You are on page 1of 8

Computers in Human Behavior 93 (2019) 106–113

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Computers in Human Behavior


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh

Full length article

A huggable communication medium can provide sustained listening support T


for special needs students in a classroom
Junya Nakanishia,b,∗, Hidenobu Sumiokab, Hiroshi Ishiguroa,b
a
Intelligent Robotics Laboratory, Graduated School of Engineering Science, Osaka University, 1–1 Yamadaoka, Suita, Osaka, 565-0871, Japan
b
Hiroshi Ishiguro Laboratory, Advanced Telecommunication Research Institute International, 2-2-2, Hikaridai, Seikacho, Sorakugun, Kyoto, 619-0237, Japan

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Poor listening ability is a serious problem for pupils who suffer from a wide range of developmental disabilities.
A huggable communication medium Although recent advances in technology provide such pupils with supportive systems that improve educational
Classroom listening acoustic environments, they do not support their mental stress while they are listening in classrooms. Our
Students with special needs current study proposes a huggable communication medium to reduce mental stress of listeners in addition to
An emotional support
preparing a better acoustic environment. We investigated its impact on pupils with special needs and long-term
A sustained support
use. Special needs pupils listened to their teachers or classmates in a general listening situation and in another
situation where they listened through a huggable communication medium called Hugvie for three months. We
used a memory test to evaluate listening performances and questionnaires about “intention to use” to evaluate
acceptance to Hugvie for long-term use. The results showed that listening through Hugvies improved the scores
of memory tests about information provided by teachers. In particular, the scores of distracted pupils with
emotional problems tended to greatly improve. The improvement of their memory test scores continued for three
months. The pupils' impressions of Hugvies also indicated their preference for long-term use. These findings
suggest that a huggable communication medium can support the classroom listening of special needs pupils in
their daily school life.

1. Introduction Hogg-Johnson, & Tannock, 2003). Thus, listening support is crucial for
education.
Listening is a crucial skill for human growth and development for Previous approaches to such support included improvement of such
success in school because such educational instructions as teaching and poor sound situations as loud noise or large distances between teachers
coaching are generally provided verbally in educational environments. and pupils. Pupils' listening is distracted by noises that are louder than
For example, 66% of the duration of German primary school classes and the normal voice levels of many teachers in classrooms (Finitzo-Hieber,
53% of U.S. college classes are consumed by students listening to tea- 1988; Ross, Maxon, & Brackett, 1982). Poor sound environments drown
chers/professors (Bohlken, 1999; Imhof & Weinhard, 2004). Un- out the speaker's voice and distract the listener's attention from it. A
surprisingly, many studies have reported that poor listening skills ne- sound field amplification system (SFA) was developed to improve
gatively influence reading, writing, and math performances (Bennett, educational environments for listening. SFA resembles a loudspeaker
Ruuska, & Sherman, 1980; Elliott, Hammer, & Scholl, 1989; that projects a teacher's voice and raises its level relative to the levels of
McClelland, Morrison, & Holmes, 2000). other sound sources. SFA minimizes the impact of poor classroom
However, young pupils are often unable to listen well during class acoustics to provide a better opportunity for students to clearly hear the
because of their immature self-control skills. They are learning the skill instructions of teachers. Many studies have reported SFA's wide-ranging
of continuing to focus on the speaker's voice, and their attention is positive impacts not only for pupils with typical development but also
easily attracted by other stimuli. In particular, it is difficult for pupils for those with special needs (Millett, 2008).
with special needs (i.e., developmental disabilities and disorders) to We focused on another factor, mental stress, which impairs im-
listen well in classrooms since their listening is further handcuffed by mature pupils' self-control ability for listening, that includes fear and
their disabilities (Dahle & McCollister, 1986; McInnes, Humphries, anxiety. Mental stress influences our self-control ability, which is the


Corresponding author. Intelligent Robotics Laboratory, Graduated School of Engineering Science, Osaka University, Machikaneyama-cho, Toyonaka, Osaka, 560-
0043, Japan.
E-mail addresses: nakanishi@irl.sys.es.osaka-u.ac.jp (J. Nakanishi), sumioka@atr.jp (H. Sumioka), ishiguro@sys.es.osaka-u.ac.jp (H. Ishiguro).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.10.008
Received 10 June 2017; Received in revised form 21 September 2018; Accepted 2 October 2018
Available online 03 October 2018
0747-5632/ © 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
J. Nakanishi et al. Computers in Human Behavior 93 (2019) 106–113

