You are on page 1of 5

Journal of the Neurological Sciences 310 (2011) 70–74

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of the Neurological Sciences


j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w. e l s ev i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / j n s

Brain structural MRI correlates of cognitive dysfunctions in Parkinson's disease


Naroa Ibarretxe-Bilbao a, b, c, d,⁎, Carme Junque a, b, Maria J. Marti a, c, Eduardo Tolosa a, c
a
Centro de Investigación en Red de Enfermedades Neurodegenerativas (CIBERNED), Hospital Clínic de Barcelona, Spain
b
Department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychobiology, University of Barcelona, Spain
c
Movement Disorders Unit, Neurology Service, Institut Clínic de Neurociències (ICN), Hospital Clínic de Barcelona, Spain
d
Department of Methods and Experimental Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Education, University of Deusto, Spain

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Cognitive dysfunction occurs at early stages of Parkinson's disease (PD). Initial studies reported that cognitive
Received 29 March 2011 dysfunction in early PD only affected fronto-striatal circuits, provoking a marked executive dysfunction.
Received in revised form 28 July 2011 Memory impairment in PD was thought to depend on a problem in retrieving stored information, therefore
Accepted 30 July 2011
also reflecting a fronto-striatal dysfunction. However, there is increasing structural MRI evidence of medial
Available online 23 August 2011
temporal lobe atrophy in PD, which may be responsible for memory dysfunction. Other neuropsychological
Keywords:
functions usually impaired in PD are semantic fluency, visuoperceptual and visuospatial functions, decision-
Parkinson's disease making and recognition of facial emotions; and impairments in these functions are associated with cortical
Cognition structural changes assessed by MRI. Overall, although the literature on the topic is scarce, there is increasing
Memory evidence of brain structural changes, detectable by MRI, which can explain the neuropsychological deficits
Magnetic resonance imaging early in the clinical disease course before dementia develops. In this review, we summarize the papers that
have used structural MRI to study the neuroanatomical correlates of cognitive dysfunctions in PD.
© 2011 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction 2. Neuroanatomical correlates of cognitive dysfunctions in PD

Cognitive dysfunctions occur at early stages of Parkinson's disease 2.1. Memory


(PD) and mostly involve impairment of memory, executive and
visuoperceptual functions [1–4]. Neuroimaging techniques, specifically Cognitive dysfunction in early stages of PD is much more extended
the use of structural MRI, have proved useful for assessing the than previously believed and is not only limited to executive
neuroanatomical correlates of these cognitive dysfunctions. The dysfunction. Initial studies reported that cognitive dysfunction in
methods most widely used to assess structural MRI changes in PD are early PD only affected fronto-striatal circuits, provoking a marked
volumetric region-of-interest (ROI) and voxel-based morphometry executive dysfunction [5]. Memory problems associated with PD have
(VBM). Combining these neuroimaging techniques and neuropsycho- traditionally been reported to be secondary to retrieval deficits and
logical testing may identify the cerebral substrate for cognitive changes therefore also reflect fronto-striatal dysfunction [6]. However, there is
occurring in PD before the onset of dementia. In this review, we increasing evidence of a learning and recognition memory deficit in
summarize the cross-sectional MRI papers that study the neuroana- both demented and non-demented PD patients, which is inconsistent
tomical correlates of cognitive dysfunctions in PD patients (see Table 1). with this retrieval deficit hypothesis [7,8]. MRI studies suggest that
A systematic literature search using MEDLINE database was carried out medial temporal lobe atrophy may be responsible for memory
on papers published in English between 1998 and March 2011. We dysfunction in PD. Using an ROI approach, Riekkinen et al. [9] reported
included the studies in PD that performed: i) analyses of T1-weighted that right and left hippocampus volume correlated with memory
structural MRI brain data; ii) neuropsychological assessment; and accuracy on the Delayed Matching to Sample Test (DMTS) in a group of
iii) correlation analyses between the neuropsychological data and non-demented PD. Using a visual scale, Bruck et al. [10] reported that
structural MRI brain data. early PD patients had more atrophy in the hippocampus and prefrontal
cortex than controls, and that atrophy of the left hippocampus
correlated with verbal memory score. Tam et al. [11] also used a
visual scale and reported medial temporal lobe atrophy in PD patients
compared to controls, but no correlation between atrophy and cogni-
tive impairment was found. Camicioli et al. [12] showed a progressive
⁎ Corresponding author at: Department of Methods and Experimental Psychology,
Faculty of Psychology and Education, University of Deusto, Avenida de las Universidades
hippocampal volume loss in PD and found that verbal memory,
24, 48007 Bilbao, Spain. Tel.: + 34 94 413 90 00x2891. assessed by the CERAD recognition memory test, was associated with
E-mail address: naroa.ibarretxe@deusto.es (N. Ibarretxe-Bilbao). left but not right hippocampal atrophy. Junque et al. [13] showed that

