You are on page 1of 12

153

.
ASM20410.1177/10731

Article
Assessment

WAIS-IV Profile of Cognition in


20(4) 462­–473
© The Author(s) 2013
Reprints and permissions:

Schizophrenia sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1073191113478153
asm.sagepub.com

Natalie M. Michel1, Joel O. Goldberg1,2, R. Walter Heinrichs1, Ashley A. Miles1,


Narmeen Ammari1, and Stephanie McDermid Vaz2,3

Abstract
The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) has been used extensively to study impairment across a range of cognitive
domains in schizophrenia. However, cognitive performance among those with the illness has yet to be examined using
the newest edition of this measure. Hence, the current study aims first, to provide WAIS-IV normative data for Canadian
individuals with schizophrenia of low average intelligence; second, to examine schizophrenia performance on all WAIS-IV
subtest, index and general intelligence scores relative to healthy comparison subjects; and third, to revalidate the pattern
of impairment identified in this clinical group using the WAIS-III, where processing speed (PS) was most affected, followed
by working memory (WM), perceptual reasoning (PR) and verbal comprehension (VC). The WAIS-IV was administered to
outpatients with schizophrenia and their performance compared with age, gender, and education matched controls. WAIS-
IV schizophrenia performance data are provided. Analyses revealed significant impairment on several tasks, including the
new Cancellation subtest and the VC supplemental subtest, Comprehension. At the index score level, group differences in
PS were significantly larger than those observed in all other cognitive domains. Impairments were also observed in WM
amid relatively preserved performance in VC, thereby confirming the pattern of impairment identified using the WAIS-III.

Keywords
schizophrenia, cognition, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, processing speed, working memory, verbal comprehension, can-
cellation

Cognitive impairment is now widely recognized as a central have been linked to poor quality of life, social functioning,
feature of schizophrenia (Heinrichs, 2005). Difficulties and community outcome (Green, Kern, & Heaton, 2004;
with attention, working memory and higher order executive Williams et al., 2008). Given the pervasive and integral
functioning are prominent in this population (Forbes, Car- nature of cognitive impairments in schizophrenia, the con-
rick, McIntosh, & Lawrie, 2008; Johnson-Selfridge & tinued refinement of methods used to quantify and qualify
Zalewski, 2001; Lee & Park, 2005; Reichenberg & Harvey, these deficits is necessary to advance the field.
2007) and deficits in processing speed are especially pro- The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) is a com-
nounced (Knowles, David, & Reichenberg, 2010). In fact, prehensive test of general intellectual ability comprising
patients perform 1.5 to 2.0 standard deviations below subtests spanning four domains of cognitive functioning,
healthy controls on many tests of cognitive functioning namely, verbal comprehension, perceptual reasoning, work-
(Keefe & Fenton, 2007). ing memory, and processing speed (Wechsler, 2008). For
Cognitive impairments in schizophrenia are posited to more than four decades, the WAIS has been used exten-
reflect an underlying neurological vulnerability to the ill- sively to examine cognitive ability in schizophrenia, with
ness since they precede illness onset, are stable over time, patients impaired relative to comparison subjects on most
persist when symptoms begin to remit, and are associated tasks (Allen et al., 1998; Dickinson & Coursey, 2002;
with frontal and medial temporal lobe dysfunction (Albus
et al., 2006; Erlenmeyer-Kimling et al., 2000; Hughes et al., 1
York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
2002; Joyce & Huddy, 2004; Rund, 1998). Research has 2
McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada
3
also shown that cognitive functioning is more accurate than St. Joseph’s Healthcare, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada
neurobiology in distinguishing schizophrenia patients from
Corresponding Author:
healthy comparison subjects (Davidson & Heinrichs, 2003; Joel O. Goldberg, Department of Psychology,York University, 4700 Keele
Heinrichs, 2005). Furthermore, difficulties in neurocogni- Street, Toronto, Ontario, M3J 1P3, Canada.
tion have important clinical implications. Cognitive deficits Email: jgoldber@yorku.ca
Michel et al. 463

Dickinson, Iannone, & Gold, 2002; Gold et al., 1995; verbal knowledge (Allen et al., 1998; Chen & Yao, 2009;
Jortner, 1970; Revheim et al., 2006). However, to our Dickinson & Coursey, 2002; Dickinson et al., 2002;
knowledge, the cognitive impairments affecting individuals Dickinson et al., 2004; Gold, Queern, Iannone, & Buchanan,
with the illness have yet to be assessed using the latest edi- 1999; Goldberg & Gold, 1995; G. Goldstein, Beers, &
tion of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, the WAIS-IV Shemansky, 1996; Lezak, 1995; Nestor, Kubicki, et al.,
(Wechsler, 2008). 2010; Nestor, Niznikiewicz, & McCarley, 2010; Nuechterlein
A number of revisions were made to enhance the et al., 2004; Psychological Corporation, 1997). It is not yet
WAIS-IV relative to its predecessors (Frazier, 2011). First, known whether this pattern of cognitive impairment will
whereas the WAIS-III (Wechsler, 1997) collapsed tasks be equally robust when performance is assessed using the
involving verbal and working memory into the Verbal IQ WAIS-IV.
score, and tasks involving processing speed and perceptual In addition, seldom have neurocognitive studies in
organization into the Performance IQ score, the WAIS-IV schizophrenia simultaneously matched control and clinical
omits the summary Verbal IQ and Performance IQ scores groups on a number of key demographic variables known
and places greater emphasis on the four distinct index- to affect cognition. Gender is one such factor. Sexual
based measures previously confirmed by factor analysis dimorphisms have been noted across cognitive domains in
(Bowden, Saklofske, & Weiss, 2011a; Dickinson et al., disordered and nondisordered populations alike, with
2002; Dickinson, Iannone, Wilk, & Gold, 2004). Second, women generally outperforming their male counterparts on
three new subtests, namely, Visual Puzzles, Figure Weights, tasks of language and memory, and men demonstrating
and Cancellation, were added; and two others, Picture an advantage on tasks that draw on spatial abilities
Arrangement and Object Assembly were eliminated (Beatty, Mold, & Gontkovsky, 2003; Feingold, 1993; J. M.
(Hartman, 2009). Subtests used to derive the Perceptual Goldstein et al., 1998; Halari, Mehrotra, Sharma, Ng, &
Reasoning Index (PRI) were also changed. Whereas the Kumari, 2006; Hoff, Riordan, O’Donnell, & DeLisi, 1991;
WAIS-III Perceptual Organization Index was derived from Maylor et al., 2007; Sota & Heinrichs, 2003). Furthermore,
Block Design, Matrix Reasoning, and Picture Completion, some research suggests that relative to healthy comparison
the core subtests of the analogous index measure in the subjects, cognitive impairments are more pronounced
WAIS-IV include the new Visual Puzzles task and omit among men with schizophrenia than among women, indi-
Picture Completion, now deemed a supplemental subtest. In cating that gender may also interact with group member-
addition, adjustments to individual items and subtests were ship to affect cognition (Heinrichs & Zakzanis, 1998;
made in an attempt to improve clarity, decrease fine motor Sota & Heinrichs, 2003). Educational achievement is yet
demands, reduce cultural biases, and decrease administra- another variable to consider (Stratta, Prosperini, Daneluzzo,
tion time (Frazier, 2011; Hartman, 2009). Discontinue rules, Bustini, & Rossi, 2001), with years of education account-
for example, were shortened for 4 of the 15 subtests, item ing for a significant proportion of the variance in neuropsy-
illustrations were simplified and subtests which made use chological test performance (G. Goldstein, Zubin, &
of similar sounding letters or numbers (e.g., P and B) were Pogue-Geile, 1991; Gontkovsky, Mold, & Beatty, 2002).
revised. Subtest instructions were also simplified and dem- This relationship is especially important to be mindful of in
onstration and sample items added. Finally, the WAIS-IV schizophrenia, since most affected individuals achieve no
standardization sample was updated to reflect our aging and more than a high school education (Heinrichs, 2005;
increasingly diverse population (Frazier, 2011; Hartman, Heinrichs & Zakzanis, 1998; Jones, Guth, Lewis, &
2009; Wechsler, Coalson, & Raiford, 2008). This has the Murray, 1994). Finally, age has also been associated with
added advantage of correcting for inflation to intelligence cognition (Jeste, Wolkowitz, & Palmer, 2011). These find-
scores which occurs when dated norms are used (Flynn, ings underscore the importance of closely matching, or
2009). As a result of these changes, the WAIS-IV boasts otherwise controlling, for gender, education, and age when
substantial improvement over its predecessors. examining WAIS-IV cognitive performance in schizophre-
At present, schizophrenia norms for the WAIS-IV sub- nia relative to controls. If not, observed between-group dif-
test, index, and general intelligence scores have not been ferences risk being an artifact of variations in group
reported in the literature, nor have they been examined demographics rather than a reflection of true cognitive
alongside a comparable group of healthy individuals. impairment in the clinical group.
Likewise, group data for the three new subtests of the Hence, in the current study, WAIS-IV cognitive perfor-
WAIS-IV are lacking. Moreover, studies using earlier ver- mance was examined in a sample of outpatients with schizo-
sions of the WAIS have demonstrated a particular pattern of phrenia or schizoaffective disorder relative to healthy
cognitive impairment, with tasks involving processing control participants individually matched for age, gender,
speed yielding the largest group differences, followed by and education. Study goals were threefold: first, to provide
working memory and perceptual organization, amid rela- mean group performance data for a Canadian reference
tively well-preserved performance on subtests involving sample of individuals with schizophrenia on each of the 15
464 Assessment 20(4)

