You are on page 1of 7

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 1969, 2, 199-205 NUMBER 3 (FALL 1969)

APPLICATION OF TIMEOUT FROM POSITIVE


REINFORCEMENT FOR INCREASING THE
EFFICIENCY OF SPEECH TRAINING'
LEIJA V. MCREYNOLDS
UNIVERSITY OF KANSAS

Language training procedures, which involved positive reinforcement for verbal imitation,
were applied to increase the appropriate verbal behavior of an almost non-verbal, brain dam-
aged, 5-yr-old boy. Two experiments assessed the effectiveness of timeout from positive rein-
forcement as a training procedure viewed as having potential punishing and negatively rein-
forcing functions. In both experiments, timein, termination of timeout and resumption of
training, was arranged to have reinforcing properties in that it presented an opportunity to
receive positive reinforcers. In Exp. I, the procedure consisted of temporarily halting language
training (timeout) following verbal jargon and resuming it (timein) contingent upon the boy
sitting quietly in his chair for approximately 30 sec. The jargon declined to almost zero for an
extended period each time the procedure was employed. In Exp. II, the procedure consisted of
halting language training (timeout) after emission of undesired verbal responses which pre-
viously had been reinforced as the desired approximation to the target verbal behavior. Re-
sumption of training (timein) was made contingent upon the emission of the then-desired
approximation of the target verbal behavior. In each experiment, the contingent timeout and
timein of the language training that involved positive reinforcement effectively reduced the
undesired and increased the desired responses.

