You are on page 1of 17

LSHSS

Article

Dynamic Assessment of Word Learning


Skills: Identifying Language Impairment
in Bilingual Children
Maria Kapantzoglou, a M. Adelaida Restrepo, a and Marilyn S. Thompson a

Purpose: Bilingual children are often diagnosed with language the new words faster than children with PLI did, showing
impairment, although they may simply have fewer opportunities greater modifiability. Further, a combination of word learning
to learn English than English-speaking monolingual children. in the receptive modality and the Learning Strategies Checklist
This study examined whether dynamic assessment (DA) of word (Lidz, 1991; Peña, 1993) provided the best accuracy in identi-
learning skills is an effective method for identifying bilingual fying PLI in these children.
children with primary language impairment (PLI). Conclusion: Findings suggest that a brief DA is a promising
Method: Fifteen 4- and 5-year-old predominantly Spanish- method for accurately differentiating children with TLD from
speaking children with typical language development (TLD) and children with PLI.
13 with PLI each participated in a 30- to 40-min session of DA of
word learning skills following a pretest–teach–posttest design.
Results: Results indicated that TLD children made associations Key Words: language impairment, bilingual, dynamic
between the phonological and semantic representations of assessment, word learning

valuation of language skills in bilingual children to learning a second language (e.g., Limbos & Geva, 2001;

E poses a significant challenge. The quantity and qual-


ity of input in their native and second languages influ-
ence the growth trajectories in those languages (Goldstein,
Samson & Lesaux, 2009). In the later elementary grades,
if bilinguals present with low academic skills, concerns in-
crease, and often there is overrepresentation of bilinguals in
2004; Hart & Risley, 1992, 1995; Pearson, Fernández, special education (e.g., Samson & Lesaux, 2009). Research
Lewedeg, & Oller, 1997). Given the diversity of language indicates that the norms used for monolingual children in
experiences in bilingual children, language proficiency and static measures (i.e., standardized measures) do not apply to
dominance vary among bilinguals and change as children bilingual /multicultural populations (Horton-Ikard & Ellis
grow according to children’s experiences in each language Weismer, 2007; Restrepo & Silverman, 2001). For example,
(Kohnert & Bates, 2002). Thus, static measures that assess Restrepo and Silverman (2001) examined the validity of
bilingual children’s existing knowledge in a particular do- the Spanish version of the Preschool Language Scale—3
main may lead to incorrect diagnoses of primary language (PLS–3; Zimmerman, Steiner, & Pond, 1993) score inter-
impairment (PLI).1 pretations and found that PLS–3 scores over-identified
At young ages, there is underrepresentation of bilinguals Spanish–English-speaking children as having PLI.
in special education because professionals often attribute Investigators have evaluated alternative methods of
early language and literacy deficits to difficulties related assessment that could reduce the bias of previous language
experiences when assessing bilinguals, such as using adap-
tations of existing measures for different cultural and lin-
a
Arizona State University, Tempe guistic populations, measuring processing skills, and using
Correspondence to Maria Kapantzoglou: mkapantz@asu.edu dynamic assessment (DA; e.g., Burton & Watkins, 2007;
Editor: Marilyn Nippold Campbell, Dollaghan, Needleman, & Janosky, 1997;
Associate Editor: Gary Troia Gutiérrez-Clellen, Peña, & Quinn, 1995; Peña, Iglesias,
Received October 29, 2010 & Lidz, 2001; Seiger-Gardner & Brooks, 2008; Windsor,
Revision received February 9, 2011
Accepted July 3, 2011 1
We used the term PLI for all terms such as language impairment, specific
DOI: 10.1044/0161-1461(2011/10-0095) language impairment, and language disorder found in the literature.

LANGUAGE, SPEECH, AND HEARING SERVICES IN SCHOOLS • Vol. 43 • 81–96 • January 2012 * American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 81
Kohnert, Loxtercamp, & Kan, 2008). DA evaluates how accordingly during the assessment (Tzuriel, 2001). Con-
much a child can learn rather than how much a child knows. versely, in more structured DA approaches such as the GPM,
Intervention is part of the assessment as the examiner teaches cues are predetermined and are presented in hierarchical
a concept, skill, or task and provides support to the child order, with the examiner providing increasing levels of sup-
during the learning process. Based on the child’s perfor- port. The posttest in DA evaluates learning, maintenance,
mance during the instruction and on the posttest, the exam- and/or transfer of the concepts or skills addressed in the
iner makes conclusions regarding gains resulting from the teaching phase. For less structured dynamic procedures to be
intervention, the strategies the child uses in learning, and successful, the examiner must be highly skilled at mediating
the amount and type of prompts the child needs to learn and assessing. Also, the less structured the method is, the
(modifiability; Feuerstein, 1979; Lidz, 1991; Peña, 1993). more difficult it is to standardize. On the other hand, Lidz
(1991) argued that structured DA may not be flexible enough
to adjust to the particular characteristics of each examinee.
DA
In comparison to static tests, DA addresses not only
DA is a method derived from Vygotsky’s (1978) cognition and/or language, but also the child’s behavioral
sociocultural theory. He observed that immigrants were and motivational factors during his or her interaction with
erroneously identified as having limited intelligence based the mediator—elements that are considered critical for learn-
on standardized static measures. According to his theory, ing (Tzuriel, 2001). Further, DA gives weight to social/
high-level mental processes, such as problem solving, vol- interpersonal interaction, thereby providing a more natural-
untary attention and memory, and concept formation, occur istic environment for learning in comparison with static tests.
and develop through interaction with the environment in In static tests, the absence of interaction and context may
daily activities and socialization. Language serves as a tool influence the child’s performance negatively, particularly
that mediates the relationship between cognition and the when the child is not familiar with a task, such as pointing to
environment. According to Vygotsky’s sociocultural theory, named pictures (e.g., Gutiérrez-Clellen, 1996). In contrast,
learning moves gradually from being regulated primarily DA reduces task familiarity biases through interpersonal
by the environment to self-regulation (internalization). The interaction and contextual assessment, thereby reducing test
more internalized learning processes become, the more anxiety (Carlson & Wiedl, 1992). The drawback is that
independently a person can learn. Vygotsky presented the such factors make DA more subjective than static tests.
concept of zone of proximal development (ZPD), which he
defined as the distance between the “actual developmental How DA Differentiates Children With
level as determined by independent problem solving and and Without PLI
the level of potential development as determined through
problem solving under adult guidance or in collaboration A number of studies have examined DA as a method to
with more capable peers” (Vygotsky, 1978, p. 86). DA is differentiate between children from culturally and linguisti-
intended to examine a child’s ZPD through the child’s inter- cally diverse backgrounds with typical language development
action with a clinician, for instance, to assess the child’s poten- (TLD) and those with PLI and have reported promising re-
tial for learning independently in a stimulating environment. sults. Specifically, studies have followed the common DA
Based on Vygotsky’s (1978) ideas or parallel to his work, pretest–teaching–posttest design with a brief intervention to
various approaches of DA have been developed, and some assess children’s learning abilities regarding narrative skills
common characteristics of these approaches have been (Peña et al., 2006), categorization skills (Ukrainetz, Harpell,
highlighted in reviews of the different types of DA (e.g., Walsh, & Coyle, 2000), and labeling skills in the Expressive
Grigorenko, 2009; Grigorenko & Sternberg, 1998; Swanson One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test—Revised (EOWPVT–R;
& Lussier, 2001; Tzuriel, 2001). In DA, a pretest–teaching– Gardner, 1990; Peña et al., 2001). Modifiability assessments
posttest method is often applied. During the pretest, the have been based on one or both of the following Likert scales:
existing level of knowledge of the participant is assessed, the Learning Strategies Checklist (LSC; Lidz, 1991; Peña,
often through standardized static tests. During the teaching 1993), which assesses the child’s responsivity (i.e., attention,
phase, teaching methods vary; some are less structured, planning, self-regulation, and motivation) and transfer of
such as those in the mediated learning experience (MLE) knowledge or skills, and the Modifiability Scale (MS; Lidz,
approach (Feuerstein, 1979), and others are more structured, 1987, 1991), which indicates the effort the examiner made
such as those in the graduated prompt method (GPM; and the support the child needed during the sessions.
Campione & Brown, 1987). In the MLE approach, there In all studies, posttest scores and modifiability yielded
is not a strict script to follow. The examiner observes how the best discrimination. For instance, in the Peña et al. (2006)
the child learns, detects problems in the learning process, study, the posttest of narrative skills and modifiability mea-
and addresses the problems during the assessment. The sures provided 100% correct classification of children in
child’s response serves as feedback for the effectiveness of both groups, with modifiability being the most accurate
the mediation applied, and the examiner adjusts the mediation individual measure. Although results from this study are

