You are on page 1of 6

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Morning types are less sensitive to pain than evening types all
day long
K.S. Jankowski
Faculty of Psychology, University of Warsaw, Poland

Correspondence Abstract
Konrad S. Jankowski
E-mail: konrad.jankowski@psych.uw.edu.pl Background: Diurnal variations in pain have been observed in
experimental protocols, post-surgery states and pathological conditions.
Funding Sources Chronotype is considered to have the most profound effect on diurnal
Manuscript preparation was partially
variations, and in addition, previous studies suggest that evening types
supported by the grant 2011/03/D/HS6/05760
from the National Science Centre, Poland.
may be more vulnerable to pain than morning types. This study aimed to
examine whether or not morning and evening chronotypes differ in terms
Conflict of interest of their daily levels and diurnal fluctuations of pain sensitivity.
None declared. Methods: A total of 16 morning-healthy and 15 evening-healthy men
were selected using the Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire and
Accepted for publication underwent nine measurements during 1 day (between 08:15 and 20:15 h),
7 December 2012
each consisting of five heat stimuli situated at the ventral side of the wrist.
doi:10.1002/j.1532-2149.2012.00274.x
Results: A marked difference between chronotypes was found, with
morning types showing less sensitivity to pain than evening types all day
long [M = 50.1; standard error (SE) = 1 and M = 47.2; SE = 1, respectively;
pain thresholds in centigrade]. Diurnal variations in pain were not
statistically significant.
Conclusions: The results showed that chronotype could be an important
factor determining sensitivity to pain, regardless of time of day.

Horne and Östberg (1976). It has been shown that


1. Introduction
chronotype is influenced by polymorphisms in par-
Factors influencing pain are of great interest to ticular genes (Wulff et al., 2009), and that genetic
researchers and clinicians, as the anticipation of pain factors explain 45% of the variance in morningness-
symptoms can increase the accuracy of pain manage- eveningness (Hur, 2007). Moreover, chronotype is
ment. One of these factors is the circadian rhythm. determined biologically, mostly by age; during adoles-
Circadian and diurnal variations in pain have been cence, a shift towards eveningness is observed, but
observed in pathological conditions (Junker and Wirz, there is a morningness preference from early adult-
2010), post-surgery states (Chassard and Bruguerolle, hood to old age (Roenneberg et al., 2007). In general,
2004) and experimental protocols conducted on particular phases of the circadian rhythm occur earlier
healthy individuals (Koch and Raschka, 2004). in morning types (M-types) than in evening types
Some individual differences in circadian variations (E-types). Differences between chronotypes in terms
in pain have been observed (Junker and Wirz, 2010); of the pattern of circadian or diurnal variations have
however, they have not been ascribed to particular been shown for many physiological and psychological
individual characteristics. Chronotype describes pref- variables. A particularly robust difference occurs
erences for activity at specified times of the day and regarding the phase of subjective energy, which con-
is considered to have the most profound effect on sists of greater subjective energy in the evening for
circadian rhythms; usually is assessed by question- E-types and in the morning for M-types (Jankowski
naires, particularly the most frequently used is the and Ciarkowska, 2008; Ottoni et al., 2011). Moreover,
Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (MEQ) by chronotypes assessed by the MEQ show differences in

1068 Eur J Pain 17 (2013) 1068–1073 © 2013 European Federation of International Association for the Study of Pain Chapters
K.S. Jankowski Chronotype and pain