One procedure that can be used for train- (jargon), which he emits spontaneously during
ing a child to acquire topographically appro- each session. Furthermore, as the training pro-
priate speech is imitation: the child is pre- gresses and more precise articulation is re-
sented with a unit of speech as a verbal quired, or the phoneme to be acquired is a
model. The child's response is reinforced each difficult one to produce, the formerly appro-
time he responds with a vocalization that priate approximations may take a considerable
approximates the model presented. At each amount of time to be extinguished.
successive approximation, the criteria for an As long as these inappropriate vocalizations
appropriate response become more stringent continue to be emitted in speech sessions,
and responses that do not meet the criteria progress in training appropriate speech is
are placed on extinction (ignored) while ap- hampered. Usually a procedure in which ap-
propriate approximations are reinforced. Al- propriate responses are reinforced while inap-
though speech can be trained by using positive propriate behavior is placed under extinction
reinforcement and extinction, it sometimes is successful in training speech. Sometimes,
requires a rather extended training period, however, the inappropriate behaviors are not
due to some of the behaviors in the child's readily modified by these procedures. It is
repertoire. Not only does the child come to possible that a more efficient program might
therapy with a low rate of emission of appro- result if a consequent event, rather than ex-
priate speech, but he frequently comes with tinction, were used to modify the jargon and
a repertoire of inappropriate vocalizations inappropriate responses.
A consequent event that has been used
"The research was supported by Research Grant HD successfully to decelerate incompatible behav-
00870-04 from the National Institute of Child Health ior in children is timeout from positive rein-
and Human Development to the Bureau of Child Re- forcement (Baer, 1962; Wolf, Risley, and
search at the University of Kansas. Reprints may be Mees, 1964). Timeout has been used also dur-
obtained from the author, Bureau of Child Research ing speech training with echolalic children to
Laboratory, Children's Rehabilitation Unit, University
of Kansas Medical Center, 39th and Rainbow Blvd., decrease the frequency of emission of inappro-
Kansas City, Kansas 66103. priate verbal statements and shrieks (Risley
199
200 LEIJA V. McREYNOLDS
and Wolf, 1967), and to decelerate incorrect experimental and control room were separated
responses (Sloane, Johnston, and Harris, 1968). by a wall containing a one-way vision window.
The results indicate that in situations in A microphone was mounted in the ceiling of
which positive reinforcers have been estab- the experimental room and led to a Roberts
lished, timeout can be an effective consequence Tape Recorder, Model 770, and a speaker lo-
for quickly decelerating inappropriate behav- cated in the control room. The training took
ior. It is possible that in attempting to modify place in the experimental room while an ob-
jargon and inappropriate responses, timeout server recorded the data in the control room.
as a consequent event will function more Three pens of a Gerbrands four-pen event
effectively than extinction, thereby increasing recorder were used to record the events in the
the efficiency of speech training. experimental room. The pens were activated
The present study sought to explore by foot switches placed on the floor of the con-
whether timeout from positive reinforcement trol room. The first pen was used to record the
within and across sessions would function (1) experimenter's presentation of the model, the
as a punishing event to decrease the frequency second recorded the subject's response, and the
of jargon vocalizations, and (2) as a negatively third recorded the consequent event if one oc-
reinforcing event to increase the probability curred. The observer, a speech pathologist,
that a specific speech unit would be emitted, transcribed in phonetic symbols on the event
rather than a previously correct (but currently recorder tape his own observations regarding
incorrect) speech response. the subject's responses. Each notation was
written directly beside the response as it oc-
METHOD curred and was recorded with the pen. In this
manner, agreement or disagreement on each
Subject response between the observer and experi-
A 5-yr-old boy, who had been diagnosed as menter was recorded. Because of the continu-
brain-damaged, served. Although his parents ously running tape, occurrences of events
had attempted to enroll him in a special class, within any specified time interval for each ses-
he had not been accepted due to his severely sion could be counted. To check reliability, a
limited behavioral repertoire. No evaluation second observer was introduced periodically
results were available because no responses had and both would make simultaneous observa-
been obtained from the child during diagnos- tion records of a session.
tic procedures. During baseline observation For the jargon behavior, percentage of
the child emitted no speech and did not re- agreement between the observers was obtained
spond appropriately to verbal directions. In by dividing the smaller number of observed
three observation periods he emitted no vocal- occurrences by the larger number when the
izations. He was accepted in our experimental frequency scores of the observers were not
speech training program after he had been ex- identical. In this case, interest was centered on
cluded from other special programs. obtaining a reliability index of frequency per
Operant procedures were used for speech se, and the amount of agreement over the total
training. In the initial phases of training, em- number of jargon occurrences.
phasis was placed on shaping vocal behavior. For the subject's imitative responses, reli-
The child's vocalizations were reinforced and ability was assessed between the experimenter
by the time the study began, he was imitating and observer as well as the two observers. The
isolated speech sounds. The imitation training percentage of agreement was obtained by di-
consisted of presenting the child with a model viding the total number of agreements by the
of the phoneme. Vocalizations that approxi- number of agreements plus the number of dis-
mated the model were reinforced with ice agreements. The percentage of agreement in
cream. No consequences were presented for in- all reliability calculations ranged from 81 to
correct responses. The present study was initi- 86%.
ated during imitative word training.
Experimental Setting Preliminary Procedures
The imitative speech training was con- The procedure for word training followed
