You are on page 1of 10

MEMORY, 2006, 14 (1), 27±36

Remembering a criminal conversation: Beyond


eyewitness testimony

Laura Campos
University of Granada, Spain

MarõÂa L. Alonso-Quecuty
University of La Laguna, Spain

Unlike the important body of work on eyewitness memory, little research has been done on the accuracy
and completeness of ``earwitness'' memory for conversations. The present research examined the effects
of mode of presentation (audiovisual/ auditory-only) on witnesses' free recall for utterances in a criminal
conversation at different retention intervals (immediate/delayed) within a single experiment. Different
forms of correct recall (verbatim/gist) of the verbal information as well as different types of errors (dis-
tortions/fabrications) were also examined. It was predicted that participants in the audiovisual modality
would provide more correct information, and fewer errors than participants in the auditory-only modality.
Participants' recall was predicted to be impaired over time, dropping to a greater extent after a delay in
the auditory-only modality. Results confirmed these hypotheses. Interpretations of the overall findings
are offered within the context of dual-coding theory, and within the theoretical frameworks of source
monitoring and fuzzy-trace theory.

Most research on witness memory is concerned shots (e.g., Huss & Weaver, 1996). Nevertheless,
with eyewitnesses (see Wells & Olsen, 2003, for a there are particular crimes that are committed
review). Empirical research on earwitnesses' tes- verbally through conversation; that is, ``language
timony is much more limited (see Yarmey, 1995, crimes'' (verbal harassment at the workplace,
for a review). However, for some crimes, such as bribery, etc.). In these cases the central point is not
those committed over the telephone (e.g., obscene who did it, but witnesses' memory of what was said
phone calls) or crimes committed in darkness (i.e., verbal content). In the legal context, witness
(e.g., hooded rape), the most important piece of statements about criminal conversations are
evidence is the auditory information. essential because they are frequently the only
Earwitness research has focused primarily on available evidence (Pezdek & Prull, 1993).
voice identification (e.g., Cook & Wilding, 2001; Observers who hear a criminal conversation
Yarmey, Yarmey, Yarmey, & Parliament, 2001), may be either earwitnesses who can both hear and
including some research on memory for different see the conversational event, or earwitnesses (e.g.,
kinds of nonverbal auditory stimuli, such as gun- blind witnesses) who can only listen to that con-

Correspondence should be addressed to Maria L. Alonso-Quecuty, Department of Cognitive Psychology, Faculty of Psychology,
University of La Laguna, Campus de Guajara, s/n 38205 La Laguna (Tenerife), Spain. Email: mlalonso@ull.es
This research was supported by Grant BSO2002-03811 from the Spanish Ministerio de Ciencia y TecnologõÂa (MCYT) and Grant
COF2003/014 from the Canary Government to both authors. Some of the data from this article were previously presented at the Fifth
Biennial Conference of the Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition (SARMAC) in Aberdeen (July 2003). We would
like to express our deep gratitude to Professor Margarita Diges from the Universidad AutoÂnoma de Madrid (Spain) for her special
assistance. We also thank Dan Wright, James Ost, and an anonymous reviewer for their helpful comments on an earlier draft of this
paper.

# 2006 Psychology Press Ltd


http://www.psypress.com/memory DOI:10.1080/09658210444000476
28 CAMPOS AND ALONSO-QUECUTY

