You are on page 1of 10

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 1983, 161,329-338 NUMBER 3 (FALL 1983)

A MODIFIED INCIDENTAL-TEACHING PROCEDURE


FOR AUTISTIC YOUTH: ACQUISITION AND
GENERALIZATION OF RECEPTIVE OBJECT LABELS
GAIL G. MCGEE, PATRICIA J. KRANTZ, DEBRA MASON,
AND LYNN E. MCCLANNAHAN
PRINCETON CHILD DEVELOPMENT INSTITUTE

A modified incidental-teaching procedure was used to increase the receptive language


skills of autistic youth who had previously experienced lengthy institutionalization. At
the time of the study, the two severely language-delayed children had recently been
transitioned to a community-based group home. Receptive-labeling skills were taught
for four sets of objects typically used in school lunch preparation. The percentage of
correct, unprompted object identifications displayed by Youth 1 increased when the in-
cidental-teaching package (gestural prompts, behavior-specific praise, and contingent
access to lunch-making supplies) was sequentially introduced in a multiple-baseline
design across sets of objects. These results were replicated with Youth 2. The youths'
newly acquired language skills also generalized to a different setting (the dining room
of the group home) and to a different activity occurring later in the day (a traditional
sit-down, discrete-trial session). This research indicates that the linguistic skills of
severely developmentally delayed autistic children can be accelerated by incidental in-
struction that is provided in the course of shaping other home-living skills.
DESCRIPTORS: autistic children, generalization, incidental teaching, language

Incidental-teaching procedures developed by topic is a prepotent reinforcer available in the


Hart and Risley (1968, 1974, 1975, 1980) environment, then requests an elaboration or
have been well-documented as effective in teach- improvement in the child's request. If the child
ing language to disadvantaged preschool chil- is unable to produce the elaborated response,
dren in naturally occurring adult-child interac- prompts or models are provided to assure cor-
tions. As defined by Hart and Risley (1982), in- rect responding, which is followed by teacher
cidental teaching is a process that occurs when approval and access to the requested materials,
the natural environment is arranged to attract activity, or information.
children to desired materials and activities, and A similar procedure was reported by Halle,
a teacher is available to provide attention, praise, Baer, and Spradlin (1981) in a study designed
and instruction when a child initiates an interac- to evaluate teachers' use of nonverbal cues to
tion related to a topic of immediate interest. If increase verbal initiations by language-delayed,
necessary, the teacher verifies that the child's mentally retarded children. Teachers were
trained to delay offers of help in selected natural-
This research was supported by Grant No. MH- istic situations when children desired objects, ac-
20030 awarded by the National Institute of Mental tivities, or assistance, thereby evoking increased
Health (Center for Studies of Crime and Delin- spontaneous use of previously acquired speech.
quency) to the Bureau of Child Research, University
of Kansas; and by grants awarded to the Princeton Although there is consensus that incidental-
Child Development Institute by Father Flanagan's teaching procedures are effective in facilitating
Boys' Home, Boys Town, Nebraska. Reprints may be spontaneous speech, a defining characteristic of
obtained from Gail G. McGee Princeton Child De-
velopment Institute, P.O. Box 2013, Princeton, New these procedures is the requirement that children
Jersey 08540. have previously acquired language skills that
329
330 GAIL G. McGEE et al.
enable them to initiate verbal interactions. In and severe skill deficits, the total amount of
a modification of standard incidental-teaching available teaching can be critical to the youths'
procedures, Rogers-Warren and Warren (1980) ability to make successful adaptations to com-
developed a mand-model technique that was munity living. Thus, incidental teaching can be
used with three language-delayed preschool chil- an invaluable tool, because it maximizes the
dren who did not typically initiate verbal inter- amount of instruction by teaching language
actions. This intervention occurred in a free-play skills while simultaneously shaping other neces-
setting, but teachers controlled teaching oppor- sary home-living skills. Additionally, language
tunities by instructing children to state or elab- training in a naturalistic home environment that
orate their requests. promotes generalization across settings can en-
There is a need for systematic investigations hance the youths' ability to participate in com-
that extend incidental-teaching procedures to munity activities such as shopping trips, eating
severely language-delayed autistic children, since in restaurants, and visiting natural families. This
certain characteristics of the procedure present study was conducted to determine whether inci-
the potential for circumventing many of the diffi- dental-teaching procedures could be modified to
culties encountered in highly structured, discrete- provide naturalistic training of receptive object
trial language teaching. Autistic children often labels to severely language-delayed autistic youth
have severe deficits in both receptive and ex- who did not initiate interactions via language or
pressive language, and although some autistic gestures, while preserving the benefits (e.g.,
youth make substantial progress when standard facilitation of generalization) that have been
language training programs are provided (i.e., afforded by standard incidental-teaching pro-
Guess, Sailor, & Baer, 1976; Lovaas, 1977), cedures.
others display extremely slow rates of acquisition
or fail to acquire targeted skills. Further, the METHOD
effectiveness of incidental-teaching procedures in Participans
promoting language generalization, which is Two autistic youth who resided in a Teaching-
characteristically difficult for autistic children to Family Model group home participated in the
achieve (Koegel & Rincover, 1974), suggests study. Both youth had been transitioned from a
that such procedures may hold special promise large state institution to the family-style, com-
for these youth. However, the autistic children munity-based group home within the year prior
most in need of innovative language program- to the study, following institutionalization for 7
ming often do not initiate interactions with yr, 5 mo and 7 yr, 7 mo, respectively. Both
others and frequently display severe problem be- youth had severe deficits in expressive and re-
haviors when access to desired materials, activi- ceptive language, and at program entry they
ties, or assistance is limited. Therefore, proce- were largely unable to label objects typically
dural modifications are necessary in order to found in a home setting. Prior to entering the
adapt incidental-teaching procedures for use with group home, communication with the youth had
many autistic youth. consisted mainly of sign language, with little
Modified incidental-teaching procedures ap- teaching of verbal language skills.
pear particularly applicable to community-based Youth 1 was 15 yr, 10 mo old; she achieved
programs that provide autistic children with in- a Leiter Mental Age of 5.7 the year prior to her
struction and treatment in the context of natural- entry to the group home. At the time of this
istic, familylike activities in a home environ- study, she was receiving individualized treat-
ment. When programming for previously insti- ment for physical aggression, inappropriate
tutionalized youngsters who display multiple noisemaking, and disruptive bedtime behavior,
MODIFIED INCIDENTAL TEACHING 331

