You are on page 1of 10

REVIEW

CURRENT
OPINION Serious video games and virtual reality for
prevention and neurorehabilitation of cognitive
decline because of aging and neurodegeneration
Arseny A. Sokolov a,b,c, Amélie Collignon a,d,
and Mélanie Bieler-Aeschlimann e,f

Purpose of review
Cognitive decline because of aging and neurodegeneration has become increasingly prevalent. This calls
for the implementation of efficacious, motivating, standardized and widely available cognitive interventions
for the elderly. In this context, serious video games and virtual reality may represent promising approaches.
Here, we review recent research on their potential for cognitive prevention and neurorehabilitation of age-
related cognitive decline and mild cognitive impairment (MCI).
Downloaded from http://journals.lww.com/co-neurology by BhDMf5ePHKbH4TTImqenVE5wmW8EcRnjNvUKc+se9prf6Sz9Uw03+O48ofbbdGav on 03/06/2020

Recent findings
The majority of currently available data in this evolving domain lacks the methodological quality to draw
reliable conclusions on the potential of novel technology for cognitive training in older people. However,
single well designed randomized controlled trials have reported promising effects of cognitive interventions
involving serious video games and virtual reality. The cognitive benefits of exergames promoting physical
exercise with and without combined cognitive training remain unclear.
Summary
The immersion into stimulating and motivating environments along with training content based on
neuroscientific and neuropsychological models may represent a significant advance as compared with
conventional computerized cognitive training. Additional research with sound methodology including
sufficient sample sizes, active control groups and meaningful outcome measures of everyday function is
needed to elucidate the potential of serious video games and virtual reality in multifactorial
neurorehabilitation of cognitive decline in aging and neurodegeneration.
Keywords
aging, cognitive neurorehabilitation, computerized cognitive training, exergames, neurotechnology, serious
video games, virtual reality

INTRODUCTION
As the proportion of older individuals increases in
a
the general population and neurological practice, Neuroscape@NeuroTech Platform & Service de Neurologie, Départe-
we will face cognitive decline more frequently. ment des Neurosciences Cliniques, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire
Vaudois (CHUV), Lausanne, bDepartment of Neurology, University Neu-
Among other challenges, this calls for efficacious
rorehabilitation, University Hospital Inselspital, University of Bern, Bern,
interventions for prevention and neurorehabilita- Switzerland, cNeuroscape Center, Weill Institute of Neuroscience,
tion of cognitive decline in the elderly. According to Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco,
some large-scale studies, conventional neuropsy- USA, dClinical and Experimental Neuropsychology Laboratory, Depart-
chological interventions may not only improve ment of Psychology, University of Geneva, Geneva, eLeenaards Memory
Centre, Département des Neurosciences Cliniques, Centre Hospitalier
the targeted domains but also benefit everyday
Universitaire Vaudois (CHUV) and fMindMaze SA, Lausanne, Switzerland
life function in healthy older adults (HOA) and
Correspondence to Arseny A. Sokolov, MD, Neuroscape@NeuroTech
people with mild cognitive impairment (MCI). For Platform & Service de Neurologie, Département des Neurosciences
instance, the Advanced Cognitive Training for Inde- Cliniques, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois (CHUV), Rue du
pendent and Vital Elderly (ACTIVE) randomized Bugnon 46, CH-1011 Lausanne, Switzerland. Tel: +41 79 55 67 355;
controlled trial (RCT) in 2832 HOA reported e-mail: arseny.sokolov@chuv.ch
domain-specific effects of 10 sessions of inductive Curr Opin Neurol 2020, 33:239–248
reasoning versus processing speed versus verbal DOI:10.1097/WCO.0000000000000791

1350-7540 Copyright ß 2020 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved. www.co-neurology.com

Copyright © 2020 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


Degenerative and cognitive diseases

people with MCI. Interventions encompassing


KEY POINTS novel technology have become widely accepted in
 Serious video games and virtual reality are promising cognitive neurorehabilitation [9]. Here, we review
approaches for cognitive training and recent research in this area, with a particular focus
neurorehabilitation in older people. on the past 2 years and the use of neurotechnology,
such as serious video games and virtual reality.
 This novel technology can increase motivation and
training effects through immersion in
stimulating environments.
COMPUTERIZED COGNITIVE TRAINING
 Closed-loop adaptivity of difficulty in real-time may Computerized cognitive training (CCT) usually rep-
represent a major advance in cognitive training. resents translation and digitalization of standard
 The currently available data do not allow drawing neuropsychological interventions in a computer-
reliable conclusions on the efficacy and assumed ized framework. Digitalization offers several advan-
advantages of serious video games and virtual reality. tages, such as a high level of standardization and
reproducibility, time efficiency and the possibility
 Large-scale RCTs with state-of-the-art methodology are
required to elucidate the potential of this for remote training. Of note, already the ACTIVE
neurotechnology for cognitive training and trial discussed above employed CCT for processing
neurorehabilitation of cognitive decline related to aging speed training [2]. Furthermore, an RCT involving
or neurodegeneration. 30 HOA and 30 younger adults indicated that 8 h of
computerized divided attention training improved
specifically attentional control capacities and dual-
task performance in HOA as opposed to sequential
episodic memory training [1,2]. Although no self- training on the same tasks that required focused
&&
reported instrumental activities of daily living attention [10 ]. Transfer was reported with respect
(IADL) benefits were observed in either group at to dual-tasking in an ecological virtual reality sce-
2 years after training [1], inductive reasoning train- nario, but no effects were found on the self-reported
ing was associated to IADL improvements at 5-year Cognitive Failures Questionnaire. Overall, system-
follow-up [3]. Somewhat surprisingly, such distal atic literature reviews on CCT in HOA and patients
transfer from specific cognitive training to everyday with dementia have suggested significant albeit
life function appeared in all three interventional modest positive effects [11,12]. However, the avail-
groups at the ten-year follow-up [4]. A recent RCT able evidence indicates only limited transfer in HOA
in 145 individuals with MCI comparing 16 h of and is insufficient with respect to benefits of CCT in
training on strategies for memory and attentional MCI [13]. Apart from the processes underlying cog-
control (MEMOþ) to a psychosocial training and a nitive decline and potentially limiting the effects of
no-contact control group reported specific improve- training irrespective of the modality, these modest
ments in memory performance on cognitive testing benefits could be because of a relative lack of moti-
and in everyday mnemonic strategies as assessed by vation and personalization in conventional neuro-
the Multifactorial Memory Questionnaire in the psychological interventions, irrespective of whether
&&
MEMOþ group [5 ]. The effects persisted at fol- digital tools are used.
low-up assessments after 3 and 6 months.
Distal transfer and long-term persistence of ben-
efits constitute the ultimate objectives of cognitive FROM COMMERCIAL TO SERIOUS VIDEO
interventions, but have been rarely observed [6]. GAMES
Proximal and transient transfer, from the trained The fun and joy of video games could enhance
cognitive function to a related cognitive domain, the inherent motivation for training in people
has been reported more frequently. Furthermore, with cognitive decline [14]. An RCT in 36 HOA
the majority of current clinical and experimental found that, when compared with a no-contact
procedures aiming at prevention or deceleration of control group, playing the action video game
cognitive decline because of aging and neurodegen- Super Mario Bros three times a week over 2 months
eration have only yielded limited benefits [7,8]. In yielded more wide-spread cognitive improvement
the era of information and communication technol- (with a particular emphasis on the visuospatial
ogy, a part of the scientific community has started and working memory domains) than the same
developing digital solutions to extend standard training dose of the reasoning-oriented, off-label
&
neuropsychological approaches for cognitive train- Dr Kawashima’s Brain Training [15 ]. Although
ing and overcome the main limitations of currently these data indicate that the rich environment and
available cognitive interventions for HOA and broad challenges of commercial action video games