82 LANGUAGE, SPEECH, AND HEARING SERVICES IN SCHOOLS • Vol. 43 • 81–96 • January 2012
promising for school-age children, a limitation to consider in levels (Caramazza, 1997). The phonological representation
evaluating preschool children is that storytelling skills, and refers to information related to the sounds or combination
particularly use of story grammar, are not useful criteria of sounds of a word, whereas semantic representation refers
given that in preschool age, these skills are just beginning to the meaning of a word. Morphological and syntactic
to emerge (Berman & Slobin, 1994; Muñoz, Gillam, Peña, information also contribute to the meaning of a word. The
& Gulley-Faehnle, 2003; Trabasso, Stein, Rodkin, Munger, form of the word and its position in a sentence provide cues
& Baughn, 1991). In the Ukrainetz et al. (2000) study, based for obtaining information about the word’s meaning (i.e.,
on the posttest scores, sensitivity of the expressive catego- syntactic bootstrapping; Gleitman, 1990).
rization subtests from the Assessing Semantic Skills Through Word learning occurs gradually (Bloom, 2001; Nelson,
Everyday Themes (ASSET; Barret, Zachman, & Huisingh, 1985; Vygotsky, 1986). Initially, a single or a few exposures
1988) was 75% and specificity was 87%. Modifiability to a novel word create a partial mental representation of
correctly classified the stronger and weaker language learn- that word, which includes only a few word features captured
ers with 87% sensitivity and 100% specificity; the MS during the initial exposures to the verbal stimulus ( fast
discriminated better than the LSC. Nevertheless, children’s mapping; Carey & Bartlett, 1978). Then, succeeding expo-
performance in a categorization task may still be influenced sures to the word in a variety of contexts increase the pho-
by the depth of their vocabulary knowledge, so bilinguals nological, semantic, and syntactic information associated
may be disadvantaged by decreased exposure to and use of with the word and strengthen its mental representations
particular vocabulary rather than due to PLI. Consequently, as shown, for example, through reduced naming errors
categorization skills are not an optimal criterion for accurately (Gershkoff-Stowe, 2001, 2002; Gershkoff-Stowe & Smith,
differentiating preschool children with and without PLI due 1997; Gray, 2004; McGregor et al., 2002), more elaborate
to biases associated with previous language experience. definitions (McGregor et al., 2002; Ordoñez, Carlo, Snow,
Finally, in the study by Peña et al. (2001), results indicated & McLaughlin, 2002), and drawings (Gray, 2004; McGregor
that after working with children on understanding the impor- et al., 2002).
tance of using “special names”/single words for objects, In English-speaking monolingual populations, numerous
versus descriptives for example, using 19 words of the test studies have shown that children with PLI have word learn-
as explanations of when to use “special names,” the sensitiv- ing difficulties (e.g., Alt, Plante, & Creusere, 2004; Gray,
ity of the EOWPVT–R based on the posttest was 78%, but 2003, 2004, 2005; Kan & Windsor, 2010; Kiernan & Gray,
specificity was very good (95%; cf. Plante & Vance, 1994). 1998; Rice, Buhr, & Nemeth, 1990; Stothard, Snowling,
Overall, results showed that DA can limit cultural biases in Bishop, Chipchase, & Kaplan, 1998). To differentiate chil-
diagnostic procedures and improve accuracy in classification dren with PLI from their TLD peers, researchers have tried
of bilinguals. Such a DA process addresses communication to identify word learning phases in which differences
style bias and potential disadvantage due to lack of test-taking between the two groups are maximized. In some studies,
experience (i.e., lack of test wiseness). However, the main investigators have assessed word learning in the initial
drawback of this approach is that it does not account for phases through fast mapping (Carey & Bartlett, 1978) or
differences in vocabulary knowledge due to different lan- quick incidental learning (QUIL) tasks (e.g., Rice, Oetting,
guage and cultural experiences. The target words are common Marquis, Bode, & Pae, 1994). In such tasks, participants
words, and average exposure to those varies in culturally are exposed to novel stimuli once or a few times, and no
diverse populations; thus, different levels of performance feedback or support is provided for learning. Other studies
do not necessarily reflect different language abilities. have assessed differences in word learning skills between
The assessment of novel-word learning skills through monolingual children with and without PLI in later phases
DA may be a more accurate method than the assessment of through supportive intervention, in which cues and/or feed-
existing vocabulary in bilinguals for identification of PLI. back are provided (e.g., Gray, 2004, 2005; Weismer &
Using DA in domains minimally related to previous language Hesketh, 1993, 1996, 1998).
experiences and culture may minimize respective biases and A number of studies have shown that word production
help accurately identify children with PLI. Further, language tasks reveal word learning limitations in children with PLI,
learning tasks would indicate children’s ability to acquire which suggests possible limitations in the perception or
language. Previous studies suggest that children with PLI storage of, or access to, the phonological representations
tend to have difficulties in word learning (e.g., Gray, 2004; of the words (e.g., Dollaghan, 1987; Kan & Windsor, 2010).
McGregor, Newman, Reilly, & Capone, 2002). In the initial stages of word learning, on a fast mapping task
of a single nonword, Dollaghan (1987) found statistically
Word Learning Differences Between Children significant differences between preschoolers with and with-
out PLI in word production, but not in word identification.
With and Without PLI
In contrast, Alt et al. (2004) assessed the ability of children
Based on models of lexical processing, words are with PLI to fast map semantic characteristics of novel words
represented at the phonological, semantic, and syntactic in receptive tasks and found that the children with PLI had

Kapantzoglou et al.: Dynamic Assessment of Word Learning Skills 83


poorer performance than their TLD peers. Past the initial stage Spanish-speaking bilinguals with PLI. The task included
of word learning, using supportive word learning interven- modeling, imitation, word identification, and production
tion, Gray (2005) found restrictions in both phonological and probes, but only production was analyzed. Restrepo did
semantic representations in preschoolers with PLI. Gray not find significant differences in learning performance
reported significant between-group differences in the num- between children with and without PLI. Similar to the trials-
ber of words learned as measured in word identification and to-criterion approaches reported earlier, it is plausible that
production tasks, regardless of the type of cues provided, the support provided to the children through intensive expo-
phonological or semantic. In a meta-analysis of word learn- sure to novel stimuli reduced cognitive demands and elim-
ing studies, Kan and Windsor (2010) reported that word inated differences in processing skills between the two
identification and recognition (forced choice given) tasks groups (e.g., Dollaghan & Campbell, 1998; Edwards &
resulted in greater effect sizes than production tasks, but Lahey, 1996; Kohnert, Windsor, & Miller, 2004; Lahey,
effect sizes were considered across various types of inter- Edwards, & Munson, 2001; Montgomery & Windsor, 2007).
vention (e.g., with both low and high numbers of exposures
to stimuli). Given that we do not know how other task Factors That Influence Word Learning in
characteristics affect results, further examination of both
Children With and Without PLI
word production and identification skills will help determine
what type of assessment works best for specific tasks for Various studies have examined factors that may influence
accurate identification of PLI. word learning in children with and without PLI, including
Kiernan and Gray (1998) examined whether a word the number of presentations in the input, neighborhood
learning task following a trials-to-criterion approach could density, and phonotactic probability. Rice et al. (1994) inves-
accurately differentiate English-speaking preschoolers with tigated the influence of the number of presentations in the
and without PLI. Intervention took place during 2–4 days input on the word learning performance of 5-year-old
and included three target words, modeling of novel words children with and without PLI in QUIL tasks. Using eight
(e.g., “This is a. . .”), imitation (e.g., “Can you say. . .?”), target words, Rice et al. (1994) assessed participants’ per-
word production (e.g., “What’s this?”), word identification formance when words were repeated three times and 10 times.
probes (e.g., “Point to. . .”), and feedback (correct word) After three presentations, only the TLD group made statis-
in structured play activities. Task administration continued tically significant gains. After 10 presentations, the group
until children achieved 75% accuracy in word production with PLI was able to fast map new words and performed
in 2 consecutive days or for 4 days maximum. Kiernan and similarly to the TLD group. Rice et al. (1990) also presented
Gray found significant group differences at the production 20 new words, five times each, in short videos to 5-year-
level, but 73% of the children with PLI produced the same old children. In a word identification posttest, the authors
number of words correctly as their TLD peers. Also, chil- found that the children with PLI scored significantly lower
dren with PLI who had low production scores were able to than the groups matched for mean length of utterance (MLU)
comprehend most of the target words. Subsequently, Gray and age. Results suggest that tasks aiming to differentiate
(2003) used a similar task to that of Kiernan and Gray using the two groups should be sufficiently easy to avoid floor
four target words. The trials-to-criterion approach classified effects but also challenging enough to reveal the processing
English-speaking preschoolers with and without PLI with limitations of children with PLI.
90% specificity considering word production and 93% In a meta-analysis, Kan and Windsor (2010) found that,
specificity considering word identification. Sensitivity was when comparing children with and without PLI, greater
77% considering word production and 53% considering numbers of exposures in a word learning task resulted in
word identification. Later, Gray (2004) found significant larger effect sizes than low numbers of exposures. These
differences between preschoolers with and without PLI in results should be interpreted with caution because first, as
production, with 30% of scores overlapping between groups. the authors indicated, there was a limited number (i.e., five)
Perhaps increased support through lengthy intervention of studies available with high numbers of exposures. Second,
facilitates word learning for children with PLI enough to there were significant methodological differences across
reduce the gap in performance between the two groups studies. For example, some studies using QUIL tasks (e.g.,
and limit classification accuracy. Rice et al., 1994) were assessed together with studies using
Despite the strong evidence of word learning limitations trials-to-criterion approaches (e.g., Gray, 2003, 2004, 2005),
in monolingual children with PLI (e.g., Alt et al., 2004; and the numbers of novel words varied across tasks. Due
Dollaghan, 1987; Gray 2005), to our knowledge, only one to methodological differences, the cognitive demands of a
study assessed differences in novel-word learning skills task in the high-number-of-exposures condition in one study
between bilinguals with PLI and their TLD peers (Restrepo, may be similar to the cognitive demands of a task in the
1998). Restrepo (1998) used a trials-to-criterion word low-number-of-exposures condition in another study.
learning task as part of a set of measures to assess whether Neighborhood density (i.e., number of similar words in a
such a task could identify 5- to 7-year-old, predominantly given language) of words and phonotactic probability (i.e.,