What is already known about this topic? perception, only men were enrolled into the study.
• Diurnal variations in pain have been observed. The participants were selected from a cohort of 483
Chronotype is considered to have the most pro- Warsaw college students, between 18 and 30 years of
found effect on diurnal variations. Previous age, on the basis of their morningness-eveningness
studies suggest that evening types may be more scores derived from the Polish adaptation of the MEQ
vulnerable to pain than morning types. of Horne and Östberg (1976). All participants had
good health and did not use any medicine. The Polish
adaptation of the MEQ has 21 items comprising a
What does this study add?
morningness-eveningness continuum, with higher
• Morning types showed less sensitivity to pain
scores indicating greater morningness and lower ones
than evening types all day long.
indicating greater eveningness. The Polish version of
• Diurnal variations in pain were statistically
the MEQ has good psychometric properties: internal
non-significant.
consistency reliability using the Cronbach’s alpha was
• Chronotype could be an important factor deter-
0.825, the test–retest reliability was 0.793 at a
mining sensitivity to pain, regardless of time of
6-month interval and 0.838 at 3 months, while valid-
day.
ity was demonstrated by the expected relationships
between morningness-eveningness scores and both
age and diurnal fluctuations in subjective energy
the timing of the peak of the core body temperature (Jankowski and Ciarkowska, 2008).
(which is the most endogenous circadian rhythm), in In the present study, the M-types were those who
cortisol (Bailey and Heitkemper, 2001) and melatonin scored from z ⱖ 1.5 upwards, while the E-types were
(Gibertini et al., 1999) and in other physiological those who scored z ⱕ -1.5, compared with the MEQ
rhythms (Kerkhof, 1985). Moreover, there are conse- score distribution in the Polish normalization group
quent differences between chronotypes assessed by (n = 9226). Sixteen subjects were categorized into the
MEQ in affective functioning consisting of disadvan- M-type group [mean age = 23.4 ⫾ standard deviation
tageous mood (Jankowski and Ciarkowska, 2008), (SD) = 2.48 years] and 15 into the E-type group
depressiveness and anxiety (Gaspar-Barba et al., 2009; (mean age = 22.3 ⫾ SD = 1.89 years); the two groups
Pabst et al., 2009) related to eveningness. Therefore, it did not differ in terms of age [t(29) = 1.305; p = 0.202].
is not surprising that E-types may be more vulnerable The 31 subjects who underwent pain measurements
to pain than M-types, irrespective of the time of day. were paid for their participation. Each subject pro-
For instance, eveningness has been suggested to be vided written informed consent, and the study was
associated with headaches (Bruni et al., 2008) and carried out in accordance with the ethical standards of
abdominal pain symptoms in adolescent girls (Negriff the University of Warsaw and International Associa-
and Dorn, 2009). Nonetheless, to date, no controlled tion for the Study of Pain’s guidelines for pain research
experimental study has been conducted to examine in humans.
the role of chronotype in diurnal variations in pain or
in sensitivity to pain regardless of the time of day.
Thus, the aim of the present study was to examine 2.2 Pain measurements
whether or not M-types and E-types differed in pain A repeated-measures experimental design was used
sensitivity in terms of their daily levels and diurnal with selected subjects examined on a single day. Par-
fluctuations. It was hypothesized that M-types were ticipants were asked to come to the laboratory nine
less sensitive to pain than E-types. Regarding the time- times during 1 day to perform the experimental task
of-day effect, a directional hypothesis was not stated as (from 08:00 to 20:00 h). The experimental task was
the studies on diurnal variations in pain sensitivity the same at each time during the day and consisted of
using healthy volunteers showed contradictory results heat-pain threshold measurements using the method
(Koch and Raschka, 2004; Bachmann et al., 2011). of limits (Yarnitsky et al., 1995). Pain was measured
using a 30 ¥ 30 mm Peltier device (Andrex, Warsaw,
Poland). Temperature was monitored at the probe–
2. Methods
skin interface by a built-in temperature sensor. The
probe was designed to fasten onto the ventral side
2.1 Subjects
(ventrum) of the wrist. The probe was initially set at
To avoid any effects of gender, oral contraceptive use 37 °C to shorten the anticipation period, because only
or the menstrual cycle phase on the subjects’ pain the heat-pain threshold but not the warmth-detection

