.90] at 09:45 22 April 2014

groups. Perhaps the weight could have influenced


The goal of the present study was to quantify tech- time-motion structure; however, at the present
nical and tactical actions, and the time-motion in moment, studies with these comparisons are scarce.
female judo matches and to compare age groups In the current investigation, no significant differ-
(pre-cadet, cadet, junior and senior). All women ences were found between the two levels and when
groups had lower percentage of combats ending comparing weight categories. The use of T-pattern
before the official combat time rule (pre-cadet = 32%, detection in judo, as previously presented in investi-
cadet = 25%, junior = 32%, senior = 34%, respec- gations conducted with visually impaired judo ath-
tively), in comparison with male athletes studies letes (Gutiérrez-Santiago et al., 2011, 2013) and
(Castarlenas & Planas, 1997; Miarka et al., 2012). used in the present study is also an important
Male fighters demonstrated 44% to 58% of cham- approach to improve technical–tactical and physical
pionship combats finishing before regular time training methods for judo athletes, based on the
(Castarlenas & Planas, 1997; Miarka et al., 2012). specific profile observed in each age group, gender
Gutiérrez-Santiago et al. (2011) demonstrated or weight category. Further studies can help to
higher values for visually impaired athletes, 81% of improve the understanding of analysis of this type
male combats and 74% of women combats finishing of data, considering both international and national
before the maximal combat time. Regarding age female groups, what could bring differences between
group differences in the present study, similar results weight category and/or competitive level, as preced-
were found by previous authors, who reported that ing studies reported (Calmet & Ahmaidi, 2004;
junior male athletes scored 14% less ippon than Franchini et al., 2008).
senior (Miarka et al., 2012). Gutiérrez-Santiago et al. (2011) showed distinct
Even with changes in the rules over the years, results from the present research, when observing
these modifications do not seem to interact with visually impaired athletes and differences between
the temporal structure of the combat (Franchini, genders of standing combat and groundwork combat
Artioli, et al., 2013; Sikorski et al., 1987). For times, where 57.7% of male combat time and 39.1%
instance, Castarlenas and Planas (1997) revealed a of female combat time corresponded to a standing
similar total combat time for male athletes when situation, while 42.2% and 60.9% to a groundwork
compared to senior female athletes in the present situation. The standing and groundwork combat
study, after more than 10 years. On the other hand, mean times were 82 s (men)/54 s (women) and 60
both results are longer than the total combat time s (men)/84 s (women), respectively.
reported by Gutiérrez-Santiago et al. (2011) with Concerning total displacement without contact
visually impaired women athletes, which demon- time, Marcon, Franchini, Jardim, and Barros Neto
strated a total of 119 s – similar to pre-cadet, cadet (2010) showed approximately 80 s of total time of
and junior combats. The differences among female displacement without contact in male combat simu-
senior and other age groups are in agreement with lations. This difference can be explained by the fact
Miarka et al. (2012), which demonstrated effects of that this period is used for recovery and for strategic
age in male combats. The authors observed longer development in championships, based on feedback
periods in standing combat time (148 ± 92 s), from previous actions during the tournament
8 B. Miarka et al.

(Calmet et al., 2010; Franchini, Artioli, et al., 2013; characteristic determining proficiency in judo
Miarka et al., 2012). matches (Calmet & Ahmaidi, 2004; Calmet et al.,
Regarding sequential combat/pause cycles, 2010). The differences between senior and other
Gutiérrez-Santiago et al. (2011) demonstrated no groups in gripping time are similar to comparisons
differences between visually able and impaired male reported by other investigations with different age
judokas in the sequential parameters; therefore, the groups in male athletes (Miarka et al., 2012). In
present study can help support similar considera- contrast, senior women athletes in the present
tions for women. The greatest difference is the num- research displayed longer gripping times than
ber of groundwork combat sequences in which reported in male judo athletes (Calmet & Ahmaidi,
impaired women presented five sequences, higher 2004; Miarka et al., 2012). Preceding research with
than the values found in all age groups, with a med- different levels of experience and ages suggested that
ian between three and four sequences. No other experts have a higher rate of technical and tactical
study with visually able women was found. gripping knowledge (Calmet et al., 2010), regardless
Concerning age group differences, results are in of age (Calmet & Ahmaidi, 2004; Calmet et al.,
agreement with previous reports with male athletes, 2006), and more experienced athletes (i.e. senior
which described between five and nine cycles of judo athletes) spend more time on this specific
combat/pause sequences for senior male matches action (Calmet et al., 2010; Miarka et al., 2012).
(Castarlenas & Planas, 1997; Marcon et al., 2010; Regarding sequences of groundwork time,
Miarka et al., 2012). Thus, the present results are in Castarlenas and Planas (1997) reported similar
Downloaded by [177.9.188.90] at 09:45 22 April 2014

