You are on page 1of 10

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/224707559

Variability in Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV Subtest Performance


Across Age

Article  in  Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology · April 2012


DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acs041 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

58 20,938

3 authors, including:

Joseph Mignogna
U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs
31 PUBLICATIONS   399 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Literature Reviews - Mental Health Integrated Care View project

Brief Motivational Interventions View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Joseph Mignogna on 18 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 27 (2012) 389–397

Variability in Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV Subtest Performance


Across Age

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at Houston Academy of Medicine-Texas Medical Center Library on May 17, 2014
Nick M. Wisdom1,2, Joseph Mignogna1,3, Robert L. Collins 4,5,*
1
Mental Health Care Line, Michael E. DeBakey VA Medical Center, Houston, TX, USA
2
Menninger Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, USA
3
VA HSR&D Houston Center of Excellence, Michael E. DeBakey VA Medical Center, Houston, TX, USA
4
Neurology Care Line, Michael E. DeBakey VA Medical Center, Houston, TX, USA
5
Department of Neurology, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX, USA
*Corresponding author at: Neurology Care Line, Michael E. DeBakey Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Houston, TX, USA. Tel.: +713-794-8939;
fax: +713-794-7786.
E-mail address: robert.collins3@va.gov (R.L. Collins).
Accepted 15 March 2012

Abstract
Normal Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS)-IV performance relative to average normative scores alone can be an oversimplification
as this fails to recognize disparate subtest heterogeneity that occurs with increasing age. The purpose of the present study is to characterize the
patterns of raw score change and associated variability on WAIS-IV subtests across age groupings. Raw WAIS-IV subtest means and standard
deviations for each age group were tabulated from the WAIS-IV normative manual along with the coefficient of variation (CV), a measure of
score dispersion calculated by dividing the standard deviation by the mean and multiplying by 100. The CV further informs the magnitude of
variability represented by each standard deviation. Raw mean scores predictably decreased across age groups. Increased variability was noted
in Perceptual Reasoning and Processing Speed Index subtests, as Block Design, Matrix Reasoning, Picture Completion, Symbol Search, and
Coding had CV percentage increases ranging from 56% to 98%. In contrast, Working Memory and Verbal Comprehension subtests were
more homogeneous with Digit Span, Comprehension, Information, and Similarities percentage of the mean increases ranging from 32%
to 43%. Little change in the CV was noted on Cancellation, Arithmetic, Letter/Number Sequencing, Figure Weights, Visual Puzzles, and
Vocabulary subtests (,14%). A thorough understanding of age-related subtest variability will help to identify test limitations as well as
further our understanding of cognitive domains which remain relatively steady versus those which steadily decline.

Keywords: Statistical methods; Norms/normative studies; Intelligence; Everyday functioning; Developmental and learning disabilities; Assessment

Introduction

An effective neuropsychological evaluation is contingent on recognizing several important mitigating factors that influence
the interpretation of collected test scores. In the evaluation of older adults, perhaps the most important issue is the differenti-
ation between normal, age-related changes and pathologic brain dysfunction (Miller, Myers, Prinzi, & Mittenberg, 2009;
Nadler, Mittenberg, DePiano, & Schneider, 1994). Normal, non-pathological aging is associated with a number of physiologic-
al changes and neurological declinations. The most common findings include the enlargement of the ventricles, cortical sulci,
and subarachnoid space; gross cortical, hippocampal, and temporal lobe atrophy; hyperintensity in the white matter and basal
ganglia; and arteriosclerosis (Drayer, 1988; Scahill et al., 2003). However, the widespread belief that neuronal loss is associated
with normal aging has recently been criticized and there is some support that decreases in brain matter are more the result of
reduced gray matter volume (Abe et al., 2008; for a review, see Keller, 2006).
These age-associated reductions in gray matter have been shown to negatively impact cognitive functioning in healthy, older
adults (Zimmerman et al., 2006). Regardless of etiology, cognitive change as a normal process of advancing age is well docu-
mented in the literature. More specifically, declines in executive functioning (Royall, Palmer, Chiodo, & Polk, 2004; West &

Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of US Government 2012.


doi:10.1093/arclin/acs041 Advance Access publication on 17 April 2012
390 N.M. Wisdom et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 27 (2012) 389–397

Alan, 2000), novel problem-solving (Hayslip & Sterns, 1979), selective attention (Parasuraman & Greenwood, 1998), process-
ing speed (Gunning-Dixon & Raz, 2000; Schaie, 1994), working memory (Foos & Wright, 1992; McEvoy, Pellouchoud,
Smith, & Gevins, 2001), and encoding of new information (Petersen, Smith, Kokmen, Ivnik, & Tangalos, 1992; Stebbins
et al., 2002) are prominent with aging. Given these findings, it is not surprising that age significantly influences many of
the tests utilized to measure intelligence (Kaufman, 1990, 2001).
Much of the aforementioned research was conducted using the most widely used and researched measure of intelligence—
the various iterations of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS; Wechsler, 1955, 1981, 1997, 2008a). At its core, the
Wechsler scales have always been comprised of point scales, in that an overall Intelligence Quotient (IQ) is based on the sum-
mation of “points earned” across various subtests. As subtests are grouped together to form both factor scores and an IQ, an
underlying assumption exists that each subtest is accorded the same weight as the others (Matarazzo, 1972). As a result, tests