critical ability to listen and maintain attention to a speaker's voice 2.2. Poor listening skills with special needs pupils
(Baumeister & Heatherton, 1996). Fear and anxiety make it difficult for
people to control themselves and concentrate on speakers (Dalman, Pupils with special needs often have difficulty listening. Actually,
2016; Elkhafaifi, 2005; Vogely, 1998). This is a serious problem for poor listening ability is a serious problem for pupils with a wide range
young pupils who are more prone to stress or anxiety in their school of developmental disabilities, such as attention deficit hyperactivity
environments, relationships with classmates, and lessons. They also disorder (ADHD), Apert Syndrome, and anxiety disorders (Bennetts &
have difficulty handling it due to immature self-control (Fabian & Flynn, 2002; Dahle & McCollister, 1986; Flexer, 1990; Leung, 2006;
Dunlop, 2007; Wong, 2015). In fact, support for mental stress has Maag, 2006, 2007; McInnes et al. 2003). For example, ADHD is a
garnered attention in educational curricula (Brigman & Webb, 2003; neurodevelopmental type of mental disorder that is characterized by
Denham, 2006; Raver & Knitzer, 2002; Thompson & Raikes, 2007; difficulty paying attention or controlling behaviors that are in-
Webster-Stratton & Reid, 2004). Thus, both sound environment pro- appropriate for a person's age (American Psychiatric Association,
blems and mental stress problems must be addressed to improve lis- 2013). Anxiety disorders are mental disorders that share the features of
tening. excessive fear and anxiety and related behavioral disturbances
We previously proposed a huggable communication medium called (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).
Hugvie, which is designed to provide users a hugging experience and Such attentional and emotional problems interrupt the self-control
verbal tele-communication. Hugvie is also an effective device to support of attention to listening. Attentional problems cause failure to control
listening (Nakanishi, Sumioka, & Ishiguro, 2016). Since such a medium one's attention to listening. Emotional problems, such as excessive an-
can transmit a voice with sufficient volume and provide a stress re- xiety or fear, make pupils more often pay attention to mental stress at
duction effect (Nakanishi, Kuwamura, Minato, Nishio, & Ishiguro, the expense of listening. Therefore, the listening of special needs pupils
2013; Sumioka, Nakae, Kanai, & Ishiguro, 2013), it might simulta- is interfered with more than that of pupils with typical development
neously solve the above problems. In fact, our previous case studies and should be supported by controlling their attention during listening.
showed that a storytelling system, which consists of Hugvie and a radio
broadcasting system, had a more calming effect on the behavior of 2.3. Stress reduction of interpersonal and mediated touches
preschool pupils than regular storytelling in classrooms (Nakanishi
et al., 2016). Such interpersonal touch as shaking hands, stroking, hugging, and
However, no empirical studies have examined the educational effect kissing has the potential to relieve mental stress. For example, holding
of a huggable communication medium with special needs pupils. It also hands or tapping another's forearm can reduce anxiety and promote
remains unclear whether such a medium can continue to support young calm (Feldman, Singer, & Zagoory, 2010). Gentle stroking and touching
pupils throughout long-term use. Therefore, this paper focused on and can reduce stress hormones, which are proportional to the degree of
investigated its impact on pupils with special needs and long-term use. psychological stress (Ditzen et al., 2007; Whitcher & Fisher, 1979).
We conducted a case study where special needs pupils used a huggable Since people empirically know the impact of interpersonal touch on
communication medium during their morning meetings in an elemen- psychological well-being, it has been employed in psychotherapy
tary school. (Phelan, 2009) and nursing (Anderson & Taylor, 2011; Gleeson &
Timmins, 2005). Stress reduction by touch is also valid for special needs
2. Theoretical framework pupils (Cullen, 2005; Polak, 2017). Interestingly, some studies report
that children with autism and ADHD also benefit from massage therapy
2.1. Listening skill and its relation to memory and self-control and show less inattentive in classrooms (Field, 2010), implying poten-
tial effect of touch on attentional problems. These results show that
According to a skills-based scheme of listening-centered commu- interpersonal touch can reduce the negative influence of attentional and
nication called the HURIER model (Wolvin, 2010), listening skills are emotional problems on people with disabilities.
divided into six processes: hearing, interpreting, understanding, eval- Recent studies with tele-communication technology have argued
uating, remembering, and responding. Hearing is the entrance of lis- that touch mediated by artificial systems also has a stress reduction
tening, in which a person makes decisions about which acoustic sti- effect that resembles interpersonal touch (Huisman, 2017). A mediated
mulus to focus on. Interpreting, understanding, and evaluating overlap hand-touch against a part of an arm reduced the heartbeat rates of
and are related to the analysis of acoustic stimulus in which a person participants after they watched a sad video clip (Cabibihan, Zheng, &
grasps the literal meaning, context, or validity of the information re- Cher, 2012). Even though this study emulated touch with hands, touch
ceived with nonverbal cues. In the remembering process, we store the with a hug has also been focused on in research of mediated inter-
analyzed stimuli that are used for our responses, either immediately or personal touch. Many virtual hug devices have been developed. In the
at some later point. The listener's response, which is the final compo- “Hug Machine,” which seems to be the first prototype of a haptic sti-
nent of the listening process, is influenced by the five processes that mulator of a hug (Grandin, 1984), a user was positioned between two,
precede it. foam rubber boards and firmly squeezed, producing soothing and
This model suggests that we can evaluate listening performances by comforting reactions. Another study showed that this machine reduced
assessing how much the listener remembers what s/he received or how stress in autistic children (Edelson, Edelson, & Kerr, 1999). “The Hug” is
properly s/he responds to what s/he received. Actually, previous stu- a robotic communication medium that reflects the shape and the ges-
dies assessed listener's memory or responses to evaluate whether lis- tures of a human hug to explore a conceptual design of a robotic pro-
tening skills improved (Dahle & McCollister, 1986; Dalman, 2016; duct that facilitates intimate communication across distances (DiSalvo,
Elkhafaifi, 2005; McInnes et al., 2003; Millett, 2008; Vogely, 1998). Gemperle, Forlizzi, & Montgomery, 2003). A hug jacket produced a
Although the HURIER model is based on a behavioral perspective, tactile stimulation of hugging with pressure or vibrators controlled by a
within the framework of cognitive psychology, listening is referred to as remote person's input (Mueller et al., 2005, pp. 1673–1676; Morikawa,
an intentional and controlled process that requires attentional capacity Hashimoto, Munakata, & Okunaka, 2006; Teh et al., 2008). As de-
and self-control (Schneider & Shiffrin, 1977; Shiffrin & Schneider, scribed above, Hugvie is a huggable communication medium that is
1977). This implies that listening is interrupted when the control of the designed to give users a hugging experience during remote conversa-
attention fails due to external or internal attractors, such as environ- tions (Minato, Nishio, & Ishiguro, 2013). While other virtual hug de-
mental acoustic noise and mental stress. In fact, noise and anxiety ne- vices do not conduct quantitative evaluation, it is reported that Hugvie
gatively impact attention control (Sanz, 1993; Eysenck et al., 2007) and reduces stress hormones and feelings of depression, hostility, and fa-
listening (Dalman, 2016; Elkhafaifi, 2005; Klatte, 2013; Vogely, 1998). tigue during remote conversations with it (Nakanishi et al., 2013;

107
J. Nakanishi et al. Computers in Human Behavior 93 (2019) 106–113

Sumioka et al., 2013). Therefore, we assume that the effect of a


mediated interpersonal touch can be applied to emotional problems
about listening; that is, mediated interpersonal touch reduces mental
stress that distracts the pupils' attention during listening tasks or si-
tuations.