Eur J Pain 17 (2013) 1068–1073 © 2013 European Federation of International Association for the Study of Pain Chapters 1069
Chronotype and pain K.S. Jankowski

threshold was measured; the probe was then heated at


0.5 °C/s. The subjects were able to cool the probe
down by 2 °C/s by pressing a button, and the cut-off
temperature was set at 59 °C. The thermal apparatus
was connected to a PC. Prior to heat stimulation, a
15-min resting period was allowed. To increase the
reliability of the measurements, each task at each time
of the day involved five heat stimuli with 30-s inter-
vals between the cessation of one stimulus and the
start of the next (Gustorff et al., 2003). The subjects
were asked to press a button as soon as the sensation
became unpleasant. The temperature value at which a
subject pressed the button was defined as the pain
threshold.
It should be noted that the instructions in heat-pain
threshold measurements using the method of limits
could be categorized into two groups. In the first cat-
egory, subjects are asked to stop the heat stimulus as Figure 1 Levels of the heat pain threshold (mean ⫾ standard error) in
soon as they feel it turning from hot to painful (e.g., morning (squares) and evening types (circles) throughout a day.
Schaffner et al., 2008). In the second group, subjects
are instructed to stop the increase in temperature as
soon as the perception of heat becomes unpleasant Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. Results of p < 0.05 were
(e.g., Gustorff et al., 2001, 2002, 2003). This slight considered statistically significant. The analyses were
difference in instructions could potentially impact the performed using Statistical Package for the Social Sci-
experiment results. Namely, using a word ‘pain’ or ences 18 for Windows (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).
‘painful’ is more likely to evoke a nocebo effect (for a
comprehensive review, see Benedetti et al., 2007).
3. Results
Therefore, using terms relating to pain may evoke
anxiety and negative expectations to perceived stimu- Regarding pain threshold, the ANOVA revealed a main
lus, which in turn leads to a lowered pain threshold effect of chronotype [F(1, 29) = 4.447; p < 0.05; hp2 =
contaminated by a nocebo effect. Moreover, as a 0.133, see Fig. 1]. Neither main effect of time of day
nocebo evokes anxiety, the instructions relating to [F(8, 232) = 2.052; p = 0.089; hp2 = 0.066; e = 0.518] nor
pain should be particularly avoided if the groups being interaction between chronotype and time of day
compared in the pain thresholds could also differ in [F(8, 232) = 1.001; p = 0.412; hp2 = 0.033; e = 0.518] were
their readiness to start becoming anxious. As discussed significant.
in the Introduction section, such a difference exists The effect of chronotype explained 13.3% of the
between M-types and E-types; the former chronotypes variance in the pain threshold and was due to higher
exhibit lower anxiety and advantageous affective pain thresholds in the M-types [M = 50.087; standard
functioning. error (SE) = 0.957] than the E-types (M = 47.186;
SE = 0.988). This means that the E-types were more
sensitive to heat pain than the M-types all day long
2.3 Statistical analyses
(see Fig. 1).
The dependent variable was pain threshold in centi-
grade, averaged across five stimuli. Two-way analysis
4. Discussion and conclusions
of variance (ANOVA) with repeated measures was
used, with chronotype (two levels: M-type, E-type) as The results of this study indicate that chronotype
the between-subjects factor and time of day (nine affected the heat-pain threshold. The M-types showed
levels: 08:15, 09:45, 11:15, 12:45, 14:15, 15:45, 17:15, a higher heat-pain threshold than the E-types all day
18:45, 20:15 h) as the within-subjects factor. Partial long. This means that the M-types seemed to be less
eta-squared was used as the effect size measure and sensitive to heat pain than the E-types during the day.
Huynh–Feldt epsilon was applied if the sphericity Moreover, both the time of day and the interaction
assumption was violated. The assumption of normality between chronotype and time of day were non-
was met for all measurements as determined by the significant. This means that a lack of diurnal variation