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at Houston Academy of Medicine-Texas Medical Center Library on May 17, 2014
with lower reliabilities (e.g., object assembly) were included in the overall IQ because intelligence was originally considered
“assortative” rather than hierarchical in that each subtest adds unique information to the overall IQ (Matarazzo, 1972). Over
time, much more emphasis has been placed on individual subtests and their semantic combinations (i.e., index scores). It is now
commonly held that there is a real benefit in being able to compare an individual’s performance across subtests and factors and
that many of these scores can be considered indicators of neurological integrity (Ruff, Allen, Farrow, Niemann, & Wylie,
1994). To permit comparisons between the various subtests, “points earned” are converted into scaled scores with an
average of 10 and a standard deviation of +3. Unfortunately, the same underlying assumption that accords equal subtest
weighting has an unintended effect of implying similar psychometric properties for each subtest. Moreover, because the
“points earned” are converted into scaled scores with uniform standard deviations (e.g., +3), any differential variability
that might exist between the various subtests is obscured.
Ardila (2007), working backward through the normative tables, examined age-related changes in variability on WAIS-III
subtests by calculating the “percentage of the mean” for each subtest. The percentage of the mean, more formally known
as the coefficient of variation (CV), is simply a ratio of the standard deviation/mean × 100 (Bartlett, 1946; Hendricks &
Robey, 1936; Pearson, 1897; Yablokov, 1974). The CVs were previously reported for the Wechsler – Bellevue index scales
(Verbal, Performance, Full Scale; Matarazzo, 1972), but Ardila (2007) was the first to examine variability and score dispersion
at the subtest level for the WAIS-III. Unfortunately, the CV is typically overlooked in the behavioral sciences despite having
two very attractive properties. First, by expressing the variability as a percentage of the mean, more information about actual
variability is conveyed; that is to say, a standard deviation score alone is not meaningful without reference to the mean. In turn,
this enables researchers and clinicians to subsequently compare the CVs within and between populations (Lande, 1977). Using
the CV methodology, Ardila demonstrated that mean scores on the WAIS-III performance generally decline across age cohorts,
while the heterogeneity for most subtests increases greatly. However, the exact percentage increase/decrease in the CV for each
subtest is difficult to discern from Ardila’s study as he chose to express this change as a “ratio percentage” instead of the per-
centage change.
As a result of several factor-analytic models, the WAIS-IV has implemented a number of changes and now has 15 subtests
across four index scales, eschewing the traditional dual IQ (Verbal and Performance) index score structure from previous itera-
tions of the WAIS (for a review, see Wechsler, 2008b). The WAIS-IV has retained all the Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI)
subtests from the WAIS-III, although Comprehension is now supplemental. Likewise, the Working Memory Index (WMI)
subtests remain unchanged, although a new sequencing task has been added to the traditional Forward/Backward Digit
Span. The WAIS-III Perceptional Organization Index has been renamed for the WAIS-IV as the Perceptual Reasoning
Index (PRI); Block Design and Matrix Reasoning have been retained and Picture Completion is now optional. In addition,
two new subtests, Visual Puzzles and Figure Weights (supplemental), have been added to the PRI as measures of non-verbal
abstract reasoning and quantitative/analogical reasoning, respectively. Finally, the Processing Speed Index (PSI) has added the
supplemental Cancellation task alongside core Coding and Symbol Search subtests. Picture Arrangement and Object Assembly
from the WAIS-III previously only loaded on the omnibus Full-Scale IQ and both have been discontinued for the WAIS-IV. For
a full review of all changes, including subtest modifications, sample characteristics, and reliability/validity statistics, see the
WAIS-IV technical manual (Wechsler, 2008b).
Given the number of substantial changes to the test content and structure found in the WAIS-IV, further evaluation of
age-related changes in subtest variability is warranted. The purpose of this study was to calculate the CVs in all of the
WAIS-IV subtests across each age grouping. It was hypothesized that mean scores would decrease with age, particularly
within the PSI and PRI, while CV scores would amplify, supporting the notion of increased intellectual heterogeneity with
aging. Also, the addition of the sequencing task to the Digit Span subtest, along with normative data tabulated for forward/
backward, allowed for a unique examination of Digit Span performance across age cohorts. Finally, the results of the
current study were compared with Ardila’s (2007) research to better capture changes between WAIS-III and WAIS-IV.
N.M. Wisdom et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 27 (2012) 389–397 391

Methods

Normative data found in the WAIS-IV Administration and Scoring Manual (Tables A.1 and C.1; Wechsler, 2008a) were
used to determine mean raw scores and CVs (i.e., dispersion scores) within age groups for each WAIS-IV subtest. The proced-
ure described by Ardila (2007) was utilized to determine these scores. Specifically, within each age group, the raw score cor-
responding to each subtest scaled score ¼ 10 provided the mean raw score for each subtest. When a range was provided for the
corresponding raw score (i.e., 71– 75), the mean of the range was used. Then, to determine the raw standard deviation within
age groups for each subtest, the raw scores corresponding to a scaled score ¼ 7 (1 SD below the mean) and a scaled score ¼ 13
(1 SD above the mean) were separately subtracted from the mean, added together, and then divided by 2. The CV for each
subtest by age was determined by dividing the standard deviation by the mean and multiplying by 100. This provides the per-