2.4. Listening support for special needs pupils

Previous studies with SFA addressed the improvement of sound


environments in the classroom not only for the listening of pupils with
typical development but also those with special needs. For example,
SFA decreased pupils' errors on a word identification task (Flexer,
1990) and improved the speed with which pupils followed their tea-
cher's instructions (Flexer, 1990; Maag, 2006, 2007). However, this Fig. 1. Hugvie: a huggable communication medium.
approach ignores pupils with listening difficulties due to emotional
problems. We expect Hugvie to support those with attentional and
emotional problems because it helps people focus on the teacher's voice use, enjoyment of use, and ease of use. Finally, based on the results, we
and reduces mental stress. discussed the possibilities of a storytelling system with a huggable
Although our previous case studies identified the impact of a hug- communication medium for educational environments.
gable communication medium on pupils with typical development
(Nakanishi et al., 2016), the impact on special needs pupils remains 3. Method
unclear. We expect that a huggable communication medium will sup-
port the listening of pupils with attentional and emotional problems 3.1. Apparatus
because its functions intrinsically support such pupils. Furthermore, our
previous studies reported only observations of hearing behavior in the Hugvie is a human-shaped cushion (75-cm high, weighing 600 g)
HURIER model; that is, they did not evaluate pupil memories or lis- that was designed as a communication device to give a hugging ex-
tening response with a quantitative measurement. Since pupils might perience (Fig. 1). It is a soft cushion filled with polystyrene microbeads
pretend to listen or pay attention to tactile stimulation, we have to and covered with spandex fiber. A hands-free mobile phone inside a
confirm whether they are actually focusing on the listening contents pocket in its head enables users to talk while hugging it. Although
and remembering them while using Hugvie. Thus, we formulated the Hugvie was originally designed for one-to-one interactive commu-
following hypothesis: nication (e.g., Sumioka et al., 2013), we introduced it into one-to-many
communication in combination with an FM radio broadcasting system
H1. A huggable communication medium improve the memory of the
(Nakanishi et al., 2016). We used the same system as the one from our
special needs pupils about the listened information.
previous study: a storytelling system that combined Hugvie with an FM
radio broadcasting system. The broadcasting system carries the sound
2.5. Long-term use of educational situations information to an unspecific number of receivers by radio waves. The
storytelling system with Hugvie conveys a speaker's voice to listeners
One concern about applying a huggable communication medium for with a radio receiver inside its pocket instead of a mobile phone. Fig. 2
pupils is whether its effect persists throughout long-term use, that is shows an overview of the storytelling system with Hugvie. A human
more than several times use. Although we expect that a huggable speaker talks to listeners by a microphone connected to an FM radio
communication medium will support the listening of special needs transmitter. Listeners using Hugvies listen to the same speaker's voice
pupils, we have not utilized it for long-term daily listening in class- near their ears by each Hugvie radio receiver. In this study, we used our
rooms. Long-term trials are important to introduce new devices because storytelling system with a Hugvie for special needs pupils who si-
long-term use might reduce the intention to use them due to decreased multaneously listen to their teachers or classmates. Note that the pupils
interest in their novelty, especially for young pupils (e.g., Kanda, Sato, can also directly listen to the teacher's voice since both are in the same
Saiwaki, & Ishiguro, 2007; Komatsubara, Shiomi, Kanda, Ishiguro, & classroom. A Hugvie voice is the strongest stimuli among other sounds
Hagita, 2014). Less intention to use might reduce the performance because an experimenter can adjust its volume in advance.
improvements produced by the devices.
Previous studies on interpersonal touch show that it enhances such 3.2. Participants
intimacy as social bonding or trust between people or groups (Henley,
1973). For instance, social touch enhances the performance of basket- Our participants were seven, six-to eleven-year-old elementary
ball teams by building cooperation (Kraus, Huang, & Keltner, 2010). school boys and girls (mean age = 8.7 years, SD = 1.9, Table 1) with
Since using a huggable communication medium produces a similar ef-
fect to interpersonal touch, a user can enhance intimacy for the medium
without getting tired of using it, like a child who plays with a stuffed
animal for a long time. Thus, we propose:
H2. H1 persists over long-term use.

2.6. Current study

Our current study addressed the above two hypotheses through


long-term use of Hugvies in the morning meetings for special needs
students in an elementary school. We tackled H1 by evaluating their
listening abilities with a memory test. H2 was evaluated based on the
stability of improved listening and questionnaires about intention to Fig. 2. Hugvie storytelling system.