agreement with previous findings for male athletes, results, with 7.4 s per cycle. In contrast, visually
which described lower values of pause sequences in impaired women presented longer time of ground-
the junior age group, with six sequences, when com- work combat, with 15.3 s per combat (Gutiérrez-
pared with other ages, which presented seven Santiago et al., 2011). The pause time of visually
sequences (Miarka et al., 2012). These facts can be impaired women, with 21.1 s was longer than the
associated with a higher number of techniques present data.
applied without score, but still with the ability to Concerning differences in technique time, the
induce the opponent to a susceptible position on longer time spent by senior women in comparison
the groundwork combat (Franchini, Takito, & with other groups could be explained by the fact that
Bertuzzi, 2005). 54% of total sutemi-waza were applied by senior
With respect to time per combat/pause sequence, athletes. These techniques require more time to be
Gutiérrez-Santiago et al. (2011) demonstrated lower applied, as they normally involve performing twists
results (i.e. 19.7 s per combat time sequence in and self-projection on the ground to throw down the
visually impaired women) than the findings in pre- opponent. Previous study pointed that sutemi-waza
cadet, junior and senior groups. Visually impaired was the counter-attack technique most often applied,
women athletes presented lower standing combat with higher efficiency, as scores were obtained in
time than all groups, with 7.8 s. This difference can 44% of the occasions during Polish Judo
be associated with the time spent in the displacement Championships disputed between 2005 and 2008
without contact, which is not explored by visually (Boguszewski, 2009). The author’s hypothesis con-
impaired athletes. When observing displacement siders the role of match processes, in which the use
without contact time per combat/pause cycle of sutemi-waza could come from a defensive position
sequences, results were similar to those presented of athletes in an attempt to avoid being attacked
by male athletes of 4 ± 1 s (Marcon et al., 2010) (Boguszewski & Boguszewska, 2006). Also, this
and 5 ± 8 s (Miarka et al., 2012). This aspect can be information can be used to prevent injuries, since
explained from the fact that displacement without previous studies also pointed that throwing techni-
contact is considered a tactical element, with two ques, defensive actions and groundwork behaviours
propositions: first, to analyse the opponent, attempt- could implicate in injuries (James & Pieter, 2003;
ing to perform interactions conducted by gripping Koshida et al., 2010; Pieter, Talbot, Pinlac, &
the judogi; second, to control the space trajectory of Bercades, 2001).
the combat and, at the same time, to manage the Ashi-waza were the most-used techniques by all age
attempts of the adversary to impose this control, and, groups, followed by te-waza, koshi-waza and sutemi-
as athletes are currently trying to grip, attack and waza. That result agrees with a preceding report that
defend actions of his/her opponent (Calmet & studied techniques used by judoists during World and
Ahmaidi, 2004; Franchini et al., 2008; Miarka Olympic Tournaments in the years 1995–1999, which
et al., 2012). found a higher percentage of ashi-waza, followed by
There is a range of primary and secondary factors the same sequence of techniques groups (Sterkowicz
influencing the development of expertise in judo & Franchini, 2000). Other authors established similar
combats, and gripping seems to be one essential results in high-level college judo athletes (Franchini
Female judo matches analysis 9