1070 Eur J Pain 17 (2013) 1068–1073 © 2013 European Federation of International Association for the Study of Pain Chapters
K.S. Jankowski Chronotype and pain

in pain sensitivity was observed in the two chronotype The third mechanism could be linked to differences
groups. This finding is in agreement with the results in in the sleep debt between chronotypes. Namely,
a study by Bachmann et al. (2011), where the laser- E-types report shorter periods of sleep duration
induced heat-pain threshold did not show any diurnal during work days compared with M-types (Roepke
variations. However, it is also possible that the sample and Duffy, 2010), which is associated with social
size in the present study was insufficient to detect any demands. As a consequence, they accumulate a sleep
statistically significant time-of-day effect. Visual debt and attempt to make up for lost sleep during
inspection of pain thresholds throughout the day sug- weekends, while sleep deprivation is known to
gests that there may be diurnal fluctuations. Never- heighten pain sensitivity (e.g., Campbell et al., 2011).
theless, the absence of a time-of-day effect does not The present study has several limitations. These
exclude the possibility that strong circadian pattern in mainly arise from the nature of the sample and the
heat-pain sensitivity could occur (Portaluppi et al., stimulus used. Regarding the nature of the sample,
2011). It may be possible that robust day-to-night due to the small number of young adult men assessed
differences in heat-pain sensitivity exist that cannot be in this study, generalization of the results should be
recorded over a 12-h period, which seems ecologically made cautiously. The results of previous studies con-
valid, as diurnal recording reflects the waking period cerning gender differences in the pain threshold sug-
of the circadian rhythm of humans. gested that there were no such differences (Schaffner
Several possible mechanisms could be suggested to et al., 2008), especially for thermal pain threshold
explain the observation of M-types showing a higher (Kindler et al., 2011), or that women are more sensi-
heat-pain threshold than the E-types. The first could tive to pain than men (Rolke et al., 2006; Fillingim
be associated with affective functioning. Generally, et al., 2009). Furthermore, the sample subjects
negative affective states have disadvantageous effects included in the present study were 18–30 years of age,
on pain perception, both in healthy individuals and in whereas it was previously found by some researchers
individuals with pathological conditions (Jansen, that subjects above 40 years old are less sensitive to
2002; Pressman and Cohen, 2005; Rhudy and Will- experimentally induced pain (Rolke et al., 2006).
iams, 2006; Haukka et al., 2011; Knudsen et al., Thus, including women and older subjects should be
2011). Hence, emotional processing seems to have a considered in future research on morningness-
key role in pain perception (e.g., Schackman et al., eveningness and circadian variations in pain. Regard-
2011). In previous studies, M-types, as compared with ing the stimulus used in the study, it should be noted
E-types, generally reported advantageous psychologi- that the heat-pain threshold mainly represents C-fibre
cal functioning (Jankowski, 2012a,b), a better mood functioning (Rolke et al., 2006) and various types of
profile during the course of the day (Matthews, 1988; pain may have distinct circadian or daily patterns.
Jankowski and Ciarkowska, 2008) and lower depres- Therefore, future studies on chronotype and the time
siveness (Chelminski et al., 1999; Pabst et al., 2009). of day should concentrate on the pain perceived by
Therefore, lower pain sensitivity in M-types, as com- patients with various diseases and healthy human
pared with E-types, could partly be associated with subjects in different pain models.
affective functioning. Interestingly, in the present research, the individu-
The second mechanism could be linked to differ- als, particularly the M-types, seemed to exhibit higher
ences between M-types and E-types in the circadian pain thresholds than in previous studies (e.g., Rolke
pattern of cortisol. It was recently found that E-types et al., 2006). One possible explanation of this finding
showed lower cortisol levels during the day (Ogińska could be the methodology used: the starting tempera-
et al., 2010) and attenuated cortisol awakening ture of the probe was set at a value higher than 32 °C
response (CAR) (Randler and Schaal, 2010) than – the starting temperature usually used in quantitative
M-types. On the other hand, men who had lower sensory testing – because the aim was to test only the
levels of salivary cortisol prior to pain induction pain threshold and not the warmth-detection thresh-
reported greater pain sensitivity (al’Absi et al., 2002), old. One could speculate whether the higher starting
and the attenuated CAR was associated with greater temperature resulted in a higher pain threshold. Pre-
levels of experimentally induced pain in both men and vious studies have shown that an alteration of the
women (Mayes et al., 2009). Therefore, the observa- adaptation temperature mostly influences the
tion in the present study that M-types are less sensitive warmth-detection threshold and not the heat-pain
to pain than E-types may also be due to the differences threshold (Kojo and Pertovaara, 1987; Pertovaara
between chronotypes in terms of the circadian cortisol et al., 1996). Moreover, when referring pain thresh-
profile. olds obtained in the present study to those reported