240 www.co-neurology.com Volume 33  Number 2  April 2020

Copyright © 2020 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


Serious video games and virtual reality Sokolov et al.

without cognitive specificity may afford a more EXERGAMES


global improvement in cognitive function, the only Physical exercise alone has been shown to yield
significant interaction across all three conditions cognitive effects in older people [27,28]. However,
favored Dr Kawashima’s Brain Training with respect the adherence to physical exercise is difficult to
to specific improvement in the Stroop test. In a maintain. Exergames are defined as video games
similar vein, an RCT in 54 HOA showed that 60 h aiming primarily at physical training [29]. Although
of an off-label, gamified visual attention and proc- exergames do not appear to outperform conven-
essing speed CCT (PositScience InSight) outper- tional physical training in terms of cognitive effects
formed the commercial action video game Crazy [30], they may be a motivating vector to promote
&
Taxi and a no-contact control group [16 ] in the adherence to physical exercise. Recent meta-analy-
Useful Field of View (UFOV) assessment of process- ses and systematic reviews indicated that playing
ing speed, selective and divided attention [17]. No exergames can benefit executive function, attention
significant differences between the groups were and visuospatial processing in HOA and MCI
found on IADL. [30,31], although the results of the former have been
These outcomes may reflect the controversy in challenged [32]. In an RCT in 78 individuals with
defining useful and meaningful outcome measures MCI, playing sports video games on the Nintendo
in research on cognitive interventions. Yet, it may Wii for 30 min three times a week over 10 weeks
also suggest that a lack of cognitive specificity and yielded more significant effects in the digit span
neuroscience-informed design limits the utility of (working memory) and also health-related quality
commercial video games for cognitive neurorehabi- of life than the same dose of the CoTras CCT pro-
&
litation. Furthermore, commercial action video gram [33 ]. Conversely, no differential effects were
games have been typically designed for younger found in verbal learning, short-term verbal memory
individuals, and may not be well accepted and or long-term visual memory.
tolerated by older people [18]. In contrast, commer- As the combination of physical and cognitive
cially available, computerized puzzle games are training appears to outperform physical or cognitive
enjoyed by HOA [19]. exercise alone in terms of cognitive benefits [34,35],
Combining the specific elements of CCT and coupling exergames with cognitive training may be a
the motivational aspects of commercial video promising avenue. In this respect, a recent promising
games has led to the development of serious video RCT in 44 individuals with MCI evaluated the cogni-
games [20,21]. Through integration of neuroscien- tive and electrophysiological effects of the off-label
tific models, neuropsychological content with Dr Kawashima’s Body and Brain Exercises on the
immersive graphics and soundtrack, serious video Microsoft Xbox 360 Kinect that allows tracking
games may bear a significant potential for cognitive movement in response to cognitive tasks displayed
&&
neurorehabilitation across several neurological and on a large screen [36 ]. The control group performed
psychiatric conditions [20,22,23]. Furthermore, nondigital motion range exercises without cognitive
serious video games can be designed to involve content and the training dose in each group was 25–
monitoring of multiple measures, such as reaction 30 min, 5 days a week during 6 weeks (12.5–15 h). As
time, response accuracy, precise duration of prac- compared with the control, the cognitive exergame
tice, as well as physiological indicators like heart afforded significant benefits on the Mini-Mental
rate, skin conductance, eye movements or brain State Examination and Montreal Cognitive Assess-
activity. These measures can be used for real-time ment scores, as well as the Trail Making Test B assess-
feedback, detailed recording of training perfor- ing cognitive flexibility. Some electrophysiological
mance and progression, as well as closed-loop adap- effects were also described. However, these encour-
tation [24]. Closed-loop adaptive video games aging results may have been somewhat confounded
(CLAVs) incorporate real-time, performance-driven by probable differences in the expectancy of cogni-
adaptation of game challenges [20]. A landmark tive benefits between the two training groups [37].
study in 46 HOA showed that a custom-designed Between-group differences other than the training
dual-task CLAV improved significantly divided content, such as training location or the presence of a
attention as opposed to single-task and no-contact therapist can further increase the divergence in
control conditions [25]. The data also indicated expectancy, and thus training outcomes.
transfer to sustained attention and working mem- The Aerobic and Cognitive Exercise Study
ory that were not targeted directly. Furthermore, (ACES) enrolled 111 older participants (including
training benefits persisted at 6 months follow-up. 83 individuals with suspected MCI) to evaluate the
Good acceptance of serious video games has been effects of stationary cycling exercise coupled with a
reported among individuals with neurodegenera- serious video game versus cycling in a virtual land-
tive disease [26]. scape (with 45 and 46 participants, respectively).