versation. Does the memory for conversations effects of mode of presentation on memory for the
differ depending on the modality of experience? event in the same design. As the findings obtained
This study aims to answer this question. by Toglia et al. suggest, it might be expected that
In previous studies of memory for conversa- witnesses in the audiovisual modality would, in
tions, the participants witness the conversational general, show better memory for conversation, as
event either in an audiovisual modality, through a well as less forgetting over time, compared to
videotape as a general rule (Bates, Masling & witnesses in the auditory-only modality. Accord-
Kintsch, 1978; MacWhinney, Keenan & Reinke, ing to the dual-coding theory (Paivio, 1971, 1975,
1982), or in an auditory-only modality through an 1986; Paivio & Csapo, 1969), dually coded items
audiotape (Hjelmquist, 1989; Jarvella & Collas, will be remembered better than unitarily coded
1974; Pezdek & Prull, 1993). Surprisingly, little items, thanks to richer encoding-elaboration pro-
research on free recall for conversations deals cesses. The nonverbal (imaginal) trace compo-
with both the auditory-only and the audiovisual nent, at least if it is visual, leads to better memory
modalities in the same design. In one of these few performance than the verbal component in their
studies, Stafford, Waldron, and Infield (1989) additive combination when items are encoded
found no significant differences in immediate free dually. Consequently, it is likely that the same
recall for utterances as a function of the mode of target memory can be retrieved from either code,
presentation (audiovisual/auditory-only). Other compared to the case where only one of the codes
studies examining the effects of audiovisual and has been stored (Paivio, 1986).
auditory-only modalities on memory have shown Another important issue in research on recall
the same results when immediate free recall for for conversations in forensic context is concerned
the verbal information conveyed through a target with different forms of retention of the verbal
material (e.g., magazine programmes, party poli- information. That is, when people remember
tical broadcasts) has been tested (e.g., Furnham, conversations, what do they remember? Previous
Proctor, & Gunter, 1988; Gunter, Furnham, & research on memory for conversations shows that
Leese, 1986). recall protocols contain largely gist memory, and
In previous witness research, however, it has that verbatim memory for utterances, therefore, is
been found that memory for an event witnessed unusual when retention is measured through a
audiovisually may differ from memory for the free recall (as opposed to a recognition) test
same event when it is presented in an auditory- (Miller, deWinstanley, & Carey, 1996; Ross &
only modality, as was shown in an interesting Sicoly, 1979; Stafford, Burggraf, & Sharkey, 1987;
study carried out by Toglia, Shlechter, and Che- Stafford & Daly, 1984; Stafford et al., 1989).
valier (1992). Toglia et al. examined memory for Furthermore, empirical studies of memory for
an event involving an argument between two sentences, models of discourse processing, and
students that was experienced either in an audio- models of memory claim that memory for the
visual modality, in which participants witnessed semantic content (gist) of to-be-remembered
the staged live event (Experiment 1), or in an (TBR) material is better retained over time than
auditory-only modality, in which participants lis- memory for the actual words and syntactic form
tened to a written version of the same event that (verbatim wording) (see e.g., Brewer & Naka-
was read aloud to them (Experiment 2). Retention mura, 1984; Gernsbacher, 1985; Kintsch, Welsch,
over time was also tested. In both modalities, it Schmalhofer, & Zimny, 1990; Sachs, 1967). Par-
was found that participants recalled more correct allel findings have been observed in previous
information related to actions and conversation studies of memory for conversations (e.g.,
immediately than 2 weeks later. Moreover, parti- Hjelmquist & Gidlund, 1985; Stafford et al., 1987).
cipants in the auditory-only modality showed a More recently, the topic of verbatim and gist
greater decrement in memory performance memory has received extensive treatment by
between the immediate and delayed tests com- Brainerd and Reyna in their Fuzzy-Trace Theory
pared to the decrement in performance showed (FFT), which provides a relevant approach to
over time by participants in the audiovisual interpret memory performance (Brainerd &
modality. Reyna, 2002; Brainerd, Reyna, & Brandse, 1995;
Unfortunately, Toglia et al. reported no data on Reyna, 2000; Reyna & Brainerd, 1995; Reyna &
possible differences between the audiovisual Kiernan, 1994). The FTT states that during
modality and the auditory-only modality in encoding of TBR material, verbatim and gist tra-
memory performance, as they did not examine the ces are formed in parallel. The verbatim trace
BEYOND EYEWITNESS TESTIMONY 29

represents the item's surface form whereas the gist 1991; Mitchell & Johnson, 2000), which is an
trace represents the semantic, relational, and ela- extension of the reality-monitoring (RM) model
borative properties of the stimulus. This theory proposed by Johnson and Raye (1981). According
posits that people retrieve dissociated verbatim to this theory, false memories are a result of
and gist traces, with correct recollection being source misattribution errors, which are less likely
supported by both kinds of traces (Brainerd & to occur when the event memory is richly detailed
Reyna, 2002). According to the FTT, forgetting is and its attributes are distinctive, uniquely char-
assumed to be the gradual fragmentation of traces, acteristic of its source (Johnson et al., 1993). After
which is higher for verbatim (as opposed to gist) a delay, the likelihood of source errors arising
representations. Therefore, people are more increases, as the perceptual characteristics of the
likely to rely on verbatim representations imme- externally generated memories fade (e.g., Frost,
diately after the TBR material is presented, but 2000; Underwood & Pezdek, 1998). From this
shift to gist after a delay (Reyna & Kiernan, 1994). view, it should be expected that participants in the
The FTT also offers a framework in which to audiovisual modality would make fewer fabrica-
interpret a variety of memory errors such as fab- tions than participants in the auditory-only mod-
rications or illusory memories (Toglia, Neuschatz, ality, because of richer perceptual features at
& Goodwin, 1999). This theory assumes that these input when a conversational event is both seen
kind of false memories occur because gist is and heard compared to when the conversational
accessed, rather than verbatim memory, and fac- event is only heard. Accordingly, fewer fabrica-
tors that influence the accessibility of gist repre- tions in the audiovisual modality compared to the
sentations (e.g., when memory testing is delayed auditory-only modality should also be expected
for several days and participants are instructed to over time.
rely on the meaning content of experience) have The purpose of the present research was to
predictable effects on memory performance study the effects of both mode of presentation
(Reyna & Kiernan, 1994). Accordingly, several (audiovisual/auditory-only) and retention interval
experiments have shown high levels of false (immediate/delayed) on witnesses' free recall for
memories persistence (e.g., Brainerd et al., 1995; utterances in a criminal conversation within a
Toglia et al., 1999). single experiment. Our hypotheses can be sum-
In addition to fabrications (i.e., recollecting marised as follows: (1) Participants in the audio-
events that never happened), false memories may visual modality will provide overall more correct
also take the form of remembering an event in a information, and fewer errors than participants in
distorted manner (e.g., incorrect recall of some the auditory-only modality; (2) Participants in the
detail of the original event) (Toglia et al., 1999). audiovisual modality will show less forgetting of
Previous research suggests that these two different correct information, and fewer errors over time
forms of false memories may represent different compared to participants in the auditory-only
psychological phenomena. Gudjonsson and Clare modality; (3) Participants overall will show
(1995) found that distortions and fabrications did superior gist over verbatim recall; (4) Participants'
not correlate significantly with each other and verbatim and gist recall will be impaired over
loaded on separate factors, so that they recom- time; (5) Forgetting of correct information over
mend considering them separately in research time will be higher for verbatim details than for
involving both types of false memories.1 In line gist; and (6) Participants will make fewer
with FTT, more distortions should be expected distortions over time, whereas the amount of
when memory is tested immediately compared to fabrications will tend to be persistent.
when memory is tested after a delay, as individuals
are more likely to rely on verbatim traces imme-
diately after the TBR material is presented. METHOD
Another theoretical approach to false mem-
ories is Johnson and colleagues' source-monitor- Participants
ing framework (SMF) (Johnson, 1988; Johnson,
Hashtroudi, & Lindsay, 1993; Lindsay & Johnson, A total of 80 undergraduate students from the
University of La Laguna in Tenerife volunteered
1
In the present study, distortions and fabrications were to participate in the experiment. There were 64
considered separately since, in a forensic investigation, the two females and 16 males. Their ages ranged from 20
may have quite different implications (Milne & Bull, 2002). to 22 years (mean 20.52).
30 CAMPOS AND ALONSO-QUECUTY