Eur J Pain 17 (2013) 1068–1073 © 2013 European Federation of International Association for the Study of Pain Chapters 1071
Chronotype and pain K.S. Jankowski

from other laboratories, the thermode size should be Fillingim, R.B., King, C.D., Riberio-Dasilva, C., Rahim-Williams,
considered (9 cm2 for the present study), as the B., Riley, III, J.L. (2009). Sex, gender, and pain: A review of
recent clinical and experimental findings. J Pain 10, 447–
smaller the thermode, the higher heat-pain thresholds
485.
appear (Defrin and Urca, 1996; Zhu and Lu, 2010). Gaspar-Barba, E., Calati, R., Cruz-Fuentes, C.S., Ontiveros-
However, taking into account the fact that only men Uribe, M.P., Natale, V., De Ronchi, D., Serretti, A. (2009).
were enrolled in the study, the pain thresholds Depressive symptomatology is influenced by chronotypes. J
obtained for the E-types seem to fall into the range of Affect Disord 119, 100–106.
Gibertini, M., Graham, C.H., Cook, M.R. (1999). Self-report of
psychophysical thresholds for heat pain (Maudreli
circadian type reflects the phase of the melatonin rhythm. Biol
et al., 2003; Rolke et al., 2006). Thus, it is possible that Psychol 50, 19–33.
a psychophysical specificity of M-types results in Gustorff, B., Felleiter, P., Nahlik, G., Brannath, W., Hoerauf,
heightened heat-pain thresholds. K.H., Spacek, A., Kress, H.G. (2001). The effect of remifentanil
In summary, the current results showed that chro- on the heat pain threshold. Anasth Analg 92, 369–374.
Gustorff, B., Hoerauf, K.H., Lierz, P., Kress, H.G. (2003). Com-
notype could be an important factor determining sen-
parison of different quantitative sensory testing methods
sitivity to pain. If this finding also proves to be valid in during remifentanil infusion in volunteers. Br J Anaesth 91,
other healthy subjects, in pathological states and post- 203–208.
surgical conditions, it may have great impact on pain Gustorff, B., Nahlik, G., Hoerauf, K.H., Kress, H.G. (2002). The
management where, depending on an individual’s absence of acute tolerance during remifentanil infusion in
volunteers. Anasth Analg 94, 1223–1228.
chronotype, more adequate doses of analgesics could
Haukka, E., Leino-Arjas, P., Ojajärvi, A., Takala, E.P., Viikari-
be administered. Juntura, E., Riihimäki, H. (2011). Mental stress and psychoso-
cial factors at work in relation to multiple-site musculoskeletal
pain: A longitudinal study of kitchen workers. Eur J Pain 15,
Acknowledgements 432–438.
Horne, J.A., Östberg, O. (1976). A self-assessment questionnaire
The experimental protocol was supervised by Prof. Wanda to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian
Ciarkowska. rhythms. Int J Chronobiol 4, 97–110.
Hur, Y.M. (2007). Stability of genetic influence on morningness-
eveningness: A cross-sectional examination of South Korean
References twins from preadolescence to young adulthood. J Sleep Res 16,
17–23.
al’Absi, M., Petersen, K.L., Wittmers, L.E. (2002). Adrenocortical Jankowski, K.S. (2012a). Morningness/eveningness and satisfac-