0022-510X/$ – see front matter © 2011 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jns.2011.07.054
N. Ibarretxe-Bilbao et al. / Journal of the Neurological Sciences 310 (2011) 70–74 71

Table 1
Summary of main MRI studies on neuroanatomical correlates of cognitive dysfunctions in PD.

Study Participants Matched control Assessment MRI analysis Neuropsychological test Correlation with
(mean age) group?a of VH? technique (impaired or not?)b brain structure

Memory
Riekkinen et al. 1998 20 mild PD (60) No control group No Manual ROI DMTS memory L and R hippocampus
15 UM severe PD (64) accuracy (Yes)
8 IM severe PD (65)
Camicioli et al. 2003 10 PD (73.3) Yes No Manual ROI CERAD recognition L hippocampus
10 PDD (74.7) (Yes)
11 AD (74.4)
12 controls (73.9)
Bruck et al. 2004 20 early PD (61.3) Yes No Visual scale VEM (not specified) L hippocampus
22 controls (65.7)
Junque et al. 2005 16 PD (72.8) Yes No Manual ROI RAVLT Learning (Yes) Amygdala hippocampus
16 PDD (70.1)
16 controls (71.3)
Bouchard et al. 2007 44 PD (71.1) Yes No Manual ROI CVLT-II (No) L and R amygdala
13 PDD (71.9) L hippocampal head
44 controls (71.1) BVMT-R (No) L hippocampal head
Ibarretxe-Bilbao et al. 2008 9 PDD (69.8) Yes Yes: 16 PD VBM RAVLT Learning (Yes) L and R hippocampal head
16 PD with VH (73.5) with VH
19 PD no-VH (72.5)
56 Controls (73)
c
Dalaker et al. 2011 11 MCI PD (67.5) Yes No Volumetric automatic CVLT-II (not specified) Fourth ventricle
32 non-MCI PD (63.3) segmentation
41 controls (64.2)

Executive function
Nagano-Saito et al. 2005 39 PD (61.8) Yes: only age matched. Yes: none VBM RCPM (not specified) Dorsolateral PFC
9 PDD (67.3) with VH
31 Controls (63.5) Parahippocampus
Camicioli et al. 2008 43 PD (70.7) Yes No VBM Index of executive Caudate, middle temporal
43 Controls (71.0) functiond gyri, precuneus cerebellume
Ibarretxe-Bilbao et al. 2009 24 early PD (56.1) Yes Yes: none VBM IGT (Yes) L lateral OFC f
24 controls (57.6) with VH

Verbal fluency
Pereira et al. 2009 32 PD (73.1) No control group Yes: 14 PD VBM Semantic fluency Inferior and middle frontal
with VH (Not specified) gyrus Temporal lobe

Visuoperceptual and visuospatial functions


Pereira et al. 2009 36 PD (73.2) Yes Yes: 18 PD VBM BFRT (yes) Fusiform gyrus
20 controls (72.7) with VH VFDT (yes) L and R superior parietal

Recognition of facial expressions of emotions


f
Ibarretxe-Bilbao et al. 2009 24 early PD (56.1) Yes Yes: none VBM Ekman test (yes) Bilateral OFC
24 controls (57.6) with VH

Others (negative results or correlations with general cognitive measures)


Apostolova et al. 2010 12 PD (68.4) Yes No Radial distance MMSE (yes) Head of the caudate
8 MCI PD (78) mapping
15 PDD (73.4) Inferior and posterior lateral
20 controls (73.6) ventricle horns
Tam et al. 2005 33 PD (75.4) Yes No Visual scale CAMCOG (yes) No significant correlations
31 PDD (71.8) (MTA rating)
25 DLB (75.3)
31 AD (78.7)
39 Controls (75.2)
Tinaz et al. 2011 15 early PD (59.7) Yes No Cortical thickness (Not impaired) No significant correlations
15 controls (59.8)