Materials instructions, with the exception that they were


going to listen to an audiotaped conversation
A videotape in which two actors staged a mock (instead of watching it).
dyadic criminal conversation was recorded on the In the second phase, all participants carried out
Campus de Guajara square of the University of La a distractor task for 15 minutes, consisting of
Laguna. The event under discussion was a planned crossword puzzles (HernaÂndez-Fernaud &
theft. The recording lasted 15 minutes approxi- Alonso-Quecuty, 1997). Afterwards, participants
mately. This length provided the conversation were asked to free recall the target conversation,
with an episodic structure (i.e., consisting of which they had to complete in writing during an
introduction, core, and ending) such as that used unlimited time. Participants in groups 1 and 3 did
in other researches (e.g., Bates, Kintsch, Fletcher, an immediate free-recall task; participants in
& Giuliani, 1980; Bates et al., 1978; Miller et al., groups 2 and 4 did it 4 days later.
1996). In order to ensure high ecological validity, Finally, all participants were asked questions
the conversation was recorded as if it had been concerning their comprehension of the conversa-
obtained by chance by one of the security cameras tion (e.g., what the speakers were going to do after
located in the campus. It was recorded at night in the conversation took place), and whether they
black and white format and with environmental had any previous knowledge of the target mate-
sound. rial, in order to eliminate those participants who
might have been familiar with such conversation.
All participants showed that they had understood
Design the conversation and were naõÈve concerning the
purpose of the experiment.
The design was 2 6 2 factorial. The first factor,
mode of presentation, was between subjects and
consisted of two groups: (1) audiovisual and (2) Scoring procedure
auditory-only. The second factor, retention inter-
val, was also between subjects and consisted of The videotape was transcribed. Next, utterances
two groups: (1) immediate and (2) delayed. were split up into idea units. An idea unit was
defined as the smallest utterance that contained a
subject and predicate (Miller & deWinstanley,
Procedure 2002; Miller et al., 1996). The final coding script
consisted of 374 idea units. Recall protocols were
Participants were randomly assigned to four similarly split up into recall units.
treatment combinations as a function of Mode of Later, each recall unit was compared to the
Presentation (audiovisual/auditory-only) and original idea units and coded as being verbatim
Retention Interval (immediate/ delayed). Partici- recall, gist recall, distortion, or fabrication. Each
pants in group 1 (n = 20) received the audiovisual recall unit was scored by giving 1 point. Recall
modality and were tested immediately; partici- protocols were coded by two independent judges,
pants in group 2 (n = 20) received the audiovisual who were doctoral students previously trained in
modality and were tested 4 days later; participants this task. They split each recall protocol up into
in group 3 (n = 20) received the auditory-only recall units and then coded each one within the
modality and were tested immediately; and following dependent measures:
participants in group 4 (n = 20) received the
auditory-only modality and were tested 4 days (1) Verbatim recall: Accurate recall of an ori-
later. ginal entire idea unit included in the conversation.
All participants went through three experi- (2) Gist recall: Accurate recall of the kernel of
mental stages. In the first phase, participants in the meaning from an original entire idea unit of which
audiovisual condition were told that they were wording differs from the one included in the
going to watch a dyadic conversation and that they conversation; that is, a paraphrase of the original
had to pay as much attention as possible to it, as if idea unit (e.g., recalling the utterance ``don't fail
they were real eyewitnesses. Then they watched in your task'' instead of the original idea unit
the videotaped conversation in small groups ``don't make any mistake when you do the job'').
comprising around 10 persons. Participants in the Gist recall also included recall units that sum-
auditory-only condition received the same marised two or more idea units accurately (e.g.,
BEYOND EYEWITNESS TESTIMONY 31