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at Houston Academy of Medicine-Texas Medical Center Library on May 17, 2014
centage of the mean represented by the standard deviation (Hendricks & Robey, 1936), and thus information about the mag-
nitude of variability represented by each standard deviation, as well as a standardized score of variability that allows for inter-
and intra-age group comparisons.
The WAIS-IV was standardized and normalized using 2,200 American examinees divided into 13 age groups: 16– 17, 18– 19,
20– 24, 25 –29, 30– 34, 35– 44, 45 – 54, 55– 64, 65– 69, 70– 74, 75– 79, 80– 84, and 85– 90. The ratio of different ethnicities
and gender were proportionally consistent with census data within each corresponding age group. The sample was drawn from
four different geographical regions in the USA (West, Midwest, South, and Northeast) and stratified according to five education
levels based on the number of years of school completed: ≤8, 9 – 11, 12, 13 – 15, and ≥16. A detailed description of the sample
can be found in the WAIS-IV Technical and Interpretive Manual (Wechsler, 2008b).

Results and Discussion

The raw mean, standard deviation, and CV scores in each age range for all subtests on the WAIS-IV can be found in Tables 1
(VCI), 2 (PRI), 3 (PSI), and 4 (WMI). In addition, Table 5 presents the highest and lowest mean scores, along with the asso-
ciated mean and CV score percentage change, within each subtest. In other words, the data presented in Table 5 can be used to
determine which subtests fluctuate the most with increasing age and which remain stable. More specifically, subtests where the
CV remains constant across the lifespan while the mean decreases suggest that the underlying cognitive construct really is de-
clining for most individuals (Ardila, 2007). For example, Visual Puzzles mean scores steadily decline with age, but the CV
(i.e., intragroup variability) remains stable in the older age groups. Clinically, this indicates that most people will demonstrate
a decline in this measure with age. Compare this with subtests where the CV increases greatly in the context of the mean score
decline (e.g., Digit Span Sequencing). Because the older age groups are more heterogeneous, statements regarding an indivi-
dual’s strength or weakness can be made with less confidence because the spectrum of “normal” is wider. Finally, Table 6
presents the percentage change in CV scores for each subtest for both WAIS-III and WAIS-IV. It is important to note that
Ardila (2007), when examining the change in the mean and the associated CV for each subtest across the lifespan, originally
calculated a ratio percentage [(lowest mean/highest mean) × 100]. Using the data presented in Ardila’s manuscript, we have

Table 1. Calculated raw scores of the WAIS-IV VCI subtests


16–17 18–19 20–24 25–29 30–34 35–44 45–54 55–64 65–69 70–74 75–79 80–84 85–89

Similarities
Mean 23.0 23.5 24.5 25.0 25.5 26.0 26.0 25.5 24.5 23.5 22.5 21.5 20.5
SD 5.3 5.3 5.5 5.3 5.5 5.8 5.8 6.0 6.3 6.3 6.3 6.3 6.3
CV 22.8 22.3 22.4 21.0 21.6 22.1 22.1 23.5 25.5 26.6 27.8 29.1 30.5
Vocabulary
Mean 30.0 31.0 33.0 34.5 36.0 37.5 38.5 38.5 37.5 36.5 35.5 34.5 33.0
SD 10.3 10.3 10.3 10.8 11.3 11.5 11.8 11.8 11.5 12.0 12.0 12.3 12.5
CV 34.2 33.1 31.1 31.2 31.3 30.7 30.5 30.5 30.7 32.9 33.8 35.5 37.9
Information
Mean 13.0 13.0 13.0 13.0 13.0 13.5 14.5 14.5 14.5 14.5 13.5 12.5 11.5
SD 4.8 4.8 5.0 5.0 5.0 5.3 5.3 5.8 5.8 5.8 5.8 5.8 5.8
CV 36.5 36.5 38.5 38.5 38.5 38.9 36.2 39.7 39.7 39.7 42.6 46.0 50.0
Comprehension (optional)
Mean 22.5 22.5 23.5 23.5 24.5 25.5 25.5 24.5 23.5 22.5 21.5 20.5 19.5
SD 5.8 6.0 6.3 5.8 6.3 5.8 6.3 6.3 6.5 6.3 6.3 6.0 6.3
CV 25.6 26.7 26.6 24.5 25.5 22.5 24.5 25.5 27.7 27.8 29.1 29.3 32.1
Note: CV ¼ coefficient of variation.
392 N.M. Wisdom et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 27 (2012) 389–397