and hemodynamic predictors of pain perception in men and tion with life in a Polish sample. Chronobiol Int 29, 780–785.
women. Pain 96, 197–204. Jankowski, K.S. (2012b). Morningness–eveningness and tem-
Bachmann, C.G., Nitsche, M.A., Pfingsten, M., Gersdorff, N., perament: The Regulative Theory of Temperament perspec-
Harder, C., Baier, P.Z., Antal, A., Treede, R.D., Paulus, W., tive. Pers Individ Dif 53, 734–739.
Happe, S. (2011). Diurnal time course of heat pain perception Jankowski, K.S., Ciarkowska, W. (2008). Diurnal variation in
in healthy humans. Neurosci Lett 4, 122–125. energetic arousal, tense arousal, and hedonic tone in extreme
Bailey, S.L., Heitkemper, M.M. (2001). Circadian rhythmicity of morning and evening types. Chronobiol Int 25, 577–595.
cortisol and body temperature: Morningness-eveningness Jansen, S.A. (2002). Negative affect and sensitization to pain.
effects. Chronobiol Int 18, 249–261. Scand J Psychol 43, 131–137.
Benedetti, F., Lanotte, M., Lopiano, L., Colloca, L. (2007). When Junker, U., Wirz, S. (2010). Review article: Chronobiology:
words are painful: Unraveling the mechanisms of the nocebo Influence of circadian rhythms on therapy of severe pain. J
effect. Neuroscience 147, 260–271. Oncol Pharm Pract 16, 81–87.
Bruni, O., Russo, P.M., Ferri, R., Novelli, L., Galli, F., Guidetti, V. Kerkhof, G.A. (1985). Inter-individual differences in the human
(2008). Relationship between headache and sleep in a non- circadian system: A review. Biol Psychol 20, 83–112.
clinical population of children and adolescents. Sleep Med 9, Kindler, L.L., Valencia, C., Fillingim, R.B., George, S.Z. (2011).
542–548. Sex differences in experimental and clinical pain sensitivity for
Campbell, M.C., Bounds, S.C., Simango, M.B., Witmer, K.R., patients with shoulder pain. Eur J Pain 15, 118–123.
Campbell, J.N., Edwards, R.R., Haythornthwaite, J.A., Smith, Knudsen, A.K., Brunelli, C., Kaasa, S., Apolone, G., Corli, O.,
M.T. (2011). Self-reported sleep duration associated with6 Montanari, M., Fainsinger, R., Aass, N., Fayers, P., Caraceni,
distraction analgesia, hyperemia, and secondary hyperalgesia A., Klepstad, P. (2011). Which variables are associated with
in the heat-capsaicin nociceptive model. Eur J Pain 15, 561– pain intensity and treatment response in advanced cancer
567. patients? – Implications for a future classification system for
Chassard, D., Bruguerolle, B. (2004). Chronobiology and anes- cancer pain. Eur J Pain 15, 320–327.
thesia. Anesthesiology 100, 413–427. Koch, H.J., Raschka, C. (2004). Diurnal variation of pain per-
Chelminski, I., Ferraro, F.R., Petros, T., Plaud, J.J. (1999). An ception in young volunteers using the tourniquet pain model.
analysis of the ‘eveningness-morningness’ dimension in Chronobiol Int 21, 171–173.
‘depressive’ college students. J Affect Disord 52, 19–29. Kojo, I., Pertovaara, A. (1987). The effects of stimulus area and
Defrin, R., Urca, G. (1996). Spatial summation of heat pain: A adaptation temperature on warm and heat pain thresholds in
reassessment. Pain 66, 23–29. man. Int J Neurosci 32, 875–880.