84 LANGUAGE, SPEECH, AND HEARING SERVICES IN SCHOOLS • Vol. 43 • 81–96 • January 2012
frequency with which phonemes and phoneme sequences (SES) participated in this study. SES was assessed based on
occur in a language) may also influence word learning. High children’s eligibility for free or reduced price lunch. Thirteen
neighborhood density increases cognitive demands for children were males (6 TLD and 7 with PLI) and 15 were
recognition and production because similar words compete females (9 TLD and 6 with PLI). The mean ages were
for semantic activation (e.g., Garlock, Walley, & Metsala, 4;9 (years;months, SD = 5.23 months) for the TLD group,
2001; Gupta & MacWhinney, 1997; Luce, Goldinger, Auer, and 4;6 (SD = 6.97 months) for the group with PLI. The
& Vitevitch, 2000; Metsala, 1997). The more neighbors a difference in ages between groups was not statistically signif-
target stimulus has, the more inhibition takes place and the icant, t(22) = 1.36, p = .19. All children were recruited from
more difficult it is for the target’s semantic information to public school programs in a metropolitan area in the South-
become activated (Storkel & Morrisette, 2002). In addition, west. Consent forms and a short letter were given to teachers
high phonotactic probability facilitates word learning in to distribute to children. Children whose parents signed and
preschoolers (Storkel, 2001); however, some studies found returned the consent forms to the teachers participated in the
the opposite pattern in children with phonological deficits study. Children attended half-day preschool programs with
(e.g., Storkel, 2004) and in Spanish speakers (e.g., Vitevitch bilingual staff, but most of the curriculum instruction was
& Stamer, 2006). in English. All participants met the following criteria:
In summary, previous studies suggest that supportive & Children did not have any significant hearing loss,
novel-word learning tasks may be a promising procedure medical problem, or cognitive problem, based on a
for the differentiation of children with and without PLI in pure-tone hearing screening (American National
monolingual populations (e.g., Gray, 2003, 2004, 2005; Kan Standards Institute [ANSI], 1969); chart review for
& Windsor, 2010). DA has shown high accuracy in differ- children with PLI; and reports from a bilingual speech-
entiating bilinguals with and without PLI in various areas language pathologist (SLP), parents, and teachers.
by taking into consideration the child’s modifiability and & Children were identified as predominantly Spanish
decreasing previous language experience biases. Nonword speaking when they met three of the following
learning tasks would further reduce such biases by ensuring four criteria:
that none of the children have previously heard the words. & Parents reported on a parent questionnaire that the
A trials-to-criterion approach may provide too much support child spoke Spanish > 50% of the time during the day
to bilinguals with PLI and, as a result, decrease classifica- or communicated with the majority of the family
tion accuracy in such populations (e.g., Kiernan & Gray, members in Spanish.
1998; Restrepo, 1998). A brief DA of nonword learning skills
& Teachers reported on a teacher questionnaire that the
in which number of presentations, word neighborhood den-
child’s English language skills were lower than those
sity, and phonotactic probability are taken into account to
of a native speaker at least at the expressive level.
control for cognitive demands could give enough support to
children to reveal their learning potential while challenging & On a language proficiency scale (Smyk, Restrepo,
them cognitively so that processing limitations consistent Gorin, & Kapantzoglou, 2009) that uses story retell in
with children with PLI could be identified correctly (e.g., both languages (English and Spanish) as a language
Kohnert, Windsor, & Yim, 2006). elicitation technique, children demonstrated better
expressive language skills in Spanish by at least .5
point on a 1- to 5-point scale with intervals of .5.
Purpose of the Study
& On the Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary
The purpose of this study was to examine whether a brief Test—Spanish–Bilingual Edition (EOWPVT–SBE;
DA of word learning skills that uses verbal and visual sup- Brownell, 2001), children named items correctly in
port differentiates preschool bilinguals with PLI from their Spanish as opposed to English >50% of the time.
TLD peers. Specifically, this study evaluated whether a set of All children classified as TLD also met the following criteria:
word production and word identification scores after nine,
& Parents reported no concerns regarding their child’s
18, or 27 exposures, combined with modifiability scores
language development (Restrepo, 1998).
based on the LSC and MS, could accurately classify pre-
school bilinguals with and without PLI. & The number of grammatical errors per terminal unit
(T-unit) in the Spanish language sample was <18%
(Restrepo, 1998).
& A bilingual SLP concurred with the children’s identi-
METHOD fication as TLD.
All children classified as having PLI also met the following
Participants criteria:
Twenty-eight predominantly Spanish-speaking children & Parents reported concerns regarding their child’s lan-
(15 TLD and 13 with PLI) with low socioeconomic status guage development.

Kapantzoglou et al.: Dynamic Assessment of Word Learning Skills 85


& The number of grammatical errors per T-unit in the productions). Language samples were rated immediately
Spanish language sample was ≥18%. after completion of the retelling task by the examiner in
& A bilingual SLP concurred with the children’s identi- each language. Evidence for the validity and reliability of
fication as having PLI. this language proficiency measure has been examined for
Spanish–English bilingual children with varying degrees of
Identification Measures English proficiency. Scale ratings were highly correlated
with language sample analysis results, for example, for
Parent report. All parents completed a questionnaire grammaticality, r = .73; for vocabulary with number of
requesting demographic information, parents’ and child’s tokens, r = .62; and for MLU, r = .66. Interrater reliability
education, child’s language skills, child’s medical history, for language proficiency levels was 96% (Smyk, Restrepo,
family history related to language and learning abilities, and Gorin, & Kapantzoglou, 2009).
child’s exposure to and use of each language. A proxy for
family SES was school lunch assistance. Parent question- DA Materials
naires and consent forms were distributed and collected by
the children’s teachers. Stimuli. Three unfamiliar target objects (an animal that
Teacher report. All teachers filled out a questionnaire could not be determined, seeds, and a bubble level presented
for each child whose parents agreed to participate in the as a toy) from three semantic categories (animal, food, and
study. Teachers provided information regarding each child’s toy) and three familiar objects (flower, pizza, and sunglasses)
Spanish and English language abilities; the frequency with were used. Five adults who work with children agreed that
which the child spoke each language; and concerns regarding preschoolers could not identify any of the three target objects.
the children’s learning, cognitive, and social skills. The target words were three CVCV (consonant/vowel/
Vocabulary tests. Children took the EOWPVT–SBE, consonant/vowel) nonwords ( fote, depa, and kina) that fol-
which includes picture labeling and in which responses in lowed Spanish phonological rules. Words were chosen to be
both languages are accepted. Naming items correctly in CVCV because this is a very frequent phonotactic sequence
Spanish > 50% of the time was used as one of the criteria for in Spanish. To minimize word misarticulations, the target
determining Spanish language dominance. The measure was words included early acquired consonants.
administered according to the instructions in the manual. Nonwords as opposed to unfamiliar real words were
Language samples and language proficiency. Children used to control for word characteristics. To reduce cognitive
completed a story retelling task in English and Spanish using demands and facilitate word learning, target words of low
the books A Boy, a Dog, and a Frog (Mayer, 1967a) and neighborhood density and of high phonotactic probability
Frog on His Own (Mayer, 1967b). Both stories were avail- (Table 1) were chosen. Very low neighborhood density words
able in both languages; the four versions were used in were avoided because words with very few neighbors tend
random order. Each language was assessed using different to sound less natural in a language. Very high phonotactic
stories to avoid memory effects. All four versions were probabilities were avoided as well because word learning may
equivalent regarding length, vocabulary, and complexity be influenced by previous language experience when using
(Smyk, Restrepo, Morgan, & Kapantzoglou, 2009). Picture
support was provided when the test administrator was
reading the story and during the child’s retell to decrease Table 1. Neighborhood density and phonotactic probability of
the novel words.
cognitive load. Samples were transcribed and coded using
Systematic Analyses of Language Transcripts, research
version (Miller & Iglesias, 2008). The language samples Neighborhood Phonotactic
were segmented in T-units, which are main clauses with their density probability
subordinate clauses (Hunt, 1965). Language samples were
evaluated for number of grammatical errors per T-unit
Fote (animal) 8 .40
(Restrepo, 1998) as a measure of language skills. Depa (food) 9 .45
The story retelling language samples were also used to Kina (toy) 9 .47
assess each child’s language proficiency based on a language
proficiency scale that measured sentence length and com- Note. Neighborhood density reflects the number of words that
plexity, grammaticality, vocabulary, and fluency in each sound similar in Spanish due to variations in one phoneme (e.g.,
language. Initially, each area was rated on a 1- to 5-point zero neighborhood density would indicate that no word in Spanish
scale with .5 intervals, and then an overall proficiency level sounds similar to the nonword with only one phoneme difference).
was determined ranging from 1 to 5 (1 = silent/observer; Phonotactic probability reflects how frequently word segments
occur in their particular word position based on a database of words
2 = a few words or formulaic phrases; 3 = short sentences encountered in Spanish (e.g., zero phonotactic probability would
and phrases with multiple grammatical errors; 4 = full indicate that none of the nonword segments was in a legal word
sentences with a few grammatical errors; 5 = native-like position for Spanish).

