You are on page 1of 14

Directiveness in Teachers’

Language Input to Toddlers and


Preschoolers in Day Care

Luigi Girolametto
University of Toronto Five subtypes of directiveness were examined in the interactions of day care
Toronto, Ontario, Canada teachers with toddler and preschooler groups. The instructional context (book
reading, play dough) yielded significant differences across all five subtypes of
Elaine Weitzman directiveness, indicating that these two activities elicited different types of teacher-
The Hanen Centre child discourse. Book reading was characterized by significantly more behavior
Toronto, Ontario, Canada and response control and less conversation control in comparison with the play-
dough activity. Correlations between teachers’ directiveness and child language
Riet van Lieshout productivity indicated that behavior control and turn-taking control were associ-
Dawna Duff ated with low levels of productivity, whereas conversation control was associated
University of Toronto with the highest levels of productivity. The results of this study confirm that
Toronto, Ontario, Canada instructional context is an important mediator of teachers’ directiveness and
suggest that subtypes of directiveness have differential effects on child language
output.
KEY WORDS: directiveness, teacher-child interaction, day care, language input

A
n increasing number of young children are spending the major-
ity of their waking hours in day care centers. One advantage of
attending day care is that interactions with childcare providers
are thought to provide the impetus for developmental progress in a
number of areas, including language acquisition (Peisner-Feinberg &
Burchinal, 1997). This premise is based, in part, on findings from longi-
tudinal studies that have demonstrated gains in receptive and expres-
sive language skills of typically developing children as a result of their
participation in quality day care (Goelman & Pence, 1987; Howes, 1997;
McCartney, 1984; McCartney, Scarr, Phillips, & Grajek, 1985). In these
studies, gains in language skills have been consistently related to global
aspects of teacher-child interaction, such as sensitivity to children’s non-
verbal cues, warmth, talkativeness, and verbal responsiveness.
The recognition of teacher-child interaction as an important source
of language input derives theoretical support from social interactionist
accounts of language acquisition—namely that language learning occurs
during everyday conversational exchanges with adults. Although adults
are generally motivated by the desire to converse rather than to teach
language (Cross, 1977; McDonald & Pien, 1982), their conversations
include important structural and discourse adaptations that are thought
to assist children in the language-learning process. First, joint atten-
tion and joint action in the interactions between caregivers and young
children permit children to make comparisons between nonlinguistic

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 43 • 1101–1114 • October 2000 • ©American Speech-Language-Hearing
Girolametto Association
et al.: Directive Input in Day Care 1101
1092-4388/00/4305-1101

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Univ of York-England User on 02/16/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
and linguistic contexts and induce the relationships a specific vocal, verbal, or action response from the child.
among objects, actions, external events, and words (Hoff- Studies of adult-child interaction report that teachers
Ginsberg, 1986; Snow, 1981). Second, during these epi- use a high proportion of response control directives in
sodes of conversational interaction, the adults’ simpli- both their individual and their group interactions in
fied and contingent language input models syntactic and day care (Schaffer & Liddell, 1984; Tizard & Hughes,
semantic content that reflects the child’s interests. Such 1984). Moreover response control directives are more
input reduces contextual ambiguities, provides redun- frequently observed with less conversationally skilled
dancy, and increases the saliency of the input such that children than with children who have well-developed
more motivational and cognitive resources can be avail- language skills (Cicognani & Zani, 1992; Polyzoi, 1997).
able for language learning (e.g., Cross, 1977; Harris, Contexts can also elicit differential use of response con-
Jones, Brookes, & Grant, 1986; Snow & Ferguson, 1978; trol directives, with teacher-directed play, such as circle
Tomasello & Todd, 1983; Yoder, Kaiser, Alpert, & Fischer, time, resulting in more response control than in child-
1993). Thus one hallmark of the social interactionist directed contexts, such as free play (O’Brien & Bi, 1995;
account of language acquisition is the facilitative role Pecyna, Feeny-Giacoma, & Neiman, 1987). Although
attributed to the adult’s responsive language input. response control is used to direct behavior (e.g., “Make
However, research on teacher-child interaction por- a snake”) and elicit responses (e.g., “What color is this?”),
trays teachers’ child-directed talk as overly directive in it is generally considered to inhibit children’s verbal
nature and unresponsive (e.g., Cicognani & Zani, 1992; productivity because it does not invite extended con-
Pellegrino & Scopesi, 1990; Polyzoi, 1997; Schaffer & versation and places few cognitive demands on the chil-
Liddell, 1984; Tizard & Hughes, 1984). Despite this char- dren to explain, predict, or provide information (Tizard
acterization, it is an area that has not been well docu- & Hughes, 1984). Therefore, although its discourse role
mented, especially in relation to contextual differences, may be to increase child participation in interaction,
age differences, and effects on child participation in in- its effect on verbal conversation and output may be
teraction. The theoretical and clinical implications of this counterproductive.
line of inquiry include (a) a description of different sub- A second type of directiveness observed in caregiver-
types of directive input used in day care, (b) the effect of child interactions is turn-taking control. Turn-taking
directiveness on children’s verbal participation in con- control is the extent to which teachers dominate the con-
versations, (c) how directiveness varies as a function of versation by using frequent verbal turns. Tannock (1988)
different contexts and age groups, and (d) the identifi- suggests that caregivers may take more turns, especially
cation of specific areas of in-service education for child- with less conversationally skilled children, in an effort
care providers. Speech-language pathologists are in- to encourage them to verbalize. However, turn-taking
creasingly becoming involved in consultative roles and dominance in group childcare settings is generally
direct treatment in day cares and preschools. To be maxi- viewed as having an inhibiting effect on child conversa-
mally effective in these new roles and contexts clinicians tion, although this has not yet been unequivocally dem-
need to understand better the communication dynam- onstrated. Investigations of turn-taking dominance in
ics in these settings and how they vary by task and age. teacher-child interaction report that teachers tend to
The specific role of directive language input in lan- dominate the conversation by taking most of the turns
guage acquisition is not clearly defined by social inter- whether they interact with children in dyads or groups
actionist theories. Some adherents of this perspective (Wells, 1978, 1981). Also, when compared to mother-
generally maintain that language learning may be in- child interaction, teachers take many more turns, with
hibited when the input is at odds with the child’s plan- the consequence that preschool-age children talk very
of-the-moment in topic and content (McDonald & Pien, little in these interactions (Cross & Horsborough, 1986;
1982; O’Brien & Bi, 1995; Ogilvy, 1992; Olsen-Fulero, Polyzoi, 1997; Tizard & Hughes, 1984).
1982). It is thought that directive input lacks semantic Finally, the third type of directive interaction that
contingency and provides few opportunities for joint Tannock (1988) describes, topic control, refers to the
engagement. In contrast, other researchers suggest that extent to which adults select the topic of the conversa-
some subtypes of directiveness can serve to facilitate tion and direct the child’s attention to adult-selected
the child’s participation in interaction and receptive lan- topics or activities. This subtype of directiveness has not
guage development (Barnes, Gutfreund, Satterly, & been directly examined in the extant literature on
Wells, 1983; Ellis & Wells, 1980; Tannock, 1988). teacher-child interaction. However, a related type of
Tannock (1988) describes three subtypes of directive- directiveness, behavior control, is similar in function and
ness that are most frequently used by caregivers of appears to be characteristic of group childcare. Behav-
young children. The most commonly studied subtype, ior control refers to utterances that are used by teach-
response control, refers to teachers’ use of commands, ers to elicit group participation around a common activ-
test questions, and yes/no questions that serve to elicit ity or to manage safety concerns. Previous studies report

1102 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 43 • 1101–1114 • October 2000