recalling the utterance ``I need you to give me TABLE 1


more information about my mission'' instead of Mean of verbatim and gist recall as a function of mode of
presentation and retention interval
the original utterances ``I don't have enough
information about the target student'' and ``Please Retention interval
tell me more about the magic key'', separately).
These were less precise and more global state- Type of correct recall Immediate Delayed Mean
ments than a paraphrase of an original idea unit
Verbatim
(Miller et al., 1996). Also, these summary state- Audiovisual 0.35 (0.58) 0.10 (0.30) 0.22 (0.48)
ments could be not directly related to any one idea Auditory-only 0.80 (1.36) 0.00 (0.00) 0.40 (1.03)
unit but rather captured the global situation being
Gist
described and permitted the scoring of a ``deeper'' Audiovisual 14.90 (6.15) 12.45 (6.56) 13.68 (6.39)
level of gist than was allowed by one-to-one idea Auditory-only 14.90 (7.15) 6.10 (3.87) 10.50 (7.22)
unit matching (Stafford & Daly, 1984; Stafford et
al., 1987, 1989). Standard deviations are presented in parentheses.
(3) Distortion: Recall unit that was in some
way discrepant with the original idea unit
0.57) than in the delayed test (mean 0.05). Indeed,
expressed in the conversation; that is, incorrect
participants' gist recall was greater when they
recall of some detail of an original utterance in the
were tested immediately (mean 14.90) than after a
conversation (e.g., recalling the distorted utter-
delay (mean 9.28). There was also a main effect of
ance ``we have already got three magic keys''
mode of presentation on gist recall. Participants in
instead of the original utterance ``we have already
the audiovisual modality recalled the gist of more
got two magic keys'').
idea units (mean 13.68) than did participants in
(4) Fabrication: Recall of something not
the auditory-only modality (mean 10.50). Mode of
expressed in the conversation (e.g., recalling that
presentation main effect was moderated by a sig-
one of the speakers asked the other for some
nificant mode of presentation 6 retention interval
money, when this utterance was not expressed in
interaction. Simple effects analyses revealed that
the conversation).
participants' gist recall was greater in the audio-
visual modality (mean 12.45) than in the auditory-
RESULTS
TABLE 2
Correct recall Mode of presentation 6 retention interval 6 type of correct
recall ANOVAs performed on mean verbatim and gist correct
A series of 2 (mode of presentation: audiovisual/ recall scores
auditory-only) 6 2 (retention interval: immedi-
Source df F o2
ate/delayed) 6 2 (type of correct recall: verbatim/
gist) analyses of variance (ANOVAs) with the Between subjects (verbatim recall)
first and second factors between subjects and the Mode of presentation (MP) 1 1.06 .0008
third factor within subjects were performed on Retention interval (RI) 1 9.62** .09
mean verbatim and gist correct recall scores. The MP 6 RI 1 2.64 .02
S within-group error 76 (0.57)
descriptive statistics for these data are displayed in
Table 1. Between subjects (gist recall)
Mode of presentation (MP) 1 5.48* .05
As expected, there was a main effect of type of
Retention interval (RI) 1 17.20*** .17
correct recall (see Table 2). Participants' gist MP 6 RI 1 5.48* .05
recall was greater (mean 12.09) than their verba- S within-group error 76 (36.79)
tim recall (mean 0.31). All the interactions were Within subjects
significant. Simple effects analyses revealed fur- Type of correct recall (CR) 1 320.88*** .67
ther that participants' gist recall was greater than CR 6 MP 1 6.49** .03
their verbatim recall in all treatment combina- CR 6 RI 1 15.05*** .08
tions. CR 6 MP 6 RI 1 4.87* .02
CR 6 S within-group error 76 (17.28)
As also shown in Table 2, there was a main
effect of retention interval on both verbatim and Values in parentheses represent mean square errors. S =
gist recall. As predicted, participants' verbatim subjects.
recall was greater in the immediate test (mean * Significant at p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001.
32 CAMPOS AND ALONSO-QUECUTY