1350-7540 Copyright ß 2020 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved. www.co-neurology.com 241

Copyright © 2020 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


Degenerative and cognitive diseases

Both the landscape and video game were displayed cognitive training on a desktop screen has also been
on a screen mounted on the stationary bike. A described as virtual reality, because of recent techno-
game-only condition without physical exercise logical and conceptual progress, only immersion in a
was also conducted, with five randomized and virtual environment via a room-sized cube (CAVE
ten attributed participants. However, no partici- system) or head-mounted displays should be referred
pant completed the 6-month training in this con- to as virtual reality.
trol group. In the other two conditions, only seven Of particular significance for older adults, virtual
participants per principal condition completed reality allows personalized and ecological assessment
6 months of training, representing an attrition of and training of IADL. In a study across 25 HOA and
87%. The study comprised pretraining and post- individuals with MCI without a control condition,
training structural MRI, saliva exosome analyses, nonimmersive computerized IADL training was
as well as plasma protein assays for brain-derived reported to improve visual memory, attention and
neurotrophic factor, C-reactive protein, insulin-like cognitive flexibility, but without affecting everyday
growth factor 1, interleukin 6 and vascular endo- life cognition [45]. Recent research in 42 older indi-
thelial growth factor. No differences between viduals with MCI concluded that IADL training and
the more passive and the cognitively demanding physical exercise in and outside virtual reality yield
exergame were found on the primary cognitive largely similar outcomes, with the immersive virtual
outcome measures [38]. However, the exer-tour reality group showing more benefits on the Trail
cycling without elevated cognitive demands Making Test B (cognitive flexibility) and gait cadence
&
afforded significantly greater benefits on the sec- during cognitive–motor dual tasking [46 ].
ondary outcomes verbal memory, physical fitness Exposure to and immersive interaction with vir-
and everyday life cognition. The greater effects for tual environments may also be useful for assessing
passive cycling as opposed to exergaming contra- and training spatial, episodic and prospective mem-
dicted the results of a preceding ACES study [39]. ory [47–49]. Furthermore, patients with MCI and
The same research group also evaluated the off- dementia have been reported to prefer immersive
label, home-based interactive Physical and Cogni- virtual reality to paper-and-pencil interventions
tive exercise system (iPACes) that involved pedaling [50]. This was particularly the case for participants
on an elliptical and completing a list of errands suffering from apathy, further underlining the moti-
along a virtual bike path displayed on a tablet- vational value of novel technology. Additional, care-
laptop. In the absence of a control group, 10 out fully designed RCTs are needed to assess the true
of 31 enrolled older individuals completed a twice- potential of immersive virtual reality for cognitive
weekly training over 3 months and exhibited sig- neurorehabilitation in HOA and people with MCI.
nificant improvement in the Stroop test and Alz-
heimer’s Disease Assessment scale delayed word
recall that measure inhibition control and verbal TOWARDS MULTIMODAL
memory, respectively [40]. Eight participants with NEUROTECHNOLOGICAL AND
MCI or mild dementia completed another pilot MULTIFACTORIAL INTERVENTIONS
study comparing stationary cycling coupled with The potential of more holistic, multifactorial inter-
cognitive challenges (such as avoiding cars or shop- ventions addressing several functional domains to
ping in a supermarket) to a no-contact control improve physical and cognitive health has become
group. The trial did not find significant differences increasingly recognized [51]. The 2-year longitudinal
in cognitive outcomes [41]. The question of Finnish Geriatric Intervention Study to Prevent Cog-
whether the inclusion of cognitive training in an nitive Impairment and Disability (FINGER) study
exergame context may be useful for maintaining or indicated that a multifactorial intervention involv-
rehabilitating cognitive function, thus remains ing diet, exercise, cognitive training and vascular risk
largely open. monitoring could improve or maintain cognitive
function in 1260 older people at-risk of cognitive
decline [52]. The currently ongoing ENGAGE RCT
VIRTUAL REALITY FOR TRAINING AND represents an interesting attempt at integrating cog-
ASSESSMENT nitive training with novel technology and leisure
Virtual reality may represent another important neu- activities [53]. One hundred and forty-four older
rotechnology for optimization of cognitive training. adults with subjective memory deficits will be
This technology allows immersion and interaction in assigned to either the ENGAGE-MUSIC/SPANISH
virtual environments. The use and manipulation of interventional or the ENGAGE-DISCOVERY active
such environments may open novel perspectives for control condition for a total of 48 h over 4 months.
cognitive neurorehabilitation [42–44]. Although ENGAGE-MUSIC/SPANISH consists of learning

242 www.co-neurology.com Volume 33  Number 2  April 2020

Copyright © 2020 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


Table 1. A summary of the design and outcomes of recent studies on neurotechnology for cognitive training in healthy older adults and people with mild cognitive
impairment, including cognitive strategy training, computerized cognitive training, video games and virtual reality
Population Design and time
Cognitive N randomized points for cognitive
First author Year domain (N analyzed) Age evaluation number Conditions Training dose Outcome measures Main results

Cognitive strategy training


Belleville 2018 Memory 145 aMCI (127) 55þ RCT Cognitive strategy 16h (2h  1d  8w) Proximal transfer Cognitive strategy training
- T0 training (N ¼ 49) - Memory composite scores significantly improved delayed
- T1 Psychosocial intervention - Psychosocial health measures memory and strategy use in
- T2 (FU at 3mo) (active control; Distal transfer daily life
- T3 (FU at 6mo) N ¼ 49) - Everyday life questionnaires
No contact (N ¼ 47) (Metamemory Questionnaire,
Complex ADL, Self-reported
memory)
Computerized cognitive training
Bier 2018 Attention 30 HOA & 30 YA 60-80 RCT Variable priority training 8h (4d  1h  2w) Direct training effect Attention modulation improved
(27 HOA) - T0 (flexible divided - In the trained tasks (visual only in those HOA undergoing
- T1 attention; N ¼ 15) detection task; alphanumeric variable priority training. The
Single-task training equation task); variable priority training group
(focused attention; Transfer in a VR dual task improved dual-task cost on
N ¼ 15) - In similar tasks involving the both transfer tasks, whereas
same cognitive skills (virtual single-task training only
car ride; alpha span task) improved Alpha span task
performance
Buitenweg 2019 Cognitive flexibility 158 HOA (142) 60-80 RCT Frequent task switching 29h (3d  0.5h  1w þ Distal transfer: No significant effects of training
(working memory, - T0 (N ¼ 56) 5d  0.5h  11w) - SF-36 or group differences between
reasoning, - T0.5 (6w training) Infrequent task switching - CFQ T0 and T1 (posttraining)
attention) - T1 (N ¼ 33) - DEX Significant improvement on CFQ
- T2 (FU at 1mo) Mock training (active - IADL and DEX at T2 (follow-up) as