86 LANGUAGE, SPEECH, AND HEARING SERVICES IN SCHOOLS • Vol. 43 • 81–96 • January 2012
words with frequent combinations of phonemic sounds (Kan Children were helped to develop a plan for this session
& Kohnert, 2008). Less frequent phonemic sounds and com- as follows: “Now I would like you to remember the names
binations are probably unfamiliar to all participants; thus, of objects we are going to see here. We are going to play
previous language experience may be less influential. Target and I am going to help you. Do your best. When we are done,
words were presented randomly in sentences of similar you can pick a sticker.” Also, while working on the first
syntactic complexity. two familiar words, the examiner reminded the children to
focus on the names of the objects when giving feedback by
saying, for example, “Yes, pizza, like this, keep giving me
DA Procedure
the names of objects.” Finally, before starting each new
The DA task was presented in Spanish, which was the phase, the examiner said, “Remember, you have to pay
children’s stronger language. It followed a pretest–teach– attention to the names of the objects. To remember their
posttest design and was completed in a single session of names really well, we are going to play this game one more
30–40 min. Given that we taught three nonwords with time.” At the end of each assessment, the examiner com-
three unfamiliar items in a brief session, knowledge of the mented on positive change by saying, for example, “You
words before teaching or generalization of word learning are learning new words!”
skills after teaching was not expected. In the pretest, we Each word was addressed individually, with the following
asked participants to name all the items to confirm that sequence of presentation: two familiar words, so the child
they could name all of the familiar items and none of the would become familiar with the task; two unfamiliar words;
unfamiliar items. No child could name the unfamiliar items. then, a familiar word followed by an unfamiliar word, using
Two children could not name one of the familiar items. In an alternating pattern to avoid frustration.
each case, that item was replaced with one the child could Each word was presented nine times in the script, includ-
name (e.g., a ball). ing the feedback (e.g., “yes, it’s a depa” for a correct re-
Target words were taught using a scripted structured play sponse or “it’s a depa, what is it?” in case of an incorrect
activity following Lidz’s (1991) mediation principles (see response). The words were always matched with their ref-
script in the Appendix) adapted to the activity and the chil- erents. Imitations were elicited on three occasions. An ad-
dren’s age. An MLE approach was used with a script for ditional imitation was requested in the cases of incorrect
consistency of procedures across participants, allowing feed- responses without providing additional input or feedback in
back adjusted to the children’s responses (Peña et al., 2001). this case, as seen in the earlier examples. Thus, all children
Interactions and the pace of the procedures were adjusted were given the opportunity to produce each word correctly
to each child’s needs (contingent responsivity and competence/ three times. Correct productions were considered those in
task regulation). For the play activity, the examiner used which the child produced all of the phonemes of the word
puppets. The script was designed for a birthday occasion. correctly. Children who presented with phonological pro-
It was one of the puppets’ birthday and her friends were cesses were identified during the EOWPVT–SBE and the
coming to bring her presents, which were the three familiar story retelling task. Their responses were considered correct
and three unfamiliar objects. if the children demonstrated consistent phonological pro-
The examiner established rapport with the children before cesses or phonemic errors.
initiating the activity. Affective involvement and enthusiasm The examiner provided support for learning for each word
were critical elements of the sessions and motivated the as follows: The examiner reported the category in which
participants. In the beginning, the goal of the activity was each item belonged (e.g., animal, food, or toy); talked about
stated explicitly (mediation of intentionality): “Now, we are its function/use (e.g., she can cook them with meat) using
going to play and learn some new words. Try to remember a corresponding gesture at the same time; provided a de-
the names of the objects. . .” Also, before each assessment, scription (e.g., it has a bubble); allowed the child to manip-
the examiner said: “Now let’s see if we remember the names ulate the object to become familiar with its characteristics
of the objects.” (e.g., shape); and asked the child to imitate the word on three
The purpose (mediation of meaning) was communicated occasions. The first imitation requested was an immediate
mainly by connecting the activities with the children’s imitation (e.g., “Mr. Cow brought her a depa. What is this?).
previous experiences (mediation of transcendence): “Try The second imitation was requested after a gesture (e.g.,
to remember the names of these toys, the same way you “Give the depa to Lola. What’s this?). Finally, the third
remember the names of objects you have at home. What imitation requested was a delayed imitation (e.g., “The depa
kinds of objects do you have at home? Do you have toys? tastes good. Lola can cook it with meat. What is this?).
What toys do you have?” If the child did not name any, Probes. The script as described previously was used three
the examiner said: “Do you have cars? Barbies? You know times in the DA session (Phase 1, Phase 2, and Phase 3).
their names!” The examiner repeated some of the toys the At the end of each phase, the examiner assessed the child’s
child mentioned and said: “These are the names of your ability to identify and name the objects. The examiner put
toys.” all objects, familiar and unfamiliar, on the table and asked the

Kapantzoglou et al.: Dynamic Assessment of Word Learning Skills 87


child to name them (word production) and then identify them with children. The RA was blind to the purpose of the study
(word identification). After using the script three times, and the children’s diagnoses. Training was provided by
the examiner had provided a total of three production and the first author in three sessions. At the end of each train-
three identification probes for each word (familiar and un- ing session, the RA completed a self-evaluation and the
familiar). Children’s scores for word production and iden- first author assessed the RA’s performance using Lidz’s
tification were obtained from the total of nine opportunities (1991) mediation principles with a pass/no pass criterion.
(3 nonwords × 3 opportunities/nonword) to produce the Training sessions were completed when both the author
nonwords and nine opportunities (3 nonwords × 3 opportu- and the RA agreed that the RA was following the mediation
nities/nonword) to identify them. Target words were pre- principles.
sented in random order in each phase to avoid order effects.
However, to minimize recency effects, target words were
Reliability
assessed at the end of each phase in the same order in which
they were presented in the script. In 57% (16/28) of the sessions, the first author was
Modifiability. Modifiability was assessed using the LSC present to ensure procedural integrity, and scored outcomes
and the MS at the end of the third phase. The LSC was used simultaneously with the RA. Interrater reliability was cal-
to assess the child’s strategies when learning new words. culated for each phase separately based on the word pro-
This measure includes 13 items scored on a 3-point scale duction and word identification scores in that phase, along
(0–2) for a total of 26 possible points. The LSC addresses with the LSC and MS scores. For the LSC scale (0–26 points
a child’s attention/discrimination skills, planning, self- possible), ratings were considered to be in agreement if
regulation /awareness, application, and motivation. The MS the total scores were within two points of difference. For the
expresses a general judgment of the child’s response to inter- MS scale (0–11 points possible), ratings were considered
vention. This measure includes three Likert-type items, two to be in agreement if the total scores were within one point
of which are scored on a 4-point scale (1–4) and one on a of difference. Levels of interrater agreement for word pro-
3-point scale (1–3), for a total of 11 possible points. This duction and identification for Phase 1, Phase 2, and Phase 3
measure addresses examiner’s effort, child’s responsivity, were 91%, 100%, and 94%, respectively. Interrater agree-
and transfer. Peña et al. (2001) calculated a total score for ment for LSC and MS scores was 94% and 97%, respec-
modifiability, called the modifiability index (MI), as the mean tively. When there was lack of agreement, the scores of the
of the z scores for each scale. In this study, however, we RA were maintained.
considered the two scales separately given that the two scales Regarding the language sample analyses, 18% (10/56) of
address different constructs. randomly selected samples were double scored. Interrater
agreement for total number of words was 99%; for T-units,
98%; and for grammatical errors, 93%.
General Procedure
Each child participated in three sessions of 30–40 min
Data Analysis
each. Children were seen during the school day in a quiet
room in the school. In the first two sessions, preliminary test- Descriptive statistics were estimated for each group and
ing was completed for participant recruitment and place- phase separately. Between-group differences for each of
ment in the appropriate group (TLD or with PLI) by the first the measures were assessed using independent t tests. Three
author, who was blind to the children’s diagnoses at that time. discriminant analyses, one for each phase, were conducted
In the first session, children completed the EOWPVT–BSE using SPSS for Windows Release 11.0.1 (SPSS Inc., 2001)
and the story retell task in one of the two languages. In to assess whether word production, word identification,
the second session, the children completed the Receptive LSC, and/or MS scores classified children with and with-
One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test—Spanish-Bilingual out PLI accurately. The Bonferroni procedure was used to
Edition (ROWPVT–SBE; Brownell, 2000) and the story control for Type I error, so alpha was set at .016 for each
retell task in the other language. Results from the ROWPVT– discriminant analysis. The three discriminant functions
SBE were not used for this study. In the third session, were assessed based on their level of significance and effect
children participated in the three-phase DA task. Sessions size.
were conducted at least 1 day apart to avoid participant Follow-up analyses were conducted for the statistically
fatigue. significant discriminant function. Sensitivity and specificity
were calculated to assess the percentage of children with
PLI who were classified as with PLI and the percentage of
DA Training
children with TLD who were classified as with TLD, respec-
All DA sessions were conducted by a single research tively. To estimate classification accuracy in a hypothetical
assistant (RA) who was a native Spanish speaker and second- new sample, the percentage of participants who were cor-
year master student with extensive experience working rectly classified was also estimated using the leave-one-out