only modality (mean 6.10) when they were tested 1.25) occurred in the auditory-only (as opposed to
after a delay, F(1, 77) = 9.05, MSE = 44.53, p = the audiovisual) modality, F(1, 78) = 17.76, MSE =
.004, o2 = .09. Nevertheless, no significant differ- 1.83, p < .001, o2 = .09. Indeed, participants
ences were found in immediate gist recall as a showed a superior amount of fabrications (mean
function of the mode of presentation. As pre- 2.07) over distortions (mean 1.00) in the delayed
dicted, a significant reduction in gist recall over (as opposed to the immediate) test, F(1, 78) =
time was observed, but solely in the auditory-only 11.85, MSE = 1.95, p = .001, o2 = .06.
modality (mean 14.90 versus mean 6.10), F(1, 77) As also shown in Table 4, there was a main
= 19.89, MSE = 38.93, p < .001, o2 = .19, which effect of retention interval on distortions. As
suggests that gist recall is more sensitive to the expected, participants tested immediately made
4-day delay in this mode of presentation, whereas more distortions (mean 1.78) than did participants
gist recall remains relatively stable from the tested after a delay (mean 1.00). There was also a
immediate retention interval (mean 14.90) to the main effect of mode of presentation on fabrica-
delayed interval (mean 12.45) in the audiovisual tions. Also in accordance with our predictions,
modality (see Figure 1). participants in the auditory-only modality made
more fabrications (mean 2.53) than did partici-
pants in the audiovisual modality (mean 1.42).
Errors

A series of 2 (mode of presentation: audiovisual/ DISCUSSION


auditory-only) 6 2 (retention interval: immedi-
ate/delayed) 6 2 (type of error: distortion/fabri- In this paper we have explored the effects that
cation) ANOVAs were performed on mean mode of presentation and delay have on wit-
distortions and fabrications scores, with the first nesses' recall for utterances of a criminal con-
and second factors between subjects and the third versation. Different forms of accurate recall
factor within subjects. The descriptive statistics for (verbatim/gist) of the verbal information as well as
these data are displayed in Table 3. different forms of false memories (distortions/
A main effect of type of error was obtained (see fabrications) have been examined.
Table 4). Participants overall made more fabri- The results showed that conversational recall
cations (mean 1.98) than distortions (mean 1.39). was not very good, consistent with those obtained
Both two-way interactions were significant. Sim- in previous research on memory for conversations
ple effects analyses revealed that superior amount in which free recall (as opposed to recognition)
of fabrications (mean 2.53) over distortions (mean tests have been used to examine retention (e.g.,

Figure 1. Mode of presentation 6 retention interval interaction on gist recall.


BEYOND EYEWITNESS TESTIMONY 33

TABLE 3 As predicted, gist recall was greater than ver-


Mean of distortions and fabrications as a function of mode of batim recall. The results also revealed that
presentation and retention interval
superior gist recall over verbatim recall occurred
Retention interval in both immediate and delayed tests. Participants'
memories were not word-for-word reproductions,
Type of error Immediate Delayed Mean as an average of only 0.31 idea units recalled
verbatim indicates (Hjelmquist & Gidlund, 1985).
Distortion
Audiovisual 1.85 (1.08) 1.20 (1.19) 1.52 (1.17)
Such a low level of verbatim recall might be due to
Auditory-only 1.70 (1.86) 0.80 (0.76) 1.25 (1.48) a floor effect; however, this might also be due to
the fact that participants overall tend to focus on
Fabrication
Audiovisual 1.45 (1.79) 1.40 (1.35) 1.42 (1.56) the kernel of meaning rather than specific details
Auditory-only 2.30 (2.75) 2.75 (1.80) 2.53 (2.30) (e.g., Stafford et al., 1987; Stafford & Daly, 1984).
According to Fuzzy-Trace Theory (FTT)
Standard deviations are presented in parentheses. (Brainerd & Reyna, 2002; Reyna, 2000; Reyna &
Brainerd, 1995; Reyna & Kiernan, 1994), indivi-
duals retrieve dissociated verbatim and gist traces,
Miller et al., 1996; Ross & Sicoly, 1979; Stafford & with accurate memories being supported by both
Daly, 1984). As predicted, there was forgetting
kinds of traces. Gist retrieval is favoured when gist
over time: participants recalled more correct
traces are strong relative to verbatim traces, as
information immediately than after the 4-day
when individuals are instructed to rely on the
delay, consistent with findings in previous studies
meaning content of experience (Brainerd &
of recall for conversations (Hjelmquist & Gidlund,
Reyna, 2002). From this view, participants in the
1985; Stafford et al., 1987). Although participants'
present study were likely to rely mainly on gist
verbatim and gist recall were both greater in the
traces, rather than verbatim traces, as intentional
immediate test than in the delayed test, extremely memory instructions were given to them (Toglia
poor verbatim recall found in the delayed test (only
et al., 1999). Conditions of test might also bias
0.05 idea units on average) suggests that verbatim
towards gist retrieval, as the participants were
traces became inaccessible more rapidly than gist
asked to free recall the target conversation, where
traces (Reyna & Brainerd, 1995).
naturally no instructions to recall the verbal
information verbatim nor to respond to specific
TABLE 4
questions were given (Lindsay & Johnson, 1991).
Mode of presentation 6 retention interval 6 type of error Participants made more fabrications (i.e., recall
ANOVAs performed on mean distortions and fabrications of something not expressed in the conversation)
scores than distortions (i.e., incorrect recall of some
detail of an original utterance in the conversa-
Source df F o2
tion), but only after the 4-day delay. According to
Between subjects (distortions) FTT, false memories vary directly with the rela-
Mode of presentation (MP) 1 0.90 .001 tive accessibility of gist versus verbatim repre-
Retention interval (RI) 1 7.18** .07 sentations, and individuals are more likely to rely
MP 6 RI 1 0.18 .01 on gist (as opposed to verbatim representations)
S within-group error 76 (1.67)
after a delay (Reyna & Brainerd, 1995). An
Between subjects (fabrications) interesting finding was that distortions and fabri-
Mode of presentation (MP) 1 6.09** .06
cations rates differed over time. As predicted,
Retention interval (RI) 1 0.20 .009
MP 6 RI 1 0.31 .008 participants made more distortions in the
S within-group error 76 (3.97) immediate test than in the delayed test. No sig-
Within subjects
nificant differences were found, however, in the
Type of error (E) 1 7.96** .04 amount of fabrications as a function of the
E 6 MP 1 10.89*** .06 retention interval. Given that forgetting is
E 6 RI 1 5.48* .03 assumed to be the gradual fragmentation of traces,
E 6 MP 6 RI 1 0.81 .001 which is higher for verbatim (as opposed to gist)
E 6 X within-group error 76 (1.73)
representations (Reyna, 2000), one would expect
Values in parentheses represent mean square errors. S = that participants would make more distortions
subjects. immediately after the target conversation was
* Significant at p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001. presented than after the 4-day delay. On the other
34 CAMPOS AND ALONSO-QUECUTY