ducted in a two-room experimental suite. The the imitation paradigm. Instead of using a
TIMEOUT AND SPEECH-TRAINING EFFICIENCY 201
gradual fading of phoneme prompts (Risley, presented, they were scored as jargon. When
1966) for word training, the procedure con- timeout was used as a punishing event for jar-
sisted of backward chaining. Each word was gon, the procedure consisted of the following
taught by first presenting the child with the sequence of events: after jargon was emitted
final phoneme in the word; when he had ac- the experimenter took the ice cream, turned in
quired imitation of this phoneme, the next to her chair, and sat with her back to the child.
the final phoneme was introduced. Chaining Turning to face the child again was contin-
continued until the child had acquired all the gent on his ceasing the jargon and remaining
phonemes in the chain and was imitating the quietly in his chair for approximately 30 sec.
complete word. When the child's imitation If the child had been sitting quietly for 30 sec,
was appropriate, the experimenter said, the experimenter turned to face him, smiled,
"Good", and immediately followed this with a and presented the verbal model. If the
bite of ice cream. It was observed, however, child imitated the model, the experimenter
that as responses with more precise topogra- reinforced the behavior by presenting the
phies were required, i.e., correct articulation child with a bite of ice cream and a verbal,
of all phonemes in a sequence, the rate of ac- "Good".
quisition of the chain of phonemes was consid- Timeout was instituted for jargon during
erably slower than it had been in the earlier training of the words "pop" and "boat". In
isolated phoneme training. The slow acquisi- both instances, timeout was contingent upon
tion was partly attributable to the fact that the emission of jargon after it had increased in
the child, after correctly imitating the new re- extinction.
sponse chain for some time, would then revert When timeout was used in a negative rein-
to the previously learned shorter chain. forcement paradigm for emission of a specific
At this time the child also began to jargon; speech response, the procedure was somewhat
i.e., after he had responded, and before the different. Timeout for incorrect responses was
experimenter presented another stimulus, the applied only after (1) the subject had previ-
child began to emit a number of irrelevant ously demonstrated that he could imitate the
vocalizations. Originally, any inappropriate new response, (2) the subject showed no in-
response or jargon was placed on extinction; crease in the correct responses within at least a
no consequence was applied. The experi- 5-min period in a training session, and (3) his
menter waited until he had stopped vocalizing responses consisted of the previous response
before presenting a new stimulus, but the jar- that had been appropriate before a new pho-
gon increased. It was evident that, whereas re- neme or word was added to the chain.
inforcement and extinction had been sufficient Timeout as a negative reinforcer was ap-
for training the speech response thus far, they plied for incorrect responses four times. It was
were less efficient for training correct articula- presented twice in the second session of train-
tion of several phonemes in the context of a ing for the chain "cream", once during the
word. At this point, the study was initiated to second session of training for the word "dog",
explore the efficiency of using a combination and once during the second session of training
of consequent events other than reinforcement for the sentence "I want pop". On all four
and extinction for word training. An attempt occasions, timeout was contingent upon con-
was made to determine if timeout from posi- tinued emission of a formerly correct, but pres-
tive reinforcement would function more effec- ently incorrect response in imitation of the ex-
tively to decrease the jargon and increase the perimenter's model. Timeout was terminated
probability that the correct response would be contingent upon the emission of the correct
emitted. response. In training for "cream", timeout was
contingent upon emission of "eam", and ter-
minated contingent on emission of "ream". In
Procedures training for "dog", timeout was contingent
Jargon was defined as unintelligible vocali- upon emission of "og", and terminated con-
zations consisting of one or more spoken tingent upon emission of "dog". Timeout was
sounds resembling English phonemes. When instituted when the subject emitted, "want
these vocalizations occurred after a response pop", and terminated when the child emitted,
had been emitted and before a new model was "I want pop".
202 LEIJA V. McREYNOLDS
On the second occasion, timeout functioned
RESULTS to decelerate jargon in much the same manner
Timeout contingent on jargon usually re- as it did in the first situation. Jargon was
sulted in a decrease in the number of vocaliza- emitted at a rate of approximately one vocali-
tions within 5 min after its application. In zation per minute during the first session of
both cases of timeout for jargon, the rate of training. In the first 10 min of the second ses-
vocalizations decreased from an average of sion, the child emitted approximately three
four vocalizations per minute before applica- jargon vocalizations per minute. Contingent
tion of timeout, to nearly zero per minute after timeout was in effect for the emission of jargon
termination of timeout. Figure 1 presents the after the first 10 min. During timeout, five jar-
results of applying timeout for jargon during gon vocalizations occurred within the first 2
training for the word "pop". min and then ceased. After a silence of 30 sec,
the experimenter terminated timeout and pre-
asl T.O. sented the model. Correct responses increased
and jargon decreased to a zero level for the re-
45 mainder of the session. Although some jargon
-J40A- was emitted in the next session, it did not in-
crease to the former high level.
30 Timeout for incorrect responses was applied
on four occasions, one during training for the
word "cream". The results are presented in
3- 0 Fig. 3.
lo-
| 5: S
z 55
o _ . a _ 0
o 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 s0 I.- 50
| SESSO I | SESSION 2 | SESSION 3 SESSION4 N 45
CUMULATIVE MINUTES -J 40
'4
U 35
a
Fig. 1. Cumulative number of jargon vocalizations
over four sessions of imitative speech training before z 30
0
and after timeout (T.O.) from positive reinforcement 0 25
for jargon. Each dot represents 2 min. lz
'4
20
15
In the first session of training for the word
"pop", the jargon vocalizations averaged one '4 10
-.8
every 2 min. No consequences were presented a
5
after these vocalizations were emitted. In the U 0 I
I I
aI I I
I