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Univ of York-England User on 02/16/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
that approximately 40% of teacher’s speech to older and extended it to include two additional subtypes of
preschoolers addresses management functions such as directiveness. The first, behavior control, refers to the
these (Perry, Colman, & Cross, 1986) and that this is use of group regulation strategies that promote safety
also a dominant mode of interaction in toddler class- or group cohesion. The second, conversation control, in-
rooms (Ogilvy, 1992). According to Pellegrino and Scopesi cludes open-ended questions, clarification questions, and
(1990), younger children receive more attention calls to conversational yes/no questions that attempt to engage
direct their focus to shared tasks, whereas older pre- the children in conversation and are indicative of a con-
schoolers receive more commands directing them to co- versation-eliciting style of interaction. The present study
operate in ongoing group activities. The precise rela- contributes to the existing literature on teacher-child
tionship between behavior control and child language interactions by clarifying the effects of child age and
productivity has not yet been examined. context on teachers’ directiveness using a systematic
One explanation for the observed directiveness in delineation of directive subtypes. This study examines
teacher-child interaction may reside in the nature of teachers’ directive language input to a group of toddlers
group childcare. As noted previously, teachers’ directive (average: 24 months old) and a group of preschool chil-
input may be necessary to promote safety, manage group dren (average: 41 months old) to determine the extent
behavior, and ensure participation in group tasks. A sec- to which teachers modify their directive language input
ond explanation suggests that directive input may be when addressing children of different age groups. Also,
influenced by the developmental level of the children, because previous studies have reported that the con-
with more directive interaction provided to less conver- text of the interaction can systematically influence teach-
sationally skilled children (e.g., Pellegrino & Scopesi, ers’ linguistic behavior (O’Brien & Bi, 1995; Rhyner,
1990; Polyzoi, 1997). In this case, directiveness (e.g., Lehr, & Pudlas, 1990), we elected to observe teachers in
response control and turn-taking control) may be used a child-directed context (i.e., play dough) as well as a
as a strategy to scaffold children’s participation in on- teacher-directed context (book reading) in order to de-
going activities and to invite them to take part in con- termine contextually bound differences in directive in-
versations (e.g., File, 1994; Ogilvy, 1992; Polyzoi, 1997). teraction styles. The results of this study extend our
A third influence on teachers’ directiveness may be the knowledge of language input in day care settings and
underlying instructional purpose or context of an activ- the role of directive interaction in facilitating or inhibit-
ity. (For a review of contextual influences on adult and ing conversation.
child behavior, see Caldera, Houston, & O’Brien, 1989; This study addresses several research questions: (a)
O’Brien & Nagle, 1987; Wanska, Pohlman, & Bedrosian, Does teachers’ use of different subtypes of directiveness
1989.) Contexts that are teacher-directed and have spe- differ when addressing a group of toddlers versus pre-
cific themes or scripts, such as circle time or book read- schoolers? (b) Does teachers’ use of subtypes of direc-
ing, have been found to elicit more directive input than tiveness differ in two naturalistic but different instruc-
child-directed free play (e.g., O’Brien & Bi, 1995; Pecyna tional contexts (i.e., play dough, book reading)? (c) Does
et al., 1987). Therefore, in part, teachers’ directiveness the amount of child talk differ systematically as a func-
may be an adaptation that is facilitative of group man- tion of age and context? (d) Are there significant rela-
agement and interaction and characteristic of certain tionships between measures of teachers’ directiveness
instructional discourse modes commonly found in day and measures of child language productivity? In keep-
care classrooms. However, if controlling and directive ing with the results of previous studies we predicted that
input is the primary means of encouraging interaction teachers would use more behavior control, response con-
and conversation there is a concern that it may also in- trol, and turn-taking control with younger children than
hibit children’s verbal productivity and reduce oppor- with older children. However, we hypothesized that
tunities for language learning (Cross & Horsborough, teachers would use more conversation control with the
1986; McDonald & Pien, 1982; O’Brien & Bi, 1995; children in the preschool group. In addition, because
Ogilvy, 1992). For this reason, it is important to observe child-directed play is associated with less teacher in-
the systematic influences of different contexts and age volvement, we predicted that teachers would use fewer
groups on teacher’s directiveness and determine the directives in the play-dough context than in the book-
relationships between subtypes of directive interaction reading activity. We also hypothesized that the play-
and children’s verbal productivity. dough context would elicit more child participation than
The present study was designed to explore, in a sys- the book-reading activity. Finally, we hypothesized that
tematic fashion, five different subtypes of conversa- there would be significant negative correlations between
tional directiveness that are characteristic of group behavior control, response control, topic control, and
childcare settings. We adapted Tannock’s (1988) delin- turn-taking control and measures of children’s language
eation of three subtypes of directiveness (including re- use, but significant positive correlations between teach-
sponse control, topic control, and turn-taking control) ers’ conversation control and child language use.

Girolametto et al.: Directive Input in Day Care 1103

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Univ of York-England User on 02/16/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
with 20 children, and three classrooms with 24 children.
Method All 20 teachers who participated in this study had com-
Participants pleted high school as well as 2 years of postsecondary
education, resulting in a diploma in Early Childhood
The participants in this study were 20 early child-
Education (ECE). None of the caregivers had received
hood education teachers who worked in licensed non-
any specific training in how to stimulate language de-
profit day care centers in the metropolitan area of
velopment. All teachers were female and had at least 2
Toronto. Each teacher selected four children from her
years experience in childcare. The average age of the
classroom to take part in the study. Ten of the teachers
teachers was approximately 30 years in both groups.
were caregivers for 40 toddlers, and the remaining 10
Descriptive data on the teachers’ characteristics can be
teachers were caregivers for 40 preschoolers. Eight of
found in Table 2.
the preschool teachers were also involved in a related
study of teacher-child interactions with children who had
disabilities (Girolametto, Hoaken, Weitzman, & van Procedures
Lieshout, 2000). The toddlers ranged in age from 17 to
33 months, and the preschoolers ranged in age from 32 A research assistant contacted the supervisors of
to 53 months. All 80 children were developing typically day care centers by letter, describing the research, and
according to parent report and had normal speech and followed up on the letter with a phone call within 2
language development as determined by the Speech and weeks. If the center agreed to take part in the research
Language Assessment Scale (Hadley & Rice, 1993) that project, the supervisor nominated a teacher with an
was completed by the teacher for each child. Most of the ECE diploma who was interested in participating, and
children attended the facility on a full-time basis (i.e., the research assistant set up appointments for three
at least 40 hours per week), and all had attended the consecutive visits to the center within a 2-week period.
particular day care for at least 2 months before the study. During the first visit, teachers were informed that the
Summary data describing the characteristics of the chil- purpose of the study was to observe teacher and child
dren can be found in Table 1. communication in small groups, but they were not in-
formed of the categories of language behavior that were
The toddler classrooms had a teacher-child ratio of under investigation. The investigator gathered demo-
1:5, and the preschool classrooms had a teacher-child graphic information on the teacher (e.g., age, training,
ratio of 1:8 as mandated by law in the Province of years of experience) and then asked each teacher to se-
Ontario. Class sizes for the toddler classrooms ranged lect four children in her classroom who displayed typi-
from 10 (four classrooms) to 15 children (six classrooms). cal development, had average language skills, and fre-
Class sizes for the preschool classrooms ranged from 12 quently played together. Information and consent forms
to 24 children and included one classroom with 12 chil- were given to the parents by the teacher. Parents who
dren, five classrooms with 16 children, one classroom allowed their children to participate in the research com-
pleted case history forms that requested information
Table 1. Characteristics of the toddlers and preschoolers. about birth history and developmental milestones.