hand, the relative stability of fabrications over mode (Toglia et al., 1992). One possible explana-
time is also consistent with FTT, which predicts tion focuses on differences in encoding elabora-
that illusory memories can be highly persistent, as tion processes. In accordance with the dual-coding
they arise from particularly stable memory theory (Paivio, 1971, 1975, 1986), dually coded
representations (gist traces) (Brainerd et al., 1995; items will be remembered better than unitarily
Toglia et al., 1999). coded items. From this view, participants who saw
In this study, participants witnessed the con- and heard the conversational event were likely to
versational event either in an audiovisual mod- encode it dually in both an imaginal and a verbal
ality or in an auditory-only modality. As code, whereas participants in the auditory-only
predicted, participants in the audiovisual modality modality were likely to represent it primarily in a
showed better recall of the conversation overall verbal code, although some referential (imaginal)
compared to participants in the auditory-only coding could also occur. It is likely, therefore, that
modality. Participants in the audiovisual modality the same target memory could be retrieved from
recalled the gist of more idea units of the con- either code in the audiovisual modality, compared
versation, and made fewer fabrications compared to one code in the auditory-only modality.
to participants in the auditory-only modality. Overall better recall of the conversation in the
However, no significant differences were found in audiovisual modality compared to the auditory-
verbatim recall, nor in the amount of distortions as only modality was bolstered by the fabrication
a function of the mode of presentation of the data. As predicted, participants in the audiovisual
conversation. As was mentioned above, the pro- modality made fewer fabrications compared to
blem here might be a floor effect. But it might also participants in the auditory-only modality. This
evidence that the typical manner of remembering result may be explained in line with the source-
a conversation is in gist when a free recall is used monitoring framework (SMF) (Johnson et al.,
to test retention (Hjelmquist & Gidlund, 1985; 1993; Johnson & Raye, 1981). According to the
Miller et al., 1996; Stafford et al., 1987; Stafford & SMF, false memories arise due to source-mon-
Daly, 1984). itoring errors, which are less likely to occur when
The mode of presentation main effect on gist the mental event is richly detailed and its attri-
recall was moderated by a significant interaction butes are distinctive, unique characteristics of its
with retention interval, which showed that super- source (Johnson et al., 1993). In line with this, to
ior gist recall in the audiovisual modality over the the extent that there are more external perceptual
auditory-only modality occurred only after the 4- features at input, it should make source errors less
day delay. Consistent with previous research on likely. In the audiovisual modality, it is obvious
immediate free recall for conversations (e.g., that there were richer perceptual features at input,
Stafford et al., 1989) as well as with other studies as there was additional perceptual (visual) infor-
examining the effects of audiovisual and auditory- mation (e.g., the environmental setting, actions,
only modalities on memory (e.g., Furnham et al., clothing and physical characteristics of partici-
1988; Gunter et al., 1986), no significant differ- pants, nonverbal information), which was not
ences were found in immediate gist recall as a presented in the auditory-only modality. Partici-
function of the mode of presentation of the con- pants in the audiovisual modality, therefore, were
versation. Because gist (as opposed to verbatim) likely to make fewer fabrications compared to
memories become inaccessible more slowly participants in the auditory-only modality.
(Kintsch et al., 1990; Reyna & Brainerd, 1995; The fact that participants in the auditory-only
Sachs, 1967; Stafford et al., 1987), it may be that an modality showed a greater fabrication rate over
immediate (as opposed to a delayed) test is not time compared to participants in the audiovisual
sensitive enough to detect any modality effects on modality is also consistent with the SMF. This
gist recall. theory predicts that because perceptual char-
One of the more noteworthy findings of this acteristics of the externally generated memories
study was that, as predicted, participants in the fade over time, the likelihood of making source
audiovisual modality showed little forgetting of errors increases (e.g., Frost, 2000; Underwood &
correct information over time compared to parti- Pezdek, 1998). In accordance with this, as fewer
cipants in the auditory-only modality. It appears, perceptual details concerning the conversational
therefore, that there are different rates of forget- event should be encoded at input in the auditory-
ting for witnessing a conversational event audio- only modality compared to the audiovisual mod-
visually and witnessing it in an auditory-only ality, it should make source errors more likely
BEYOND EYEWITNESS TESTIMONY 35