second session, the jargon vocalizations in- O 10 20 30 40 50 60


creased to a rate of approximately 3-per-min
by the fourteenth minute. At this time, time-
ISESSION I I SESSION 2 I SESSION 3 I

out was applied, contingent on the emission of CUMULATIVE MI NUTES


the next jargon vocalization. During the first Fig. 2. Cumulative number of jargon vocalizations
2 min of timeout, the subject emitted eight over three sessions of imitative speech training before
vocalizations, and then gradually the vocaliza- and after timeout (T.O.) from positive reinforcement
tions decreased to three within the last 2 min. for jargon. Each dot represents 2 min.
After 30 sec of silence, the experimenter
turned to face the subject and presented the The chain for "cream" consisted of a se-
model. A correct response was reinforced. No quence of four training phases: imitation of
jargon occurred during the rest of Session 2. /m/, then /im/, /rim/ and finally /krim/.
Except for three vocalizations in the next ses- Timeout from positive reinforcement was in-
sion, jargon remained at a zero rate. troduced during the addition of the third pho-
Figure 2 presents the data on application of neme to the chain. Before timeout, the subject
timeout for jargon during training of the word had reached an 80% level of correct imitations
"boat". of /im/. Therefore, the /r/, a somewhat diffi-
TIMEOUT AND SPEECH-TRAINING EFFICIENCY 203

to110 90r
cS 100
z
o9. 90
801-
hi
or
80
,_ 70
0
hi 60
z
0
70p
o 50 T.O. C.
U
T.0.
h, 40 601
0
-
I- 30
-J 20
X 10
I- 501
0 o[ LI
0 I I I I
O 10 20 30 40
-

50 60 70 0 40p
1 SESSION II SESSION 2 | SESS ION 3 |
T.4
CUMULAT IVE MINUTES 30.-
Fig. 3. Cumulative number of correct responses over
three sessions when timeout (T.O.) was used to in-
crease the probability of emission of "cream". Each dot
201
represents 2 min. a