Table 2. Calculated raw scores of the WAIS-IV PRI subtests

16–17 18–19 20–24 25–29 30–34 35–44 45–54 55–64 65–69 70–74 75–79 80–84 85–89

Block Design
Mean 46.5 46.5 46.5 46.0 45.0 43.0 39.5 35.5 32.5 30.0 28.0 25.0 22.0
SD 12.5 12.3 12.5 12.8 13.0 13.3 13.0 12.3 11.3 10.8 10.3 9.8 9.3
CV 26.9 26.3 26.9 27.7 28.9 30.8 32.9 34.5 34.6 35.8 36.6 39.0 42.0
Matrix Reasoning
Mean 19.0 19.0 19.0 19.0 18.5 18.5 16.5 15.0 13.5 12.0 11.0 9.5 8.0
SD 4.5 4.5 4.5 4.3 4.3 4.8 4.8 4.8 5.0 5.0 4.8 4.0 3.8
CV 23.7 23.7 23.7 22.4 23.0 25.7 28.8 31.7 37.0 41.7 43.2 42.1 46.9

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at Houston Academy of Medicine-Texas Medical Center Library on May 17, 2014
Visual Puzzles
Mean 16.5 16.5 16.5 15.5 15.5 15.0 14.0 13.0 11.5 11.0 10.0 9.0 8.0
SD 5.3 5.3 5.0 5.0 4.8 4.5 4.0 3.8 3.8 3.5 3.3 3.0 2.5
CV 31.8 31.8 30.3 32.3 30.6 30.0 28.6 28.8 32.6 31.8 32.5 33.3 31.3
Figure Weights (optional)
Mean 16.5 16.5 16.5 15.5 15.5 14.5 13.0 12.0 12.0
SD 5.3 5.3 5.3 5.3 4.8 4.8 4.0 4.0 4.3
CV 31.8 31.8 31.8 33.9 30.6 32.8 30.8 33.3 35.4
Picture Completion (optional)
Mean 14.0 14.0 14.0 14.0 13.5 13.0 13.0 12.0 11.0 10.0 9.0 8.0 7.0
SD 3.8 3.8 3.8 3.8 3.8 3.8 3.5 4.0 4.0 4.0 4.0 3.5 3.5
CV 26.8 26.8 26.8 26.8 27.8 28.8 26.9 33.3 36.4 40.0 44.4 43.8 50.0
Note: CV ¼ coefficient of variation.

Table 3. Calculated raw scores of the WAIS-IV PSI subtests

16–17 18–19 20–24 25–29 30–34 35–44 45–54 55–64 65–69 70–74 75–79 80–84 85–89

Symbol Search
Mean 34.5 34.0 34.0 34.0 33.5 32.5 30.5 28.0 26.0 23.5 21.0 17.5 15.5
SD 8.0 8.5 8.5 8.5 8.0 8.3 7.8 7.5 7.3 7.5 7.3 6.8 6.5
CV 23.2 25.0 25.0 25.0 23.9 25.4 25.4 26.8 27.9 31.9 34.5 38.6 41.9
Coding
Mean 73.0 73.0 73.0 73.0 72.0 70.0 65.0 59.5 54.0 49.5 44.5 40.0 34.0
SD 16.8 16.8 16.8 16.8 16.5 16.3 16.0 15.5 15.5 15.5 15.0 15.3 15.0
CV 22.9 22.9 22.9 22.9 22.9 23.2 24.6 26.1 28.7 31.3 33.7 38.1 44.1
Cancellation (optional)
Mean 42.0 42.0 42.0 41.0 40.5 39.5 38.0 37.0 36.0
SD 9.8 9.8 9.8 9.8 9.8 9.8 9.5 9.5 9.0
CV 23.2 23.2 23.2 23.8 24.1 24.7 25.0 25.7 25.0
Note: CV ¼ coefficient of variation.

instead calculated the more intuitive percentage increase/decrease for both the mean and CV for each subtest [(amount of
change/highest mean or associated CV) × 100] and presented them in Table 6.

Verbal Comprehension Index

In general, all raw mean scores followed a similar and predictable pattern, increasing from younger to middle age before
steadily declining into older adulthood. All the VCI subtests decline by about 20% when measuring the percentage change
from the highest to the lowest mean scores; however, with the exception of Vocabulary, the intragroup variability as repre-
sented by the CV increases moderately. More specifically, the associated CV increases by a moderate 40% when moving
from the highest to the lowest mean scores on the Similarities, Information, and Comprehension subtest. Vocabulary is
unique in that it is the only WAIS-IV subtest where the lowest mean scores are not observed in the oldest age group, but
rather the youngest (16 – 17 years). Vocabulary is also distinctive among VCI subtests because there is little intragroup vari-
ability in performance, irrespective of age, as the CV only varies 12% when comparing the associated highest and lowest mean
scores.
N.M. Wisdom et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 27 (2012) 389–397 393

Table 4. Calculated raw scores of the WAIS-IV WMI subtests

16–17 18–19 20–24 25–29 30–34 35–44 45–54 55–64 65–69 70–74 75–79 80–84 85–89

Digit Span, Overall


Mean 28.5 28.5 28.5 28.5 28.5 28.5 27.5 26.5 25.5 24.5 23.5 22.5 21.5
SD 5.8 5.5 6.0 6.0 6.0 5.8 5.8 6.0 6.0 6.0 5.8 5.8 5.8
CV 20.2 19.3 21.1 21.1 21.1 20.2 20.9 22.6 23.5 24.5 24.5 25.6 26.7
Digit Span, Forward
Mean 10.5 10.5 11.0 11.0 11.0 11.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 10.0 9.5 9.0
SD 2.0 2.5 2.5 2.3 2.5 2.5 2.5 2.8 2.5 2.5 2.5 2.3 2.0
CV 19.0 23.8 22.7 20.5 22.7 22.7 25.0 27.5 25.0 25.0 25.0 23.7 22.2