subtests, four index scores and two general intelligence existing differences, whereas increases beyond that ratio do
measures of the WAIS-IV; second, to assess schizophrenia little to improve power, we considered a control sample five
performance on these scales relative to healthy comparison times the size of our schizophrenia group. Each participant
subjects; and third, to obtain data on the pattern of perfor- with schizophrenia was individually matched to five indi-
mance across the four cognitive domains identified by the viduals from the standardization sample who were of the
WAIS-IV in the clinical group relative to controls. same gender, had attained the same level of education and
Consistent with previous research using the WAIS (Allen et who most closely approached the patient in age. This
al., 1998; Dickinson & Coursey, 2002; Dickinson et al., yielded a control sample of 185 participants. Information
2004; Gold et al., 1999; Nestor, Kubicki, et al., 2010; regarding exclusion criteria for the control sample can be
Nestor, Niznikiewicz, & McCarley, 2010; Psychological found in the WAIS-IV Technical and Interpretive Manual
Corporation, 1997), we hypothesized that individuals with (Wechsler et al., 2008). Table 1 provides the demographic
schizophrenia will demonstrate a differential pattern of per- characteristics of the study participants.
formance wherein the PRI is least impaired, followed by the
Working Memory Index (WMI) and finally, the Processing
Speed Index (PSI) where the greatest impairments are Measures
expected (i.e., PRI > WMI > PSI). Since verbal comprehen- Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I disor-
sion is generally stable over time and relatively resistant to ders–patient edition (SCID-I/P; First et al., 2002). The SCID is
clinical illness (G. Goldstein et al., 1996; Lezak, 1995; a semistructured interview that informs diagnostic deci-
Nuechterlein et al., 2004), we expected patients to evince sions based on DSM-IV-TR criteria. The current study used
preserved performance on verbal tasks relative to controls. the SCID-I/P, an adaptation of this measure designed spe-
cifically for use with individuals who have been identified
as psychiatric patients.
Method Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (Kay, Fiszbein, &
Participants Opler, 1987). The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale is
a widely used measure of symptom severity for patients
Data for 37 outpatients with schizophrenia or schizoaffec- with schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders. Follow-
tive disorder and 185 healthy controls were collected for ing a semistructured interview, patients are rated on 30
this study. Patients were recruited from the Hamilton symptom items which range in severity from 1 (absent) to 7
Program for Schizophrenia, a community-based, case (extreme). Items can be categorized into three dimensions,
management and psychiatric rehabilitation program in namely positive symptoms, negative symptoms, and
Ontario, Canada. Flyers describing the study were posted general symptoms.
at the location and individuals interested in participating Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–fourth edition (Wechsler,
were asked to communicate their interest via their case- 2008). The WAIS-IV is a comprehensive test of intellectual
workers. All clients were assessed using the Structured functioning. It consists of 10 core and 5 supplemental sub-
Clinical Interview for the Diagnostic and Statistical tests. Raw subtest scores are converted into scaled scores
Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition, text revision corrected for age group. Subtest scaled scores are standard-
(DSM-IV-TR SCID-I; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, ized to a mean of 10, with one standard deviation reflected
2002) and met DSM-IV-TR criteria (American Psychiatric in three point increments.
Association, 2000) for schizophrenia or schizoaffective Subtests measuring similar aspects of cognitive ability
disorder. Participants who presented with Cushing’s dis- are combined to derive one of four index scores: Verbal
ease, mental retardation, or a comorbid substance use Comprehension (VCI; Similarities, Vocabulary, and
disorder, learning disorder, developmental disability, neu- Information), Perceptual Reasoning (PRI; Block Design,
rological disorder, or a thyroid or endocrine problem were Matrix Reasoning, and Visual Puzzles), Working Memory
excluded. Participants were also required to have normal (WMI; Digit Span and Arithmetic), and Processing Speed
or corrected-to-normal vision and hearing, as well as (PSI; Symbol Search and Coding). Supplemental subtests
English as a first language. associated with these indices are Comprehension (VCI),
Data for healthy controls were extracted from Pearson Figure Weights and Picture Completion (PRI), Letter–
Assessment’s American standardization sample, which Number Sequencing (WMI), and Cancellation (PSI). These
includes 800 examinees ranging from 16 to 90 years of age. are meant to replace a core subtest when performance on the
Given limitations in collecting additional data for our clini- latter has been compromised because of factors unrelated to
cal group, we chose to consider a larger subset of controls the participant or task in question. For example, a partici-
so as to improve statistical power. Guided by Grimes and pant’s Arithmetic score may not be reflective of his or her
Schulz (2005), who note that increasing the number of con- true ability if during the administration of the test, he or she
trols to a ratio of 4:1 improves a study’s ability to detect was distracted by loud noises (Lichtenberger & Kaufman,
Michel et al. 465