1350-7540 Copyright ß 2020 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.
control; N ¼ 50) - HAD compared with all other
- CIS-F timepoints in all groups
- Subjective memory problems
questionnaire
Flak 2019 Working memory 85 MCI (68) 43-88 RCT Adaptative working 12.5–16.75h Proximal transfer No significant group differences
- T0 memory training (5d  0.5– Primary outcome:
- T1 (N ¼ 42) 0.67h  5w) - Working memory (Digit and
- T2 (FU at 4mo) Nonadaptative working spatial span backward, letter-
memory training number sequencing)
(N ¼ 43) Secondary outcome:
- NPS assessments of attention,
processing speed, verbal and
visuospatial memory, EF
Video games
Perrot 2019 EF, processing 36 HOA (35) 60-71 RCT Kawashima Brain 24h (3d  1h  8w); Proximal transfer Inhibition: KBT > SMB and KBT
speed and - T0 Training (KBT; - Cognitive flexibility (TMT-B) >> Ctrl;
visuospatial - T1 N ¼ 12) - Inhibition (Stroop) Reasoning: KBT & SMB > Ctrl;
abilities Super Mario Bros (SMB; 24h (3d  1h  8w); - Reasoning (Matrix, WAIS) Speed and visuomotor: SMB >

Copyright © 2020 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


N ¼ 12) - Speed and visuomotor Ctrl;
No contact (N ¼ 12) 0h coordination (DSST) Spatial memory: SMB  Ctrl;
- Spatial memory (Corsi) Speed and spatial relations: SMB
- Speed and spatial relations > Ctrl

www.co-neurology.com
(number comparison, DAT5)

243
Serious video games and virtual reality Sokolov et al.
244
Table 1 (Continued)
Population Design and time
Cognitive N randomized points for cognitive
First author Year domain (N analyzed) Age evaluation number Conditions Training dose Outcome measures Main results

Belchior 2019 Mixed 71 HOA (54) 65-86 RCT Crazy Taxi (N ¼ 26) 60h (5d  1h  12w); Improvement in game skills: Direct
- T0 PositScience InSight 60h (5d  1h  12w); - Crazy Taxi - Improved performance on
- T1 (CCT; N ¼ 20) - InSight trained task in both training
- T2 (FU at 3mo) No contact control 0h Proximal transfer groups
(N ¼ 25) Primary outcome: Proximal transfer
- attention: UFOV - UFOV: InSight >> Crazy Taxi
Secondary outcomes: at T1 and T2
- Visual attention (Multiple object - Multiple object tracking: similar
tracking, attentional blinks) benefits in both training
- Visuospatial (block design, groups at T1

www.co-neurology.com
judgment of line orientation, - Attentional blinks: Crazy Taxi
mental rotation, object >> InSight (T1), Crazy
Degenerative and cognitive diseases

rotation) Taxi ¼ InSight (T2)


Distal transfer Distal transfer
- Road sign test - Both training groups became
- IADL faster in IADL at T2
- Pos. and neg. affect scale - Geriatric Depression Scale:
- Geriatric Depression Scale Crazy Taxi > InSight at T2
- Verbal memory (HVLT)
Exergames
Park and Park 2018 Mixed 78 aMCI (78) 60þ RCT Nintendo Wii sports 15h (3d  0.5h  10w) Proximal transfer: Nintendo Wii training only
- T0 games (N ¼ 39) - working memory (WAIS digit yielded significantly greater
- T1 CoTras – Specific CCT span) benefits in working memory
(attention, memory, - visuo-spatial abilities (WAIS- and several aspects of health-
and visual spatial Revised Block Design Test) and related quality of life
abilities; N ¼ 39) memory (ROCFT)
- verbal learning (RAVLT)
- cognitive flexibility (TMT-B)
- inhibition (Stroop)
Distal transfer:
- SF-36
Amjad 2019 Global cognition, 44 MCI (38) N/A RCT Xbox 360 Kinect 15h (5d  0.5h  6w) Global cognition: Significant improvements on
speed processing, - T0 cognitive exergames - MMSE MMSE, MoCA, TMT-A and
EF - T1 (N ¼ 22) - MoCA TMT-B in the Exergame group
Range of motion Processing speed and EF: only. but no time  group
exercises (stretching, - TMT (A and B) interaction analysis
gentle movements;
N ¼ 22)
Anderson- 2018a EF 83 MCI and 28 58-98 RCTa Exer-score (cycling and 0–3mo: Proximal transfer: The interactions time  condition
Hanley HOA (42) - T0 playing; high From 7.9h - Inhibition (Stroop) did not reach significance.
- T1 cognitive load; (2d  0.33h  12w) to - Cognitive flexibility (Color Trails) Overall, the high cognitive
- T2 (FU at 3mo) N ¼ 46) 45h - Working memory (Digit span) load training yielded less
Exer-tour (cycling in a (5d  0.75h  12w); Distal transfer: benefits after 3 months of

Copyright © 2020 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


virtual landscape; low From 3mo to 6mo: - MoCA training than the low cognitive
cognitive load; 45h - Subjective cognitive function load training, and similar
N ¼ 45) (5d  0.75h  12w) (Ecological Validity benefits after 6 months training
Cognitive game without Questionnaire) completion. Of note, only 14
exercise (N ¼ 20a) - Verbal memory (ADAS Word participants adhered to the full
Pedal-only (N ¼; 33a) List) 6 months of training.
- Physical ability (get-up-and-go
test)

Volume 33  Number 2  April 2020


Table 1 (Continued)
Population Design and time
Cognitive N randomized points for cognitive
First author Year domain (N analyzed) Age evaluation number Conditions Training dose Outcome measures Main results