Abbreviations: BFRT = Benton's facial recognition test; BVMT-R = Brief visuospatial memory test, revised; CAMCOG = Cambridge Cognitive Examination; CERAD = Consortium to
Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease; CVLT-II = California Verbal Learning Test; DMTS = Delayed Matching to Sample Test; GM = gray matter; ICV = intracranial volume; IGT =
Iowa Gambling Task; L = left; MCI = mild cognitive impairment; MI = memory impaired; MTA = medial temporal lobe atrophy; OFC = orbitofrontal cortex; PD = Parkinson's disease
patients without dementia; PDD = Parkinson's disease patients with dementia; PFC = prefrontal cortex; R = right; RAVLT = Rey's auditory verbal learning test; RCPM = Raven colored
progressive matrices; ROI = region-of-interest; UM = unimpaired memory; VBM = voxel-based morphometry; VEM = Wechsler memory scale-revised (WMS-R) test for verbal
memory; VFDT = visual form discrimination test; VH = visual hallucinations.
a
Age-, gender-, education-matched control group.
b
The table only includes the neuropsychological tests associated with brain structure.
c
Correlation corrected for age, gender and education.
d
Executive function index includes: Stroop interference, TMT A and B; digit ordering.
e
Correlation corrected for age, gender and ICV.
f
Correlation corrected for total GM volume.

verbal memory scores not only correlated with the volume of the associated hippocampal atrophy in PD. In the whole PD sample, left
hippocampus but also with the volume of the amygdala in PD patients. hippocampal volume, specifically the volume of the head of the hip-
Using an ROI method Bouchard et al. [14] reported significant age- pocampus, correlated with recall on the California Verbal Learning Test
72 N. Ibarretxe-Bilbao et al. / Journal of the Neurological Sciences 310 (2011) 70–74