Table 1. Demographic Characteristics. scores alone. All four index scores, in addition to the FSIQ
and GAI, are standardized to a mean of 100, with one stan-
Schizophrenia Controls
dard deviation reflected in 15-point increments.
N 37 185
Sex male, n (%) 25 (67.6) 125 (67.6)
Age in years (mean ± SD)a 45.08 ± 7.83 44.49 ± 9.63 Procedure
Education in years, n (%)   This research protocol was approved by the Ethics Review
  0-11 (Did not graduate from 9 (24.32) 45 (24.32) Board and the Human Participants Review Sub-Committee
high school) at York University. It also conforms to the standards of the
  12 (High school graduate or 16 (43.24) 80 (43.24) Canadian Tri-Council research ethics guidelines.
equivalent)
Prior to beginning the study, participants were thor-
  13-15 (Some postsecondary) 6 (16.21) 30 (16.21)
oughly briefed on the research protocol and intent, and writ-
  16 or More (completed 6 (16.21) 30 (16.21)
postsecondary degree or ten informed consent was obtained. Participants in our
more) clinical group were assessed on two occasions and were
Ethnicity, n (%)   provided remuneration for their time. During the initial
 Caucasian 34 (91.89)   assessment, chart reviews and brief semistructured inter-
 Non-Caucasian 2 (5.41)   views were carried out to gather relevant demographic and
 Undisclosed 1 (2.70)   clinical information. This included the participant’s age,
Clinical diagnosis, n (%)   date of birth, education level, ethnicity, formal diagnosis,
 Schizophrenia 33 (89.19)   age at onset of illness, and a list of his or her prescribed
  Schizoaffective disorder 4 (10.81)   medications. Comorbid psychiatric condition(s) were also
Age at onset of illness in years 20.31 ± 3.98   noted. The SCID-I/P was then administered to confirm a
(mean ± SD) diagnosis of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder and
Duration of illness in years 24.71 ± 8.27   participants’ symptoms were assessed using the Positive
(mean ± SD)
and Negative Syndrome Scale rating measure. During a
PANSS scoreb (mean ± SD)  
second meeting, participants completed the core and sup-
 Positive 18.14 ± 5.11  
plemental subtests of the WAIS-IV.
 Negative 15.92 ± 5.19  
 General 31.95 ± 7.35  
Antipsychotics, n (%)   Data Analysis
  Taking typical or first- 6 (16.21)  
generation antipsychotics Scaled scores. Data for this study were collected as part of
  Taking atypical or second- 28 (75.68)   a larger examination of a new measure of social perception
generation antipsychotics developed by Pearson Assessment, for which only American
  Taking a combination 2 (5.41)   standardization information is presently available. This has
of typical and atypical two noteworthy and related implications for the current
antipsychotics study. First, since funding was awarded for the express pur-
  Not taking any antipsychotic 1 (2.70)   pose of providing remuneration to patients, control data
medication
were extracted from the WAIS-IV American standardization
  Dosage in mg/dayc (mean 14.16 ± 10.07  
sample and as such, our Canadian clinical group was com-
± SD)
pared with an American control population. Second, WAIS-
Note. PANSS = Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale. IV cognitive raw scores for our schizophrenia sample were
a. Independent samples t test revealed the difference in age across groups converted into subtest and index scaled scores using Ameri-
was not statistically significant.
b. Mean PANSS scores are reflective of individuals who are relatively can norms so as to maintain consistency in the derivation of
stable but receiving outpatient treatment through a rehabilitation pro- clinical and control group data used in our analyses. Note,
gram (Whelton, Pawlick, & Cook, 1999). scaled scores for the schizophrenia group were also derived
c. In Haldol equivalents (Woods, 2003). using Canadian norms, and are provided as a reference in
Table 2 since Canadian population norms for this clinical
group have not yet been published in the literature.
2009). In this instance, the test administrator could use the Although using American norms to derive scaled scores
Letter–Number Sequencing score instead of the Arithmetic for the schizophrenia group introduces a potential bias,
score to calculate the WMI for this participant. findings generated in this manner are argued to be conser-
All core subtest scores contribute to the determination of vative. Research suggests that on average, Canadians score
the Full Scale IQ (FSIQ), whereas the General Ability Index slightly higher on the WAIS-IV than Americans do (Bowden
(GAI) is composed of the six VCI and PRI core subtest et al., 2011a, 2011b). For example, mean raw score
466 Assessment 20(4)

Table 2. Group Differences on WAIS-IV Index and Subtest Scaled Scores.

Schizophrenia (n = 37) Controls (n = 185)


Cohen’s
Tests Meana SD Meanb SD Meanb SD Fc pc dc
Verbal Comprehension Index 92.05 17.35 95.76 14.86 96.92 14.15 0.21 .650 0.08
 Similarities 8.16 3.44 8.81 2.92 9.42 2.70 1.53 .217 0.21
 Vocabulary 8.49 3.81 9.38 3.25 9.33 2.91 0.01 .928 −0.03
 Information 9.24 2.72 9.54 2.64 9.72 2.93 0.12 .732 0.07
 Comprehension 6.49 3.68 7.76 3.26 9.56 3.08 10.37 .001 0.56
Perceptual Reasoning Index 86.41 17.35 91.38 16.20 97.01 15.26 4.11 .044 0.36
  Block Design 7.27 3.07 8.24 2.86 9.61 2.92 6.74 .010 0.48
  Matrix Reasoning 8.27 3.18 8.92 3.10 9.29 3.10 0.43 .511 0.13
  Visual Puzzles 7.76 3.40 8.46 3.41 9.69 3.19 4.50 .035 0.36
  Figure Weights 7.89 2.98 8.32 3.07 9.59 3.21 4.85 .029 0.41
  Picture Completion 7.41 2.64 7.32 2.59 9.83 3.49 17.17 <.001 0.81
Working Memory Index 84.19 19.50 89.35 18.77 96.57 15.27 6.37 .012 0.42
  Digit Span 7.24 3.44 7.95 3.54 9.49 3.08 7.32 .007 0.45
 Arithmetic 7.51 3.74 8.32 3.70 9.35 3.04 3.22 .074 0.33
  Letter–Number Sequencing 7.76 3.32 8.51 2.98 9.30 2.73 2.47 .118 0.28
Processing Speed Index 78.97 15.43 82.95 14.60 95.71 14.69 23.32 <.001 0.87
  Symbol Search 6.49 2.98 7.11 2.91 9.28 2.98 16.46 <.001 0.75
 Coding 6.16 2.76 6.57 2.78 9.16 3.10 22.21 <.001 0.88
 Cancellation 5.84 2.58 5.92 2.52 9.47 3.04 44.45 <.001 1.30
Full Scale IQ 82.84 18.85 88.51 17.02 95.85 14.53 7.45 .007 0.47
General Ability 88.16 18.03 92.86 16.39 96.66 14.66 1.99 .160 0.24
Note. WAIS-IV = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–fourth edition. Mean differences in scaled scores derived using American versus Canadian norms are
0.61 (SD = 0.34) and 4.46 (SD = 0.72) for subtests and index scaled scores, respectively.
a. Derived using Canadian norms.
b. Derived using American norms.
c. Analysis of variance results comparing control and patient groups’ WAIS-IV performance derived using American norms.