Anderson- 2018b EF 24 MCI (15) 50þ Pilot Home-based exergame 12–30h (2– Proximal transfer: Significantly higher benefits in
Hanley - T0 at least 2/w 5d  0.5h  12w); - Inhibition (Stroop) inhibition and verbal memory
- T0.5: 6 w (N ¼ 10) - Cognitive flexibility (Color Trails) for high versus low-dose
- T1 Home-based exergame 0–6h (1d  0.5h  12w) - Working memory (Digit span) training
 1/w (N ¼ 5) Distal transfer:
- Verbal memory (ADAS Word
List)
Jirayucharoensak 2019 Mixed (mainly 65 aMCI and 54 60-80 RCTa Neurofeedback training 10h (in total 20 sessions Proximal transfer The neurofeedback group
attention) HOA (119a) - T0 (N ¼ 25) of 0.5h  2– - NPS battery: CANTAB showed specific benefits in
- T1 Exergame training 3d  12w) - spatial working memory (SWM) working memory and
(N ¼ 58) - short & long term memory (DMS sustained visual attention
Care as usual (no and PRM);
contact; N ¼ 36a) - speed processing (RVP)
- verbal working memory and
attention (SSP)
Mrakic-Sposa 2018 Mixed 6 MCI and 4 65þ RCT Cycling exergame with 13.25h Proximal transfer: No significant differences
dementia (8) - T0 ADL tasks (N ¼ 5) (3d  0.67h  1w þ - MMSE between the groups
- T1 No-contact control group 3d  0.75h  5w) - Episodic verbal memory
(N ¼ 5) (RAVLT_I and RAVLT_D)
- Visuo-spatial functions (ROCFT,
- Attentional Matrices Test, TMTA)
- EF (FAB, TMT-B)
- Verbal fluency test
- FAQ
Virtual reality
Gamito 2019 Mixed 25 MCI and HOA 65-85 Single-arm ADL and navigation in a 6h (2d  0.5h  6w) Proximal transfer Significant improvement in

1350-7540 Copyright ß 2020 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.
(22) - T0 nonimmersive Global cognition: perseveration (perseverative
- T1 environment (N ¼ 25) - MMSE errors in the WCST, visual
- MoCA memory (Rey complex figure
- NPS battery (EF, memory trial total score) and
visuoconstructive abilities, visual attention (d2 errors)
visual memory, attention/
concentration)
Distal transfer
- Mood
- Functional behavior
Liao 2019 Mixed 42 MCI (34) 65þ RCT Immersive physical 36h (3d  1h  12w) - TMT-A and TMT-B Comparable outcomes, apart
- T0 exercise and ADL and - Stroop from significantly greater
- T1 sports game scenarios improvement in the VR group
(N ¼ 21) on the TMT-B
Physical exercise
combined with motor-
cognitive dual-tasks
and ADL training
outside of VR (N ¼ 21)

Copyright © 2020 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


ADAS, Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale; (I)ADL, (Instrumental) Activities of Daily Living; CCT, computerized cognitive training; CFQ, Cognitive Failure Questionnaire; CSI-F, Checklist Individual Strength-Fatigue; CT,
computerized training; Ctrl, control; d2, d2 Test of Attention; DAT5, Differential Aptitude Test; DEX, Dysexecutive Functioning Questionnaire; DMS, Delayed Matching to Sample; DSST, Digit-Symbol Substitution Test; EF,

www.co-neurology.com
Executive Functions; FAB, Frontal Assessment Battery; FAQ, Functional Activity Questionnaire; HAD, Hospital Anxiety Depression Scale; HOA, Healthy older adults; HVLT, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test; MCI, mild
cognitive impairment (aMCI ¼ amnestic MCI); MMSE, Mini-Mental State Evaluation; mo, months; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; NPS, Neuropsychological tests; PRM, Pattern Recognition Memory; RAVLT-I/D,
Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test – Immediate/Delayed recall; ROCFT, Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test; ROM, Range of Motion; RVP, Rapid Visual Information Processing; SF-36, Short Form Health Survey 36
items; SSP, Spatial Span Length; TMT-A/B, Trail Making Test version A/B; UFOV, Useful Field of View; VR, Virtual Reality; w, Weeks; WAIS, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.
a

245
Serious video games and virtual reality Sokolov et al.

(Partially) nonrandomized assignment; time points: T0 ¼ baseline; T0.5 ¼ mid-training; T1 ¼ posttraining (see dose for training duration); T2 and T3 ¼ follow-up (FU).
Degenerative and cognitive diseases

attentional control and memory strategies, applying serious video games with and without coupled phys-
this knowledge to a leisure activity (learning music or ical exercise and virtual reality for improving cogni-
Spanish as a second language), and playing commer- tion in older people (Table 1). However, so far, the
cially available video games targeting attention. majority of research published in this realm lacks
ENGAGE-DISCOVERY involves psychoeducation the methodological quality to afford reliable con-
(on cognition and the brain), cultural and social clusions on the efficacy of this technology.
interactions (documentary viewing with discussions) Future research using state-of-the-art methodol-
and playing commercial video games with a low ogy will help evaluate whether serious video games
cognitive load. The primary outcome is an episodic and virtual reality can be efficacious in preventing
memory composite score. Secondary outcome mea- cognitive decline related to normal and pathological
sures focus on attentional control. Furthermore, the aging. Such RCTs should employ approaches that
study will assess effects on psychological health and are easily accessible and accepted by older people
daily life as well as structural and functional brain [22,26], as adherence may represent a significant
plasticity. In addition, evaluation of potential cova- issue [38,40]. The intervention design should take
riates such as cognitive reserve (education and life- into account the specific needs of the elderly popu-
style), sex and genotype (apolipoprotein E4, brain- lation [18]. This would not only ensure sufficient
derived neurotrophic factor and catechol-O-methyl- sample sizes for data interpretation but also indicate
transferase) will be used towards differentiated data feasibility of the interventions in real life.
interpretation. Multifactorial interventions targeting Irrespective of the vector, the training content is
not only specific cognitive functions and emotional a key factor. Mere computerization of cognitive
processing but also meta-cognitive abilities, as well as training appears insufficient for harnessing the con-
physical and social activities appear more promising siderable opportunities afforded by information and
than single-domain training. communication technology [6]. In addition to the
Other neurotechnological approaches should highly engaging contexts, serious video games and
also be considered as alternatives or additional ele- particularly immersive virtual reality may transport
ments in multifactorial interventions. For instance, the training participants outside of the clinical or
positive cognitive effects of a neurofeedback brain– interventional environment, and thus, further pro-
computer interface approach were shown in a recent mote motivation, adherence and performance. Vir-
&
study [54 ]. One hundred and nineteen HOA and tual reality may become a cost-efficient and mobile
people with MCI were assigned to three conditions: tool for highly ecological yet standardized cognitive
10 h of neurofeedback þ standard care, 10 h of exer-
&
training [22,44,46 ,55] and evaluation [55–57].
game þ standard care and standard care alone. In the Most important, virtual reality allows environmen-
neurofeedback condition, participants were trained tal manipulations that would be impossible other-
to control a set of desktop video games by optimizing wise or require substantial efforts and resources [44].
their sustained attention-related power ratio of beta Adaptivity is considered a key advantage of seri-
to alpha frequency bands recorded by electroenceph- ous video games, although CCT can also be
alography. The exergame condition consisted of five endowed with adaptivity. Closed-loop real-time
serious video games combining physical and cogni- adaptivity is believed to maintain the participant
tive challenges. The games were displayed on a giant in an optimal range of effort, and thus, promote
screen and controlled by optical whole-body move- training effects [20]. The nature and time scale of
ment tracking. Both HOA and MCI participants in the adaptivity may play an important role. Level-wise
neurofeedback condition exhibited significant adaptivity to constantly maximal effort did not
improvements in sustained attention and spatial enhance cognitive training effects when compared
working memory strategy as compared with the exer- with similar nonadaptive CCT or video games
game and standard care conditions. Conversely, indi- [58,59]. Instant adaptation of training with both
viduals in the cognitive exergame condition showed increases and reductions in difficulty based on per-
greater improvements in visuospatial working mem- formance [25] or correlates of brain activity [24]
ory (spatial span length). may, therefore, be more useful.
Furthermore, the choice of an appropriate active
control condition appears indispensable. The
CONCLUSION AND OUTLOOK absence of control groups or a no-contact control
Taken together, the domain of serious video games group do not allow for sufficiently meaningful con-
and virtual reality for cognitive training and neuro- clusions. The active control should match the
rehabilitation has just started to evolve. Single well expectancy of the intervention without targeting
designed and sufficiently powered RCTs over the the same cognitive mechanisms [37,60]. Defining
past 2 years shed light on the potential of CCT, useful primary and secondary outcome measures