(CVLT-II) and the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test, revised (BVMT-R). semantic deficits in PD patients. The most interesting results of that
In addition, they also found a correlation between amygdala volumes study were that semantic fluency scores in non-demented PD patients
and CVLT-II recall. Using VBM, Ibarretxe-Bilbao et al. [15] showed a correlated with GM density in the inferior and middle frontal gyrus
pattern of hippocampal atrophy affecting the head and the tail of this (BA 10, 46) and in several areas in the temporal lobe (BA 20, 21, 22,
structure in demented PD patients, but restricted just to the hippo- 38), assessed by VBM. On the other hand, no significant correlation
campal head in patients at risk for dementia (patients with visual was found between global brain GM density and phonemic fluency
hallucinations). When PD patients participating in the study were scores. These results suggest that semantic fluency tests reflect
considered altogether, a significant correlation was found between cortical dysfunctions in PD better than phonemic fluency.
RAVLT learning scores and GM density in the head of left and right
hippocampus [15]. 2.4. Visuospatial and visuoperceptual functions
Not only limbic atrophy but ventricular enlargement as well has
been associated with memory dysfunctions in PD. Using freesurfer Pereira et al. [28] evaluated visuoperceptual functions with
Dalaker et al. [16] found that fourth ventricle volume correlated with Benton's facial recognition test (BFRT) in a group of non-demented
memory scores assessed by CLVT-II after including age, sex and PD patients and found that performance on this test correlated with
education as covariates. In another study, the enlargement of the GM density in the fusiform gyrus (BA 19, 36), parahippocampus, the
inferior and posterior lateral ventricle horns correlated with MMSE middle occipital gyrus (BA 19), and the inferior frontal gyrus. Patients
scores in PD [17]; and a longitudinal study showed that ventricular were also assessed by means of the Visual Form Discrimination Test
change over 36 months correlated with change in dementia rating (VFDT), a test that not only evaluates visuoperceptual functions but
scale (DRS) and MMSE in patients with PD [18]. Ventricular involves visuospatial abilities as well, and correlations between
enlargement and its relation to cognition in PD patients could be performance in this test and GM density in bilateral parietal regions
interpreted as a sign of brain GM and WM loss due to progressive (BA 7, 40) were observed. Other significant GM correlations were
neurodegeneration. detected in the superior occipital (BA19), middle frontal (BA 9), and
inferior frontal gyrus. Taken together, these findings suggest that
2.2. Executive functions visuospatial and visuoperceptual dysfunctions in PD patients mainly
reflect structural GM changes in posterior cerebral regions, specifi-
Executive dysfunction in PD is frequently attributed to the cally in temporo-parietal cortical regions.
dopamine loss in the striatum that may affect frontal lobe function
by disrupting activity within basal ganglia–thalamocortical circuits 2.5. Recognition of facial emotions
[19]. However, MRI studies show that morphological brain structural
changes may also be involved. Camicioli et al. [20] assessed executive Several studies have reported impairment of recognition of facial
functions in PD using a composite measure which included the Stroop expressions of emotions in PD [21,29–33]. Specifically, Sprengelmeyer
test, Trail Making Test, and digit ordering test; and significant et al. [33] reported that de novo PD patients scored significantly lower
correlations were found between this composite executive function in recognizing sadness and fear than healthy controls. These results
measure and GM volume in bilateral caudate, bilateral middle suggested that recognition of these emotions is impaired very early in
temporal gyri, precuneus and cerebellum. On the other hand, in the disease and is independent of treatment. In a group of early PD
early PD one VBM study [21] found a significant correlation between patients we found atrophy of bilateral OFC and amygdala using VBM
decision-making, assessed by the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), and [21]. The bilateral OFC atrophy correlated with the scores in the Ekman
atrophy in left lateral orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), which is considered 60 faces test, a measure of recognition of facial emotions, in the PD
to be the limbic association cortex. Decision-making is usually group. Another MRI study [34] also reported degeneration of the
discussed in the context of executive dysfunction. However, a recent OFC in early PD patients but no significant correlations between the
review showed that only 4 out of 12 studies reported a correlation behavioral tests and the imaging data were found.
between executive functions and performance on decision-making
tasks in PD [22]. Finally, we also included in this review as depicted in 3. Discussion
Table 1, one study [23] that reported an association between the
performance on the Raven Colored Progressive Matrices (RCPM) and We reviewed MRI studies aimed at identifying brain morphological
dorsolateral PFC and parahippocampal atrophy. The authors of this changes associated with neuropsychological dysfunctions in PD; and
study suggested this test as a measure of executive and visuospatial most of these studies provided evidence of specific structural brain
functions. However, the RCPM should be better considered as a changes associated with specific neuropsychological deficits in PD. The
measure of visual inductive reasoning and/or a measure for evaluating most consistent finding refers to memory function. Structural MRI
general intelligence. studies using different techniques have shown reduced hippocampal
and amygdalar volumes in PD patients, and atrophy of these limbic
2.3. Verbal fluency structures has been related to verbal memory deficits in PD. This
review also shows that other neuropsychological functions usually
Impairment in verbal fluency is frequently observed in PD and impaired in early stages of PD are associated with cortical structural
occurs at the early stages of the disease [24]. Verbal fluency includes changes detectable by MRI.
semantic and phonemic fluency, and early impairment in semantic Pathological explanations for cognitive decline in PD include
fluency is a predictor of conversion to dementia in PD [25]. Deficits in coexistent Lewy body degeneration and Alzheimer-type changes
phonemic and semantic fluencies are considered secondary to frontal [35]. All these changes might lead to morphological brain changes,
lobe dysfunctions, as both types of fluency are impaired in patients including atrophy, which can be detected in vivo by structural MRI
with structural frontal lesions. Semantic fluency is, however, more studies. The six-stage system of brain pathology in PD proposed by
closely related to temporal damage than phonemic fluency [24]. In Braak et al. [36] suggests a predictable sequence of topographically
terms of language organization, recent data points to a role for the ascending LB pathology. As the disease progresses, components of the
posterior lateral superior temporal lobe in analyzing phonemic and autonomic, limbic, and somatomotor systems become pathologically
lexical features of speech, with more anterior regions playing some involved. During stages 1–2, inclusion body pathology is confined to
role in higher order sentence level processing [26]. Only one study the medulla oblongata/pontine tegmentum and olfactory bulb/
[27] has investigated the neuroanatomical correlates of phonemic and anterior olfactory nucleus. These stages are thought to correspond to
N. Ibarretxe-Bilbao et al. / Journal of the Neurological Sciences 310 (2011) 70–74 73