cult phoneme to learn to articulate, was added


0I*
to the chain. 01
As shown in the first session of /rim/ train- I I l -j
ing, the subject had imitated /rim/ 28 times 10 20 30 40
during the first 15 min of training. He was also SESSION I SESS I ON
emitting some /im/ responses, but as long as
he continued to increase the number of /rim/ CUMULAT I VE MINUTES
responses no consequences were applied for
Fig. 4. Cumulative number of correct responses over
the /im/. The child reverted to the /im/ re- two sessions when timeout (T.O.) was used to increase
sponse in the final 2 min of Session 1. In the the probability of "dog". Each dot represents 2 min.
initial 6 min of the next session, his emission
rate of /rim/ was zero. His response consisted model presented by the experimenter. In the
of the two phonemes /im/. Timeout was ap- first 10 min of the first session, the subject had
plied after the next /im/ response and re- correctly emitted /dog/ approximately 30
mained in effect until the child emitted /rim/. times. After the first 10 min of Session 1, he
When he said /rim/ the experimenter turned stopped imitating /dog/. At this time, he re-
to face him, presented the model, and rein- verted to emitting the two-phoneme chain
forced /rim/ with ice cream and verbal ap- /og/ each time the stimulus was presented.
proval. As shown in Fig. 3, the child correctly After 8 min of incorrect responses, timeout for
imitated five /rim/ presentations, and then /og/ was applied. After approximately 3 min
reverted once more to the /im/ response. of timeout, the subject said, /dog/ and the
Therefore, a second timeout period was insti- experimenter turned to face him. The experi-
tuted after 5 min. After timeout had been in menter presented the word /dog/ and a correct
effect for 2.5 min, the child said /rim/. The response was reinforced. As shown in Fig. 4,
experimenter turned to face him and delivered the subject quickly increased the frequency of
a reinforcement for correctly imitating /rim/. imitating /dog/. In the next session, he
The frequency of /rim/ responses continued reached criterion and progressed to a new
to increase while /im/ responses decreased. phase of training.
Another application of timeout for incorrect Timeout contingent on inappropriate re-
responses occurred during training of the word sponses also was used in training the subject to
"dog". Results are presented in Fig. 4. Time- emit a three-word chain. The subject had
out in this situation was contingent on emis- learned to emit "want pop" and the third item
sion of the response /og/ when /dog/ was the in the chain, "I", was added to the response.
204 LEIJA V. McREYNOLDS
The model presented by the experimenter con- sions and progressed to the next step in train-
sisted of "I want pop". Results of the fourth ing.
application of timeout for incorrect responses
are presented in Fig. 5.
DISCUSSION
a 110 Timeout contingent on emission of jargon
z
successfully decreased vocalizations when ex-
0

IL
SO
90 tinction (ignoring the jargon) was ineffective
a
so
a
for this purpose. Although timeout did not
I- 70
eliminate the jargon for the remainder of the
0 50
program, it reduced vocalizations across sev-
40 eral sessions to a point at which the jargon no
30 longer interfered with training. Subsequently,
I-I 20
when jargon again increased in frequency,
a
Uj 10
timeout effectively reduced it within a short
0 a a I I 2 *a I I I
time.
0 10 20 30 40 50 G0 70 50 90
| SESSION I SESSION 2 I SESSION 3 1 The present results are similar to those ob-
CUMULATIVE MINUTES tained by Risley and Wolf (1967) in a training
program in which timeout functioned to de-
Fig. 5. Cumulative number of correct responses over
three sessions when timeout (T.O.) was used to increase celerate crying, temper tantrums, and inappro-
the probability of emission of "I want pop". Each dot priate imitative vocalizations. The imitative
represents 2 min. vocalizations were somewhat similar to the jar-
gon vocalizations emitted by the child in the
In the initial 18 min of Session 1, the sub- present study. In both studies, timeout con-
ject had correctly emitted the three-item chain sisted of the experimenter remaining in the
approximately 30 times, but the responses al- room and turning away from the child. Ter-
ternated between "I want pop", and "want mination of timeout was contingent on the
pop". At the end of the session, all his re- child's ceasing the inappropriate behavior.
sponses consisted of the two-word chain. The Vocalizations for the children in the two
same response, "want pop", continued to be studies decreased in frequency within the ses-
emitted in the initial 10 min of Session 2, so sion in which timeout was applied, and re-
timeout was applied. After 2 min had elapsed, mained close to zero throughout the following
the subject said, "I want pop". The experi- sessions.
menter turned in her chair and presented the In the present study, the probability that a
model "I want pop". The two-word chain de- specific phoneme chain would be emitted was
creased in frequency and emission of the three- increased when timeout was used as negative
word chain increased. By the end of the third reinforcement in situations in which positive
session, he was emitting the three-word chain reinforcement and extinction had been ineffec-
at criterion level and training for that phase tive. Timeout, however, was used for this pur-
was terminated. pose only when the child had demonstrated
In general, timeout contingent on emission that the response was in his repertoire, and
of incorrect responses resulted in a rapid in- when the problem was one of increasing the
crease in emission of the complete chain of frequency of emission, rather than shaping the
phonemes or words with more precise articu- topography of the response. When the child
lation on each phoneme. Timeout for incor- had emitted the correct response at approxi-
rect responses was applied on four occasions. mately a 30 to 40% level during a session, it
On each occasion after timeout was terminated was concluded that the response was present
contingent on the emission of a correct re- and available to the child.
sponse, the number of correct responses in- Timeout has been successfully used to de-
creased rapidly. Conversely, the number of in- celerate emission of incorrect responses by
correct responses rapidly decreased and were Sloane, Johnston, and Harris (1968). They did
gradually eliminated from the child's reper- not, however, use timeout as a negative rein-
toire. Usually the child reached criterion in forcer. That is, their procedure consisted of
that phase of training within the next two ses- the teacher dropping her head on the chest,
TIMEOUT AND SPEECH-TRAINING EFFICIENCY 205