Toddler Preschool
During the second visit, the teachers were asked to
Group Group complete the Speech and Language Assessment Scale
Variables (n = 40) (n = 40) (Hadley & Rice, 1993) separately for each of the four
children in order to provide information on their speech
Age (months) M 24.9 41.1
SD 3.9 4.5
Table 2. Characteristics of the teachers.
Median 24.0 41.0
Range 17–33 32–53
Toddler Preschool
Gender Male 20 19 Group Group
Female 20 21 Variables (n = 10) (n = 10)
Time in day carea
2–6 8 3 Age (in years) M (SD) 29.9 (8.1) 30.0 (6.5)
7–12 17 6 Range 23–46 23–44
13+ 15 31
Years of education M (SD) 14.8 (0.7) 14.2 (.42)
Attendance Full Time 35 39 Range 14–16 14–15
Part Time 5 1
Years of experience 2–5 6 4
a
Length of time in months that child has attended the particular day 6–10 4 2
care center. 11+ 0 4

1104 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 43 • 1101–1114 • October 2000

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Univ of York-England User on 02/16/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
and language development. Also, the case history forms four Sesame Street finger puppets (Big Bird, Grover,
that were completed by parents were collected and re- Oscar, Ernie).
viewed. In three instances, children who did not have The third and final visit occurred one week later,
average language skills as determined by the scale or but because of illness or absence some visits were post-
parent report were excluded and replaced. The investi- poned for up to 3 weeks. During this visit the teachers
gator then videotaped a 30-min teacher-group interac- and children were videotaped using the same protocol
tion involving two different instructional contexts (play described previously. Following this visit the childcare
dough, book reading). This first videotape was used to providers completed an informal questionnaire that
familiarize the children and the teachers with the vid- asked them to rate their impressions of the representa-
eotaping procedure and was not used for analysis. Only tiveness of their interaction on a 5-point scale (1 = very
the children participating in the study were videotaped; typical, 3 = typical, 5 = not typical). The scale measured
the other children played with similar materials in an- four indices: the amount of talk, the rate of talk, their
other room or in a different area of the same classroom, comfort level, and the amount of playtime. All teachers
or participated in outdoor play. The group size was set rated their amount of talk, rate of speech, and amount
at four children because previous research indicated of playtime as typical (mean rating = 3.0, 2.9, and 2.8,
that teacher language input was adversely affected by respectively). In addition, the childcare providers deter-
larger group sizes (Pellegrino & Scopesi, 1990) and that mined that their comfort level was typical of an unob-
young children were more interactive in small rather served playtime (mean rating = 2.8). Thus, these rat-
than large group settings (McCabe et al., 1996). There- ings provided some assurance that the childcare
fore, the findings of this study may be generalizable providers believed their interactions to be similar to
only to similar group sizes. A portable camera with a other, unobserved, situations in the day care.
directional microphone was used to permit the research
assistant to position herself so that the behaviors of the Transcription
teacher and children could be videotaped simultaneously.
She was instructed to capture as many frontal views of Ten min of the play-dough activity and 10 min of
the children as possible without constraining the par- the book-reading session, obtained during the third
ticipants to sit in any particular order. Thus, the video- visit, were transcribed using the Systematic Analysis
tape frequently captured the back of the teachers’ heads of Language Transcripts (SALT; Miller & Chapman,
and profiles of the participants. The groups were video- 1992) to yield a total of 20 min of transcription per
taped for 15 min in a book-reading activity followed by childcare provider. The first 5 min of each videotaped
15 min in a play-dough activity following procedures session were omitted to permit a brief warm-up period.
used in a previous study of teacher-child interaction Transcripts included the adult’s utterances and all in-
within integrated day care settings (Girolametto, telligible utterances spoken by each of the four chil-
Hoaken, et al., 2000). The book reading activity took dren on separate speaker lines. In addition, the chil-
place on the floor of the center’s designated book cen- dren addressed by the adult were noted at the end of
ter. Typically, the teacher and children sat on the floor, each utterance. This was accomplished by noting
on a carpet or on pillows, in a circle. The investigator whether the adult used the child’s name, referred to a
provided the teacher with a set of three books, The specific child’s toy or activity, or responded to a specific
Mixed-Up Chameleon (Carle, 1998), Spot Bakes a Cake child’s question/comment. All other utterances were
(Hill, 1994), and Barney and Baby Bop Go to the Gro- considered to be addressed to all four children in the
cery Store (Cooner, 1997), and one set of toys that ac- group. Transcripts were prepared by a research assis-
companied the latter book and permitted thematic tant, and every transcript was verified by the second
play. For example, the book Barney and Baby Bop Go author. The second author read the prepared transcripts
to the Grocery Store was accompanied by a toy shop- and observed the accompanying videotape, noting dis-
ping basket with boxes of pretend cereal, cake mix, agreements on the transcripts. Each disagreement was
granola bars, pizza mix, two plastic eggs, an apple, an subsequently discussed with the original transcriber and
orange, a banana, a slice of pizza, and two plastic muf- resolved in joint meetings during which the videotapes
fins. The order of books was not constrained, and use of and transcripts were reviewed. Where the individuals
the toys was optional. The play-dough activity took place reached a consensus, a change was entered on the tran-
nearby at a child-size table with five chairs. The activ- scripts. If there was a disagreement that could not be
ity included the following objects that were provided resolved, then the word or utterance was noted as unin-
by the investigator: a plastic container with three dif- telligible by typing an X in the appropriate speaker line,
ferent colors of play dough, three rectangular wooden and the utterance was excluded from further analyses.
blocks, three cubic wooden blocks, four spoons, four Similarly, disagreements concerning the addressee of the
knifes, four plates, two cars, two boxes with lids, and adult’s utterance were resolved through discussion and