108
J. Nakanishi et al. Computers in Human Behavior 93 (2019) 106–113

Table 1 pupils held regular meetings and ten times as a Hugvie condition where
Participant characteristics. the pupils listened to their teachers by Hugvies. The two conditions
Grade in elementary school Gender Main problem were held alternately with an intra-individual design.
The morning meeting included a greeting, attendance, self-reports
1 Male Intellectual from the pupils about their mental and physical conditions, setting the
1 Male Intellectual
daily goal to be achieved by all pupils (goal-setting activity, hereafter),
3 Male Emotional
5 Male Emotional
a brief recounting of a recent event by one of the pupils (speech ac-
5 Female Intellectual tivity), and the day's news from the teachers (information-telling ac-
5 Female Intellectual tivity). In the goal-setting activity, pupils voted on the daily goals of the
6 Male Emotional class. The chairperson pupil of the morning meeting asked for each
pupil's opinion and listed them on the blackboard, and then common
goals were decided by a majority vote. The selected goals included
mild disabilities and listening problems who attend a class for special
being on time, studying hard, or not running in the corridors. In the
needs pupils in Kyoto. Both their parents and teachers gave them per-
case of ties, all of the tied opinions were chosen. In the speech activity,
mission to attend the special class in agreement that was concluded
a pupil related a recent memorable event or experience to the rest of the
through a discussion based on the pupils' examination results of their
class. Then the others asked questions about it. In information-telling
emotion or intelligence and their behavior in school. Three of the seven
activity, each teacher shared important information that the pupils
were judged to mainly suffer from emotional problems, and the re-
should remember that day or for several days. Examples of such in-
maining four had intellectual problems. Their emotional problems in-
formation included committee activities, health concerns, and re-
cluded struggling to remain patient during long listening and judging
quirements for the next day's activities. In the Hugvie condition, the
the feelings of others. Their intellectual problems included intellectual
pupils listened to all of the spoken contents through it. At the afternoon
levels that greatly lagged behind the average level of the same year in
meetings that are regularly held at around 3 p.m. before they go home,
the school. Although the pupils gathered in their special class classroom
the pupils took a memory test (described below) about the contents of
during morning and afternoon meetings, they attended general or
the morning meeting: goal-setting, speech, and information-telling ac-
special classes of each subject area based on the progress of their in-
tivities. After all the experiments were finished, they filled out a
dividual developmental states. The special classes occupied more than
questionnaire about intention to use, enjoyment of use, and ease of use.
half of their daily schedules. Three special class teachers participated as
We also interviewed a class teacher about the pupils' behavior in the
speakers.
experiment after all of them were finished.
This study was approved by the ethics committee of the Advanced
Telecommunication Research Institute International (approved
3.3. Experimental design
number: 14-502-8). We also received permission from the elementary
school to conduct an experiment for research purposes.
The experiments were conducted from late November 2014 to the
middle of February 2015 except for the New Year's holidays and several
4. Measurement and analysis
days that were excluded due to various experimenter or school cir-
cumstances. Hugvies were introduced into the morning meetings that
4.1. Memory test
are regularly held on weekday mornings in the special needs classroom
(Fig. 3). The pupils sat in their assigned seats at the “P” places. Three
We evaluated whether the listening of the pupils improved with a
teachers talked to them somewhere in the “T” places. Before the ex-
memory test about the spoken contents of each morning meeting. It
periments started, the subject pupils were instructed how to use Hug-
consisted of three questions about the goal-settings, speech, and in-
vies, and then they listened to a 10-min story by Hugvies as a trial. The
formation-telling activities: (a) “What was/were today's goal/goals?”;
Hugvies remained in the classroom during the experiments, and the
(b) “What did [pupil X] talk about today?”; and (c) “What information
pupils could freely interact with them during their free time, although
did [teacher Y] give us today?” This multiple choice test included both
no radio receiver was inserted. The experiments were recorded through
correct and incorrect answers. The pupils could choose one or more
a video camera. We observed the morning meetings twenty times with/
answers from eight options for question (a) and one answer from five
without Hugvies: ten times as a general condition where teachers and
options for questions (b) and (c). The options included a “Can't re-
member” choice. The correct and incorrect options were made by an
experimenter who attended the morning meeting. Before the memory
test was conducted, the teachers confirmed that the content had no such
problem for pupils and the school as hurtful or rude options.
The memory performance of each pupil was evaluated with average
accuracy scores for ten days. We calculated them by counting the
number of correct answers and dividing it by the correct answers. In
most cases, the score of each question on each day was zero or one
because the number of the original correct answers was only one, but in
some cases for question (a), the number of original correct answers
exceeded one. In the case, we calculated the score with a formula: the
number of correct answers divided by the number of original correct
answers. For example, when a pupil chose only one of two original
correct answers, he/she only received a half score, namely 0.5. No data
were recorded when a pupil was absent. When a pupil chaired a
morning meeting in the goal-setting activity and the Hugvie condition,
his/her score for question (a) was excluded from the evaluation because
he/she did not use a Hugvie due to writing on the blackboard. When a
Fig. 3. Classroom environment during morning meetings. Note. Three teachers pupil was giving a brief talk in the speech activity, his/her score of
talk to pupils somewhere in the “T” places. question (b) was also excluded because s/he was not a listener. Each

109
J. Nakanishi et al. Computers in Human Behavior 93 (2019) 106–113

Table 2
Questionnaire details about intention to use, enjoyment of use, and ease of use.
Intention to use I want to continue using Hugvie at the morning meetings.
I want to use Hugvie in the school lessons.
I want to use Hugvie at home.
I want to use Hugvie every day.
Enjoyment of use I enjoyed listening by Hugvie.
I am interested in Hugvie.
I was bored with Hugvie.
I had fun in the morning meetings with Hugvie.
Ease of use Hugging Hugvie was easy.
I easily understood how to use Hugvie.
Listening by Hugvie was easy.
Using Hugvie was easy.

pupil's average scores of each question in each condition were calcu-


lated and compared between the general and Hugvie conditions.
Furthermore, for the questions that showed significant improvement,
we also compared the average scores of the first- and second-halves
among the pupils in the Hugvie condition to evaluate the stability of
their improved listening.
Fig. 4. Average accuracy rates scores in each memory question of each condi-
4.2. Questionnaire about intention to use, enjoyment of use, and ease of use tion. Note. “x” mark denotes the mean value of scores.