et al., 2005). These data suggest that the groups of when compared European Championships of these
techniques applied in elite judo combat tend to follow two consecutive years. In the present study, the age
this order of preference. However, the present data is groups did not present effects in the number of
limited to regional and state level and it could not be projections, penalties, different attacks applied and/
transferable to higher level athletes. Also, previous or in technical orientations.
reports suggested that effects of weight were asso- The present research found a number of attacks
ciated with the preferential attack (Almansba et al., applied similar to preceding report, which analysed
2008; Claessens, Beunen, Wellens, & Geldof, 1987). 108 male athletes (52 of pre-cadet, 24 of cadet, 12 of
However, these studies were not made in a contextual junior and 20 of senior) (Calmet et al., 2010). These
tournament situation. In addition, the findings sug- authors reported differences in the number of attacks
gested that preferential attacks seem to be more asso- applied during the combat between senior (3.3 ± 0.9)
ciated with stature proportionality, body composition and pre-cadet (2.8 ± 1.3), while high-level athletes
and somatotype than weight categories (Claessens used 4.7 ± 0.8 attack directions (Calmet & Ahmaidi,
et al., 1987; Sterkowicz-Przybycien & Franchini, 2004). However, this study did not consider the
2013). direction of attacks related to gripping variations
The current results showed that (1) the number of used by the judoka. In another study, authors
ippon obtained through ashi-waza and te-waza were observed a higher number of attacks in super elite
not different from each other, but these were more and elite judo players, with a mean of 9 ± 6 and
effective than other techniques (i.e. resulted in more 6 ± 4 throwing techniques per match, respectively
Downloaded by [177.9.188.90] at 09:45 22 April 2014

ippon). In addition, ashi-waza values were higher in (Franchini et al., 2008), suggesting that this aspect
pre-cadet, cadet and junior when compared to can be an important factor in the technical develop-
senior. This difference could be explained by the ment of judo athletes. Furthermore, in the present
fact that a lot of leg attacks are easy to apply, because research, lower values of directions in all groups
they normally do not require twists and self-projec- were observed in comparison with super elite and
tion, such as koshi-waza, te-waza and sutemi-waza elite, who presented 3.8 ± 0.5 and 2.9 ± 1.0 attack
techniques (Sterkowicz et al., 2013). Regarding kan- directions (considering 4 as the maximum number of
setsu-waza and shime-waza, the difference between directions), respectively (Franchini et al., 2008).
senior and cadet can be explained by the fact that
cadet is the first age category in which these techni-
ques can be used; thus, athletes in this age group are Conclusion
starting to use them in competitions and it is The current study was the first to describe time-
expected that they use them less than more experi- motion patterns for female judo matches and to
enced athletes (IJF, 2013). conduct a comparison among four different age
The European Judo Union observed less score by groups using a high number of matches. This knowl-
projections than penalties scores in the last Olympic edge contributes to create prophylaxis programmes
Games in London 2012 (EJU, 2012). For example, according to the actions patterns observed. Our
about 573 penalties and and only 204 ippon (max- results indicated that the senior age group presented
imum score) occurred during this competition. In higher values of total combat time, standing combat
addition, the data of Olympic Games in Beijing 2008 time and gripping time than all other groups.
demonstrated a considerable increase in scores by Therefore, the prescription of training for pre-
penalty compared to previous years’ competitions cadet, cadet and junior female judo athletes should
(IJF, 2013). Even with the rule modifications in consider the specific demands of the competitive
2009, the number of penalties increased in the match, with maturation being considered as an indi-
Masters World Championship 2009, with 1.58 cator of performance capacity.
penalties/min. In Junior World Championship, this
index was 1.46 penalties/min and in the under 23
years European Championship, it was 2.17 penalties/ Funding
min combat (Heinisch, Oswald, & Büsch, 2010). In
This study was funded by FAPESP n. 08/51571-0;
2013, a new rule modification was done with the aim
the last author thanks the CNPq support [236768/
to reduce the number of penalties and to make the
2012-3].
combat more attractive to the public. However, in a
comparison of the European Championship among
the years 2012 and 2013, Franchini, Takito and References
Calmet (2013) demonstrated an increase in the
Almansba, R., Franchini, E., Sterkowicz, S., Imamura, R. T.,
number of penalties and a decrease in wazari and Calmet, M., & Ahmaidi, S. (2008). A comparative study of
yuko scores for women and men and an increase in speed expressed by the number of throws between heavier and
ippon and hansokumake (maximal penalty) for males, lighter categories in judo. Science & Sports, 23, 186–188.
10 B. Miarka et al.