the so called premotor phase of the disease [37]. In stages 3–4, the Acknowledgments
substantia nigra and other nuclear grays of the midbrain and forebrain
become the main focus of pathology. At this point, most individuals This work was supported by Generalitat de Catalunya [2009 SGR
probably cross the threshold to the symptomatic motor phase of the 0941 to C.J., 2009 SGR 0836 to E.T., PSI2010-16174 to C. J.]; CIBERNED
illness. In Braak et al. stages 5–6, the process extends to the neocortex. and University of Deusto.
Dementia in PD has been shown to occur frequently, but not always, in
patients eventually found to have on pathological examination References
abundant cortical LBs as it occurs in Braak et al. stages 5 and 6 [38].
VBM studies agree with Braak et al. staging in the sense that non- [1] Aarsland D, Bronnick K, Larsen JP, Tysnes OB, Alves G, Norwegian ParkWest Study
Group. Cognitive impairment in incident, untreated Parkinson disease: the
demented PD patients have limbic (amygdala and hippocampus) as Norwegian ParkWest study. Neurology 2009;72(13):1121–6.
well as paralimbic (cingulate cortex) and prefrontal GM reductions. [2] Elgh E, Domellof M, Linder J, Edstrom M, Stenlund H, Forsgren L. Cognitive function
However, in patients with early stages of motor impairment (Hoehn in early Parkinson's disease: a population-based study. Eur J Neurol 2009;16(12):
1278–84.
and Yahr I and II), which one would presume to reflect Braak et al.
[3] Muslimovic D, Post B, Speelman JD, Schmand B. Cognitive profile of patients with
stages 3 and 4, correlation between cognitive deficits and GM newly diagnosed Parkinson disease. Neurology 2005;65(8):1239–45.
reductions are observed in the OFC but not in the medial temporal [4] Foltynie T, Brayne CE, Robbins TW, Barker RA. The cognitive ability of an incident
lobe [21]. cohort of Parkinson's patients in the UK. The CamPaIGN study. Brain 2004;127(3):
550–60.
On the other hand, cognitive dysfunctions in PD, mainly executive [5] Owen AM, James M, Leigh PN, Summers BA, Marsden CD, Quinn NP, et al. Fronto-
dysfunctions, have been usually attributed to dopamine depletion striatal cognitive deficits at different stages of Parkinson's disease. Brain 1992
affecting fronto-striatal circuits. MRI studies reviewed here show Dec;115(6):1727–51.
[6] Cooper JA, Sagar HJ, Jordan N, Harvey NS, Sullivan EV. Cognitive impairment in
degeneration of brain regions involved in other circuits such as early, untreated Parkinson's disease and its relationship to motor disability. Brain
brainstem nuclei, limbic structures, and cerebral cortex that may 1991;114(5):2095–122.
reflect degeneration of other systems including noradrenergic, [7] Higginson CI, Wheelock VL, Carroll KE, Sigvardt KA. Recognition memory in
Parkinson's disease with and without dementia: evidence inconsistent with the
serotoninergic, and cholinergic projections which are also known to retrieval deficit hypothesis. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2005;27(4):516–28.
be involved in the cognitive dysfunction in PD [39]. Finally, it should [8] Beatty WW, Ryder KA, Gontkovsky ST, Scott JG, McSwan KL, Bharucha KJ.
be borne in mind that a combination of Lewy- and Alzheimer-type Analyzing the subcortical dementia syndrome of Parkinson's disease using the
RBANS. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2003;18(5):509–20.
pathologies is a robust pathological correlate of dementia in PD [40].
[9] Riekkinen Jr P, Kejonen K, Laakso MP, Soininen H, Partanen K, Riekkinen M.
However, none of the MRI studies included in this manuscript used Hippocampal atrophy is related to impaired memory, but not frontal functions in
cases validated by autopsy. The combination of both post-mortem non-demented Parkinson's disease patients. Neuroreport 1998;9(7):1507–11.
[10] Bruck A, Kurki T, Kaasinen V, Vahlberg T, Rinne JO. Hippocampal and prefrontal
pathologic studies and in vivo MRI studies would be crucial to
atrophy in patients with early non-demented Parkinson's disease is related to
understand the neuroanatomical correlates of cognitive dysfunctions cognitive impairment. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2004;75(10):1467–9.
in PD. [11] Tam CW, Burton EJ, McKeith IG, Burn DJ, O'Brien JT. Temporal lobe atrophy on MRI
The main methodological problems in the MRI studies discussed in in Parkinson disease with dementia: a comparison with Alzheimer disease and
dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurology 2005;64(5):861–5.
the current review are as follows: Firstly, the heterogeneity of samples [12] Camicioli R, Moore MM, Kinney A, Corbridge E, Glassberg K, Kaye JA. Parkinson's
studied; it is not always well defined whether the PD patients disease is associated with hippocampal atrophy. Mov Disord 2003;18(7):784–90.