88 LANGUAGE, SPEECH, AND HEARING SERVICES IN SCHOOLS • Vol. 43 • 81–96 • January 2012
procedure. The correlation between each measure and the Table 3. Correlations among measures for the group with and
discriminant function (structure correlation coefficients), without PLI.
and the unique contribution of each measure with respect
to the discriminant function in differentiating groups
P2 P3 I1 I2 I3 LSC MS
(standardized discriminant function coefficients) were
provided.
TLD (n = 15)
P1 –.11 .46 .00 .00 .05 –.31 .30
P2 — –.03 .19 .22 –.23 .40 .26
RESULTS P3 — .46 .16 .46 .36 .49
I1 — .20 .56* .28 .30
Means and standard deviations for word production and I2 — .00 .28 .40
word identification for each group for Phases 1–3 are I3 — .05 –.33
presented in Table 2. As expected, means tended to be higher LSC — .36
PLI (n = 13)
in the TLD group. A series of t tests allowing for unequal
P1 –.16 .43 –.07 .37 .27 –.06 –.05
variances between groups showed significant between-group P2 — .82** .06 .09 .20 .50 .60*
differences for word identification in Phase 1, t(24) = 2.16, P3 — .01 .30 .34 .42 .52
p = .04, and for LSC, t(21) = 2.92, p < .01. Between-group I1 — –.35 .43 .09 .00
differences were not significant for MS, p = .06, or for word I2 — .36 .23 .18
I3 — .20 .16
production in Phase 1, p = .10, Phase 2, p = .83, and Phase 3,
LSC — .90**
p = .34; or for word identification in Phase 2, p = .10, and
Phase 3, p = .32. Correlations among the variables for each
Note. P1 = Production Phase 1; P2 = Production Phase 2; P3 =
group are shown in Table 3. For the TLD group, a significant Production Phase 3; I1 = Identification Phase 1; I2 = Identification
correlation was found between word identification in Phases 1 Phase 2; I3 = Identification Phase 3.
and 3, r = .56, p = .03. For the group with PLI, a significant *p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01.
correlation was found between word production in Phases 1
and 3, r = .82, p < .01; word production in Phase 2 and MS,
r = .60, p = .03; and LSC and MS, r = .90, p < .01. analyses were conducted again using only word production,
Three discriminant analyses, one for each phase, were word identification, and LSC scores as predictors. Results
conducted with word production scores, word identification were very similar for analyses with and without inclusion
scores, LSC scores, and MS scores entered as predictors. of MS; therefore, we present only the results of the latter
Considering the high correlation between the LSC and the analysis with the most parsimonious group of predictors.
MS in the group with PLI and the small unique contribution Effect sizes and the classification accuracies of the discrim-
of MS with respect to the discriminant function in differ- inant analyses by phase are given in Table 4.
entiating groups, MS was removed and the discriminant The discriminant function for Phase 1 was statistically
significant, h2 = .39, L = .61, c2(3, N = 28) = 12.31, p = .01,
indicating that Phase 1 word production and word identifi-
Table 2. Means and standard deviations of measures by group. cation scores, in combination with the LSC scores, differ-
entiated significantly the group with and without PLI. The
overall Wilk’s lambda was neither significant for Phase 2,
TLD PLI h2 = .31, L = .69, c2(3, N = 28) = 9.22, p = .03, nor for
Measure M SD M SD Phase 3, h2 = .27, L =.73, c2(3, N = 28) = 7.65, p = .05.
We focused our follow-up analyses on Phase 1 measures
because the greatest differentiation was observed at this
Production Phase 1 .33 .49 .08 .28
Production Phase 2 .27 .46 .23 .44
phase.
Production Phase 3 .53 .74 .31 .48 For Phase 1, the discriminant function classified 78.6%
Identification Phase 1 2.00 .85 1.23 1.01 of the sample correctly as TLD or PLI. Sensitivity was 76.9%,
Identification Phase 2 2.20 .86 1.46 1.27 with 10 of the 13 children with PLI classified correctly.
Identification Phase 3 2.33 .90 1.92 1.19 Specificity was 80%, with 12 of the 15 TLD children classi-
LSC 18.13 3.68 13.00 5.34
MS 9.53 1.13 8.31 1.93
fied correctly. The leave-one-out procedure yielded cross-
validation classification accuracy of 78.6%. Table 5 shows
the structure coefficients and standardized discriminant
Note. TLD = children with typical language development; PLI =
children with primary language impairment; LSC = Learning function coefficients for the Phase I discriminant analysis.
Strategies Checklist (Lidz, 1991; Peña, 1993); MS = Modifiability Based on both sets of coefficients, word identification and
Scale (Lidz, 1991; Peña, 1993). especially LSC demonstrated strong relationships with

Kapantzoglou et al.: Dynamic Assessment of Word Learning Skills 89


Table 4. Effect sizes and classification accuracy for discriminant analyses by phase.

Classification accuracy/ Sensitivity Specificity


h2 crossvalidation (PLI classified correctly) (TLD classified correctly)

Phase 1 .39* 78.6%/78.6% 76.9% (10/13) 80% (12/15)


Phase 2 .31 67.9%/53.6% 53.8% (7/13) 80% (12/15)
Phase 3 .27 67.9%/64.3% 53.8% (7/13) 80% (12/15)

*p < .016.