After all the experimental morning meetings were finished, the


SD = 0.17, Median(Mdn) = 0.76, Q1 = 0.76, Q3 = 0.92, Hugvie:
pupils filled out a questionnaire about their intention to use, enjoyment
M = 0.79, SD = 0.15, Mdn = 0.75, Q1 = 0.69, Q3 = 0.90; Speech:
of use, and ease of use (see Iwamura, Shiomi, Kanda, Ishiguro, & Hagita,
General: M = 0.79, SD = 0.34, Mdn = 0.76, Q1 = 0.76, Q3 = 1.00,
2011) to evaluate their acceptance of Hugvies for long-term use. For
Hugvie: M = 0.72, SD = 0.26, Mdn = 0.78, Q1 = 0.52, Q3 = 0.94;
accepting and using new technology for a long term, intention to use is
Informative telling: General: M = 0.57, SD = 0.20, Mdn = 0.56,
critical (Davis, 1993). Enjoyment of use and ease of use are related to
Q1 = 0.39, Q3 = 0.72, Hugvie: M = 0.75, SD = 0.20, Mdn = 0.80,
the intention to use factor (Heerink, Kröse, Evers, & Wielinga, 2008).
Q1 = 0.62, Q3 = 0.89). The scores in the information-telling activity
Therefore, the questionnaire results examined the effects of the users'
were significantly improved by Hugvie (t(6) = 2.65,
acceptance over the long-term use of Hugvies.
p = 0.038 < 0.05, ES: d = 0.89), but no significant differences were
The questionnaires consisted of four questions in each category with
shown among the general and Hugvie conditions in the goal-setting and
a five-point Likert scale from 1 to 5: “strongly disagree,” “slightly dis-
speech activities (Setting goals: t(6) = -0.30, p = 0.77, Speech: Z
agree,” “neither,” “slightly agree,” and “strongly agree” (Table 2). The
(6) = 4, p = 0.42). Scrutinizing the individual scores of the informa-
score of the third question, “I was bored with Hugvie,” in the enjoyment
tion-telling activity, higher improvements (approximately double) of
of use was inverted because the question's meaning is contrary to the
two pupils with emotional problems are shown in the Hugvie condition
category. Although the original questionnaire consists of a seven point
compared with the general condition.
Likert scale, we used a five point scale because of teachers' concern that
Fig. 5 shows the average scores of the information-telling activity in
a seven point scale is difficult for young pupils to answer.
the first and second-halves of this experiment in the Hugvie condition
for evaluating the stability of the listening improvements (First half:
4.3. Teacher interviews M = 0.68, SD = 0.30, Mdn = 0.68, Q1 = 0.68, Q3 = 0.80; Second half:
M = 0.82, SD = 0.20, Mdn = 0.80, Q1 = 0.78, Q3 = 1.00). No sig-
We interviewed the class teachers about the pupils' behavior in the nificant differences are shown between the average scores of the two
experiment after it finished to gather opinions from a professional
viewpoint about the school life of the pupils. We asked them to describe
how calm their pupils were while they listened to confirm the stress
reduction effect of Hugvies.

4.4. Statistical analysis

As a memory test, we used a paired t-test or a Wilcoxon signed-rank


test to evaluate the differences of the average scores. In the case of the
normality that was confirmed with a Shapiro-Wilk normality test, we
used a paired t-test. Otherwise, a Wilcoxon signed-rank test was used.
For the questionnaire about intention to use, we used a one-sample t-
test to evaluate the difference from a middle value (value = 3) that
indicates a neutral opinion to Hugvie.

5. Results

5.1. Improvement of memory performance

Individual average scores of each memory question of each condi- Fig. 5. Average accuracy rates scores in first and second halves of this experi-
tion are shown in Fig. 4 (Setting goals: General: Mean(M) = 0.81, ment. Note. “x” mark denotes the mean value of scores.

110
J. Nakanishi et al. Computers in Human Behavior 93 (2019) 106–113

probably explains why improvement was only observed in the in-


formation-telling activity. Thus, we argue that Hugvies effectively
supported the listening of special needs pupils when the provided
contents are hard to remember.
Therefore, the memory test results support hypothesis 1: a huggable
communication medium improves the memory of the listened in-
formation for special needs pupils. Although our previous studies only
confirmed improved hearing behavior with a huggable communication
medium (Nakanishi et al., 2016), this study demonstrated that pupils
using a huggable communication medium paid more attention to the
listening contents and remembered more of them in the general con-
dition. The impact of a huggable communication medium is valid not
only for pupils with typical development but also for special needs
pupils.

6.2. Stable acceptance to Hugvies in long-term use

The stability of the improvement caused by Hugvies is shown by


comparing the memory test scores of the information-telling activity
between the first- and second-half average scores in the Hugvie con-
dition. The pupils maintained their improved performance that was
Fig. 6. Average questionnaire scores in each category. Note. “x” mark denotes produced by the devices. In long-term field trials with a humanoid
the mean value of scores.
robot, pupils tended to get tired of it with decreased novelty after about
two weeks (Kanda et al., 2007; Komatsubara et al., 2014). Considering
halves among the pupils (Z(6) = 2, p = 0.34). those results, stability over approximately three months suggests that
Hugvie might provide continuous support in more long-term use.
5.2. Acceptance of Hugvies for long-term use Relatively good perceptions and impressions of Hugvies are shown
by the questionnaire results about intention to use, enjoyment of use,
The average scores in each category of the questionnaire are shown and ease of use. In studies with a humanoid robot, most of the ques-
in Fig. 6 (Intention to use: M = 4.21, SD = 0.92, Mdn = 4.75, tionnaire scores of the experimental conditions showed over half of the
Q1 = 3.88, Q3 = 4.88; Enjoyment of use: M = 4.43, SD = 0.56, max scores while those of the control conditions showed around less
Mdn = 4.50, Q1 = 4.00, Q3 = 5.00; Ease of use: M = 4.64, SD = 0.42, than half of the scores (Iwamura et al., 2011; Kanda, Shimada, &
Mdn = 5.00, Q1 = 4.25, Q3 = 5.00). Each score was significantly Koizumi, 2012; Shiomi & Hagita, 2015). Although we cannot directly
higher than the middle value (value = 3) by a t-test (Intension to use: t compare our results with these studies since we used simpler scales and
(6) = 3.23, p = 0.017 < 0.05, Enjoyment of use: t(6) = 6.22, no guarantee confirms that the Hugvie scores are sufficiently practical,
p = 8.0 × 10−4 < 0.05, Ease of use: t(6) = 9.59, p = 7.3 × 10−5 < the scores suggest at least a positive influence on users' perceptions and
0.05). This result indicates that pupils had relatively good perceptions impressions due to long-term use of Hugvies.
and impressions of Hugvies in each category. These results support hypothesis 2: H1 persists in long-term use. We
confirmed that improvement was maintained and that pupils continued
5.3. Teacher interviews to accept a huggable communication medium. Our finding suggests the
potential of haptic interaction, which plays an important role for
In an interview, one class teacher commented, “Pupils using maintaining the preference of users to communication devices. This
Hugvies appeared to be calmer than usual. Hugging may be good for finding is supported by Harlow's experiments with infant macaques
avoiding boredom while listening.” This teacher added: “They made raised by artificial surrogate mothers. Harlow emphasized the im-
huge efforts while listening through Hugvies.” The teachers did not portance of contact comfort in the development of affectional responses
report any pupil behavior that indicated either excited or stressful states (Harlow, 1958). A previous study with a social robot also supports our
in the Hugvie condition. result, suggesting the importance of touch interactions with a robot to
achieve long-term bonding and socialization between the robot and
6. Discussion toddlers (Tanaka, Cicurel, Movellan, & Javier, 2007).