Atkinson, G., & Nevill, A. M. (2001). Selected issues in the impaired men and women. Journal of Sports Sciences, 29,
design and analysis of sport performance research. Journal of 1443–1451.
Sports Sciences, 19, 811–827. Heinisch, H. D., Oswald, R., & Büsch, D. (2010).
Bland, J. M., & Altman, D. G. (2007). Agreement between meth- Entwicklungstendenzen der wettkampfleistung im judo unter
ods of measurement with multiple observations per individual. berücksichtigung des einflusses von regeländenrungen [Trends
Journal of Biopharmaceutical Statistics, 17, 571–582. in the competitive performance in judo considering the influ-
Boguszewski, D. (2009). Defensive actions of contestants during ence of regulation changes]. Leistungssport, 40, 13–20.
Polish Judo Championships in the years 2005–2008. Baltic Hughes, M., & Franks, I. M. (2004). Notational analysis – a
Journal of Health and Physical Activity, 1, 111–117. review of the literature. In M. Hughes & I. M. Franks (Eds.),
Boguszewski, D., & Boguszewska, K. (2006). Dynamics of judo Notational analysis of sport (pp. 59–106). London: Routledge.
contests performed by finalists of European Championships International Judo Federation. (2012). Sports and organization rules
(Rotterdam 2005). Archives of Budo, 6, 40–44. of the International Judo Federation. Retrieved from http://www.
Calmet, M., & Ahmaidi, S. (2004). Survey of advantages obtained ijf.org
by judoka in competition by level of practice. Perceptual and International Judo Federation. (2013). Sports and organization rules
Motor Skills, 99, 284–290. of the International Judo Federation. Retrieved from http://www.
Calmet, M., Miarka, B., & Franchini, E. (2010). Modeling of ijf.org
grasps in judo contests. International Journal of Performance James, G., & Pieter, W. (2003). Injury rates in adult elite judoka.
Analysis in Sport, 10, 229–240. Biology of Sport, 20, 25–33.
Calmet, M., Trezel, N., & Ahmaidi, S. (2006). Survey of system Koshida, S., Deguchi, T., Miyashita, K., Iwai, K., & Urabe, Y.
of attacks by judoka in regional and interregional matches. (2010). The common mechanisms of anterior cruciate ligament
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 103, 835–840. injuries in judo: A retrospective analysis. British Journal of Sports
Castarlenas, J. L., & Planas, A. (1997). Estudio de la estructura Medicine, 44, 856–861.
temporal del combate de judo [Study of the temporal structure Lago, C. (2009). The influence of match location, quality of oppo-
Downloaded by [177.9.188.90] at 09:45 22 April 2014