246 www.co-neurology.com Volume 33  Number 2  April 2020

Copyright © 2020 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


Serious video games and virtual reality Sokolov et al.

that assess not only specific training effects but also Financial support and sponsorship
transfer to related cognitive domains (proximal The preparation of this article was supported by fellow-
transfer) and everyday life function (distal transfer) ships from the Leenaards Foundation, Swiss National
represents another significant challenge [61]. Ulti- Science Foundation, Synapsis Foundation – Alzheimer
mately, an important follow-up goal related to real- Research Switzerland ARS, SICPA Foundation and
life and clinical use of interventions yielding prom- grants from the Helmut Horten Foundation and the
ising initial effects will be to determine the optimal Fondation Anna and André Livio Glauser to AAS. The
training intensity and dose. contribution of MBA was supported by salary from Mind-
Most important, among others, the studies Maze SA and a grant received from the Active and
reviewed here highlighted the compensatory poten- Assisted Living Programme, a European instance devoted
tial of both HOA and individuals with MCI. One may to promoting technological solutions to elderly people.
argue that the underlying processes diminish the
utility and, in particular, long-term benefits of cog- Conflicts of interest
nitive interventions in the elderly. However, similar M.B.A. has been an employee of MindMaze SA, Lau-
to younger patients with neurological conditions sanne, Switzerland.
[62,63], lower cognitive ability appears to predispose
to greater cognitive benefits after training in older
people [45,64]. This may also illustrate the need for REFERENCES AND RECOMMENDED
personalized interventions tailored to the individu- READING
al’s specific needs and deficits. This personalization Papers of particular interest, published within the annual period of review, have
been highlighted as:
or stratification may also involve information & of special interest
&& of outstanding interest
beyond the cognitive profile, such as brain volumetry
[65] or multimodal analyses of brain connectivity 1. Ball K, Berch DB, Helmers KF, et al., Advanced Cognitive Training for
and its relationship to behavior [66]. If pharmacolog- Independent and Vital Elderly Study Group. Effects of cognitive training
interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA 2002;
ical options halting or decelerating neurodegenera- 288:2271–2281.
tion become available, cognitive interventions in 2. Jobe JB, Smith DM, Ball K, et al. ACTIVE: a cognitive intervention trial to promote
independence in older adults. Control Clin Trials 2001; 22:453–479.
MCI may eventually evolve into neurorehabilitation 3. Willis SL, Tennstedt SL, Marsiske M, et al., ACTIVE Study Group. Long-term
aiming at recovery. effects of cognitive training on everyday functional outcomes in older adults.
JAMA 2006; 296:2805–2814.
In a nutshell, serious video games and virtual 4. Rebok GW, Ball K, Guey LT, et al., ACTIVE Study Group. Ten-year effects of
reality may complement existing neuropsychologi- the advanced cognitive training for independent and vital elderly cognitive
training trial on cognition and everyday functioning in older adults. J Am
cal interventions by affording a more engaging, stan- Geriatr Soc 2014; 62:16–24.
dardized yet personalized context for cognitive 5. Belleville S, Hudon C, Bier N, et al. MEMOþ: efficacy, durability and effect of
&& cognitive training and psychosocial intervention in individuals with mild
training and neurorehabilitation. However, addi- cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc 2018; 66:655–663.
tional research with well designed content and con- This study illustrated the positive effects of mnemonic strategy learning on memory
performance and everyday mnemonic strategies in individuals with MCI, as well as
trol conditions taking into account the specific needs durability of the benefits.
and expectations of the elderly and using state-of- 6. Simons DJ, Boot WR, Charness N, et al. Do ‘brain-training’ programs work?
Psychol Sci Public Interest 2016; 17:103–186.
the-art methodology is required to elucidate the true 7. Bahar-Fuchs A, Martyr A, Goh AM, et al. Cognitive training for people with mild
potential of this neurotechnology. Given previous to moderate dementia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2019; 3:CD013069.
8. Martin M, Clare L, Altgassen AM, et al. Cognition-based interventions for
evidence, it appears unlikely that serious video games healthy older people and people with mild cognitive impairment. Cochrane
or virtual reality approaches will become stand- Database Syst Rev 2011; CD006220.
9. Cicerone KD, Goldin Y, Ganci K, et al. Evidence-based cognitive rehabilita-
alone, highly efficacious approaches for age-related tion: systematic review of the literature from 2009 through 2014. Arch Phys
cognitive decline or MCI. They will rather comple- Med Rehabil 2019; 100:1515–1533.
10. Bier B, Ouellet E, Belleville S. Computerized attentional training and transfer
ment multifactorial interventions involving physical && with virtual reality: effect of age and training type. Neuropsychology 2018;
exercise, diet, lifestyle adaptation, cognitive training 32:597–614.
This study introduced the first ecological training outcome measure in immersive
and learning meta-cognitive strategies for coping virtual reality, and confirmed that variable dual-task training improves attentional
and compensation. Neurotechnological approaches control and ecological dual-task abilities.
11. Kueider AM, Parisi JM, Gross AL, Rebok GW. Computerized cognitive
could represent a decisive component in multifacto- training with older adults: a systematic review. PLoS One 2012; 7:e40588.
rial interventions for prevention and neurorehabili- 12. Garcia-Casal JA, Loizeau A, Csipke E, et al. Computer-based cognitive
interventions for people living with dementia: a systematic literature review
tation of cognitive decline related to normal aging and meta-analysis. Aging Ment Health 2017; 21:454–467.
and neurodegeneration. 13. Butler M, McCreedy E, Nelson VA, et al. Does cognitive training prevent
cognitive decline?: a systematic review. Ann Intern Med 2018; 168:63–68.
14. Bavelier D, Green CS, Pouget A, Schrater P. Brain plasticity through the life span:
Acknowledgements learning to learn and action video games. Annu Rev Neurosci 2012; 35:391–416.
15. Perrot A, Maillot P, Hartley A. Cognitive training game versus action videogame:
The authors wish to thank Petr Grivaz and Alexis & effects on cognitive functions in older adults. Games Health J 2019; 8:35–40.
Renaud for technical assistance in preparation of this This work suggested more global cognitive benefits of a commercial action video
game as opposed to a reasoning-oriented cognitive training video game, but only
manuscript. found significant interaction effects on cognitive flexibility for the latter.