performance on the Block Design task is approximately 41 (ANOVAs) were then carried out to explicate between
for Canadians and 34 for Americans. However, when devel- group differences on each of these scales. Group differences
oping standardization data for each population, mean sub- on the index and global cognitive scores were also assessed
test raw scores are independently set to a scaled score of 10 using univariate ANOVAs. Alpha levels were adjusted at
(i.e., in the example above, both raw scores would convert each stage of the analysis using a Bonferroni correction. To
to a scaled score of 10 using the appropriate norms). evaluate the magnitude of the effect of group on each of the
Conversely, raw scores typically convert into higher scaled dependent variables, effect size calculations (Cohen’s d;
scores when Americans norms are used resulting in a slight Cohen, 1992) were undertaken by dividing the mean group
overestimate in performance relative to a Canadian refer- difference by the pooled standard deviation for each of
ence sample (see Table 2). Therefore, the chances of finding the scales.
mean differences between the patient and control groups in To investigate the pattern of performance across groups
this study are reduced, and any significant group differ- on the VCI, PRI, WMI, and PSI, a multivariate approach to
ences which are observed are likely reflective of legitimate repeated measures analysis was used, followed by post hoc
cognitive impairment in the schizophrenia group. contrasts.
WAIS-IV performance and group differences. Mean group
scaled scores were calculated for each of the 15 subtests,
four index scores, and two general intelligence measures of Results
the WAIS-IV. Scaled scores derived using American norms WAIS-IV Cognitive Profile in Schizophrenia
were used in the analyses that follow.
The effect of group on the WAIS-IV subtest scores was WAIS-IV mean subtest, index, and general intelligence
examined using a multivariate analysis of variance scaled scores derived using American and Canadian norms
(MANOVA). Post hoc univariate analyses of variance are presented in Table 2.
Michel et al. 467

10.50
10.00
9.50

Subtest Scaled Scores


9.00
8.50
8.00
7.50
7.00
6.50
6.00
5.50
5.00

Schizophrenia Healthy Controls

Figure 1. Group performance on WAIS-IV cognitive subtest scaled scores.


Note. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals. WAIS-IV = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–fourth edition.
*p < .003.

MANOVA was first used to assess whether schizophre- PRI, WMI, and PSI, with the PSI yielding large effect size
nia group membership conferred an overall disadvantage on differences across groups (p < .001, Cohen’s d = 0.87).
the 15 WAIS-IV subtests relative to mentally healthy con- Differences on the WMI were also significant (p = .012,
trols. All assumptions were met and analyses revealed a Cohen’s d = 0.42), but the PRI only showed a trend toward
main effect of group, Λ = 0.72, F(15, 206) = 5.25, p < .001. impairment after adjusting for multiple comparisons (p =
Results for the post hoc univariate ANOVAs are presented .044, Cohen’s d = 0.36). In contrast, patient performance on
in Table 2 and illustrated in Figure 1. Mean differences for the VCI was comparable with controls (p = .65). Group dif-
5 of the 15 subtests were significant after adjusting the ferences on the WAIS-IV index scores are presented in
alpha level using a Bonferroni correction. The magnitudes Table 2 and depicted in Figure 2.
of group differences on the Comprehension and Symbol Between-group differences on the GAI and FSIQ were
Search subtests were in the medium range (Cohen’s d = also assessed using univariate ANOVAs. Analyses revealed
0.56 and 0.75, respectively). Large effect sizes were noted that schizophrenia group performance differed significantly
for the Picture Completion subtest (Cohen’s d = 0.81), as from healthy comparisons on the FSIQ (F = 7.42, p = .007,
well as two of the three processing speed subtests: Cohen’s d = 0.47) but not the GAI (F = 1.99, p = .160,
Cancellation and Coding (Cohen’s d = 1.30 and 0.88, Cohen’s d = 0.24).
respectively). Mean differences on the two new PRI sub-
tests, Figure Weights, and Visual Puzzles, were not signifi-
cant following a Bonferroni correction (p = .029 and p = Pattern of Performance on the WAIS-IV
.035, respectively). All scaled scores were normally distrib- Index Scores
uted. However, the assumption of equality of variance was A multivariate approach to repeated measures analysis was
met for all but the Picture Completion variable. Hence, used to investigate the pattern of performance across cogni-
group differences on this subtest were further examined tive domains in the schizophrenia group relative to con-
using post hoc independent samples t tests (equal variances trols. All assumptions were met, and analyses revealed a
not assumed), as well as a Mann–Whitney nonparametric significant group-by-domain interaction, Λ = 0.91, F(3,
test. Results converged on both post hoc tests, demonstrat- 218) = 6.84, p < .001. Follow-up post hoc contrasts
ing a significant between-group difference, t(65.17) = 5.04, revealed that relative to controls, the degree of impairment
p < .001; U = 1998.50, z = −4.01, p < .001. on the PSI in the schizophrenia group was significantly
Univariate ANOVAs were then carried out for each of larger than that observed on the VCI, PRI, and WMI, F(1,
the four index scores. All assumptions were met, and results 220) = 19.31, p < .001; F(1, 220) = 7.17, p = .008; and F(1,
were in keeping with our hypothesis. The schizophrenia 220) = 3.90, p = .050, respectively. Individuals with schizo-
group evinced a pattern of increasing impairment on the phrenia also performed more poorly on the WMI than they
468 Assessment 20(4)

102.00
100.00
98.00
96.00
Index Scaled Scores 94.00
92.00
90.00
88.00
86.00
84.00
82.00
80.00
78.00
76.00
Verbal Perceptual Working
* *
Processing
Comprehension Reasoning Memory Speed

Schizophrenia Healthy Controls

Figure 2. Group performance on WAIS-IV cognitive index scores.


Note. Error bars reflect 95% confidence intervals. WAIS-IV = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–fourth edition.
*p < .0125.