Girolametto et al.: Directive Input in Day Care 1105

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Univ of York-England User on 02/16/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
observation of the videotape. Agreement reliability was the existing coding system and were time-consuming to
conducted on 25% (i.e., 100 min) of the corrected tran- transcribe and code. Turn-taking control was rated in
scripts selected at random using the following formula: an item that assessed teachers’ turn dominance, and
number of agreements / the number agreements + dis- topic control was rated in a second item that assessed
agreements × 100. Agreement reliability for the childcare teachers’ ability to follow the child’s lead. See Appendix
providers was 96% for utterance boundaries, 99% for B for a full description of these two items. Before using
words, and 95% for addressee. Reliability for the chil- the scale, a research assistant was trained to use the
dren was 93% for utterance boundaries and 91% for scale items to a level of 85% agreement (within 1 point)
words. An examination of the intelligibility of the speak- with the first author. The trained observer watched each
ers’ utterances revealed that teachers in both groups of the videotapes, recorded examples of the strategies
were highly intelligible at 99%. For toddlers and pre- as they occurred, and noted missed opportunities. After
schoolers the proportion of intelligible utterances was viewing the tape, the observer rated the teacher’s be-
88% and 91%, respectively. havior on the scale and then watched the videotape a
second time to note strategies that were missed during
Coding the first pass. Interrater reliability was determined by
having a second independent observer assign ratings to
Each utterance (excluding direct reading of the text
20% of the data (i.e., 8 videotaped interactions: 4 tod-
in the books) was coded to capture the functional use of
dler teachers and 4 preschool teachers). Reliability was
language input. Individual codes were mutually exclu-
computed, allowing a margin of error of 1 scale point;
sive and were combined to create five superordinate
overall interrater reliability was 87.5% and ranged from
variables: (a) behavior control (i.e., attention calls, group
75% to 100% for all 14 items. Specific interrater reli-
management), (b) response control (i.e., commands, test
ability for the two individual scale items used in this
questions, directive yes/no questions, choice questions),
study was 88% for Follow Child’s Lead and 88% for Turn-
(c) conversation control (e.g., Wh-questions, conversa-
Taking. Intrarater reliability was computed one month
tional yes/no questions, clarification questions), (d) lan-
after the initial ratings were finished. The original ob-
guage modelling (e.g., label, imitate, expand, comment),
server recoded 20% of the videotaped samples without
and (e) other utterances (acknowledgments, evaluative
referring to the original ratings. Overall intrarater reli-
statements). Only the first three categories that reflected
ability was 100%, and there were no disagreements (al-
directive input were analyzed. A complete description
lowing 1 point spread) for any of the items, including
of the coding system with examples can be found in Ap-
Follow the Child’s Lead and Turn-Taking.
pendix A. The research assistant was trained to code
transcripts using this system until an 85% level of agree-
ment was reached on each individual code. The research
assistant subsequently coded all transcripts, and the Results
first author independently coded a random sample of The results are presented in three sections: (a) com-
eight 10-min transcripts. Interrater reliability was cal- parisons of teachers’ directive interaction by age group
culated using percentage agreement (i.e., the number and context, (b) comparisons of child language use in
of agreements / the number agreements + disagreements the toddler versus the preschool groups, and (c) correla-
× 100), and reliability for the individual teacher codes tions between subtypes of teachers’ directiveness and
ranged from 90% to 98%; overall agreement for teacher child language productivity. Wherever appropriate,
codes was 93% (N = 1427 codes). MANOVAS were used to compare groups in order to re-
duce the family-wise error rate. Univariate follow-up
Rating tests were conducted only if the omnibus test was sig-
The Teacher Interaction and Language Rating Scale nificant. The two-tailed probability was set at .05.
(Girolametto, Weitzman, & van Lieshout, 1999) was used
to assess the teacher’s use of 14 interaction strategies.
Teachers’ Directiveness
Each of the strategies was judged for its quality (i.e.,
how completely and consistently a teacher carries out a This set of analyses compares overall aspects of
strategy) and was rated from 1 (inadequate) to 7 (excel- teacher’s directive language input (i.e., percentages of
lent). Only two scale items (balanced turn-taking, follow behavior control, response control, conversation control,
the child’s lead) that captured directive interaction styles topic control, turn-taking control). Both percentage and
were used in the present study. (The entire scale is avail- frequency summary data for these variables are found in
able from the first author.) These two subjective rating Table 3, although only the percentage data were analyzed.
scale items were used because they measured directive Two codes measuring Behavior Control (attention calls,
aspects of dyadic interaction that were not captured by group management) were entered into a MANOVA, with

1106 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 43 • 1101–1114 • October 2000

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Univ of York-England User on 02/16/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Table 3. Means and standard deviations for measures of teachers’ directiveness to toddlers and pre-
schoolers in two contexts.

Toddler Group (n = 40) Preschool Group (n = 40)

Variable/Context M (SD) Min-Max M (SD) Min-Max

Behavior Control
%Attention Calls B 7.5 (3.9) 3.1–14.6 4.5 (3.4) 0.9–12.4
P 4.7 (3.8) 0.0–12.0 2.1 (1.5) 0.0–4.1
#Attention Calls B 15.3 (7.8) 5.0–25.0 8.0 (8.9) 2.0–32.0
P 8.9 (7.5) 0.0–22.0 3.9 (3.1) 0.0–10.0
%Grp Management B 4.0 (2.2) 1.0–7.6 4.3 (2.9) 0.8–10.0
P 4.3 (4.3) 1.0–14.1 3.8 (4.2) 0.0–14.0
#Grp Management B 8.4 (6.2) 1.0-20.0 7.0 (4.3) 1.0-15.0
P 7.5 (6.5) 2.0-20.0 7.0 (7.2) 0.0-22.0

Response Control
%Commands B 5.9 (3.7) 1.4–13.5 8.1 (4.4) 3.0–15.8
P 8.4 (4.2) 1.8–17.0 7.9 (3.1) 3.4–14.2
#Commands B 13.0 (11.0) 2.0–41.0 14.5 (11.0) 4.0–41.0
P 16.1 (8.2) 3.0–28.0 14.6 (6.1) 8.0–24.0
%Test Questions B 11.1 (5.7) 4.1–20.4 19.0 (8.7) 6.0–31.0
P 3.5 (2.0) 0.0–5.7 3.6 (4.2) 0.0–11.0
#Test Questions B 24.1 (16.1) 6.0–53.0 11.4 (4.9) 5.5–20.7
P 6.8 (4.5) 0.0–14.0 2.0 (2.3) 0.0–6.8
%Directive Y/N B 5.7 (2.7) 2.1–12.2 8.0 (4.4) 0.0–13.4
P 4.9 (2.2) 0.9–7.7 3.8 (2.2) 1.6–9.0
#Directive Y/N B 11.5 (5.2) 3.0–22.0 14.1 (8.9) 0.0–29.0
P 9.5 (5.3) 2.0–17.0 7.5 (5.3) 3.0–20.0
%Choice Questions B 0.4 (0.5) 0.0–1.1 0.8 (0.9) 0.0–2.8
P 0.3 (0.3) 0.0–0.7 0.7 (0.9) 0.0–2.7
#Choice Questions B 0.8 (0.9) 0.0–2.0 1.3 (1.3) 0.0–3.0
P 0.4 (0.5) 0.0–1.0 1.6 (2.2) 0.0–7.0

Conversation Control
%Wh-Questions B 4.1 (2.6) 1.2–10.2 5.5 (4.1) 0.0–14.2
P 6.1 (2.9) 2.2–11.5 9.6 (3.6) 3.8–13.5
#Wh-Questions B 8.3 (4.9) 2.0–16.0 8.7 (5.5) 0.0–17.0
P 11.3 (5.3) 5.0–19.0 19.2 (9.8) 6.0–30.0
% Conv Y/N B 7.7 (4.6) 3.0–18.5 8.4 (4.8) 3.0–20.0
P 10.9 (3.8) 5.1–15.3 12.9 (5.9) 4.1–23.5
# Conv Y/N B 14.8 (7.2) 5.0–29.0 13.5 (7.2) 4.0–27.0
P 20.5 (7.3) 10.0–28.0 26.7 (17.0) 4.0–62.0
%Clarification B 4.7 (3.5) 0.6–11.5 3.6 (3.0) 0.0–10.0
P 8.6 (3.1) 5.4–14.1 8.3 (2.2) 5.7–11.4
#Clarification B 8.9 (6.1) 1.0–20.0 5.6 (3.9) 0.0–12.0
P 15.9 (5.4) 9.0–23.0 16.2 (6.8) 6.0–30.0

Note. B = book reading, P = play dough. Percentages denote the number of adult utterances that were coded in a
given way over the total number of adult utterances.

repeated measures with Group (toddlers, preschoolers) effect for Attention Calls only [F(1, 36) = 7.3, p ≤ .01].
and Context (book reading, play dough) as the indepen- Similarly, follow-up analyses for Context yielded a sig-
dent variables. This analysis revealed a significant main nificant univariate effect for Attention Calls [F(1, 36) =
effect for Group [Hotellings F(2, 35) = 3.8, p ≤ .05] and 6.4, p ≤ .05]. An examination of the data in Table 3 indi-
for Context [Hotellings F(2, 35) = 3.3, p ≤ .05]. Follow- cates that teachers of toddlers used more attention calls
up analyses for Group revealed a significant univariate than did teachers of preschool-age children and more