1350-7540 Copyright ß 2020 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved. www.co-neurology.com 247

Copyright © 2020 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


Degenerative and cognitive diseases

16. Belchior P, Yam A, Thomas KR, et al. Computer and videogame interventions 42. Cipresso P, Giglioli IAC, Raya MA, Riva G. The past, present, and future of
& for older adults’ cognitive and everyday functioning. Games Health J 2019; virtual and augmented reality research: a network and cluster analysis of the
8:129–143. literature. Front Psychol 2018; 9:2086.
These data indicated that gamified computerized cognitive training may yield more 43. Montana JI, Tuena C, Serino S, et al. Neurorehabilitation of spatial memory
specific effects than commercial action video games. using virtual environments: a systematic review. J Clin Med 2019; 8:; pii:
17. Ball K, Owsley C. The useful field of view test: a new technique for evaluating E1516.
age-related declines in visual function. J Am Optom Assoc 1993; 64:71–79. 44. Perez-Marcos D, Bieler-Aeschlimann M, Serino A. Virtual reality as a vehicle to
18. Bediou B, Adams DM, Mayer RE, et al. Meta-analysis of action video game impact empower motor-cognitive neurorehabilitation. Front Psychol 2018; 9:2120.
on perceptual, attentional, and cognitive skills. Psychol Bull 2018; 144:77–110. 45. Gamito P, Oliveira J, Morais D, et al. Cognitive stimulation of elderly individuals
19. Chesham A, Wyss P, Muri RM, et al. What older people like to play: genre with instrumental virtual reality-based activities of daily life: pre-post treatment
preferences and acceptance of casual games. JMIR Serious Games 2017; study. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 2019; 22:69–75.
5:e8. 46. Liao YY, Chen IH, Lin YJ, et al. Effects of virtual reality-based physical and
20. Mishra J, Anguera JA, Gazzaley A. Video games for neuro-cognitive optimiza- & cognitive training on executive function and dual-task gait performance in
tion. Neuron 2016; 90:214–218. older adults with mild cognitive impairment: a randomized control trial. Front
21. Robert PH, König A, Amieva H, et al. Recommendations for the use of serious Aging Neurosci 2019; 11:162.
games in people with Alzheimer’s disease, related disorders and frailty. Front One of the first immersive virtual reality cognitive intervention studies showing that
Aging Neurosci 2014; 6:54. real-life and immersive virtual reality everyday activity training yield largely similar
22. Manera V, Ben-Sadoun G, Aalbers T, et al. Recommendations for the use of benefits in individuals with mild cognition.
serious games in neurodegenerative disorders: 2016 Delphi Panel. Front 47. La Corte V, Sperduti M, Abichou K, Piolino P. Episodic memory assessment
Psychol 2017; 8:1243. and remediation in normal and pathological aging using virtual reality: a mini
23. Sokolov AA, Grivaz P, Bove R. Cognitive deficits in multiple sclerosis: recent review. Front Psychol 2019; 10:173.
advances in treatment and neurorehabilitation. Curr Treat Options Neurol 48. Lecouvey G, Morand A, Gonneaud J, et al. An impairment of prospective
2018; 20:53. memory in mild Alzheimer’s disease: a ride in a virtual town. Front Psychol
24. deBettencourt MT, Cohen JD, Lee RF, et al. Closed-loop training of attention 2019; 10:241.
with real-time brain imaging. Nat Neurosci 2015; 18:470–475. 49. Tuena C, Serino S, Dutriaux L, et al. Virtual enactment effect on memory in
25. Anguera JA, Boccanfuso J, Rintoul JL, et al. Video game training enhances young and aged populations: a systematic review. J Clin Med 2019; 8:; pii:
cognitive control in older adults. Nature 2013; 501:97–101. E620.
26. Ben-Sadoun G, Sacco G, Manera V, et al. Physical and cognitive stimulation 50. Manera V, Chapoulie E, Bourgeois J, et al. A feasibility study with image-based
using an exergame in subjects with normal aging, mild and moderate cognitive rendered virtual reality in patients with mild cognitive impairment and demen-
impairment. J Alzheimers Dis 2016; 53:1299–1314. tia. PLoS One 2016; 11:e0151487.
27. Kelly ME, Loughrey D, Lawlor BA, et al. The impact of exercise on the cognitive 51. Livingston G, Sommerlad A, Orgeta V, et al. Dementia prevention, interven-
functioning of healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. tion, and care. Lancet 2017; 390:2673–2734.
Ageing Res Rev 2014; 16:12–31. 52. Ngandu T, Lehtisalo J, Solomon A, et al. A 2 year multidomain intervention of
28. Saez de Asteasu ML, Martinez-Velilla N, Zambom-Ferraresi F, et al. Role of diet, exercise, cognitive training, and vascular risk monitoring versus control to
physical exercise on cognitive function in healthy older adults: a systematic prevent cognitive decline in at-risk elderly people (FINGER): a randomised
review of randomized clinical trials. Ageing Res Rev 2017; 37:117–134. controlled trial. Lancet 2015; 385:2255–2263.
29. Kooiman BJ, Sheehan DP. Interacting with the past, present, and future of 53. Belleville S, Moussard A, Ansaldo AI, et al. Rationale and protocol of the
exergames: at the beginning of a new life cycle of video games? Society ENGAGE study: a double-blind randomized controlled preference trial using a
Leisure 2015; 38:55–73. comprehensive cohort design to measure the effect of a cognitive and leisure-
30. Stojan R, Voelcker-Rehage C. A systematic review on the cognitive benefits based intervention in older adults with a memory complaint. Trials 2019;
and neurophysiological correlates of exergaming in healthy older adults. J Clin 20:282.
Med 2019; 8:pii: E734. 54. Jirayucharoensak S, Israsena P, Pan-Ngum S, et al. A game-based neurofeed-