did on the VCI, F(1, 220) = 7.06, p = .008. In contrast, including Coding. This finding is interesting in light of
group differences on the PRI did not differ significantly recent meta-analytic work by Dickinson, Ramsey, and Gold
from those observed on the VCI and WMI, F(1, 220) = (2007) that shows that impairments on Digit Symbol Coding
3.74, p = .054 and F(1, 220) = 0.35, p = .557, respectively. are more substantial than those observed on all other neuro-
psychological measures in the review, and may therefore
reflect the single largest cognitive deficit in schizophrenia.
Discussion Future research should examine WAIS-IV performance on
The present study is the first to examine cognitive perfor- both the Coding and Cancellation subtests, so as to deter-
mance in schizophrenia using the latest edition of the mine whether the latter task is in fact a more sensitive mea-
WAIS. Results demonstrate that the WAIS-IV is sensitive to sure of processing speed impairment in this population.
schizophrenia, with patients performing below demograph- This is especially important given that the mean scatter of
ically matched control participants on most subtests and scores derived using Canadian and American norms dif-
index scores. Statistically significant impairments were fered in this study, and as a result, individual subtest dis-
evident on the processing speed and working memory sum- crepancies may be more or less evident when norms from
mary indices, as well as 5 of the 15 subtests after applying one country are used over another.
a Bonferroni correction. In addition, schizophrenia patients Also noteworthy is the variability in performance across
showed the expected pattern of differential impairment subtests of a given domain. For instance, although perfor-
across the four cognitive domains, as previously identified mance on the core subtests of the VCI was relatively intact
using the WAIS-III. in the patient group, individuals with schizophrenia evinced
The introduction of new WAIS subtests has the potential significant impairment on the supplemental subtest of this
to uncover subtle but important differential sensitivities, to index, namely Comprehension. A similar pattern of results
which the clinician needs to be alert in case work. Three has been noted using the WAIS-IV in other clinical groups
new subtests were added to the WAIS-IV. Although results as well (Holdnack, Goldstein, & Drozdick, 2011). Several
are suggestive of a trend toward impairment on Visual items in this subtest rely on abstract reasoning. Participants
Puzzles and Figure Weights, patients demonstrated greatest may be asked, for example, to explain metaphors or the
difficulty on the PSI supplemental subtest, Cancellation, a meaning of less well-known proverbs (Lichtenberger &
search task where subjects are asked to identify target Kaufman, 2009). This may be especially challenging for
objects of a particular shape and color from a visual display individuals who engage in ‘unusual and idiosyncratic’
(McCrae & Robinson, 2011). In fact, effect size analyses thinking, which is often the case in schizophrenia.
revealed that the mean group difference on Cancellation These findings call attention to the potential implications
surpassed that observed on all other WAIS-IV subtests, of substituting core subtest scores with supplemental subtest
Michel et al. 469

scores when working with individuals affected by schizophre- Performance differences on Arithmetic were more mild (p =
nia. The supplemental VCI subtest, Comprehension, appears .074), however, demonstrating variability in task perfor-
to be more challenging than the core VCI subtests for this mance across WMI subtests.
population. Therefore, performance on this subtest may not Still, effect size differences on the WMI and its subtests
be comparable to performance on Vocabulary, Similarities, were smaller than anticipated. Whereas previous research
and Information. Should clinicians use Comprehension to using the WAIS-III working memory subtests and similar
replace a spoiled core subtest in calculating the VCI, they measures has yielded effects sizes in the medium to large
should be aware that this may result in a lower index level range (Dickinson et al., 2007; Forbes et al., 2008; Lee &
score than expected. A similar argument may be made for Park, 2005), small to medium effect sizes were noted in the
the Cancellation subtest of the PSI. Further research directly current study. We offer a few potential explanations for this
assessing the impact of supplemental subtest substitution in discrepancy. To begin, the clinical sample in the present
the WAIS-IV across clinical and nonclinical groups is war- study consisted of a group of outpatients with schizophre-
ranted, particularly since substantial variability in perfor- nia. Conversely, meta-analytic studies identifying medium
mance across core and supplemental subtests has not been to large effect size differences in working memory have
observed in nonschizophrenia normative samples (see con- collapsed data across inpatient and outpatient samples (see
trol group data Table 2; see also Bowden et al., 2011a, Forbes et al., 2008; Lee & Park, 2005). In addition, as part
2011b; Ryan & Glass, 2011). of the changes made to the WAIS-IV, a number of WMI
Results from analyses examining the differential pattern subtest items were revised. For instance, WAIS-III Letter–
of impairment across the four cognitive domains were con- Number Sequencing and Digit Span items that were
sistent with previous literature (Allen et al., 1998; Chen & deemed confusing (e.g., items that included similar sound-
Yao, 2009; Dickinson & Coursey, 2002; Dickinson et al., ing digits or letters), were adjusted or removed to improve
2002; Dickinson et al., 2004; Gold et al., 1999; Goldberg & task clarity in the WAIS-IV (Hartman, 2009). The
Gold, 1995; G. Goldstein et al., 1996; Lezak, 1995; Nestor, Arithmetic subtest was also altered to decrease the empha-
Kubicki, et al., 2010; Nestor, Niznikiewicz, & McCarley, sis on mathematical skills and the English measurement
2010; Nuechterlein et al., 2004; Psychological Corporation, system. Together, these changes may have resulted in a
1997) and in line with our predictions. Individuals with slight decrease in the measure’s sensitivity to impairments
schizophrenia evinced most impairment on the PSI relative in schizophrenia than was previously the case. Future con-
to age, education, and gender-matched controls. Impairments current validity research comparing WAIS-III and WAIS-IV
were also noted on the WMI and a trend toward impairment WMI scores with other measures of working memory
was noted on the PRI. In contrast, preserved performance should clarify this discrepancy.
was observed on the VCI. WAIS-IV PRI clinical group performance was compa-
In keeping with meta-analytic work by Knowles et al. rable to previous literature using the WAIS-III in this popu-
(2010), group differences on the PSI were especially nota- lation (Dickinson et al., 2002; Nestor, Kubicki, et al., 2010;
ble. Effect sizes were in the large range, and between-group Nestor, Niznikiewicz, & McCarley, 2010). Relative to con-
differences were significantly larger than those observed on trols, individuals with schizophrenia expressed greater dif-
all other index measures. Likewise, schizophrenia perfor- ficulty in perceptual reasoning with group differences
mance on the PSI subtests (Symbol Search, Coding, and showing a trend toward significance after adjusting for the
Cancellation) was also indicative of substantial impairment, family-wise error rate.
with effect sizes in the medium to large range. However, mean performance differences on the PRI
Impaired functioning in the schizophrenia group was were not significantly larger than those observed on the
also apparent on the WAIS-IV WMI, and group differences VCI, where preserved performance was found. Findings
here were significantly larger than those observed on the here are not surprising. Previous literature supports the
VCI. This is consistent with research in the field, which has notion that tasks relying on perceptual organization skills
repeatedly found that the short-term maintenance and are generally more difficult than tasks assessing verbal
manipulation of information in memory is an area of diffi- abilities in schizophrenia (Allen et al., 1998; Dickinson et
culty for this population (Forbes et al., 2008; Gold, al., 2004; Nuechterlein et al., 2004), as was noted in the
Carpenter, Randolph, Goldberg, & Weinberg, 1997; Lee & present study. However, findings regarding the degree of
Park, 2005). Although, mean differences on tasks associ- impairment on Perceptual Reasoning tasks are mixed.
ated with the WMI were not significant on their own, core Whereas some studies find medium to large group differ-
WMI subtests certainly showed a trend toward impairment. ences on PRI subtests (Dickinson et al., 2007), other
In fact, group differences on Digit Span would have reached research finds that impairment on these tasks is more mod-
significance (p = .007) were it not for the strict application erate (Dickinson et al., 2004).
of a Bonferroni correction to control for the family-wise Moreover, given recent changes to the tasks that com-
error rate (alpha required for statistical significance = .003). prise the core subtests of the PRI in the WAIS-IV, it was
470 Assessment 20(4)