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at Houston Academy of Medicine-Texas Medical Center Library on May 17, 2014
Digit Span, Backward
Mean 7.5 9.0 9.0 9.0 9.0 9.0 9.0 8.0 8.0 8.0 8.0 7.0 6.5
SD 2.5 2.5 2.5 2.5 2.8 2.8 2.5 2.5 2.5 2.3 2.0 2.0 1.8
CV 33.3 27.8 27.8 27.8 30.6 30.6 27.8 31.3 31.3 28.1 25.0 28.6 26.9
Digit Span, Sequencing
Mean 9.0 9.0 9.0 9.0 9.0 9.0 8.5 8.0 8.0 7.0 7.0 6.0 6.0
SD 2.0 2.0 2.0 2.3 2.5 2.5 2.5 2.3 2.5 2.5 2.3 2.5 2.5
CV 22.2 22.2 22.2 25.0 27.8 27.8 29.4 28.1 31.3 35.7 32.1 41.7 41.7
Arithmetic
Mean 13.5 14.0 14.0 14.0 14.0 14.5 14.5 14.0 14.0 13.0 12.5 12.0 11.0
SD 3.8 4.0 4.0 4.0 4.0 3.8 3.8 3.8 3.5 3.8 3.5 3.5 3.3
CV 27.8 28.6 28.6 28.6 28.6 25.9 25.9 26.8 25.0 28.8 28.0 29.2 29.5
L-N Sequencing (optional)
Mean 20.5 20.5 20.5 20.5 20.5 20.5 20.5 20.0 19.0
SD 3.8 3.8 3.8 4.0 4.0 4.0 4.3 4.0 3.8
CV 18.3 18.3 18.3 19.5 19.5 19.5 20.7 20.0 19.7
Note: CV ¼ coefficient of variation.

Table 5. Variations in mean and CV scores


Subtest Highest score Lowest score Percentage change
Age Mean CV Age Mean CV Mean (%) CV (%)

Similarities 35–54 26 22.1 85–89 20.5 30.5 21.2 38.0


Vocabulary 45–64 38.5 30.5 16–17 30 34.2 22.1 12.1
Information 45–74 14.5 36.2– 39.7 85–89 11.5 50 20.7 38.1
Comprehension 35–54 25.5 22.5– 24.5 85–89 19.5 32.1 23.5 42.7
Block Design 16–24 46.5 26.3– 26.9 85–89 22 42 52.7 56.1
Matrix Reasoning 16–29 19 22.4– 23.7 85–89 8 46.9 57.9 97.9
Visual Puzzles 16–24 16.5 30.3– 31.8 85–89 8 31.3 51.5 21.6
Figure Weights* 16–24 16.5 31.8 55–69 12 33.3– 35.4 27.3 11.3
Picture Completion 16–29 14 26.8 85–89 7 50 50.0 86.6
Digit Span, Overall 16–44 28.5 19.3– 21.1 85–89 21.5 26.7 24.6 32.2
Digit Span, Forward 20–44 11 20.5– 22.7 85–89 9 22.2 18.2 22.2
Digit Span, Backward 18–54 9 27.8– 30.6 85–89 6.5 26.9 27.8 23.2
Digit Span, Seq. 16–44 9 22.2– 27.8 80–89 6 41.7 33.3 87.8
Arithmetic 35–54 14.5 25.9 85–89 11 29.5 24.1 13.9
Letter-Number Seq.* 16–54 20.5 18.3– 20.7 65–69 19 19.7 7.3 7.7
Symbol Search 16–17 34.5 23.2 85–89 15.5 41.9 55.1 80.6
Coding 16–29 73 22.9 85–89 34 44.1 53.4 92.6
Cancellation* 16–24 42 23.2 65–69 36 25 14.3 7.8
Notes: CV ¼ coefficient of variation. Subtests with an asterisk means they are optional/supplemental subtests.

Perceptual Reasoning Index

A general pattern of the mean score emerges when examining the PRI subtests in that all of the mean scores strongly decline
with age—the highest and lowest mean scores are found in the youngest and oldest age groups, respectively. Excluding
Figure Weights, all the PRI subtest mean scores decline by about 53%. Figure Weights mean scores only decline by 27%,
394 N.M. Wisdom et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 27 (2012) 389–397

Table 6. Percentage change in CV scores for the WAIS-III and WAIS-IV subtests

Subtest WAIS-III (Ardila, 2007) WAIS-IV


High-score age Low-score age CV (%) High-score age Low-score age CV (%)

Similarities 30– 54 85– 89 40 35– 54 85– 89 38.0


Vocabulary 45– 54 85– 89 33 45– 64 16– 17 12.1
Information 45– 54 85– 89 16 45– 74 85– 89 38.1
Comprehension 45– 54 85– 89 36 35– 54 85– 89 42.7
Block Design 16– 29 85– 89 2 16– 24 85– 89 56.1
Matrix Reasoning 16– 17 85– 89 100 16– 29 85– 89 97.9
Picture Completion 16– 44 85– 89 134 16– 29 85– 89 86.6