Girolametto et al.: Directive Input in Day Care 1107

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Univ of York-England User on 02/16/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
attention calls were used overall in the book-reading Table 4. Means and standard deviations for ratings of teachers’
than in the play-dough context. directiveness to toddlers and preschoolers in two contexts.
A second MANOVA with repeated measures was
Toddler Group Preschool Group
conducted on four codes that were included in Response
(n = 40) (n = 40)
Control (commands, test questions, directive yes/no ques-
tions, choice questions). This analysis revealed a signifi- Min- Min-
cant main effect for Context only [Hotellings F(4, 33) = Variable/Context M (SD) Max M (SD) Max
11.5, p ≤ .001]. There was no significant main effect for Topic Controla B 4.6 (1.5) 2–6 4.2 (0.8) 3–5
Group. Follow-up analyses for Context revealed signifi- P 3.8 (1.2) 2–5 3.1 (1.1) 2–5
cant univariate effects for both test questions and di-
rective yes/no questions [Fs(1, 36) = 44.5 and 6.8, ps ≤ Turn-Taking Controla B 4.7 (0.8) 3–6 4.7 (0.8) 3–6
P 3.8 (0.9) 2–5 3.2 (0.9) 2–5
.001 and .05, respectively]. An examination of the means
in Table 3 reveals that teachers used significantly more Note. B = book reading, P = play dough.
of these two question types in the book-reading context a
Ratings on the Teacher Language and Interaction Rating Scale. For the
than in the play-dough activity. purposes of this study, the scale was reverse scored so that high ratings
A third MANOVA was conducted on three codes that corresponded to high levels of control.
were included in the measure of Conversation Control
(Wh-questions, conversational yes/no questions, clarifica- more Topic Control during the book-reading activity than
tion questions). This analysis revealed a significant main in the child-directed play-dough activity.
effect for Context only [Hotellings F(3, 34) = 7.3, p ≤
.001]. Follow-up analyses for Context revealed signifi-
cant univariate effects for all three variables: Wh-ques- Child Participation in Talk
tions, conversational yes/no questions, and clarification This section compares the two groups (toddlers,
questions [Fs(1, 36) = 6.4, 8.2, and 20.8; ps ≤ .01, .05, preschoolers) in terms of the children’s verbal produc-
and .001, respectively]. An examination of the means in tivity in group conversations (i.e., number of utterances,
Table 3 reveals that teachers used significantly more of number of different words, number of combinations, and
these three question types in the play-dough activity length of the longest utterance). The summary data for
than in the book-reading context. Although the main these analyses can be found in Table 5.
effect for Group did not reach conventional levels of sig-
Measures of the children’s productivity were en-
nificance [F(3, 34) = 2.5, p = .077], the data in Table 3
tered into a MANOVA, with repeated measures with
generally indicate that teachers used less conversation
Group (toddler, preschool) and Context (play dough, book
control with toddlers than with preschoolers. When ex-
reading) as the independent variables. This analysis re-
ploratory follow-up tests were conducted on these vari-
vealed a significant multivariate main effect for Group
ables, there was a significant univariate effect only for
Wh-question types [F(1, 36) = 5.5, p ≤ .05]. Teachers may
Table 5. Means and standard deviations for amount of child talk in
use significantly more Wh-questions with preschoolers
the toddler and preschool groups.
than with toddlers.
Next, ratings assigned to two items on the Teacher Toddler Group Preschool Group
Interaction and Language Rating Scale that evaluated (n = 40) (n = 40)
the teachers’ Topic Control and Turn-Taking Control
Min- Min-
were analyzed using a series of nonparametric Mann- Variable/Context M (SD) Max M (SD) Max
Whitney U tests. These two items were reverse scored
so that low ratings were associated with low levels of # Utterances B 19.6 (16.5) 1–58 26.4 (17.1) 0–77
control and high ratings indicated high levels of control. P 23.1 (17.2) 0–67 39.0 (19.1) 2–88
The data for these items are summarized in Table 4. First,
the ratings were compared between Groups (toddlers, # Different Words B 18.3 (16.6) 2–79 33.4 (20.6) 0–84
P 22.4 (16.8) 0–72 55.6 (24.1) 2–105
preschoolers). Then, they were compared between Con-
texts (play dough, book reading). There were no differ-
# Combinations B 7.5 (8.3) 0–27 12.3 (9.8) 0–39
ences between the toddler and preschool age groups for P 12.5 (9.9) 0–35 29.1 (15.6) 0–70
either variable. However, when these items were compared
across the two Contexts, play dough received significantly Longest Utterance B 3.8 (2.1) 1–10 6.5 (3.6) 0–19
lower ratings for both Topic Control and Turn-Taking in Words P 4.4 (2.0) 1–10 9.4 (4.1) 1–23
Control (Us = 117.0 and 71.0, ps ≤ .05 and .001, respec-
tively). Turn-taking was less balanced, and teachers used Note. B = Book reading, P = Play dough

1108 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 43 • 1101–1114 • October 2000

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Univ of York-England User on 02/16/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
[Hotellings F(4, 153) = 23.3, p ≤ .001] for all measures of and child language output. In addition, there were also
productivity when analyzed together. Significant significant negative correlations between ratings for
univariate differences were found for all four measures teachers’ Turn-Taking Control and the number of ut-
of child language productivity [Fs(1, 156) = 14.4–62.5, terances [r(18) = –0.60, p ≤ .01], different words [r(18)
ps ≤ .001]. Not surprisingly, preschoolers talked signifi- = –0.50, p ≤ .05], and the number of multiword utter-
cantly more often and used a higher level of complexity ances used by children [r(18) = –0.55, p ≤ .01]. These
than children in the toddler group. The context of inter- correlation coefficients are indicative of moderate lin-
action also exerted a systematic effect on the children’s ear relationships between turn-taking control and child-
participation [Hotellings F(4, 153) = 17.7, p ≤ .001]. Re- language output (Silverman, 1998). Only conversation
gardless of the age group, children were more produc- control was positively related to children’s language
tive during the play-dough activity than during book measures—that is, with the number of children’s utter-
reading. Follow-up univariate analyses revealed that all ances [r(18) = 0.51, p ≤ .05], different words [r(18) = 0.42,
four variables contributed to this finding [Fs(1, 156) = p ≤ .05], and multiword combinations [r(18) = 0.46, p ≤
10.5–62.5, ps ≤ .001]. .05]. According to accepted standards of behavioral re-
search, the correlation coefficients for conversation con-
trol range from small (for different words and combina-
Relation Between Teachers’ Directiveness tions) to moderate (for number of utterances) (Silverman,
and Child-Language Productivity 1998). There were no significant correlations among any
In this section the five subtypes of teachers’ direc- of the four measures of child-language productivity and
tiveness were correlated with indices of child-language Response Control or Topic Control. Therefore, teachers’
productivity measured during the same interaction. use of conversation control was associated with higher
Because there were four children in each teacher-group values of language productivity. In contrast, teachers’
interaction, the average values of the children were se- use of behavior control and turn-taking control was as-
lected for these correlational analyses such that each sociated with children who spoke least often and used
teacher’s language input was correlated to the mean language that was less complex.
productivity of the four children in her group. Because
all hypotheses were directional, the Pearson product
moment correlations were conducted using a one-tailed Discussion
probability with an alpha level of .05. To increase the The results of this study reveal several important find-
power of the analyses, teachers’ values for directiveness ings concerning the subtypes of teachers’ directive lan-
and children’s data for participation in interaction were guage input to children in group childcare settings. For
summed across the two contexts (i.e., book reading and the most part, children in both the toddler and preschool
play dough). The results of all analyses between teachers’ age groups received similar amounts of directive input
input and child productivity are presented in Table 6. from their teachers. Exceptions include the use of more
There were significant negative correlations be- attention calls with younger children (who were on aver-
tween teachers’ Behavior Control and three of the four age 24 months old) and the use of more Wh-questions with
measures of productivity—that is, children’s number of older children (who were on average 41 months old). The
different words [r(18) = –0.39, p ≤ .05], use of multiword finding for attention calls replicates previous work by
combinations [r(18) = –0.41, p ≤ .05], and average length Pellegrino and Scopesi (1990), who reported that teach-
of the three longest utterances in words [r(18) = –0.40, ers used more attention calls with younger children
p ≤ .05]. According to standards of behavioral research when attempting to manage group behavior. However,
(Silverman, 1998), these correlation coefficients indicate the absence of group differences for the remaining sub-
relatively weak relationships between behavior control types of directiveness stands in sharp contrast to the

Table 6. Relations between measures of teachers’ directiveness and child language productivity (N = 20).