difficult to predict whether the index score would be more or hypothesis, with patients expressing most impairment on
less sensitive to perceptual reasoning impairments in our tasks of processing speed, and least impairment on tasks rely-
clinical group, relative to studies examining similar deficits ing on verbal knowledge. It is likely that this pattern broadly
using previous versions of the WAIS. Results of the present characterizes schizophrenia as a diagnostic group. Findings
study indicate that omitting Picture Completion in favor of of the present study provide a reference against which clini-
Visual Puzzles as a core PRI subtest may result in an index cians and researchers may compare WAIS-IV impairment
score that is less sensitive to impairment in schizophrenia. patterns among people with schizophrenia, and may serve to
Of all the PRI subtests, Picture Completion appears to be the complement more comprehensive clinical and cognitive
most challenging for individuals with the illness, with effect assessments. Schizophrenia group scaled score performance
sizes in the large range (p < .001, Cohen’s d = 0.81). In con- derived using Canadian norms may also prove useful in dis-
trast, our clinical group was less impaired on Visual Puzzles, criminating meaningful impairment when individual perfor-
a subtest which is new to the WAIS. Future research may mance on a particular subtest or index score is of interest.
attempt to assess the relative sensitivity of these measures to However, clinicians are cautioned against applying the
impairments in perceptual organization in schizophrenia. pattern of performance observed herein as some kind of
In contrast to all other index measures, performance on definitive schizophrenia rubric. Significant variability in
the VCI was not different between groups, reflecting pre- performance across cognitive domains exists between par-
served verbal ability in our outpatient sample relative to ticipants. Not all individuals with schizophrenia show
demographically matched controls. This finding was greatest impairment on the PSI, for instance, and some
expected since a majority of VCI subtest items draw on express difficulty on verbal tasks. Similarly, some individu-
crystallized verbal knowledge (Dawes, Jeste, & Palmer, als with the illness appear cognitively unimpaired relative
2011), which is known to be relatively resilient to clinical to comparable healthy subjects, scoring in the high average
illness and stable over time (Dickinson et al., 2002; Gold et range across subtests. Furthermore, our sample consisted of
al., 1999; Goldberg & Gold, 1995; G. Goldstein et al., 1996; a relatively high-functioning group of Canadian outpatients,
Groth-Marnat, 2009; Nuechterlein et al., 2004). Mean VCI and differences in the degree of impairment across scores
performance in our group was also comparable to prior are also likely affected by other factors, such as the severity
research using the WAIS-III to assess cognition in schizo- of illness and geographic location. Therefore, clinicians are
phrenia (Dickinson et al., 2002; Nestor, Niznikiewicz, & encouraged instead to use the current study’s findings as
McCarley, 2010). general guidelines for the idiographic interpretation of
Nevertheless, the finding of preserved verbal task perfor- neurocognitive assessments.
mance in schizophrenia is a controversial one, with other Study limitations include a relatively small schizophre-
research finding impairment on the Vocabulary, Similarities, nia sample size. Future research should endeavor to collect
and Information subtests of the WAIS (Dickinson et al., larger samples, as this would allow for a more detailed
2007). Variations in the way control and clinical groups are examination of WAIS-IV cognitive performance than was
matched may partially explain this difference. It is not possible here. The effect of ethnicity and antipsychotic dos-
always the case, for instance that groups are matched on age, for instance, which have previously been found to
demographic variables known to affect cognitive perfor- influence cognitive performance in this population
mance. This is problematic when between-group differences (Knowles et al., 2010; Walker, Batchelor, & Shores, 2009)
along these variables are apparent. As an example, in many may be assessed if larger samples are available. Also, ran-
studies control participants are on average slightly more dom selection of study participants was not possible.
educated than individuals affected by schizophrenia Samples were instead selected based on availability, affect-
(Dickinson et al., 2007), thereby conferring a disadvantage ing the generalizability of these findings. Additionally,
to patient groups as compared with controls on cognitive future studies should endeavor to examine patient and con-
measures. In other research, participants are matched on trol samples of the same nationality. This will likely result
parental level of education. Parents of individuals with in greater between-group performance discrepancies for
schizophrenia typically achieve higher levels of education some subtests and index scores. Studies examining clinical
than their children whose academic pursuits are often inter- and control groups matched on other variables of interest,
rupted by illness onset. Hence, matching on parental, and not such as socioeconomic status and parental level of educa-
participant, level of education may similarly result in tion, may also provide new insights on WAIS-IV cognitive
increased performance discrepancies between schizophrenia performance in schizophrenia.
and control groups. Continued research examining perfor-
mance on VCI subtests between patients and demographi-
cally matched controls is needed to determine if, in fact, Conclusions
these measures are intact in schizophrenia. The present study was the first to examine cognitive per-
In summary, the overall pattern of impairment observed formance in a group of individuals with schizophrenia
across WAIS-IV index scores was in keeping with our using the WAIS-IV (Wechsler, 2008). Results indicate that
Michel et al. 471