the discriminant function and were therefore important in were lower than the 90% classification accuracy suggested
distinguishing between groups with and without PLI. for measures of PLI (Plante & Vance, 1994). Nonetheless,
considering the low effectiveness or efficiency of existing
measures, and that methodological modifications could im-
prove the identification accuracy of the current task, results
DISCUSSION suggest that DA of word learning skills is a promising tool
This study examined whether a DA of word learning for screening language skills for PLI in bilinguals. Based
skills can accurately differentiate predominantly Spanish- on the findings from the discriminant analysis, the word
speaking preschoolers with PLI from their TLD peers. Spe- identification and LSC scores were the best predictors of
cifically, this study investigated whether word production children’s diagnosis as TLD and with PLI.
and word identification scores after nine, 18, or 27 exposures Our findings are consistent with those of Ukrainetz
to novel words (Phases 1, 2, and 3), combined with chil- et al. (2000), who found that modifiability scores were the
dren’s modifiability scores, could accurately classify chil- strongest component that separated strong and weak lan-
dren with and without PLI. The target words were three guage learners in Native American kindergartners. However,
nonwords that were associated with three unfamiliar referents. Ukrainetz et al. reported that the MS was a stronger predictor
Word production, word identification, and modifiability were than the LSC, which is opposite to the findings in the current
addressed using a pretest–teach–posttest design. It was hypoth- study. In this study, the unique contribution of the MS
esized that word learning skills assessed based on word with respect to the discriminant function in differentiating
production, word identification, and modifiability would groups was small, and classification accuracy remained the
classify participants with high accuracy. same when MS was removed from the predictors. Given
Three discriminant analyses were conducted using the the greater variety of behaviors specified and scored with
word production and identification scores in each phase, LSC compared to MS, perhaps the LSC facilitated a more
respectively, combined with the LSC and the MS scores. refined assessment of children’s modifiability during the
Classification was more accurate when scores from Phase 1 task. The results of the current study are also consistent
were considered in combination with the LSC modifiability with those reported by Peña et al. (2001), who found that
scale. Phase 1 included nine exposures to each novel word; the classification accuracy of a dynamic version of the
three word imitations; and verbal and visual cues such as EOWPVT–R improved when scores were combined with
iconic gestures, descriptions, physical manipulations, the modifiability. Peña et al. combined LSC and MS scale scores
main function, and the semantic category of each word. by taking the mean of the z scores on each scale, so the
Overall, 78.6% of the participants were classified accurately, individual contribution of each scale to classification
with 76.9% sensitivity and 80% specificity. These results accuracy was not reported.
The results of higher classification accuracy when con-
sidering word learning after only nine exposures to the novel
Table 5. Coefficients from the discriminant analysis for Phase 1.
words coincide with research suggesting that children with
PLI have limited abilities to fast map the semantic charac-
Structure Standardized discriminant teristics of novel words (e.g., Alt et al., 2004; Dollaghan,
coefficients function coefficients 1987). In addition, the DA process indicated that word
identification scores are a better predictor of children’s
Predictor Function 1 Function 1
language skills in comparison with expressive scores. This
is consistent with results by Alt et al. (2004), which suggested
LSC .73 .76 that children with PLI have limited abilities to fast map
Identification .53 .42 the semantic characteristics of novel words in receptive
Production .41 .55
tasks in relation to their TLD peers. It may be that additional

90 LANGUAGE, SPEECH, AND HEARING SERVICES IN SCHOOLS • Vol. 43 • 81–96 • January 2012
exposures to novel stimuli facilitate learning for the chil- learning differences for word identification but not for word
dren with PLI, at least at the receptive level, and therefore production, which appears to be a more challenging task.
reduce the difference between the two groups. Results from By including a brief intervention, DA perhaps helped TLD
follow-up t tests for each phase (within-time differences) children establish strong enough representations for word
supported this claim, with statistically significant between- identification while being challenging enough for children
group differences after nine exposures but not after 18 or with PLI to reveal their limitations in word learning at
27 exposures. A larger sample is necessary to assess group the identification level. Methodological variations in
differences across time with adequate precision and power. word learning activities seem to influence the outcomes
Limited fast mapping abilities could be an indicator of considerably.
poor information processing skills, which appears to be a Follow-up t tests for each phase showed no significant
key point of difference between children with and without differences between the groups with and without PLI in word
PLI (e.g., Dollaghan & Campbell, 1998; Gray, 2004; Kohnert production after any of the three teaching phases; neverthe-
et al., 2006). less, interpretation of such results should be made cautiously
The result of better differentiation in Phase 1 than in because all children scored poorly on the expressive task.
Phase 2 or 3 contrasts those found in Kan and Windsor’s Children in both groups had difficulties naming the novel
(2010) meta-analysis; their effect size estimates indicated words accurately even after 27 presentations, suggesting
that higher numbers of exposures better shows the differ- that the naming task may have been too difficult for the
ences in learning between groups with and without PLI. participants. Considering similar results from previous
As discussed earlier, the meta-analytic results should be studies (e.g., Gray, 2003), learning novel words well enough
interpreted with caution due to the limited number of studies to name their referents seems to require not only a variety
available with high numbers of exposures and to method- of cues, but also a great number of presentations of the novel
ological differences in the studies examined. For example, words in a variety of supportive contexts. The current study
the number of words used in the tasks varied across studies: used a 30- to 40-min session for the intervention with a
Rice et al. (1994) used eight words, Gray (2003, 2004, 2005) maximum of 27 presentations of each novel word, whereas
used four words, and three were used in the present study. previous research has used more than one session (e.g., Gray,
Also, Gray (2003, 2004, 2005) used trials-to-criterion ap- 2003, used up to four sessions) with similar numbers of
proaches, providing minimal contextual support for learn- presentations per session.
ing, which probably required more exposures for learning In the current study, even though a variety of cues were
than the current study in which there was more contextual provided and word characteristics such as neighborhood
support. Further, what was considered “low” and “high” density and phonotactic probability were manipulated to
numbers of presentations also varied across studies. For decrease cognitive demands for production, this brief task
instance, in the study by Rice et al. (1994), the condition was not sufficient to help children produce the novel words,
with the higher number of presentations included 10 presen- even for the TLD group. However, making the task easier
tations of each of the eight words, whereas in the current by reducing the number of words or increasing the number
study, the condition with the lowest number of presentations, of presentations, for example, may increase differentiation
Phase 1, included almost the same amount of presentations based on production but may also impact word identification,
(nine) for each of the three words. Several task characteris- which differentiated the children well. Perhaps these two
tics need to be taken into account to make judgments areas should be assessed separately while providing the
regarding the cognitive demands of each task. Cross-study adequate level of support in each case to avoid floor or
comparisons at this point do not seem helpful for drawing ceiling effects. It is also possible that evaluating partial word
conclusions regarding how a particular task characteristic, learning may be more productive than examining full pro-
such as number of presentations, influences task effective- duction probes, as children were able to retrieve a label
ness, because other task features were not held constant on the incorrect referent.
across studies. At times, children with and without PLI were able to
Better differentiation from word identification rather retrieve the novel words but with an incorrect referent, sug-
than expressive scores is consistent with Kan and Windsor’s gesting early phonological mapping of the word. On other
(2010) meta-analytic findings that word identification and occasions, children reported the semantic category or similar
recognition tasks resulted in greater effect sizes than word real words differing in one or more phonemes. The variety
production tasks. However, these results contrast with those of errors reflects the diversity of challenges that children may
of Gray (2003, 2004) and Kiernan and Gray (1998), who face during word learning and demonstrates that partial word
used trials-to-criterion approaches and found that word pro- learning is taking place, especially in a brief session. Such
duction measures were more predictive of PLI than word errors support results from previous studies (e.g., McGregor
identification in word learning studies. It is possible that et al., 2002) indicating that encoding happens gradually
the support provided to children through a trials-to-criterion and that, although children may be able to produce a word,
approach helped the children with PLI and minimized their they may not comprehend it totally. Further, partial word

Kapantzoglou et al.: Dynamic Assessment of Word Learning Skills 91


learning results indicate that in the initial stages of word needed to examine more accurately group differences across
learning, children may have the phonological or semantic times. Examination of partial word learning may also help
map, but not both. Therefore, use of measures that are more improve discrimination between groups with and without
sensitive to partial word learning may lead to better differ- PLI. Further, in some circumstances, DA of other skills
entiation between groups. may be more accurate, considering that not all children with
Data from the current experimental study yielded prom- PLI may have word learning difficulties (Gray, 2004). For
ising results for using DA as an alternative method for example, Roseberry and Connell (1991) and Anderson
evaluating vocabulary learning skills in predominately (2001) used rule learning rather than a word learning task
Spanish-speaking preschoolers. Static vocabulary measures to examine differences between preschool Spanish speakers
are not accurate in differentiating children with and without with and without PLI.
PLI and are often biased for bilinguals (e.g., Gray, Plante, In summary, the current DA method of word learning
Vance, & Henrichsen, 1999; Peña et al., 2001). Children’s shows promise as an adequate tool for screening Spanish-
potential to change through adult guidance in this word speaking children who are at risk of PLI. It is possible that
learning task was a good indicator of their overall language this measure alone is not sufficient for 90% accuracy in
abilities (ZPD; Vygotsky, 1978). Further support of such classification (Plante & Vance, 1994). However, classification
outcomes would have significant clinical implications for accuracy is good for screening purposes, and given the
the differentiation of bilinguals who present with language brief administration time, the task is clinically efficient. The
difference from those who have language impairment. results indicate that word comprehension after nine expo-
Assessing language learning skills as opposed to existing sures in combination with the LSC classified children
knowledge reduces biases of previous language experience with near 80% accuracy. Future research may make small
(e.g., Peña et al., 2006; Ukrainetz et al., 2000). In addition, modifications to the task to examine improvement in clas-
assessment of the child’s attention, self-regulation, planning, sification accuracy.
motivation, and response to intervention seems to provide
valuable information for an accurate diagnosis (e.g., Peña,
2000; Peña et al., 2001, 2006; Ukrainetz et al., 2000).
Competent SLPs may intuitively take such factors into con- REFERENCES
sideration in both assessment and intervention and provide Alt, M., Plante, E., & Creusere, M. (2004). Semantic features
additional support for learning. In DA, the clinician addresses in fast-mapping: Performance of preschoolers with specific
these areas by following the mediation principles and eval- language impairment versus preschoolers with normal language.
uates them more systematically as part of the assessment Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47,
process. These results suggest that brief word learning tasks 407–420.
reveal slower learning processes in children with PLI, but American National Standards Institute. (1969). Specifications
longer adult–child interactions also provide necessary infor- for audiometers. New York, NY: Author.
mation regarding children’s learning strategies and modifi- Anderson, R. T. (2001). Learning an invented inflectional mor-
ability. Assessing word learning skills at various levels may pheme in Spanish by children with typical language skills and
be the key to an accurate diagnosis of PLI. with specific language impairment (SLI). International Journal
of Language and Communication Disorders, 36, 1–19.
Limitations and Future Directions Barret, M., Zachman, L., & Huisingh, R. (1988). Assessing
Semantic Skills Through Everyday Themes. Moline, IA:
The current study indicates that DA of word learning Linguisystems.
skills can be used to screen language skills for PLI in young Berman, R. A., & Slobin, D. I. (1994). Relating events in
bilinguals. However, it is important to consider that the cog- narrative: A cross linguistic developmental study. Hillsdale, NJ:
nitive demands of the word learning task are highly sensitive Erlbaum.
to methodological variations and may influence classification Bloom, P. (2001). Précis of how children learn the meanings of
accuracy. This study took into consideration a number of words. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(6), 1095–1103.
task characteristics, but results indicate that further method- Brownell, R. (2000). Receptive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test
ological manipulations could facilitate word production and (Spanish–Bilingual ed.). Novato, CA: Academic Therapy
improve classification accuracy. Systematic manipulations Publications.
of one task characteristic at a time would facilitate better Brownell, R. (2001). Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary
understanding of how task demands change and influence Test (Spanish–Bilingual ed.). Novato, CA: Academic Therapy
classification accuracy. The current study assessed changes Publications.
in classification accuracy when the numbers of exposures Burton, V. J., & Watkins, R. V. (2007). Measuring word
changed, but more exposures and/or imitations seem nec- learning: Dynamic versus static assessment of kindergarten
essary for greater performance across groups in word pro- vocabulary. Journal of Communication Disorders, 40(5),
duction. Such modifications and a larger sample size are 335–356.