Behavior Response Conversation Topic Turn-Taking


Variables Control Control Control Control Control

# Utterances –.37 –.04 .50* –.29 –.60**


# Different Words –.39* –.05 .42* –.22 –.50*
# Multiword Combinations –.41* –.06 .46* –.25 –.55**
Longest Utterance –.40* .04 .25 –.23 –.38

Note. One-tailed significance. *p ≤ .05, **p ≤ .01.

Girolametto et al.: Directive Input in Day Care 1109

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Univ of York-England User on 02/16/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
findings of earlier studies which report that teachers children by asking more clarification questions, conver-
use more closed questions, commands, and turns with sational yes/no questions, and open-ended Wh-questions.
less conversationally skilled children (Cicognani & Zani, In this activity, each child had his or her own play dough
1992; Polyzoi, 1997). In part, differences in methodolo- and implements and ordinarily was free to construct
gies may explain the absence of group findings in our whatever he or she chose, thus inviting open-ended ques-
study. First, our toddlers were older and more verbal tions such as “What are you making?” or yes/no ques-
than the 12-month-old infants in the Cicognani and Zani tions that matched the child’s plan-of-the-moment and
study, who were predominantly preverbal. Second, we attempted to initiate a conversation (e.g., “Is that a snake
constrained the interactions by providing novel toys, you’re making?”). An examination of the transcripts re-
restricting group size to four children, and confining the vealed that teachers were engaged in more individual
location of filming—thereby limiting the need for teach- conversations during the play-dough activity, thereby
ers to use differential directive input to elicit the children’s inviting specific children to take turns. This activity was
attention and participation. Thus, it is possible that teach- also associated with the greatest amount of child talk in
ers may be more directive with children who are much terms of volubility (number of utterances, different
younger than those included in our study or in more words) and complexity (combinations, length). It is pos-
naturalistic settings where the group task is flexible and sible that the teachers’ style of using more conversation
group size and membership are constantly changing. control relative to other types of directives encouraged
The results of the present study also reveal that the children to increase their verbal participation in this
teacher’s directiveness is more a function of the context play context. This finding corroborates a similar find-
of the interaction than of the children’s ages. This find- ing by O’Brien & Bi (1995), who also reported that child-
ing indicates a stable pattern of directiveness across directed play contexts in which teachers used less con-
toddlers and preschool groups who display significantly trol yielded more complex speech and more talk overall.
different language abilities. Moreover, this finding sug- The pattern of correlations between teachers’
gests that sampling multiple contexts of teacher-child directiveness and child language productivity indicates
interaction is important for speech-language patholo- that teacher input that constrains behavior (behavior
gists who consult to day care centers and evaluate teach- control) and dominates turn-taking is associated with
ers’ language input. As hypothesized, book reading elic- restricted and less complex language use by the chil-
ited more behavior control (attention calls), response dren. In contrast, the teachers’ use of conversation con-
control (test questions, directive yes/no questions), and trol (Wh-questions, clarification questions, conversa-
topic control. This teacher-directed activity was also tional yes/no questions) is related to the greatest amount
associated with less balanced turn-taking overall. One of child talkativeness, lexical diversity, and complexity.
feature that may have accounted for the systematic dif- Topic control did not yield any significant association
ference between the two activities is the perceived dis- with child output, suggesting that this variable was not
course role of the activity (i.e., the underlying instruc- an important characteristic of the two contexts sampled
tional purpose). Book reading is a scripted activity, and in this study. However, the lack of significant correla-
the teachers’ general expectation appeared to be for the tions for response control is puzzling because test ques-
children to listen, attend, and respond to specific ques- tions, directive yes/no questions, and commands have
tions related to the book’s content. An examination of previously been associated with inhibiting interaction
the transcripts revealed that the book-reading sessions styles (McDonald & Pien, 1982; Olsen-Fulero, 1982).
tended to elicit more test questions (e.g., “What color is There are two alternative explanations for the ob-
this?”) and directive yes/no questions (e.g., “Can you served pattern of correlations. On the one hand, if it is
show me the orange?”). Also, teachers tended to direct assumed that teacher input drives the interaction, the
more of their utterances to the group as a whole, and current results indicate that, for the age groups in this
this style of interaction did not invite an individual child study, conversation control is helpful for promoting lan-
to participate; often, these questions remained unan- guage productivity. Also, they indicate that high levels
swered. Overall, book reading was associated with the of behavior control and turn-taking control may inhibit
least amount of child talk. Thus, the directive style that language productivity and may be incompatible with
characterized book reading generally tended to inhibit the basic goal of promoting conversational interaction.
the children’s participation in interaction. On the other hand, the bidirectional theory of influ-
In contrast, play dough was less directive than book ence (Bell, 1979) suggests an alternative explanation.
reading. Teachers used fewer attention calls, asked fewer It is possible that low levels of child participation may
test questions, and used fewer directive yes/no questions. elicit higher levels of teacher directiveness. If this is
They followed the child’s lead more often, and the turn- the case, the observed levels of teachers’ directive lan-
taking was more balanced. In contrast to book reading, guage input may be interpreted as constructive adap-
teachers also tended to promote interaction with the tations used to scaffold conversation and interaction

1110 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 43 • 1101–1114 • October 2000