of judo combat]. Apunts – Educación Fisica y Deportes, 47, 32–39. sition, and match status on possession strategies in professional
Claessens, A. L. M., Beunen, G. P., Wellens, R., & Geldof, G. association football. Journal of Sports Sciences, 27, 1463–1469.
(1987). Somatotype and body structure of world top judoists. Marcon, G., Franchini, E., Jardim, J. R., & Barros Neto, T. L.
Journal of Sports Medicine, 27, 105–113. (2010). Structural analysis of action and time in sports: Judo.
Del Vecchio, F. B., Hirata, S. M., & Franchini, E. (2011). A Journal of Quantitative Analysis in Sport, 6, 1–13.
review of time-motion analysis and combat development in Matsumoto, D., Takeuchi, M., Nakajima, T., & Iida, E. (2000).
mixed martial arts matches at regional level tournaments. Competition anxiety, self-confidence, personality and competi-
Perceptual Motor Skills, 112, 639–648. tion performance of American elite and non-elite judo athletes.
Drust, B. (2010). Performance analysis research: Meeting the Research Journal of Budo, 32, 12–21.
challenge. Journal of Sports Sciences, 28, 921–922. Miarka, B., Hayashida, C. R., Julio, U. F., Calmet, M., &
European Judo Union. (2012). EJU provides statistical analyses of Franchini, E. (2011). Objectivity of FRAMI-software for judo
Olympic games. Retrieved from http://www.eju.net match analysis. International Journal of Performance Analysis in
Ferreira Julio, U., Panissa, V. L. G., Miarka, B., Takito, M. Y., & Sport, 11, 254–266.
Franchini, E. (2013). Home advantage in judo: A study of the Miarka, B., Marques, J. B., & Franchini, E. (2011).
world ranking list. Journal of Sports Sciences, 31, 212–218. Reinterpreting the history of women’s judo in Japan. The
Field, A. (2005). Discovering statistics using SPSS. London: SAGE. International Journal of the History of Sport, 28, 1016–1029.
Franchini, E., Artioli, G. G., & Brito, C. J. (2013). Judo combat: Miarka, B., Panissa, V., Julio, U. F., Vecchio, F. B. D., Calmet,
Time-motion analysis and physiology. International Journal M., & Franchini, E. (2012). A comparison of time-motion
Performance Analysis in Sport, 13, 624–641. performance between age groups in judo matches. Journal of
Franchini, E., Del Vecchio, F. B., Matsushigue, K. A., & Sports Sciences, 30, 899–905.
Artioli, G. G. (2011). Physiological profiles of elite judo ath- Pieter, W., Talbot, C., Pinlac, V., & Bercades, L. T. (2001).
letes. Sports Medicine, 41, 147–166. Injuries at the Konica Asian Judo Championships. Acta
Franchini, E., Sterkowicz, S., Meira, C. M., Gomes, F. R., & Kinesiologiae Universitatis Tartuensis, 6, 102–111.
Tani, G. (2008). Technical variation in a sample of high level Santos, V. G. F., Franchini, E., & Lima-Silva, A. E. (2011).
judo players. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 106, 859–869. Relationship between attack and skipping in taekwondo contests.
Franchini, E., Takito, M. Y., & Bertuzzi, R. C. M. (2005). Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 25, 1743–1751.
Morphological, physiological and technical variables in high- Sikorski, W., Mickiewicz, G., Majle, B., & Laksa, C. (1987).
level college judoists. Archives of Budo, 1, 1–17. Structure of the contest and work capacity of the judoist (pp. 59–
Franchini, E., Takito, M. Y., & Calmet, M. (2013). European 65). Spala: Institute of Sport, Department of Theory of Sport,
Judo Championships: Impact of the new rule changes on points Department of Physiology, Data Base Center, and Polish Judo
and penalties. International Journal Performance Analysis in Association.
Sport, 13, 474–479. Sterkowicz, S., & Franchini, E. (2000). Techniques used by judo-
Guerandel, C., & Mennesson, C. (2007). Gender construction in ists during the World and Olympic tournaments 1995–1999.
judo interactions. International Review for the Sociology of Sport, Human Movement, 2, 24–33.
42, 167–186. Sterkowicz, S., Sacripanti, A., & Sterkowicz-Przybycień, K.
Gutiérrez-Santiago, A., Prieto, I., Ayán, C., & Cancela, J. M. (2013). Techniques frequently used during London Olympic
(2013). T-Pattern detection in judo combat: An approach to judo tournaments: A biomechanical approach. Archives of Budo,
training male judokas with visual impairments according to 9, 51–58.
their weight category. International Journal of Sports Science & Sterkowicz-Przybycien, K., & Franchini, E. (2013). Demographic
Coaching, 8, 385–394. profile of combat sports athletes: A comparative analysis
Gutiérrez-Santiago, A., Prieto, I., Camerino, O., & Anguera, T. between genders and competitive achievement in London
(2011). The temporal structure of judo bouts in visually 2012. Archives of Budo, 9, 149–159.

You might also like