92 LANGUAGE, SPEECH, AND HEARING SERVICES IN SCHOOLS • Vol. 43 • 81–96 • January 2012
Campbell, T., Dollaghan, C., Needleman, H., & Janosky, J. Gray, S. (2005). Word learning by preschoolers with specific
(1997). Reducing bias in language assessment: Processing- language impairment: Effect of phonological or semantic cues.
dependent measures. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48,
40(3), 519–525. 1452–1467.
Campione, J. C., & Brown, A. L. (1987). Linking dynamic Gray, S., Plante, E., Vance, R., & Henrichsen, M. (1999). The
assessment with school achievement. In C. S. Lidz (Ed.), diagnostic accuracy of four vocabulary tests administered to
Dynamic assessment: An interactional approach to evaluating preschool-age children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services
learning potential (pp. 82–115). New York, NY: Guilford in Schools, 30(2), 196–206.
Press. Grigorenko, E. L. (2009). Dynamic assessment and response to
Caramazza, A. (1997). How many levels of processing are intervention: Two sides of one coin. Journal of Learning
there in lexical access? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14(1), Disabilities, 42, 111–132.
177–208. Grigorenko, E. L., & Sternberg, R. J. (1998). Dynamic testing.
Carey, S., & Bartlett, E. (1978). Acquiring a single new word. Psychological Bulletin, 124(1), 75–111.
Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 15, 17–29.
Gupta, P., & MacWhinney, B. (1997). Vocabulary acquisition
Carlson, J. S., & Wiedl, K. H. (1992). The dynamic assessment and verbal short-term memory: Computational and neural bases.
of intelligence. In H. C. Hollywood & D. Tzuriel (Eds.), Brain and Language, 59(2), 267–333.
Interactive assessment (pp. 167–186). Berlin, Germany:
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F. (1996). Language diversity: Implications
Springer-Verlag.
for assessment. In K. N. Cole, P. S. Dale, & D. J. Thal (Eds.),
Dollaghan, C. (1987). Fast mapping in normal and language- Assessment of communication and language (Vol. 6, pp. 29–56).
impaired children. The Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
52(3), 218–222.
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F., Peña, E. D., & Quinn, R. (1995).
Dollaghan, C., & Campbell, T. F. (1998). Nonword repetition Accommodating cultural differences in narrative style: A multi-
and child language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, cultural perspective. Topics in Language Disorders, 15(4), 54–67.
and Hearing Research, 41(5), 1136–1146.
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1992). American parenting of language
Edwards, J., & Lahey, M. (1996). Auditory lexical decisions of learning children: Persisting differences in family–child inter-
children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech actions observed in natural home environments. Developmental
and Hearing Research, 39(6), 1263–1273. Psychology, 28, 1096–1105.
Feuerstein, R. (1979). The dynamic assessment of retarded per- Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the
formers: The learning potential assessment device, theory, instru- everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore,
ments, and techniques. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press. MD: Brookes.
Gardner, M. F. (1990). Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Horton-Ikard, R., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2007). A preliminary
Test—Revised. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications. examination of vocabulary and word learning in African
Garlock, V. M., Walley, A. C., & Metsala, J. L. (2001). Age-of- American toddlers from middle and low socioeconomic status
acquisition, word frequency, and neighborhood density effects homes. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology,
on spoken word recognition by children and adults. Journal of 16(4), 381–392.
Memory and Language, 45(3), 468–492. Hunt, K. (1965). Grammatical structures written at three grade
Gershkoff-Stowe, L. (2001). The course of children’s naming levels (Research Report No. 3). Champaign, IL: National Council
errors in early word learning. Journal of Cognition & Develop- of Teachers of English.
ment, 2(2), 131–155. Kan, P. F., & Kohnert, K. (2008). Fast mapping by bilingual
Gershkoff-Stowe, L. (2002). Object naming, vocabulary growth, preschool children. Journal of Child Language, 35(3), 495–514.
and the development of word retrieval abilities. Journal of
Kan, P. F., & Windsor, J. (2010). Word learning in children with
Memory and Language, 46(4), 665–687.
primary language impairment: A meta-analysis. Journal of
Gershkoff-Stowe, L., & Smith, L. B. (1997). A curvilinear trend Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 739–756.
in naming errors as a function of early vocabulary growth.
Kiernan, B., & Gray, S. (1998). Word learning in a supported-
Cognitive Psychology, 34(1), 37–71.
learning context by preschool children with specific language
Gleitman, L. (1990). The structural sources of verb meanings. impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
Language Acquisition, 1(1), 3–56. Research, 41, 161–171.
Goldstein, B. A. (2004). Bilingual language development and Kohnert, K., Windsor, J., & Miller, R. (2004). Crossing borders:
disorders in Spanish–English speakers. Baltimore, MD: Brookes. Recognition of Spanish words by English-speaking children with
Gray, S. (2003). Word-learning by preschoolers with specific and without language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics,
language impairment: What predicts success? Journal of Speech, 25(4), 543–564.
Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 56–67. Kohnert, K., Windsor, J., & Yim, D. (2006). Do language-based
Gray, S. (2004). Word learning by preschoolers with specific processing tasks separate children with language impairment
language impairment: Predictors and poor learners. Journal of from typical bilinguals? Learning Disabilities Research &
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 1117–1132. Practice, 21(1), 19–29.