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Univ of York-England User on 02/16/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
with a nonparticipating child. Future research is needed of children in day care. First, this study suggests that
to assess the longer-term effects of these directive sub- teachers may benefit from training to reduce some sub-
types on child language productivity and to ascertain types of directiveness (e.g., behavior control, response
the direction of influence. control, turn-taking control, topic control) and to increase
It is important to note that this study did not mea- their use of conversation control (e.g., Wh-questions,
sure the impact of teachers’ directive interaction on the clarification questions, conversational yes/no questions)
children’s linguistic development. Rather, the focus was during book reading. Evidence from previous studies
on the children’s overt verbal performance and partici- indicates that caregivers can be trained to modify their
pation in the classroom, which is presumed to be ulti- input during book reading and to enhance child language
mately important for the development of their full dis- development (e.g., Dale & Cole, 1996; Valdez-Menchaca
course and linguistic competencies. Thus, although & Whitehurst, 1992; Whitehurst et al., 1988). Second,
teachers can be directive in book-reading activities, there it also suggests a collaborative role for speech-language
can nevertheless be strong language learning underway pathologists within day care centers in that they can
in the form of enhanced comprehension and vocabulary provide teachers with appropriate support and training
growth that was not tapped by this study (M. Rice, per- in naturalistic language facilitation (e.g., Arnett, 1989;
sonal communication, March 22, 2000). Buysse, Wesley, Keyes, & Bailey, 1996). This may be
particularly important if unidentified children who are
Several limitations in the methodology must be
at risk of language delays or who have identified lan-
noted in interpreting the findings of this study. First,
guage delays are integrated into regular childcare set-
all teachers in this study were well educated, elected to
tings (Girolametto, Hoaken, et al., 2000). Because
participate, and were supported in this endeavor by their
directiveness may facilitate the participation of less con-
supervisors. Thus, the teacher-child interactions sampled
versationally skilled children, teachers’ use of directive
in this study may not be representative of all caregiver-
strategies should be examined in terms of their impact
child interactions that occur in day care centers. Sec-
on the child. If directiveness is an attempt to re-engage
ond, the teacher-children interactions occurred under
an uninvolved child and to elicit higher levels of partici-
very specific, structured conditions with small group
pation, then this differential handling should be re-
sizes. Although the videotaping took place in childcare
spected. If, on the other hand, directiveness is used to
centers (familiar settings for both the children and teach-
override the child’s topic, redirect the child’s attention,
ers), the time period was limited, the group size was
or constrain the child’s response, alternative strategies
restricted to four children, and the investigators pro-
should be taught. Third, the recognition that play con-
vided specific books and toys. Observations of teacher-
texts elicit different patterns of teacher-child interac-
child interactions in more naturalistic contexts and with
tion and child verbal participation can help teachers and
larger groups of children may reveal that teachers use
speech-language pathologists select naturalistic environ-
different interaction styles during their everyday inter-
ments that are conducive to child talk. Simply training
actions. Third, the results of this study may not be gen-
teachers to reduce their overall directiveness may place
eralized to teachers from diverse educational back-
them at odds with their expectations for the children’s
grounds or cultural groups, as these characteristics may
participation in group interactions. For some teachers,
widely influence the nature of adult-child interactions
the instructive value of teaching children to sit, listen,
and yield results different from those obtained in this
and respond to didactic questions during book reading
study. Nor can these results be applied wholesale to chil-
may override the usefulness of modifying this interac-
dren from different age groups, as there is evidence to
tion. In addition, the contextual differences noted in this
suggest that directiveness may facilitate productivity
study suggest that it is necessary to evaluate teacher-
in children who are much younger than those in the
child interactions in different contexts in order to cap-
current study (Cicognani & Zani, 1992; Pellegrino &
ture the variability in their language use.
Scopesi, 1990). Finally, day care providers are a hetero-
geneous group of individuals. The presentation of group
results in this study masks their diverse and distinc- Acknowledgments
tive approaches towards individual children. Future
descriptive and ethnographic studies may contribute to This study was sponsored by a grant from Social
Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada. We
our understanding of how background and experiential
thank Sophie Kaegi and Megan Wiigs, research assistants,
and attitudinal variables interact with different styles for their help with data transcription and data entry. We are
of language input to the children in their care. grateful to Lisa Hoaken, speech-language pathologist, for
The application of social interactionist theory to her assistance in subject recruitment and data collection.
teacher-child interactions suggests a number of implica- Above all, we are deeply appreciative of the participation of
the teachers and the children.
tions for stimulating the language use and development

Girolametto et al.: Directive Input in Day Care 1111

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Univ of York-England User on 02/16/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
References resource for assessment and IEP planning. Topics in
Speech and Language, 14, 278–288.
Arnett, J. (1989). Caregivers in day-care centers: Does Harris, M., Jones, D., Brookes, S., & Grant, J. (1986).
training matter? Journal of Applied Developmental Relations between the non-verbal context of maternal
Psychology, 10, 541–552. speech and rate of language development. British Journal
Barnes, S., Gutfreund, M., Satterly, D., & Wells, G. (1983). of Developmental Psychology, 4, 261–268.
Characteristics of adult speech which predict children’s Hill, E. (1994). Spot bakes a cake. New York: The Penguin
language development. Child Language, 10, 65–84. Group.
Bell, R. Q. (1979). Parent, child, and reciprocal influences. Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1986). Function and structure in
American Psychologist, 34, 821–826. maternal speech: Their relation to the child’s development
Buysse, V., Wesley, P., Keyes, L., & Bailey, D. B., Jr. of syntax. Developmental Psychology, 22, 155–163.
(1996). Assessing the comfort zone of child care teachers in Howes, C. (1997). Children’s experiences in center-based
serving young children with disabilities. Journal of Early child care as a function of teacher background and adult:
Intervention, 20, 189–203. child ratio. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 43, 404–425.
Caldera, Y., Houston, A., & O’Brien, M. (1989). Social McCabe, J. R., Jenkins, J. R., Mills, P. E., Dale, P. S.,
interactions and play patterns of parents and toddlers Cole, K. N., & Pepler, L. (1996). Effects of play group
with feminine, masculine, and neutral toys. Child variables on language use by preschool children with
Development, 60, 70–76. disabilities. Journal of Early Intervention, 20, 329–340.
Carle, E. (1998). The mixed-up chameleon. New York: McCartney, K. (1984). Effect of quality of day care environ-
Harper Collins. ment on children’s language development. Developmental
Cicognani, E., & Zani, B. (1992). Teacher-children interac- Psychology, 20, 244–260.
tions in a nursery school: An exploratory study. Language McCartney, K., Scarr, S., Phillips, D., & Grajek, S.
and Education, 6, 1–11. (1985). Day care as intervention: Comparisons of varying
Cooner, D. (1997). Barney and Baby Bop go to the grocery quality programs. Journal of Applied Developmental
store. Allen, TX: Barney. Psychology, 6, 247–260.
Cross, T. (1977). Mother’s speech adjustments: The contri- McDonald, L., & Pien, D. (1982). Mother conversational
butions of selected child listener variables. In C. Snow & behaviour as a function of interactional intent. Journal of
C. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input Child Language, 9, 337–358.
and acquisition (pp. 157–188). Cambridge, MA: Cam- Miller, J. F., & Chapman, R. S. (1992). Systematic analysis
bridge University Press. of language transcripts. Madison: University of Wisconsin–
Cross, T. G., & Horsborough, K. (1986). Adult speech, Madison.
child language and their interaction in the preschool. O’Brien, M., & Bi, X. (1995). Language learning in context:
Paper presented at the Fourth Australian Language and Teacher and toddler speech in three classroom play areas.
Speech Conference, Melbourne, Australia. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 15, 148–163.
Dale, P., & Cole, K. (1996). Parent-child book reading as an O’Brien, M., & Nagle, K. (1987). Parents’ speech to
intervention technique for young children with language toddlers: The effect of play context. Journal of Child
delays. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 16, Language, 14, 269–279.
213–235.
Ogilvy, C. M. (1992). How might psychological research
Ellis, R., & Wells, G. (1980). Enabling factors in adult-child findings be applied to increase the effectiveness of
discourse. First Language, i, 46–62. nurseries as learning contexts? Early Child Development
File, N. (1994). Children’s play, teacher-child interactions, and Care, 81, 65–75.
and teacher beliefs in integrated early childhood pro- Olsen-Fulero, L. (1982). Style and stability in mother
grams. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 9, 223–240. conversational behaviour: A study of individual differ-
Girolametto, L., Hoaken, L., Weitzman, E., & van ences. Journal of Child Language, 9, 543–564.
Lieshout, R. (2000). Patterns of adult-child linguistic Pecyna, P., Feeny-Giacoma, M., & Neiman, G. (1987).
interaction in integrated day care groups. Language, Development of the object permanence concept in cleft lip
Speech, and Hearing Services in the Schools, 31, 154–167. and palate and noncleft lip and palate infants. Journal of
Girolametto, L., Weitzman, E., & van Lieshout, R. Communication Disorders, 20, 233–243.
(1999). The Teacher Interaction and Language Rating Peisner-Feinberg, E. S., & Burchinal, M. R. (1997).
Scale. Unpublished manuscript, University of Toronto, Relations between preschool children’s child-care experi-
Toronto, ON. ences and concurrent development: The cost, quality, and
Goelman, H., & Pence, A. R. (1987). Effects of child care, outcomes study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 43, 451–477.
family, and individual characteristics on children’s Pellegrino, M. L. M., & Scopesi, A. (1990). Structure and
language development: The Victoria Day Care Research function of baby talk in a day-care centre. Journal of
Project. In D. A. Phillips (Ed.), Quality in child care: What Child Language, 17, 101–113.
does the research tell us? (Vol. 1, pp. 89–104). Washington,
DC: National Association for the Education of Young Perry, F., Colman, M., & Cross, T. G. (1986). Conversa-
Children. tions with children in kindergartens. In T. G. Cross & L.
M. Riach (Eds.), Aspects of child development (pp. 66–76).
Hadley, P. A., & Rice, M. L. (1993). Parental judgments of Melbourne: AE Press.
preschoolers’ speech and language development: A