included in the study are impaired in the specific neuropsychological [13] Junque C, Ramirez-Ruiz B, Tolosa E, Summerfield C, Marti MJ, Pastor P, et al.
Amygdalar and hippocampal MRI volumetric reductions in Parkinson's disease
test and/or if they present VH. For example, from papers reviewed in with dementia. Mov Disord 2005;20(5):540–4.
Table 1 only 5 assessed whether patients presented VH. Patients with [14] Bouchard TP, Malykhin N, Martin WR, Hanstock CC, Emery DJ, Fisher NJ, et al. Age
VH and/or cognitive impairment usually present greater atrophy and dementia-associated atrophy predominates in the hippocampal head and
amygdala in Parkinson's disease. Neurobiol Aging 2008;29(7):1027–39.
compared to patients without these characteristics and therefore not [15] Ibarretxe-Bilbao N, Ramirez-Ruiz B, Tolosa E, Marti MJ, Valldeoriola F, Bargallo N,
taking into consideration these variables may bias the results. et al. Hippocampal head atrophy predominance in Parkinson's disease with
Secondly, the inconsistency in controlling the influence of age, gender hallucinations and with dementia. J Neurol 2008;255(9):1324–31.
[16] Dalaker TO, Zivadinov R, Ramasamy DP, Beyer MK, Alves G, Bronnick KS, et al.
and education in the correlation analyses between brain and function.
Ventricular enlargement and mild cognitive impairment in early Parkinson's
Most of the studies did not control the confounding effect of these disease. Mov Disord 2011;26(2):297–301.
sociodemographic variables in the analyses. Finally, the limitations of [17] Apostolova LG, Beyer M, Green AE, Hwang KS, Morra JH, Chou YY, et al.
Hippocampal, caudate, and ventricular changes in Parkinson's disease with and
structural MRI analyses techniques. On the one hand, manual ROI
without dementia. Mov Disord 2010;25(6):687–8.
method takes into consideration the variability across subjects, but it [18] Camicioli R, Sabino J, Gee M, Bouchard T, Fisher N, Hanstock C, et al. Ventricular
also depends on the subjective criteria of the investigator. For this dilatation and brain atrophy in patients with Parkinson's disease with incipient
reason, inter- and intra-rater validations should be mandatory in this dementia. Mov Disord Mar 25 2011 [Electronic publication ahead of print]. doi:10.
1002/mds.23700.
kind of analysis, but this is not always the case. On the other hand, [19] Lewis SJ, Dove A, Robbins TW, Barker RA, Owen AM. Cognitive impairments in
automatic MRI measures such as VBM provide a non-biased measure. early Parkinson's disease are accompanied by reductions in activity in frontos-
However, the normalization stage within the VBM analysis, which is triatal neural circuitry. J Neurosci 2003;23(15):6351–6.
[20] Camicioli R, Gee M, Bouchard TP, Fisher NJ, Hanstock CC, Emery DJ, et al. Voxel-
required to ensure that the same brain regions can be compared based morphometry reveals extra-nigral atrophy patterns associated with
between subjects, transforms the shape of the brain image and may dopamine refractory cognitive and motor impairment in parkinsonism. Parkin-
distort the abnormal tissue and artificially inflate the atrophied areas sonism Relat Disord 2009;15(3):187–95.
[21] Ibarretxe-Bilbao N, Junque C, Tolosa E, Marti MJ, Valldeoriola F, Bargallo N, et al.
[41]. Surface-based methods and 3D modeling analyses still scarce in Neuroanatomical correlates of impaired decision-making and facial emotion
PD are proving successful. recognition in early Parkinson's disease. Eur J Neurosci 2009;30(6):1162–71.
Further studies are needed to corroborate the results of the studies [22] Gleichgerrcht E, Ibanez A, Roca M, Torralva T, Manes F. Decision-making cognition
in neurodegenerative diseases. Nat Rev Neurol 2010;6(11):611–23.
reviewed here. Major challenges that remain in the field of structural
[23] Nagano-Saito A, Washimi Y, Arahata Y, Kachi T, Lerch JP, Evans AC, et al. Cerebral
MRI studies in PD are the need to identify brain structures which are atrophy and its relation to cognitive impairment in Parkinson disease. Neurology
vulnerable to early degeneration and which could be considered as 2005;64(2):224–9.
[24] Henry JD, Crawford JR. Verbal fluency deficits in Parkinson's disease: a meta-
biomarkers predicting different disease outcomes.
analysis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2004;10(4):608–22.
[25] Williams-Gray CH, Foltynie T, Brayne CE, Robbins TW, Barker RA. Evolution of
cognitive dysfunction in an incident Parkinson's disease cohort. Brain 2007;130(7):
Conflict of interest 1787–98.
[26] Rogalsky C, Rong F, Saberi K, Hickok G. Functional anatomy of language and music
perception: temporal and structural factors investigated using functional
None. magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci 2011;31(10):3843–52.
74 N. Ibarretxe-Bilbao et al. / Journal of the Neurological Sciences 310 (2011) 70–74