over time in the auditory-only modality because Cook, S., & Wilding, J. (2001). Earwitness testimony:
the memory for the conversational event loses its Effects of exposure and attention on the face over-
shadowing effect. British Journal of Psychology, 92,
perceptual details as time progresses.
617±629.
The results of the present research can be Frost, P. J. (2000). The quality of false memory over
summarised as follows. First, when participants time: Is memory for misinformation ``remembered''
are asked to free recall a conversation they tend to or ``known''? Psychonomic Bulletin and Review,
remember the gist, showing little verbatim mem- 7(3), 531±536.
Furnham, A., Proctor, E., & Gunter, B. (1988). Memory
ory of what was said. Second, the retention
for material presented in the media: The superiority
interval between witnessing a conversation and of written communication. Psychological Reports,
later recall test impairs witnesses' performance. 63, 935±938.
Finally, the extent to which retention interval Gernsbacher, M. A. (1985). Surface information loss in
impairs memory performance depends on the comprehension. Cognitive Psychology, 17, 324±363.
Gudjonsson, G. H., & Clare, I. C. H. (1995). The rela-
mode of presentation (audiovisual/auditory-only)
tionship between confabulation and intellectual
of the conversation. The findings of the present ability, memory, interrogative suggestibility and
research provide evidence that participants in the acquiescence. Personality and Individual Differ-
auditory-only modality made more fabrications ences, 19(3), 333±338.
and showed a greater forgetting of accurate Gunter, B., Furnham, A., & Leese, J. (1986). Memory
for information from a party political broadcast as a
information over time compared to participants in
function of the channel of communication. Social
the audiovisual modality. This issue is important Behaviour, 1, 135±142.
for the psychology of testimony because, in some HernaÂndez-Fernaud, E., & Alonso-Quecuty, M. L.
cases, a criminal conversation takes place under (1997). The cognitive interview and lie detection: A
circumstances in which the participants in the new magnifying glass for Sherlock Holmes? Applied
Cognitive Psychology, 11, 55±68.
conversation cannot be seen (e.g., the area is in
Hjelmquist, E. (1989). Recognition memory for utter-
darkness), but what they say is heard. Further, ances in conversation. Scandinavian Journal of Psy-
witnesses do not usually make statements imme- chology, 30, 168±176.
diately (at the scene), but after a delay (at the Hjelmquist, E., & Gidlund, A. (1985). Free recall of
police station or in court) following the occur- conversations. Text, 5(3), 169±185.
Huss, M. T., & Weaver, K. A. (1996). Effect of mod-
rence of the criminal conversation. In this sense,
ality in earwitness identification: Memory for verbal
accounts of ``earwitnesses'' who could only listen and nonverbal stimuli presented in two contexts.
to a criminal conversation should be treated with The Journal of General Psychology, 123(4),
extreme caution in court. 277±287.
Jarvella, R. J., & Collas, J. G. (1974). Memory for the
Manuscript received 7 July 2003 intentions of sentences. Memory and Cognition, 2(1),
Manuscript accepted 22 November 2004 185±188.
PrEview proof published online 5 April 2005 Johnson, M. K. (1988). Discriminating the origin of
information. In T. F. Oltmanns & B. A. Maher
(Eds.), Delusional beliefs (pp. 34±65). New York:
REFERENCES Wiley.
Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993).
Bates, E., Kintsch, W., Fletcher, C. R., & Giuliani, V. Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114(1),
(1980). The role of pronominalization and ellipsis in 3±28.
texts: Some memory experiments. Journal of Johnson, M. K., & Raye, C. L. (1981). Reality mon-
Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and itoring. Psychological Review, 88, 67±85.
Memory, 6, 671±691. Kintsch, W., Welsch, D., Schmalhofer, F., & Zimny, S.
Bates, E., Masling, M., & Kintsch, W. (1978). Recog- (1990). Sentence memory: A theoretical analysis.
nition memory for aspects of dialogue. Journal of Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 133±159.
Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Lindsay, D. S., & Johnson, M. K. (1991). Source mon-
Memory, 4(3), 187±197. itoring and recognition memory. Bulletin of the
Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (2002). Fuzzy-trace Psychonomic Society, 29, 203±205.
theory and false memory. Current Directions in MacWhinney, B., Keenan, J. M., & Reinke, P. (1982).
Psychological Science, 11(5), 164±169. The role of arousal in memory for conversation.
Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F., & Brandse, E. (1995). Are Memory and Cognition, 10(4), 308±317.
children's false memories more persistent than their Miller, J. B., & deWinstanley, P. A. (2002). The role of
true memories? Psychological Science, 6, 359±364. interpersonal competence in memory for conversa-
Brewer, W. F., & Nakamura, G. V. (1984). The nature tion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
and functions of schemas. In R. S. Wyer Jr. & T. K. 28(1), 78±89.
Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (pp. 119± Miller, J. B., deWinstanley, P., & Carey, P. (1996).
160). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc. Memory for conversation. Memory, 4(6), 615±631.
36 CAMPOS AND ALONSO-QUECUTY