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at Houston Academy of Medicine-Texas Medical Center Library on May 17, 2014
Digit Span, Overall 16– 29 85– 89 28 16– 44 85– 89 32.2
Arithmetic 45– 54 85– 89 21 35– 54 85– 89 13.9
Letter-Number Seq. 16– 44 85– 89 100 16– 54 65– 69 7.7
Symbol Search 18– 19 85– 89 74 16– 17 85– 89 80.6
Coding/Digit Symbol 18– 19 85– 89 106 16– 29 85– 89 92.6
Notes: WAIS ¼ Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; CV ¼ coefficient of variation.

but it is important to note that normative data for individuals over the age of 70 were not reported in the WAIS-IV manual.
Figure Weights would likely follow a similar pattern through older adulthood as all the related PRI subtest mean score declines
are comparable in the 65– 69 age grouping. Despite the uniform decline in mean scores, a bimodal distribution of CV scores
suggests more variability both between and within measures that might otherwise be overlooked by focusing exclusively on
changes in mean scores. When measuring the percentage change in CV scores associated with the highest and lowest
means, a strong increase is observed on the Block Design (56.1%), Picture Completion (86.6%), and Matrix Reasoning
(97.9%) subtests, suggesting increasingly heterogeneous performance within each subsequent age grouping that is masked
by the mean. The intragroup variability for Visual Puzzles, however, actually decreases with age by 1.6%. Similarly,
Figure Weights increases only slightly by 11.3%. This change cannot be explained by the lack of older adult normative infor-
mation since the other subtest CV scores greatly surpass this minimal increase before reaching the 65– 69 age range.

Processing Speed Index

A strong decline in performance with aging is evident, as the highest and lowest PSI subtest mean scores are found in the
youngest and oldest age groups. Symbol Search and Coding exhibit a similar trajectory: mean scores strongly decline with
increases in age by about 54%, while associated CVs strongly increase by 81% and 93%, respectively. Cancellation,
however, appears to be more resilient to the effects of aging as the mean only declines by 14% with a small 8% increase in
intragroup variability. Although the complete performance curve for the Cancellation subtest cannot be calculated as it was
not normed in individuals over the age of 70, it is worth noting that the magnitude of mean and CV change for Coding and
Symbol Search is moderately larger than Cancellation in the 65– 69 age range. The increased intragroup variability noted
on these measures is particularly noteworthy as processing speed is a cognitive construct that has been cited as the primary
underlying process for loss of cognitive functioning in normal aging (Salthouse, 1996).

Working Memory Index

The pattern of decline from the highest means scores to the lowest is less discernible among the WMI subtests.
Letter-Number Sequencing exhibits the least amount of mean score change, declining by 7%, but this appears to be an artifact
given the lack of normative data for the oldest age groups; the other WMI subtests have a similar pattern of decline before
reaching the 65 – 69 age range. The remaining WMI subtests demonstrate small to middling mean score decreases with age,
ranging from 18.2% (Digit Span Forward) to 33.3% (Digit Span Sequencing). With the exception of Digit Span
Sequencing, the associated CV changes are relatively uniform. Intragroup variability actually decreases with advancing age
on the Digit Span Forward (22.2%) and Digit Span Backward (23.2%) subtests. In addition, minimal CV score increases
are observed on the Letter-Number Sequencing (7.7%) and Arithmetic (13.9%) subtests.
In contrast, Digit Span Sequencing appears to be inherently unique in that it demonstrates a very different pattern of score
dispersion. Digit Span Sequencing is suggested to be similar to Digit Span Forward and Backward in that both aspects of
working memory and mental manipulation are cognitively taxed (Wechsler, 2008b). However, Digit Span Sequencing is
thought to theoretically differ from Digit Span Forward (attention efficacy) and Digit Span Backward (transformation of
N.M. Wisdom et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 27 (2012) 389–397 395

information), in that sequencing is a more demanding task requiring simultaneous activation of phonological and semantic
information as a precursor to task comprehension (MacDonald, Almor, Henderson, Kempler, & Andersen, 2001; Werheid
et al., 2002). In our analysis of WAIS-IV Digit Span subtests, we found that Digit Span Sequencing appeared to change dif-
ferentially across age groups, with both Digit Span Forward and Backward raw and CV scores remaining relatively stable,
whereas Digit Span Sequencing raw scores decreased by 33% from the youngest to the oldest groups and the CV increased
by 87.8%. Creating composite WAIS Digit Span subtest scores (e.g., combining Forward and Backward) has been criticized
previously, as both are felt to provide distinct information (Banken, 1985; Reynolds, 1997). This would also appear to be the
case with Digit Span Sequencing and the distinction between the Digit Span subtests appears to be amplified across age
cohorts.