6.1. Improvement of memory performance of special needs pupils through 6.3. Impact of stress reduction on listening and possibilities
Hugvie
We assume that huggable communication improves listening
The memory test results show that the memory performance of the through stress reduction in addition to preparing a better acoustic en-
pupils improved more when they listened to the information by Hugvies vironment. But, is that supposition actually valid in our case study? We
than direct telling. Hugvie provided a better opportunity for them to argue that the following reasons mention the contribution of a stress
concentrate on listening to the teacher's instructions. reduction effect. First, in this experiment, the distance between the
On the other hand, we did not find any significant improvements in pupils and the teachers in the general condition seemed so short that
the goal-setting and speech activities. We infer that this result was their voices were louder than other sound sources (Fig. 3). Teachers
caused by the ceiling effect. Most pupils showed almost perfect scores in generally talked to the pupils near them. In spite of that, the pupils'
the general condition (Fig. 4). The questions about the goal-setting and listening improved in the Hugvie condition, denoting that Hugvies
speech activities might have been too easy to answer because the items supported their listening by another factor other than the effect of a
to remember were based on insufficient listening contents. For example, clear acoustic environment. Thus, we believe that in such listening si-
in setting the goals, pupils often made short comments: “I want to study tuations, Hugvie relieves emotional problems like stress and anxiety
hard.” The questions about the information-telling activity were more reduction. Second, we found that high improvements in the memory
difficult because teachers gave them too much information. This test scores (approximately double) were found in the two pupils with