remaining mute, and averting her eyes for 60 responses because they have a greater proba-
sec if the child made two consecutive incorrect bility of emission when other behaviors are not
responses or unsatisfactory approximations. occurring.
Termination of timeout, however, was con- The present study replicated the findings of
tingent on passage of time in their study, not other studies in which timeout functioned to
on the emission of a particular correct re- reduce inappropriate behavior during speech
sponse by the child. training. In addition, it demonstrated that the
The procedures used in the two studies in- specific speech response being trained could be
creased the frequency of correct responses. In required to terminate timeout in speech train-
the Sloane et al. (1968) study, timeout func- ing. In some phases of training, timeout would
tioned as a punishing event to decelerate incor- appear to have greater value for functioning
rect responses. As the incorrect responses de- as negative reinforcement to increase directly
creased, the frequency of the correct response the frequency of the correct response, rather
increased, but emission of the correct response than as a punishing event that "clears the
was not required to terminate timeout. way" for correct responses to occur.
In the present study, the behavior that ter-
minated the aversive stimulus was specified as
the emission of the correct response. In this REFERENCES
capacity, timeout functioned as a negative re- Baer, D. M. Laboratory control of thumbsucking by
inforcer to increase the emission of the correct withdrawal and representation of reinforcement.
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior,
response as well as a punisher to decrease in- 1962, 5, 525-528.
correct responses. Once the required response Risley, T. The establishment of verbal behavior in
had occurred, timein was instituted. With the deviant children. Ph.D. dissertation, University of
resumption of training, emission of the correct Washington, 1966.
Risley, T. and Wolf, M. M. Establishing functional
response was maintained by positive reinforc- speech in echolalic children. Behavior Research and
ers. Therapy, 1967, 5, 73-88.
Sloane, H., Johnston, Margaret, and Harris, Florence.
Remedial procedures for teaching verbal behavior
CONCLUSIONS to speech deficient or defective young children. In
H. N. Sloane and B. D. MacAulay (Eds.), Operant
Timeout from positive reinforcement in procedures in remedial speech and language train-
speech training has been used primarily as a ing. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Co., 1968. Pp. 77-101.
decelerating consequence for inappropriate Wolf, M. M., Risley, T., and Mees, H. Application of
behavior, not directly to increase emission of operant conditioning procedures to the behavior
problems of an autistic child. Behavior Research and
the appropriate speech response. As a punish- Therapy, 1964, 1, 305-312.
ing event, it functions to eliminate behaviors
that are incompatible with, or interfere with Received 2 October 1967.
training. Elimination of inappropriate behav- (Revised 18 December 1968.)
iors indirectly results in an increase in correct

You might also like