[27] Pereira JB, Junque C, Marti MJ, Ramirez-Ruiz B, Bartres-Faz D, Tolosa E. Structural [35] Emre M. What causes mental dysfunction in Parkinson's disease? Mov Disord
brain correlates of verbal fluency in Parkinson's disease. Neuroreport 2009;20(8): 2003;18(Suppl. 6):S63–71.
741–4. [36] Braak H, Del Tredici K, Rub U, de Vos RA, Jansen Steur EN, Braak E. Staging of brain
[28] Pereira JB, Junque C, Marti MJ, Ramirez-Ruiz B, Bargallo N, Tolosa E. Neuroana- pathology related to sporadic Parkinson's disease. Neurobiol Aging 2003;24(2):
tomical substrate of visuospatial and visuoperceptual impairment in Parkinson's 197–211.
disease. Mov Disord 2009;24(8):1193–9. [37] Tolosa E, Gaig C, Santamaria J, Compta Y. Diagnosis of the premotor phase of
[29] Kan Y, Kawamura M, Hasegawa Y, Mochizuki S, Nakamura K. Recognition of Parkinson disease. Neurology 2009;72(Suppl. 2):S12–20.
emotion from facial, prosodic and written verbal stimuli in Parkinson's disease. [38] Braak H, Rub U, Jansen Steur EN, Del Tredici K, de Vos RA. Cognitive status
Cortex 2002;38(4):623–30. correlates with neuropathologic stage in parkinson disease. Neurology 2005;64:
[30] Yip JT, Lee TM, Ho SL, Tsang KL, Li LS. Emotion recognition in patients with 1404–10.
idiopathic Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord 2003;18(10):1115–22. [39] Sonnen JA, Postupna N, Larson EB, Crane PK, Rose SE, Montine KS, et al. Pathologic
[31] Lawrence AD, Goerendt IK, Brooks DJ. Impaired recognition of facial expressions of correlates of dementia in individuals with Lewy body disease. Brain Pathol
anger in Parkinson's disease patients acutely withdrawn from dopamine 2010;20(3):654–9.
replacement therapy. Neuropsychologia 2007;45(1):65–74. [40] Compta Y, Parkkinen L, O'Sullivan SS, Vandrovcova J, Holton JL, Collins C, et al.
[32] Ariatti A, Benuzzi F, Nichelli P. Recognition of emotions from visual and prosodic Lewy- and Alzheimer-type pathologies in Parkinson's disease dementia: which is
cues in Parkinson's disease. Neurol Sci 2008;29(4):219–27. more important? Brain 2011;134(5):1493–505.
[33] Sprengelmeyer R, Young AW, Mahn K, Schroeder U, Woitalla D, Buttner T, et al. [41] Ridgway GR, Omar R, Ourselin S, Hill DL, Warren JD, Fox NC. Issues with threshold
Facial expression recognition in people with medicated and unmedicated masking in voxel-based morphometry of atrophied brains. Neuroimage 2009;44(1):
Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia 2003;41(8):1047–57. 99–111.
[34] Tinaz S, Courtney MG, Stern CE. Focal cortical and subcortical atrophy in early
Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord 2011;26(3):436–41.

You might also like