111
J. Nakanishi et al. Computers in Human Behavior 93 (2019) 106–113

emotional problems. This also denotes that the stress reduction effect Technology Agency (JST), the Core Research of Evolutional Science and
improved listening. Third, in an interview teachers thought that pupils Technology (CREST) research promotion program. Part of this work
became calmer in the Hugvie conditions. From the above, we conclude was supported by JST, the Exploratory Research for Advanced
that Hugvies contributed not only to the clear conveyance of a voice but Technology (ERATO), ISHIGURO symbiotic human-robot interaction
also to the stability of the pupils' mental states while listening. This project, Grant Number JPMJER1401, JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number
indicates advances in listening problems compared with SFA. 16J05713, Japan. The authors would like to thank the staff at the
In SFA studies, the pupils' ability, which was supported by produ- elementary school and all participants (students and their parents) for
cing a clear acoustic environment, has had a wide-range of positive their cooperation.
impacts on pupil performance: accurate discrimination of words and
spoken languages, increases of verbal involvement in discussions, References
phonological awareness, responses to teacher's statements, learning
English as a Second Language, reading, writing and numeracy (Millett, American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental dis-
2008). Perhaps Hugvies can also produce these positive impacts with orders (DSM-5®). American Psychiatric Pub.
Anderson, J. G., & Taylor, A. G. (2011). Effects of healing touch in clinical practice: A
solutions for emotional problems for a wider-range of pupils including systematic review of randomized clinical trials. Journal of Holistic Nursing, 29(3),
those with special needs than SFA does. 221–228.
On the other hand, note that Hugvies face challenges before prac- Baumeister, R. F., & Heatherton, T. F. (1996). Self-regulation failure: An overview.
Psychological Inquiry, 7, 1–15.
tical introduction into educational environments, including a great deal Bennett, F. C., Ruuska, S. H., & Sherman, R. (1980). Middle ear function in learning-
of maintenance. To establish Hugvie storytelling systems, we have to disabled children. Pediatrics, 66(2), 254–260.
provide a Hugvie for every pupil, which may be complicated for tea- Bennetts, L., & Flynn, M. (2002). Improving the classroom listening skills of children with
Down syndrome by using sound-field amplification. Down Syndrome: Research and
chers. Hygiene is another issue because Hugvies get dirty very easily
Practice, 8(1), 19–24.
due to contact with users. Keeping them clean is time consuming. Such Bohlken, B. (1999). Substantiating the fact that listening is proportionately most used
maintenance costs explain one reason why Hugvies have not already language skill. The Listening Post, 70, 5.
Brigman, G. A., & Webb, L. D. (2003). Ready to learn: Teaching kindergarten students
been introduced into educational environments.
school success skills. The Journal of Educational Research, 96(5), 286–292.
Cabibihan, J. J., Zheng, L., & Cher, C. (2012). Affective tele-touch. Social Robotics,
6.4. Limitation 348–356.
Cullen, L. A., Barlow, J. H., & Cushway, D. (2005). Positive touch, the implications for
parents and their children with autism: An exploratory study. Complementary
Our study has some limitations. First, the degree of difficulty in the Therapies in Clinical Practice, 11(3), 182–189.
memory tests might have varied every day due to the different contents Dahle, A. J., & McCollister, F. P. (1986). Hearing and otologic disorders in children with
provided by the teacher. Although we counterbalanced the order of the Down syndrome. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 90(6), 636–642.
Dalman, R. M. (2016). The Relationship between listening anxiety, listening compre-
conditions, it is possible that the tests in the Hugvie conditions were hension strategies, and listening performance among Iranian EFL university students.
easier than those in the general condition. Thus, the effect of Hugvies International Journal of Modern Language Teaching and Learning, 1(6), 241–252.
on memory must be experimentally verified. Second, we failed to in- Davis, F. D. (1993). User acceptance of information technology: System characteristics, user
perceptions and behavioral impacts.
vestigate which Hugvie features contribute to our results. The same Denham, S. A. (2006). Social-emotional competence as support for school readiness: What
results might not be found with other huggable communication devices. is it and how do we assess it? Early Education and Development, 17(1), 57–89.
Hugvies resemble a soft cushion with a human-like shape in addition to DiSalvo, C., Gemperle, F., Forlizzi, J., & Montgomery, E. (2003). The hug: An exploration
of robotic form for intimate communication. Robot and human interactive commu-
a huggable communication device, which might have influenced our nication, 2003. Proceedings.ROMAN 2003. The 12th IEEE international workshop on (pp.
results. For example, a human shape lets pupils feel a human presence 403–408). IEEE.
during the hugs, which might reduce more stress. Finally, the Hugvie Ditzen, B., Neumann, I. D., Bodenmann, G., von Dawans, B., Turner, R. A., Ehlert, U.,
et al. (2007). Effects of different kinds of couple interaction on cortisol and heart rate
effect on pupils with special needs should be verified more in depth
responses to stress in women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 32(5), 565–574.
because we only had seven subjects who just suffered from mild dis- Edelson, S. M., Edelson, M. G., & Kerr, D. (1999). Physiological and behavioral changes of
abilities. We must investigate whether the same results are found in deep pressure: A pilot study investigating the efficacy of temple Grandin's hug ma-
pupils with other disabilities. chine. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 53, 145–152.
Elkhafaifi, H. (2005). Listening comprehension and anxiety in the Arabic language
classroom. The Modern Language Journal, 89(2), 206–220.
7. Conclusion Elliott, L. L., Hammer, M. A., & Scholl, M. E. (1989). Fine-grained auditory discrimination
in normal children and children with language-learning problems. Journal of Speech,
Language, and Hearing Research, 32(1), 112–119.
We addressed the following research propositions through the long- Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive
term use of a huggable communication medium called Hugvie in the performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7(2), 336.
morning meetings in an elementary school special needs class: Fabian, H., & Dunlop, A. W. (2007). Outcomes of good practice in transition processes for
children entering primary school. Working papers in early childhood development: No.
Hypothesis 1: a huggable communication medium improves the 42. The Hague: Bernard van Leer Foundation.
memory of the listened information for special needs pupils; Hypothesis Feldman, R., Singer, M., & Zagoory, O. (2010). Touch attenuates infants' physiological
2: H1 persists in long-term use. The memory test results showed that reactivity to stress. Developmental Science, 13(2), 271–278.
Field, T. (2010). Touch for socioemotional and physical well-being: A review.
Hugvies improved their listening, particularly when they needed to Developmental Review, 30, 367–383.
concentrate to listen to many contents. Distracted pupils with emo- Finitzo-Hieber, T. (1988). Classroom acoustics. In R. Roeser (Ed.). Auditory disorders in
tional problems tended to make larger improvements, which suggests school children (pp. 221–233). (2nd ed.). New York: Thieme-Stratton.
Flexer, C., Millin, J. P., & Brown, L. (1990). Children with developmental disabilities: The
that Hugvies strongly relieved emotional problems during listening si-
effect of sound field amplification on word identification. Language, Speech, and
tuations. The pupils maintained their improved performance that was Hearing Services in Schools, 21, 177–182.
produced by the devices over a three-month period. Their perceptions Gleeson, M., & Timmins, F. (2005). A review of the use and clinical effectiveness of touch
and impressions of Hugvies were positive for long-term use. Our hug- as a nursing intervention. Clinical Effectiveness in Nursing, 9(1), 69–77.
Grandin, T. (1984). My experiences as an autistic child and review of selected literature.
gable communication medium showed the possibility of supporting the Journal of Orthomolecular Psychiatry, 13(3), 144–174.
classroom listening of distracted special needs pupils for daily listening Harlow, H. F. (1958). The nature of love. American Psychologist, 13(No. 12), 673–685.
in a classroom. We believe that our result will inspire a new avenue of Heerink, M., Kröse, B., Evers, V., & Wielinga, B. J. (2008). The influence of social presence
on acceptance of a companion robot by older people. Journal of Physical Agents, 2(2),
educational support for pupils with emotional problems. 33–40.
Henley, N. M. (1973). Status and sex: Some touching observations. Bulletin of the psycho-
Acknowledgments nomic society.
Huisman, G. (2017). Social touch technology: A survey of haptic technology for social
touch. IEEE Transactions on Haptics, 10(3), 391–408.
This work was mainly supported by the Japan Science and