relative to age-, gender-, and education level–matched of the WAIS-R in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia
controls, patients express difficulty on several WAIS-IV Research, 34, 87-94. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(98)00090-5
subtests and domains. They further suggest that the pattern American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statisti-
of cognitive impairment identified in schizophrenia using cal manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision). Wash-
the WAIS-III is robust and remains when cognitive ability ington, DC: Author.
is assessed using the WAIS-IV. Patient and control group Beatty, W. W., Mold, J. W., & Gontkovsky, S. T. (2003). RBANS
performance was comparable on tasks relying on crystal- performance: Influences of sex and education. Journal of
lized verbal knowledge. In contrast, the schizophrenia Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 1065-1069.
group performed below controls on measures of perceptual doi:10.1076/jcen.25.8.1065.16732
reasoning, working memory, and processing speed, with Bowden, S. C., Saklofske, D. H., & Weiss, L. G. (2011a). Aug-
impairments on the latter being significantly larger than menting the core battery with supplementary subtests:
those observed on all other index measures. Similar cogni- Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–IV measurement invari-
tive profiles have been observed in other clinical groups ance across the United States and Canada. Assessment, 18,
including individuals with traumatic brain injury and 133-140. doi:10.1177/1073191110381717
Alzheimer’s disease, and may reflect a common underly- Bowden, S. C., Saklofske, D. H., & Weiss, L. G. (2011b). Invari-
ing neuropathology (Wechsler et al., 2008). Findings may ance of the measurement model underlying the Wechsler
have implications for predicting functional competence Adult Intelligence Scale-IV in the United States and Canada.
and community independence in schizophrenia (Heinrichs, Educational and Psychological Measurement, 71, 186-199.
Ammari, Miles, & McDermid Vaz, 2010). doi:10.1177/0013164410387382
Chen, H., & Yao, S. Q. (2009). A comparison between nonclini-
Acknowledgments cal and schizophrenia sample on the results measured by the
The authors would like to thank Ryan Barnhart, Hugh McCague, WAIS-III Chinese version. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psy-
Mirka Ondrack, and Xiaobin Zhou for their support in this chology, 17, 157-160.
research; the clients and staff at the Hamilton Program for Cohen, J. (1992). Statistical power analysis. Current Directions
Schizophrenia for their participation and assistance; and the in Psychological Science, 1, 98-101. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.
reviewers of this article for their thoughtful comments and sug- ep10768783
gestions. We would also like to thank NCS Pearson for granting Davidson, L., & Heinrichs, R. W. (2003). Quantification of brain
us permission to use the American standardization data from the imaging findings on the frontal and temporal lobes in schizo-
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV). phrenia: A meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimag-
Copyright © 2008 NCS Pearson, Inc. All rights reserved. ing, 122, 69-87. doi:10.1016/S0925-4927(02)00118
Dawes, S. E., Jeste, D. V., & Palmer, B. W. (2011). Cognitive pro-
Declaration of Conflicting Interests files in persons with chronic schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical
The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33, 929-936. doi:10.1080/
to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. 13803395.2011.578569
Dickinson, D., & Coursey, R. D. (2002). Independence and over-
Funding lap among neurocognitive correlates of community function-
The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support ing in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 56, 161-170.
for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00229-8
Natalie M. Michel was supported by a Frederick Banting and Dickinson, D., Iannone, V. N., & Gold, J. M. (2002). Factor struc-
Charles Best Canada Graduate Scholarship from the Canadian ture of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–III in schizophre-
Institutes of Health Research. Minor grant funding for client nia. Assessment, 9, 171-180. doi:10.1177/10791102009002008
honoraria was provided by the Community Social and Dickinson, D., Iannone, V. N., Wilk, C. M., & Gold, J. M.
Vocational Rehabilitation Foundation (CSVR) in Hamilton, (2004). General and specific cognitive deficits in schizophre-
Ontario. nia. Biological Psychiatry, 55, 826-833. doi:10.1016/j.bio-
psych.2003.12.010
References Dickinson, D., Ramsey, M. E., & Gold, J. M. (2007). Overlook-
Albus, M., Hubmann, W., Mohr, F., Hecht, S., Hinterberger-Weber, ing the obvious: A meta-analytic comparison of digit symbol
P., Seitz, N. N., & Küchenhoff, H. (2006). Neurocognitive func- coding tasks and other cognitive measures in schizophrenia.
tioning in patients with first-episode schizophrenia: Results Archives of General Psychiatry, 64, 532-542. doi:10.1001/
of a prospective 5-year follow-up study. European Archives archpsyc.64.5.532
of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 256, 442-451. Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L., Rock, D., Roberts, S. A., Janal, M.,
doi:10.1007/s00406-006-0667-1 Kestenbaum, C., Cornblatt, B., & Gottesman, I. I. (2000).
Allen, D. N., Huegel, S. G., Seaton, B. E., Goldstein, G., Gurklis, J. Attention, memory, and motor skills as childhood predictors
A., & Kammen, D. P. V. (1998). Confirmatory factor analysis of schizophrenia-related psychoses: The New York High Risk
472 Assessment 20(4)

Project. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 1416-1422. Green, M. F., Kern, R. S., & Heaton, R. K. (2004). Longitudinal
doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.157.9.1416 studies of cognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia:
Feingold, A. (1993). Cognitive gender differences: A devel- Implications for MATRICS. Schizophrenia Research, 72, 41-51.
opmental perspective. Sex Roles, 29, 91-112. doi:10.1007/ doi:10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.009
BF00289998 Grimes, D. A., & Schulz, K. F. (2005). Compared to what? Find-
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (2002). ing controls for case-control studies. Lancet, 365, 1429-1433.
Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I disorders, doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(05)66379-9
research version, patient edition (SCID-I/P). New York: Bio- Groth-Marnat, G. (2009). Comparing scores on the WAIS-IV and
metrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute. the WMS-IV. In G. Groth-Marnet (Ed.), Handbook of psy-
Flynn, J. R. (2009). The WAIS-III and WAIS-IV: Daubert chological assessment (5th ed., pp. 183-206). New York, NY:
motions favor the certainly false over the approximately Wiley.
true. Applied Neuropsychology, 16, 98-104. doi:10.1080/ Halari, R., Mehrotra, R., Sharma, T., Ng, V., & Kumai, V. (2006).
09084280902864360 Cognitive impairment but preservation of sexual dimorphism
Forbes, N. F., Carrick, L. A., McIntosh, A. M., & Lawrie, S. M. (2008). in cognitive abilities in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research,
Working memory in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Psycholog- 141, 129-139. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2005.07.021
ical Medicine, 39, 889-905. doi:10.1017/S0033291708004558 Hartman, D. E. (2009). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale IV
Frazier, T. W. (2011). Introduction to the special section on (WAIS-IV): Return of the gold standard. Applied Neuropsy-
advancing WAIS-IV and WMS-IV clinical interpretation. chology, 16, 85-87. doi:10.1080/09084280802644466
Assessment, 18, 131-132. doi:10.1177/1073191111408581 Heinrichs, R. W. (2005). The primacy of cognition in schizophre-
Gold, J. M., Blaxton, T. A., Hermann, B. P., Randolph, C., nia. American Psychologist, 60, 229-242. doi:10.1037/0003-
Fedio, P., Goldberg, T. E., & Weinberger, D. R. (1995). 066X.60.3.229
Memory and intelligence in lateralized temporal lobe epi- Heinrichs, R. W., Ammari, N., Miles, A. A., & McDermid Vaz,
lepsy and schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 17, 59-65. S. (2010). Cognitive performance and functional competence
doi:10.1016/0920-9964(95)00030-P as predictors of community independence in schizophrenia.
Gold, J. M., Carpenter, C., Randolph, C., Goldberg, T. E., & Wein- Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36, 381-387. doi:10.1093/schbul/
berg, D. R. (1997). Auditory working memory and Wisconsin sbn095
Card Sort Test performance in schizophrenia. Archives of Gen- Heinrichs, R. W., & Zakzanis, K. K. (1998). Neurocognitive deficit
eral Psychiatry, 54, 159-165. in schizophrenia: A quantitative review of the evidence. Neu-
Gold, J. M., Queern, C., Iannone, V. N., & Buchanan, R. W. ropsychology, 12, 426-445. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.12.3.426
(1999). Repeatable battery for the assessment of neuropsycho- Hoff, A. L., Riordan, H., O’Donnell, D. W., & DeLisi, L. E.
logical status as a screening test in schizophrenia, I: Sensitiv- (1991). Cross-sectional and longitudinal neuropsycho-
ity, reliability, and validity. American Journal of Psychiatry, logical test findings in first episode schizophrenic patients.
156, 1944-1950. Schizophrenia Research, 5, 197-198. doi:10.1016/0920-
Goldberg, T., & Gold, J. (1995). Neurocognitive functioning in 9964(91)90070-8
patients with schizophrenia: An overview. In D. Kupfer & F. Holdnack, J., Goldstein, G., & Drozdick, L. (2011). Social per-
Bloom (Eds.), Psychopharmacology: A fourth generation of ception and WAIS-IV performance in adolescents and adults
progress (pp. 1245-1257). New York, NY: Raven Press. diagnosed with Asperger’s syndrome and autism. Assessment,
Goldstein, G., Beers, S. R., & Shemansky, W. J. (1996). Neuro- 18, 192-200. doi:10.1177/1073191110394771
psychological differences between schizophrenic patients Hughes, C., Kumari, V., Soni, W., Das, M., Binneman, B., Drozd,
with heterogeneous Wisconsin Card Sorting Test perfor- S., & Sharma, T. (2002). Longitudinal study of symptoms and
mance. Schizophrenia Research, 21, 13-18. doi:10.1016/0920- cognitive function in chronic schizophrenia. Schizophrenia
9964(96)00019-9 Research, 59, 137-146. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00393-0
Goldstein, G., Zubin, J., & Pogue-Geile, M. F. (1991). Hospitaliza- Jeste, D. V., Wolkowitz, O. M., & Palmer, B. W. (2011). Diver-
tion and the cognitive deficits of schizophrenia: The influences gent trajectories of physical, cognitive, and psychosocial
of age and education. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, aging in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 37, 451-455.
179, 202-206. doi:10.1097/00005053-199104000-00004 doi:10.1093/schbul/sbr026
Goldstein, J. M., Seidman, L. J., Goodman, J. M., Koren, D., Lee, H., Johnson-Selfridge, M., & Zalewski, C. (2001). Moderator vari-
Weintraub, S., & Tsuang, M. T. (1998). Are there sex differences ables of executive functioning in schizophrenia: Meta-analytic
in neuropsychological functions among patients with schizo- findings. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 27, 305-316.
phrenia? American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 1358-1364. Jones, R., Guth, L., Lewis, S., & Murray, R. (1994). Low intel-
Gontkovsky, S. T., Mold, J. W., & Beatty, W. W. (2002). Age and edu- ligence and poor educational achievement precede early onset
cational influences on RBANS index scores in a nondemented psychosis. In A. S. David & J. C. Cathy (Eds.), The neuro-
geriatric sample. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 16, 258-263. psychology of schizophrenia (pp. 131-144). Hove, England:
doi:10.1076/clin.16.3.258.13844 Lawrence Erlbaum.
Michel et al. 473