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at Houston Academy of Medicine-Texas Medical Center Library on May 17, 2014
Comparison of WAIS-III and WAIS-IV

In general, the majority of the WAIS-IV subtests demonstrate similar CV changes as those observed on the WAIS-III,
despite a number of material and administration differences. This suggests that either these subtests (e.g., Symbol Search)
are psychometrically similar between the WAIS-III and WAIS-IV iterations and/or that the underlying latent construct
(e.g., processing speed) is robust to measurement. A few notable exceptions to this pattern include Digit Span,
Letter-Number Sequencing, Block Design, and Picture Completion. The variability in overall Digit Span scores among the
older age groups is much higher on the WAIS-IV, although this is entirely a result of the additional Sequencing task which
was previously not included in the WAIS-III. Likewise, Letter-Number Sequencing appears to vary much less on the
WAIS-IV, although as previously noted, this is most likely because it was not normed among individuals over the age of
70 as was previously done on the WAIS-III. Block Design and Picture Completion, however, demonstrate notable volatility.
The CV change between the WAIS-III and WAIS-IV indicates that performance among the older age groups on Block Design
and Picture Completion greatly increases and decreases, respectively. The reason for this variability is not entirely clear al-
though it does suggest that these subtests have changed considerably and/or the underlying construct of visuospatial perception
is being inconsistently measured. Although caution is warranted when clinically trying to transpose any research conducted
using previous WAIS iterations onto the WAIS-IV (Loring & Bauer, 2010), particular attention should be directed at
studies that have evaluated Block Design and Picture Completion (Ronnlund & Nilsson, 2006; Schoenberg, Scott, Duff, &
Adams, 2002).

Conclusion

Healthy aging occurs in conjunction with a decline in many cognitive faculties as measured by the WAIS-IV. However,
examining the WAIS-IV subtest mean scores in isolation neglects the variability in performance within each age grouping.
A seemingly forgotten, yet valuable way of examining the dispersion of scores is to calculate the CV for each subtest
(Hendricks & Robey, 1936; Yablokov, 1974). The CV represents the percentage of the mean score that is accounted for by
the spread of 1 SD. Assuming that the standard deviation remains constant, declines in mean scores with aging like those
observed on nearly all WAIS subtests necessarily mean increased within-group variability in performance on the subtest.
The current study examined the dispersion of scores on the various WAIS-IV subtests across all age ranges included in the
normative manual. The findings from this study suggest that there is more heterogeneity in cognitive functioning among
older adults than is observed in younger adults. This pronouncement is not particularly surprising as older adults have had sig-
nificantly more opportunities to experientially shape their cognitive abilities. However, examining only the mean scores creates
the false belief that cognition declines uniformly with age when in actuality the intragroup variability amplifies with age.
There are several limitations of this study that should be targeted for improvement in follow-up experiments. Most import-
antly, this study compares different groups of subjects across different age ranges. This cross-sectional approach often results in
a “cohort effect” as each generational group has their own shared temporal life experiences. As a result, the cohort effect may
explain some of the differences between age groups rather than real changes in cognitive functioning across the lifespan.
Ideally, longitudinal studies should be conducted measuring these cognitive abilities in the same subjects across the lifespan
to help improve the internal validity of these findings. Another limitation is that the means and standard deviations represented
here are a very close estimation of the observed scores based on the information found in the WAIS-IV manual, but the actual
data could not be obtained. Finally, it is important to note that this study only describes and sheds light on the pattern of cog-
nitive decline as measured by the WAIS-IV, but offers no explanation for the changes in intragroup variability observed on
many of the subtests. Continued research to help establish the internal validity of these measures in older adults may help
explain some of these changes. Studies implementing a double-dissociation design can help differentiate the specificity of
these measures for underlying cognitive abilities (Weiskrantz, 1991).
396 N.M. Wisdom et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 27 (2012) 389–397

Conflict of Interest

None declared.

References

Abe, O., Yamasue, H., Aoki, S., Suga, M., Yamada, H., Kassai, K., et al. (2008). Aging in the CNS: Comparison of gray/white matter volume and diffusion
tensor data. Neurobiology of Aging, 29, 102– 116.
Ardila, A. (2007). Normal aging increases cognitive heterogeneity: Analysis of dispersion in WAIS-III scores across age. Archives of Clinical