Kapantzoglou et al.: Dynamic Assessment of Word Learning Skills 93


Kohnert, K. J., & Bates, E. (2002). Balancing bilinguals II: Peña, E. D., Gillam, R. B., Malek, M., Ruiz-Felter, R.,
Lexical comprehension and cognitive processing in children Resendiz, M., Fiestas, C., & Sabel, T. (2006). Dynamic
learning Spanish and English. Journal of Speech, Language, and assessment of school-age children’s narrative ability: An
Hearing Research, 45, 347–359. experimental investigation of classification accuracy. Journal
Lahey, M., Edwards, J., & Munson, B. (2001). Is processing of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 1037–1057.
speed related to severity of language impairment? Journal of Peña, E. D., Iglesias, A., & Lidz, C. S. (2001). Reducing test bias
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 1354–1361. through dynamic assessment of children’s word-learning ability.
Lidz, C. S. (1987). Dynamic assessment: An interactional approach American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 10(2), 138–154.
to evaluating learning potential. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Plante, E. D., & Vance, R. (1994). Selection of preschool lan-
Lidz, C. S. (1991). Practitioner’s guide to dynamic assessment. guage tests: A data-based approach. Language, Speech, and
New York, NY: Guilford Press. Hearing Services in Schools, 25, 15–24.
Limbos, M. M., & Geva, E. (2001). Accuracy of teacher Restrepo, M. A. (1998). Identifiers of predominantly Spanish-
assessments of second-language students at risk for reading speaking children with language impairment. Journal of Speech,
disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34(2), 136–151. Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 1398–1411.
Luce, P. A., Goldinger, S. D., Auer, E. T. Jr., & Vitevitch, Restrepo, M. A., & Silverman, S. (2001). The validity of the
M. S. (2000). Phonetic priming, neighborhood activation, and Spanish Preschool Language Scale for use with bilingual
PARSYN. Perception & Psychophysics, 62(3), 615–625. children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology,
10, 382–393.
Mayer, M. (1967a). A boy, a dog, and a frog. New York, NY: Pied
Piper Books. Rice, M. L., Buhr, J. C., & Nemeth, M. (1990). Fast mapping
word-learning abilities of language-delayed preschoolers. The
Mayer, M. (1967b). Frog on his own. New York, NY: Pied Piper
Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55(1), 33–42.
Books.
McGregor, K. K., Newman, R. M., Reilly, R. M., & Capone, Rice, M. L., Oetting, J. B., Marquis, J., Bode, J., & Pae, S.
N. C. (2002). Semantic representation and naming in children (1994). Frequency of input effects on word comprehension of
with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Lan- children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech
guage, and Hearing Research, 45, 998–1014. and Hearing Research, 37(1), 106–122.
Metsala, J. L. (1997). An examination of word frequency and Roseberry, C., & Connell, P. (1991). Use of an invented language
neighborhood density in the development of spoken-word rule in the differentiation of normal and specific language-
recognition. Memory & Cognition, 25(1), 47–56. impaired Spanish-speaking children. Journal of Speech and
Hearing Research, 34, 596–603.
Miller, J. F., & Iglesias, A. (2008). Systematic Analysis of Lan-
guage Transcripts, Bilingual SE Version [Computer software]. Samson, J. F., & Lesaux, N. K. (2009). Language-minority
SALT Software, LLC. learners in special education: Rates and predictors of identifica-
Montgomery, J. W., & Windsor, J. (2007). Examining the tion for services. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42(2),
language performances of children with and without specific 148–162.
language impairment: Contributions of phonological short-term Seiger-Gardner, L., & Brooks, P. J. (2008). Effects of onset-
memory and speed of processing. Journal of Speech, Language, and rhyme-related distractors on phonological processing in
and Hearing Research, 50, 778–797. children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech,
Muñoz, M. L., Gillam, R. B., Peña, E. D., & Gulley-Faehnle, Language, and Hearing Research, 51, 1263–1281.
A. (2003). Measures of language development in fictional Smyk, E., Restrepo, M. A., Gorin, J., & Kapantzoglou, M.
narratives of Latino children. Language, Speech, and Hearing (2009, November). A global rating scale of language proficiency
Services in Schools, 34, 332–342. in Spanish–English bilingual children. Paper session presented at
Nelson, K. (1985). Making sense: The acquisition of shared the annual meeting of the American Speech-Language-Hearing
meaning. Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Association, New Orleans, LA.
Ordoñez, C., Carlo, M. S., Snow, C. E., & McLaughlin, B. Smyk, E., Restrepo, M. A., Morgan, G., & Kapantzoglou, M.
(2002). Depth and breadth of vocabulary in two languages: (2009, June). Development of parallel measures for story re-
Which vocabulary skills transfer? Journal of Educational telling and comprehension tasks for Spanish-speaking first
Psychology, 94(4), 719–728. graders. Poster session presented at the annual Symposium on
Research in Child Language Disorders, Madison, WI.
Pearson, B. Z., Fernández, S. C., Lewedeg, V., & Oller, K.
(1997). The relation of input factors to lexical learning by SPSS for Windows. (2011). Release 11.0.1 [Computer software].
bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18(1), 41–58. Chicago, IL: SPSS Inc.
Peña, E. (1993). Dynamic assessment: A non-biased approach for Storkel, H. L. (2001). Learning new words: Phonotactic proba-
assessing the language of young children. (Doctoral dissertation). bility in language development. Journal of Speech, Language,
Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses. (Order and Hearing Research, 44, 1321–1337.
No. 9316518) Storkel, H. L. (2004). The emerging lexicon of children with
Peña, E. (2000). Measurement of modifiability in children from phonological delays: Phonotactic constraints and probability
culturally and linguistically diverse backgrounds. Communica- in acquisition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
tion Disorders Quarterly, 21(2), 87–97. Research, 47, 1194–1212.

94 LANGUAGE, SPEECH, AND HEARING SERVICES IN SCHOOLS • Vol. 43 • 81–96 • January 2012
Storkel, H. L., & Morrisette, M. L. (2002). The lexicon and Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language (Rev. ed.).
phonology: Interactions in language acquisition. Language, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 33, 24–37. Weismer, S. E., & Hesketh, L. J. (1993). The influence of
Stothard, S. E., Snowling, M. J., Bishop, D., Chipchase, B. B., prosodic and gestural cues on novel word acquisition by children
& Kaplan, C. A. (1998). Language-impaired preschoolers: A with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and
follow-up into adolescence. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 36(5), 1013–1025.
Hearing Research, 41, 407–418. Weismer, S. E., & Hesketh, L. J. (1996). Lexical learning by
Swanson, H. L., & Lussier, C. M. (2001). A selective synthesis of children with specific language impairment: Effects of linguistic
the experimental literature on dynamic assessment. Review of input presented at varying speaking rates. Journal of Speech
Educational Research, 71(2), 321–363. and Hearing Research, 39(1), 177–190.
Trabasso, T., Stein, N. L., Rodkin, P. C., Munger, M. P., & Weismer, S. E., & Hesketh, L. J. (1998). The impact of emphatic
Baughn, C. (1991). Knowledge of goals and plans in the on-line stress on novel word learning by children with specific language
narration of events. Cognitive Development, 7, 133–170. impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
Research, 41, 1444–1458.
Tzuriel, D. (2001). Dynamic assessment of young children.
New York, NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum. Windsor, J., Kohnert, K., Loxtercamp, A. L., & Kan, P.
(2008). Performance on nonlinguistic visual tasks by children
Ukrainetz, T. A., Harpell, S., Walsh, C., & Coyle, C. (2000).
with language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29(2),
A preliminary investigation of dynamic assessment with Native
237–268.
American kindergartners. Language, Speech, and Hearing
Services in Schools, 31, 142–154. Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & Pond, R. E. (1993).
Preschool Language Scale—3: Spanish Edition. San Antonio,
Vitevitch, M. S., & Stamer, M. K. (2006). The curious case of
TX: The Psychological Corporation.
competition in Spanish speech production. Language and
Cognitive Processes, 21(6), 760–770.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of
higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.

Kapantzoglou et al.: Dynamic Assessment of Word Learning Skills 95


APPENDIX. SCRIPT

The following instructions were provided in Spanish:


“Now, we are going to play and learn some new words. Try to remember the names of the objects.
The same way you remember the names of objects you have at home. What kinds of objects do
you have at home? Do you have toys? What toys do you have? (If the child doesn’t name any items
say: “Do you have cars? Barbies?”) You know their names! (Repeat some of the toys the child
mentioned and say: “these are the names of your toys”). Now, I would like you to remember the
names of objects we are going to see here. We are going to play and I am going to help you. Do
your best. When we are done, you can pick a sticker.”

While working on the first two familiar words, the examiner emphasized to the children to remember the
names of the objects when giving feedback by saying for example “yes, pizza, like this, keep giving me
the names of objects.” Also, before assessing word identification and production the examiner said: “Now
let’s see if we remember the names of the objects.” After the assessment, the examiner said: “You are
doing very well! You are learning new words! To remember them really well, we are going to play this game
one more time. Remember, you have to pay attention to the names of the objects.”
Example of script (English translation):
(Mr. Chicken/Food - DEPA)
Knock knock. . . Who is it? It’s Mr. Chicken! The chicken brought her DEPA.
Imitation: What’s this?
Correct response: Yes, DEPA.
Incorrect response: _____________DEPA, what is it? _____________
Another incorrect response: MOVE TO THE NEXT STEP. NO FEEDBACK.
Lola wants you to look at it (use Lola). “Look at my DEPA” (puppet gives object to the child -
manipulation 5 sec). Give the DEPA to Lola.
Imitation: What is this?
Correct response: Yes, DEPA.
Incorrect response: _____________DEPA, what is it? _____________
Another incorrect response: MOVE TO THE NEXT STEP. NO FEEDBACK.
Lola liked the DEPA. The DEPA is food! Look, they are small balls. The DEPA tastes good! Lola can
cook it (gesture) with meat.
Imitation: What is this?
Correct response: Yes, DEPA.
Incorrect response: _____________DEPA, what is it? _____________
Another incorrect response: MOVE TO THE NEXT STEP. NO FEEDBACK.

96 LANGUAGE, SPEECH, AND HEARING SERVICES IN SCHOOLS • Vol. 43 • 81–96 • January 2012
Copyright of Language, Speech & Hearing Services in Schools is the property of American Speech-Language-
Hearing Association and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv
without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.

You might also like