31. Stanmore E, Stubbs B, Vancampfort D, et al. The effect of active video games & back training system to enhance cognitive performance in healthy elderly
on cognitive functioning in clinical and nonclinical populations: A meta- subjects and in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Clin Interv
analysis of randomized controlled trials. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2017; Aging 2019; 14:347–360.
78:34–43. An interesting study comparing neurofeedback to cognitive exergame training.
32. Sala G, Tatlidil KS, Gobet F. Still no evidence that exergames improve 55. Liu Y, Tan W, Chao C, et al. A review of the application of virtual reality
cognitive ability: a commentary on Stanmore et al. Neurosci Biobehav Rev technology in the diagnosis and treatment of cognitive impairment. Front
2019; doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.11.015. [Epub ahead of print] Aging Neurosci 2019; 11:280.
33. Park JH, Park JH. Does cognition-specific computer training have better 56. Ouellet E, Boller B, Corriveau-Lecavalier N, et al. The Virtual Shop: a new
& clinical outcomes than nonspecific computer training? A single-blind, rando- immersive virtual reality environment and scenario for the assessment of
mized controlled trial. Clin Rehabil 2018; 32:213–222. everyday memory. J Neurosci Methods 2018; 303:126–135.
The authors report more health-related quality of life benefits for nonspecific video 57. Parsons TD, Barnett M. Validity of a newly developed measure of memory:
game interventions as opposed to specific computerized cognitive training. feasibility study of the virtual environment grocery store. J Alzheimers Dis
34. Lauenroth A, Ioannidis AE, Teichmann B. Influence of combined physical and 2017; 59:1227–1235.
cognitive training on cognition: a systematic review. BMC Geriatr 2016; 58. Buitenweg JIV, Van De Ven RM, Ridderinkhof KR, Murre JMJ. Does cognitive
16:141. flexibility training enhance subjective mental functioning in healthy older
35. Sacco G, Caillaud C, Ben Sadoun G, et al. Exercise plus cognitive perfor- adults? Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2019;
mance over and above exercise alone in subjects with mild cognitive impair- 26:688–710.
ment. J Alzheimers Dis 2016; 50:19–25. 59. Flak MM, Hol HR, Hernes SS, et al. Adaptive computerized working memory
36. Amjad I, Toor H, Niazi IK, et al. Xbox 360 Kinect Cognitive Games Improve training in patients with mild cognitive impairment. A randomized double-blind
&& Slowness, complexity of EEG, and cognitive functions in subjects with mild active controlled trial. Front Psychol 2019; 10:807.
cognitive impairment: a randomized control trial. Games Health J 2019; 60. Rolle CE, Anguera JA, Skinner SN, et al. Enhancing spatial attention and
8:144–152. working memory in younger and older adults. J Cogn Neurosci 2017;
Promising evidence on improvement of global cognition measures and cognitive 29:1483–1497.
flexibility in MCI through exergaming coupled with cognitive training. 61. Ballesteros S, Voelcker-Rehage C, Bherer L. Editorial: cognitive and brain
37. Foroughi CK, Monfort SS, Paczynski M, et al. Placebo effects in cognitive plasticity induced by physical exercise, cognitive training, video games, and
training. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2016; 113:7470–7474. combined interventions. Front Hum Neurosci 2018; 12:169.
38. Anderson-Hanley C, Barcelos NM, Zimmerman EA, et al. The Aerobic and 62. Mattioli F, Stampatori C, Bellomi F, et al. A RCT comparing specific intensive
Cognitive Exercise Study (ACES) for community-dwelling older adults with or cognitive training to aspecific psychological intervention in RRMS: the SMICT
at-risk for mild cognitive impairment (MCI): neuropsychological, neurobiolo- Study. Front Neurol 2014; 5:278.
gical and neuroimaging outcomes of a randomized clinical trial. Front Aging 63. Sumowski JF, Chiaravalloti N, Deluca J. Retrieval practice improves memory in
Neurosci 2018; 10:76. multiple sclerosis: clinical application of the testing effect. Neuropsychology
39. Barcelos N, Shah N, Cohen K, et al. Aerobic and Cognitive Exercise (ACE) 2010; 24:267–272.
pilot study for older adults: executive function improves with cognitive 64. Whitlock LA, McLaughlin AC, Allaire JC. Individual differences in response to
challenge while exergaming. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2015; 21:768–779. cognitive training: using a multimodal, attentionally demanding game-based
40. Anderson-Hanley C, Stark J, Wall KM, et al. The interactive Physical and intervention for older adults. Comput Hum Behav 2012; 28:1091–1096.
Cognitive Exercise System (iPACES): effects of a 3-month in-home pilot 65. Peter J, Schumacher LV, Landerer V, et al. Biological factors contributing to
clinical trial for mild cognitive impairment and caregivers. Clin Interv Aging the response to cognitive training in mild cognitive impairment. J Alzheimers
2018; 13:1565–1577. Dis 2018; 61:333–345.
41. Mrakic-Sposta S, Di Santo SG, Franchini F, et al. Effects of combined physical 66. Sokolov AA, Zeidman P, Erb M, et al. Structural and effective brain con-
and cognitive virtual reality-based training on cognitive impairment and oxidative nectivity underlying biological motion detection. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
stress in MCI patients: a pilot study. Front Aging Neurosci 2018; 10:282. 2018; 115:E12034–E12042.

248 www.co-neurology.com Volume 33  Number 2  April 2020

Copyright © 2020 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.

You might also like