1072 Eur J Pain 17 (2013) 1068–1073 © 2013 European Federation of International Association for the Study of Pain Chapters
K.S. Jankowski Chronotype and pain

Matthews, G. (1988). Morningness-eveningness as a dimension Rhudy, J.L., Williams, A.E. (2006). Gender differences in pain:
of personality: Trait, state, and psychophysiological correlates. Do emotions play a role. Gend Med 2, 208–226.
Eur J Pers 2, 277–293. Roenneberg, T., Kuehnle, T., Juda, M., Kantermann, T., All-
Maudreli, A.P., Vierck, C.J., Cannon, R.L., Rodrigues, A., Shen, ebrandt, K., Gordijn, M., Merrow, M. (2007). Epidemiology of
C. (2003). Relationship between skin temperature and tem- the human circadian clock. Sleep Med Rev 11, 429–438.
poral summation of heat and cold pain. J Neurophysiol 90, Roepke, S.E., Duffy, J.F. (2010). Differential impact of chrono-
100–109. type on weekday and weekend sleep timing and duration. Nat
Mayes, L.A., McGuire, L., Page, G.G., Goodin, B.R., Edwards, Sci Sleep 2010(2), 213–220.
R.R., Haythornthwaite, J. (2009). The association of the cor- Rolke, R., Baron, R., Maier, C., Tölle, T.R., Treede, R.-D., Beyer,
tisol awakening response with experimental pain ratings. Psy- A., Binder, A., Birbaumer, N., Birklein, F., Bötefür, I.C.,
choneuroendocrinology 34, 1247–1251. Braune, S., Flor, H., Huge, V., Klug, R., Landwehrmeyer, G.B.,
Negriff, S., Dorn, L.D. (2009). Morningness/eveningness and Magerl, W., Maihöfner, C., Rolko, C., Schaub, C., Scherens,
menstrual symptoms in adolescent females. J Psychosom Res 67, A., Sprenger, T., Valet, M., Wasserka, B. (2006). Quantitative
169–172. sensory testing in the German Research Network on Neuro-
Ogińska, H., Fafrowicz, M., Golonka, K., Marek, T., Mojsa-Kaja, pathic Pain (DFNS): Standarized protocol and reference
J., Tucholska, K. (2010). Chronotype, sleep loss, and diurnal values. Pain 123, 231–243.
pattern of salivary cortisol in a simulated daylong driving. Schackman, A.J., Salomons, T.V., Slagter, H.A., Fox, A.S.,
Chronobiol Int 27, 959–974. Witner, J.J., Davidson, R.J. (2011). The integration of negative
Ottoni, G.L., Antoniolli, E., Lara, D.R. (2011). The Circadian affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulated cortex. Nat
Energy Scale (CIRENS): Two simple questions for a reliable Rev Neurosci 12, 154–167.
chronotype measurement based on energy. Chronobiol Int 28, Schaffner, N., Wittwer, A., Kut, E., Folkers, G., Benninger, D.H.,
229–237. Candia, V. (2008). Heat pain threshold and tolerance show no
Pabst, S.R., Negrif, S., Dorn, L.D., Susman, E.J., Huang, B. left-right perceptual differences at complementary sites of the
(2009). Depression and anxiety in adolescent females: The human forearm. Neurosci Lett 440, 309–313.
impact of sleep preference and body mass index. J Adolesc Wulff, K., Porcheret, K., Cussans, E., Foster, R.G. (2009). Sleep
Health 44, 554–560. and circadian rhythm disturbances: Multiple genes and mul-
Pertovaara, A., Kauppila, T., Hämäläinen, M.M. (1996). Influ- tiple phenotypes. Curr Opin Genet Dev 19, 237–246.
ence of skin temperature on heat pain threshold in humans. Yarnitsky, D., Sprecher, E., Zaslansky, R., Hemli, J.A. (1995).
Exp Brain Res 107, 497–503. Heat pain thresholds: Normative data and repeatability. Pain
Portaluppi, F., Fabbian, F., Manfredini, F., Manfredini, R. (2011). 60, 329–332.
Lack of time-of-day differences is not necessarily indicative of Zhu, Y.J., Lu, T.J. (2010). A multi-scale view of skin thermal
lack of circadian influences. Chronobiol Int 28, 731–733. pain: From nociception to pain sensation. Philos Transact A
Pressman, S.D., Cohen, S. (2005). Does positive affect influence Math Phys Eng Sci 368, 521–559.
health? Psychol Bull 131, 925–971.
Randler, C., Schaal, S. (2010). Morningness-eveningness,
habitual sleep-wake variables and cortisol level. Biol Psychol
85, 14–18.

Eur J Pain 17 (2013) 1068–1073 © 2013 European Federation of International Association for the Study of Pain Chapters 1073

You might also like