Milne, R., & Bull, R. (2002). Back to basics: A com- Sachs, J. S. (1967). Recognition memory for syntactic
ponential analysis of the original cognitive interview and semantic aspects of connected discourse. Per-
mnemonics with three age groups. Applied Cognitive ception & Psychophysics, 2, 437±442.
Psychology, 16, 743±753. Stafford, L., Burggraf, C. S., & Sharkey, W. F. (1987).
Mitchell, K. J., & Johnson, M. K. (2000). Source mon- Conversational memory: The effects of time, recall
itoring: Attributing mental experiences. In E. Tul- mode, and memory expectancies on remembrances
ving & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Oxford handbook of of natural conversations. Human Communication
memory (pp. 179±195). New York: Oxford Uni- Research, 14(2), 203±229.
versity Press. Stafford, L., & Daly, J. A. (1984). Conversational
Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New memory: The effects of recall mode and memory
York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. expectancies on remembrances of natural conversa-
Paivio, A. (1975). Coding distinctions and repetition tions. Human Communication Research, 10,
effects in memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psy- 379±402.
chology of learning and motivation (pp. 179±214). Stafford, L., Waldron, V. R., & Infield, L. L. (1989).
New York: Academic Press. Actor±observer differences in conversational mem-
Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations. A dual coding ory. Human Communication Research, 15(4),
approach. New York: Oxford University Press. 590±611.
Paivio, A., & Csapo, K. (1969). Concrete-image and Toglia, M. P., Neuschatz, J. S., & Goodwin, K. A.
verbal memory codes. Journal of Experimental Psy- (1999). Recall accuracy and illusory memories:
chology, 80, 279±285. When more is less. Memory, 7(2), 233±256.
Pezdek, K., & Prull, M. (1993). Fallacies in memory for Toglia, M. P., Shlechter, T. M., & Chevalier, D. S.
conversations: Reflections on Clarence Thomas, (1992). Memory for directly and indirectly experi-
Anita Hill, and the like. Applied Cognitive Psychol- enced events. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 6,
ogy, 7, 299±310. 293±306.
Reyna, V. F. (2000). Fuzzy-trace theory and source Underwood, J., & Pezdek, K. (1998). Memory sug-
monitoring. An evaluation of theory and false- gestibility as an example of the sleeper effect. Psy-
memory data. Learning and Individual Differences, chonomic Bulletin and Review, 5(3), 449±453.
12, 163±175. Wells, G. L., & Olsen, E. A. (2003). Eyewitness testi-
Reyna, V. F., & Brainerd, C. J. (1995). Fuzzy-trace mony. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 277±295.
theory: An interim synthesis. Learning and Indivi- Yarmey, A. D. (1995). Earwitness speaker identifica-
dual Differences, 7(1), 1±75. tion. Psychology, Public Policy and Law, 1(4),
Reyna, V. F., & Kiernan, B. (1994). Development of gist 792±816.
versus verbatim memory in sentence recognition: Yarmey, A. D., Yarmey, A. L., Yarmey, M. J., &
Effects of lexical familiarity, semantic content, Parliament, L. (2001). Commonsense beliefs and the
encoding instructions, and retention interval. identification of familiar voices. Applied Cognitive
Developmental Psychology, 30(2), 178±191. Psychology, 15(3), 283±299.
Ross, M., & Sicoly, F. (1979). Egocentric biases in
availability and attribution. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 37(3), 322±336.

You might also like