112
J. Nakanishi et al. Computers in Human Behavior 93 (2019) 106–113

Imhof, M., & Weinhard, T. (2004). What did you listen to in school today. 25th annual Nakanishi, J., Sumioka, H., & Ishiguro, H. (2016). Impact of mediated intimate interac-
convention of the international listening association in Ft. Myers, FL. tion on education: A huggable communication medium that encourages listening.
Iwamura, Y., Shiomi, M., Kanda, T., Ishiguro, H., & Hagita, N. (2011). Do elderly people Frontiers in Psychology, 7.
prefer a conversational humanoid as a shopping assistant partner in supermarkets? Phelan, J. E. (2009). Exploring the use of touch in the psychotherapeutic setting: A
Proceedings of the 6th international conference on human robot interaction (pp. 449– phenomenological review. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training,
456). . 46(1), 97.
Kanda, T., Sato, R., Saiwaki, N., & Ishiguro, H. (2007). A two-month field trial in an Polak, R., Hutchinson, D., Perryman, L., & Brueck, S. (2017). A calming cushion as a
elementary school for long-term human–robot interaction. IEEE Transactions on therapeutic wellness tool for youths with disabilities and history of trauma. Social
Robotics, 23(5), 962–971. Work, 62(4), 359–365.
Kanda, T., Shimada, M., & Koizumi, S. (2012). Children learning with a social robot. Raver, C. C., & Knitzer, J. (2002). Ready to enter: What research tells policymakers about
Proceedings of the 7th international conference on human robot interaction (pp. 351– strategies to promote social and emotional school readiness among three-and four-year-old
358). . children. New York, NY: National Center for Children in Poverty.
Klatte, M., Bergström, K., & Lachmann, T. (2013). Does noise affect learning? A short Ross, M., Maxon, A., & Brackett, D. (1982). Hard of hearing children in regular schools.
review on noise effects on cognitive performance in children. Frontiers in Psychology, Prentice Hall.
4, 578. Sanz, S. A., García, A. M., & García, A. (1993). Road traffic noise around schools: A risk
Komatsubara, T., Shiomi, M., Kanda, T., Ishiguro, H., & Hagita, N. (2014). Can a social for pupil's performance? International Archives of Occupational and Environmental
robot help children's understanding of science in classrooms? Proceedings of the second Health, 65(3), 205–207.
international conference on human-agent interaction (pp. 83–90). Tsukuba: ACM. Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information
Kraus, M. W., Huang, C., & Keltner, D. (2010). Tactile communication, cooperation, and processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 1–66.
performance: An ethological study of the NBA. Emotion, 10(5), 745. Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information
Leung, S. W., & McPherson, B. (2006). Classrooms for Children with Developmental processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory.
Disabilities: Sound‐field and public address amplification systems compared. Psychological Review, 84, 127–190.
International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 53(3), 287–299. Shiomi, M., & Hagita, N. (2015). Social acceptance of a childcare support robot system.
Maag, J. W., & Anderson, J. M. (2006). Effects of sound-field amplification to increase 24th IEEE international symposium on robot and human interactive communication (pp.
compliance of students with emotional and behavior disorders. Behavioral Disorders, 13–18). .
31(4), 378–393. Sumioka, H., Nakae, A., Kanai, R., & Ishiguro, H. (2013). Huggable communication
Maag, J. W., & Anderson, J. M. (2007). Sound-field amplification to increase compliance medium decreases cortisol levels. Scientific Reports, 3.
to directions in students with ADHD. Behavioral Disorders, 32(4), 238–253. Tanaka, F., Cicurel, A., Movellan, & Javier, R. (2007). Socialization between toddlers and
McClelland, M. M., Morrison, F. J., & Holmes, D. L. (2000). Children at risk for early robots at an early childhood education center. Proceedings of the National Academy of
academic problems: The role of learning-related social skills. Early Childhood Research Sciences of the USA, 104(No. 46), 17954–17958.
Quarterly, 15(3), 307–329. Teh, J. K. S., Cheok, A. D., Peiris, R. L., Choi, Y., Thuong, V., & Lai, S. (2008). Huggy
McInnes, A., Humphries, T., Hogg-Johnson, S., & Tannock, R. (2003). Listening com- pajama: A mobile parent and child hugging communication system. Proceedings of the
prehension and working memory are impaired in attention-deficit hyperactivity 7th international conference on Interaction design and children (pp. 250–257). ACM.
disorder irrespective of language impairment. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, Thompson, R. A., & Raikes, H. A. (2007). The social and emotional foundations of school
31(4), 427–443. readiness. Social and emotional health in early childhood: Building bridges between ser-
Millett, P. (2008). Sound field amplification research summary. Deaf and hard of hearing vices and systems (pp. 13–36). .
program. York: Faculty of Education. Vogely, A. J. (1998). Listening comprehension anxiety: Students' reported sources and
Minato, T., Nishio, S., & Ishiguro, H. (2013). Evoking an affection for communication solutions. Foreign Language Annals, 31, 67–80.
partner by a robotic communication medium. Demonstration session proceeding of the Webster-Stratton, C., & Reid, M. J. (2004). Strengthening social and emotional compe-
8th ACM/IEEE international conference on human-robot interaction D: Vol. 7 (Tokyo). tence in young children—the foundation for early school readiness and success:
Morikawa, O., Hashimoto, S., Munakata, T., & Okunaka, J. (2006). Embrace system for Incredible years classroom social skills and problem‐solving curriculum. Infants &
remote counseling. Proceedings of the 8th international conference on Multimodal in- Young Children, 17(2), 96–113.
terfaces (pp. 318–325). ACM. Whitcher, S. J., & Fisher, J. D. (1979). Multidimensional reaction to therapeutic touch in a
Mueller, F. F., Vetere, F., Gibbs, M. R., Kjeldskov, J., Pedell, S., & Howard, S. (2005). Hug hospital setting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(1), 87.
over a distance. CHI'05 extended abstracts on Human factors in computing systems. ACM. Wolvin, A. D. (Ed.). (2010). Listening and human communication in the 21st century. John
Nakanishi, J., Kuwamura, K., Minato, T., Nishio, S., & Ishiguro, H. (2013). Evoking af- Wiley & Sons.
fection for a communication partner by a robotic communication medium. The first Wong, M. (2015). Voices of children, parents and teachers: How children cope with stress
international conference on human-agent interaction (iHAI 2013) (pp. 1–8). . during school transition. Early Child Development and Care, 185, 658–678.

113

You might also like