Jortner, S. (1970). Overinclusion responses to WAIS similarities Reichenberg, A., & Harvey, P. D. (2007). Neuropsychological
as suggestive of schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical Psychol- impairment in schizophrenia: Integration of performance-
ogy, 26, 346-348. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(197007) based and brain imaging findings. Psychological Bulletin,
Joyce, E., & Huddy, V. (2004). Defining the cognitive impairment 133, 833-858. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.5.833
in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 34, 1151-1155. Revheim, N., Schechter, I., Kim, D., Silipo, G., Allingham, B.,
doi:10.1017/S0033291704003472 Butler, P., & Javitt, D. C. (2006). Neurocognitive and symp-
Kay, S. R., Fiszbein, A., & Opler, L. A. (1987). The positive tom correlates of daily problem-solving skills in schizophre-
and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. nia. Schizophrenia Research, 83, 237-245. doi:10.1016/j.
Schizophrenia Bulletin, 13(2), 261-276. doi: 10.1093/schbul/ schres.2005.12.849
13.2.261 Rund, B. R. (1998). A review of longitudinal studies of cognitive
Keefe, R. S. E., & Fenton, W. S. (2007). How should DSM-V cri- function in schizophrenia patients. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 24,
teria for schizophrenia include cognitive impairment? Schizo- 425-435.
phrenia Bulletin, 33, 912-920. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbm046 Ryan, J. J., & Glass, L. A. (2010). Substitution of supplementary
Knowles, E. E. M., David, A. S., & Reichenberg, A. (2010). Pro- subtests for core subtests on composite reliability of WAIS-
cessing speed deficits in schizophrenia: Reexamining the IV indexes. Psychological Reports, 106, 13-18. doi:10.2466/
evidence. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 828-835. PR0.106.1.13-18
doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09070937 Sota, T. L., & Heinrichs, R. W. (2003). Sex differences in verbal
Lee, J., & Park, S. (2005). Working memory impairments in memory in schizophrenia patients treated with “typical” neu-
schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychol- roleptics. Schizophrenia Research, 62, 175-182. doi:10.1016/
ogy, 114, 599-611. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.599 S0920-9964(02)00373-0
Lezak, M. D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). Stratta, P., Prosperini, P., Daneluzzo, E., Bustini, M., & Rossi, A.
New York, NY: Oxford University Press. (2001). Educational level and age influence spatial work-
Lichtenberger, E. O., & Kaufman, A. S. (2009). Essentials of ing memory and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test perfor-
WAIS-IV assessment. New York, NY: Wiley. mance differentially: A controlled study in schizophrenic
Maylor, E. A., Reimers, S., Choi, J., Collaer, M. L., Peters, M., patients. Psychiatry Research, 102, 39-48. doi:10.1016/
& Silverman, I. (2007). Gender and sexual orientation differ- S0165-1781(01)00230-X
ences in cognition across adulthood: Age is kinder to women Walker, A. J., Batchelor, J., & Shores, A. (2009). Effects of edu-
than to men regardless of sexual orientation. Archives of Sex- cation and cultural background on performance on WAIS-
ual Behavior, 36, 235-249. doi:10.1007/s10508-006-9155-y III, WMS-III, WAIS-R and WMS-R measures: Systematic
McCrae, S. M., & Robinson, T. P. (2011). Visual puzzles, figure review. Australian Psychologist, 44, 216-223. doi:10.1080/
weights, and cancellation: Some preliminary hypotheses on 00050060902833469
the functional and neural substrates of these three new WAIS- Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Third
IV subtests. International Scholarly Research Network, 2011, edition. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
1-19. doi:10.5402/2011/123173 Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Fourth
Nestor, P. G., Kubicki, M., Nakamura, M., Niznikiewicz, M., edition. San Antonio, TX: Pearson Assessment.
McCarley, R. W., & Shenton, M. E. (2010). Comparing pre- Wechsler, D., Coalson, D. L., & Raiford, S. E. (2008). WAIS-
frontal gray and white matter contributions to intelligence and IV technical and interpretive manual. San Antonio, TX:
decision making in schizophrenia and healthy controls. Neuro- Pearson.
psychology, 24, 121-129. doi:10.1037/a0016981 Whelton, C., Pawlick, J., & Cook, P. E. (1999). Growing with
Nestor, P. G., Niznikiewicz, M., & McCarley, R. W. (2010). people with a psychiatric disability in a psychosocial rehabili-
Distinct contributions of working memory and social com- tation program. Psychiatric Rehabilitation, 22, 290-293.
prehension failures in neuropsychological impairment in Williams, L. M., Whitford, T. J., Flynn, G., Wong, W., Liddell, B.
schizophrenia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disorders, 198, J., Silverstein, S., & Gordon, E. (2008). General and social
206-212. doi:10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181d14143 cognition in first episode schizophrenia: Identification of sep-
Nuechterlein, K. H., Barch, D. M., Gold, J. M., Goldberg, T. arable factors and prediction of functional outcome using the
E., Green, M. F., & Heaton, R. K. (2004). Identification of IntegNeuro test battery. Schizophrenia Research, 99, 182-191.
separable cognitive factors in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia doi:10.1016/j.schres.2007.10.019
Research, 72, 29-39. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.007 Woods, S. W. (2003). Chlorpromazine equivalent doses for the
Psychological Corporation. (1997). WAIS-III WMS-III technical newer atypical antipsychotics. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry,
manual. San Antonio, TX: Author. 64, 663-667.

You might also like