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at Houston Academy of Medicine-Texas Medical Center Library on May 17, 2014
Neuropsychology, 22, 1003– 1011.
Banken, J. A. (1985). Clinical utility of considering Digits Forward and Digits Backward as separate components of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-
Revised. Neuropsychological Assessment, 41, 686 –691.
Bartlett, M. S. (1946). The coefficient of variation. Nature, 158, 520–521.
Drayer, B. P. (1988). Imaging of the aging brain: Part I. normal findings. Radiology, 166, 785–796.
Foos, P. W., & Wright, L. (1992). Adult age differences in the storage of information in working memory. Experimental Aging Research, 2, 51–57.
Gunning-Dixon, F. M., & Raz, N. (2000). The cognitive correlates of white matter abnormalities in normal aging: A quantitative review. Neuropsychology, 14,
224–232.
Hayslip, B., Jr., & Sterns, H. L. (1979). Age differences in relationships between crystallized and fluid intelligence and problem solving. Journal of
Gerontology, 34, 404– 414.
Hendricks, W. A., & Robey, K. W. (1936). The sampling distribution of the coefficient of variation. Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 7, 129– 132.
Kaufman, A. S. (1990). Assessing adolescent and adult intelligence. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Kaufman, A. S. (2001). WAIS-III IQs, Horn’s theory, and generational changes from young adulthood to old age. Intelligence, 29, 131– 167.
Keller, J. N. (2006). Age-related neuropathology, cognitive decline, and Alzheimer’s disease. Ageing Research Reviews, 5, 1 –13.
Lande, R. (1977). On comparing coefficients of variation. Systematic Zoology, 26, 214– 217.
Loring, D. W., & Bauer, R. M. (2010). Testing the limits: Cautions and concerns regarding the new Wechsler IQ and Memory scales. Neurology, 74, 685– 690.
MacDonald, M. C., Almor, A., Henderson, V. W., Kempler, D., & Andersen, E. S. (2001). Assessing working memory and language comprehension in
Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Language, 78, 17–42.
Matarazzo, J. D. (1972). Wechsler’s measurement and appraisal of adult intelligence: Fifth and enlarged edition. New York: Oxford University Press.
McEvoy, L. K., Pellouchoud, E., Smith, M. E., & Gevins, A. (2001). Neurophysiological signals of working memory in normal aging. Cognitive Brain
Research, 11, 363–376.
Miller, L. J., Myers, A., Prinzi, L., & Mittenberg, W. (2009). Changes in intellectual functioning associated with normal aging. Archives of Clinical
Neuropsychology, 24, 681–688.
Nadler, J. D., Mittenberg, W., DePiano, F. A., & Schneider, B. A. (1994). Effects of patient age on neuropsychological test interpretation. Professional
Psychology: Research and Practice, 25, 288– 295.
Parasuraman, R., & Greenwood, P. M. (1998). Selective attention in aging and dementia. In R. Parasuraman (Ed.), The attentive brain (pp. 461–487).
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Pearson, K. (1897). The chances of death, and other studies in evolution. London: Edwards Arnold.
Petersen, R. C., Smith, G., Kokmen, E., Ivnik, R. J., & Tangalos, E. G. (1992). Memory function in normal aging. Neurology, 42, 396– 401.
Reynolds, C. R. (1997). Forward and backward memory span should not be combined for clinical analysis. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 12, 29–40.
Ronnlund, M., & Nilsson, L. G. (2006). Adult life-span patterns in WAIS-R Block Design performance: Cross-sectional versus longitudinal age gradients and
relations to demographic factors. Intelligence, 34, 63– 78.
Royall, D. R., Palmer, R., Chiodo, L. K., & Polk, M. J. (2004). Declining executive control in normal aging predicts change in functional status: The Freedom
House study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 52, 346–352.
Ruff, R. M., Allen, C. C., Farrow, C. E., Niemann, H., & Wylie, T. (1994). Figural fluency: Differential impairment in patients with left versus right frontal lobe
lesions. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 9, 41– 55.
Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403–428.
Scahill, R. I., Frost, C., Jenkins, R., Whitwell, J. L., Rossor, M. N., & Fox, N. C. (2003). A longitudinal study of brain volume changes in normal aging using
serial registered magnetic resonance imaging. Archives of Neurology, 60, 989– 994.
Schaie, K. W. (1994). The course of adult intellectual development. The American Psychologist, 49 (4), 304–313.
Schoenberg, M. R., Scott, J. G., Duff, K., & Adams, R. L. (2002). Estimation of WAIS-III intelligence from combined performance and demographic variables:
Development of the OPIE-3. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 16, 426–438.
Stebbins, G. T., Carrillo, M. C., Dorfman, J., Dirksen, C., Desmond, J. E., Turner, D. A., et al. (2002). Aging effects on memory encoding in the frontal lobes.
Psychology and Aging, 17, 44–55.
Wechsler, D. (1955). Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. New York: Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (1981). WAIS-R Manual: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (1997). WAIS-III Administration and Scoring Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (2008a). WAIS-IV Administration and Scoring Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (2008b). WAIS-IV Technical and Interpretive Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Weiskrantz, L. (1991). Dissociations and associates in neuropsychology. In R. G. Lister, & H. J. Weingartner (Eds.), Perspectives on cognitive neuroscience.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Werheid, K., Hoppe, C., Thone, A., Muller, U., Mungersdorf, M., & Yves von Cramon, D. (2002). The Adaptive Ordering Test: Clinical application, reliability,
and validity of a verbal working memory test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17, 547–565.
N.M. Wisdom et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 27 (2012) 389–397 397

West, R., & Alan, C. (2000). Age-related decline in inhibitory control contributes to the increased Stroop effect observed in older adults. Psychophysiology, 37,
179–189.
Yablokov, A. V. (1974). Variability of mammals. New Delhi: Amerind Publishing.
Zimmerman, M. E., Brickman, A. M., Paul, R. H., Grieve, S. M., Tate, D. F., Gunstad, J., et al. (2006). The relationship between frontal gray matter and cog-
nition varies across the healthy adult lifespan. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 14, 823– 833.

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at Houston Academy of Medicine-Texas Medical Center Library on May 17, 2014

View publication stats

You might also like