1112 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 43 • 1101–1114 • October 2000

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Univ of York-England User on 02/16/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Polyzoi, E. (1997). Quality of young children’s talk with Valdez-Menchaca, M. C., & Whitehurst, G. J. (1992).
adult caregivers and peers during play interactions in the Accelerating language development through picture book
day care setting. Canadian Journal of Research in Early reading: A systematic extension to mexican day care.
Childhood Education, 6, 21–30. Developmental Psychology, 28, 1106–1114.
Rhyner, P. M. P., Lehr, D. H., & Pudlas, K. A. (1990). An Wanska, S., Pohlman, J., & Bedrosian, J. (1989). Topic
analysis of teacher responsiveness to communicative maintenance in preschoolers’ conversation in three play
initiations of preschool children with handicaps. Lan- situations. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 4, 393–402.
guage, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 21, 91–97. Wells, C. G. (1978). Talking with children: The complemen-
Schaffer, H. R., & Liddell, C. (1984). Adult-child interac- tary roles of parents and teachers. English in Education,
tion under dyadic and polyadic conditions. British Journal 12, 15–38.
of Developmental Psychology, 2, 33–41. Wells, C. G. (1981). Learning through interaction. London:
Silverman, F. (1998). Research design and evaluation in Cambridge University Press.
speech-language pathology and audiology. Needham Whitehurst, G., Falco, F., Lonigan, C., Fischel, J., De
Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Baryshe, B., Valdez-Menchaca, M., & Caulfield, M.
Snow, C., & Ferguson, C. (Eds.). (1978). Talking to (1988). Accelerating language development through picture
children. London: Cambridge University Press. book reading. Developmental Psychology, 24, 552–559.
Snow, C. E. (1981). Social interaction and language Yoder, P. J., Kaiser, A. P., Alpert, C., & Fischer, R. (1993).
acquisition. In P. S. Dale & D. Ingram (Eds.), Child Following the child’s lead when teaching nouns to
language: An international perspective (pp. 195–213). preschoolers with mental retardation. Journal of Speech
Baltimore: University Park Press. and Hearing Research, 36, 158–167.
Tannock, R. (1988). Mothers’ directiveness in their
interactions with their children with and without Down Received January 11, 2000
syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 93,
154–165. Accepted June 28, 2000
Tizard, B., & Hughes, M. (1984). Young children learning. Contact author: Luigi Girolametto, PhD, Department of
London: Fontana Paperbacks. Speech-Language Pathology, University of Toronto, 6
Queen’s Park Crescent West, Toronto, ON, Canada M5S
Tomasello, M., & Todd, J. (1983). Joint attention and 3H2. Email: l.girolametto@utoronto.ca
lexical acquisition style. First Language, 4, 197–212.

Appendix A. Teacher-Child Coding System

All codes were mutually exclusive. If an utterance satisfied (b) Directive Yes/No Questions (YN-D): Questions that
the criterion for two codes the utterance was assigned a code can be answered with a yes or no but that suggest or
corresponding to the order in which the codes are listed in this direct the child to play in a certain way or to speak in
appendix. For example, the utterance “John, can you make a a certain way (e.g, “Can you make a snake?”).
snake?” was coded as Attention Call and not as a Directive (c) Test Questions (TQ): Questions that test the child’s
Yes/No Question. knowledge; the answer is usually one word and
1. Behavior Control: Includes all utterances coded as Group obvious from the context (e.g., “What color is your
Management and Attention Calls. shirt?” “How many do you see?”).
(a) Group Management (GM): Utterances that suggest or (d) Choice Questions (CQ): This category includes choice
direct the child’s behavior in order to promote safety questions (e.g., “Do you want a cookie or a ba-
and to encourage participation in the group activity nana?”).
(e.g., “Push your chair forward.” “Take that out of 3. Conversation Control: Includes all open-ended Wh-
your mouth please.” “Come here and play.”). questions, clarification questions that repair breakdowns,
(b) AC (Attention Calls): Utterances that direct child’s and yes/no questions that promote conversation without
attention to an object or activity or call child’s name directing behavior.
to get child’s attention (e.g., “Look here.” “Julian.” (a) Clarification Questions (CL): Questions that clarify the
“Let’s see what’s in the bag.”). child’s meaning in the preceding utterance. They are
2. Response Control: Includes all utterances that serve to usually yes/no questions (e.g., C: “pak” / T: “He’s
constrain the child’s response. going to the park?”).
(a) Commands (CO): Utterances that suggest or direct the (b) Wh-Questions (WH): These are questions that
child to play in a certain way; commands used to includes a wh-word (i.e., what, who, why, where,
elicit words (e.g, “Make a snake with the play how). They are considered to be open-ended
dough.” Say ‘excuse me.’” “Go and get the giraffe.”). questions because the answer is not constrained (e.g.,

Girolametto et al.: Directive Input in Day Care 1113

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Univ of York-England User on 02/16/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
“Who likes grapes?” “What did you have for lunch following carrier phrases: That is X. Here’s an X. Red
yesterday?”). X. Another X. You have X.
(c) Conversational Yes/No Questions (YN-C): These are (c) Recasts (RE): Expansions that use words from the
yes/no questions that can be answered with a yes or child’s utterance in a longer utterance (e.g., C: “Car
no but do not suggest action or constrain behavior broken.” / T: “The car’s broken.” and C: “Green
(e.g., “Can I use some of yours?” “Baby Bop is going apples.” / T: “You like green apples.”).
shopping, isn’t she?”). (d) Comments (CM): Utterances that describe the
4. Language-Modeling Utterances (not used in this study). children’s or the teacher’s own activity (e.g., “You built
a house.” / “I’m making a cookie.”).
(a) Imitations (IM): Direct imitations of the child’s
verbalization/vocalizations (e.g., C: “Spaghetti” / T: 5. Other Utterances: (not used in this study)
“Spaghetti”). Imitations with evaluative tags were (a) Other Utterance (OU): Usually short utterances that
considered imitations (e.g., C: “Eye” / T: “Eye, that’s acknowledge, evaluate, or praise the child (e.g.,
right”). “Good job.” “Thank you.” “Oh good, I’m so glad.” “I
(b) Labels (LA): Short utterances that label objects. Most know.” “Nice sharing.”).
labels were used in short utterances that used the

Appendix B. Teacher Interaction and Language Rating Scale

1. Topic Control
Inadequate Minimal Good Excellent

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 N/A

Follow the Teacher never follows Teacher sometimes If children initiate (verbally Same as in #5, plus
Children’s Lead the children’s lead, comments on the children’s or nonverbally), teacher teacher is consistent
never comments on the topic or lead if verbal but responds frequently and throughout the
children’s activities, and may ignore or redirect promptly by commenting, interaction.
ignores or redirects the nonverbal leads. answering, and/or
children. participating nonverbally.

Comments:

2. Turn-Taking Control

Use Balanced Teacher dominates the Teacher sometimes takes Conversational turn-taking Same as #5, plus
Turn-Taking conversation or turns with children, but is balanced—children the teacher is consistent.
interaction. Does all the her turns are more and teacher take equal Matches the children’s
talking and/or completes frequent and longer. numbers of turns and turns pace.
most of the activity. Teacher talk still dominates. of similar length.

Comments:

1114 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 43 • 1101–1114 • October 2000

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a Univ of York-England User on 02/16/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx

You might also like