as well as individualized instruction in dressing, areas in a naturalistic home environment. In


following multiple directions, sight-word read- addition, verbal language skills were taught both
ing, and handwriting. She had acquired basic di- in discrete-trial sessions and throughout ongoing
rection-following skills since program entry; activities (e.g., dinner, greeting guests); indi-
stereotyped and repetitive finger play was tar- vidualized treatment programs were in effect
geted for treatment at the completion of this throughout the children's waking hours to help
study. Sign-language training prior to group them learn to control their severe behavior prob-
home entry had not facilitated this youth's ex- lems. The teaching-parent couple and two as-
pressive speech; language training at the group sociate therapists, professionally trained at the
home focused on verbal productions. When this Princeton Child Development Institute, pro-
study was initiated, expressive speech was largely vided teaching, training, and parenting in ses-
limited to prompted expressions of desired rein- sions with child-teacher ratios ranging from 1:1
forcers (e.g., "What do you want?"; "I want to 3: 1. The duration of planned activities ranged
cookie, please"), with some specifically trained from 15 min to 1 hr, depending on the nature of
words and phrases occurring under structured the task, the children's attending skills, and the
stimulus conditions (e.g., Edmark reading ses- number of children included in the session.
sions). The two participants in this study used token
Youth 2 was 12 yr, 7 mo old; he obtained a systems, earning poker chips that were inserted
Leiter Mental age of 5.0 the year prior to his into clear plastic holders. Tokens were earned
arrival in the group home. Concurrent with this on continuous reinforcement schedules for cor-
reseaarch he was receiving individualized treat- rect responses and on individualized fixed-inter-
ment of noncontextual laughing and crying and val schedules for responses incompatible with
hand-biting, and individualized instruction in target behaviors. When five tokens were earned,
sight-word reading, handwriting, and following the youth exchanged them for their choice of
multiple directions. Like Youth 1, he had previ- food or drink reinforcers. For purposes of
ously acquired basic direction-following skills; this study, however, the rate of reinforcement
vocal noisemaking was targeted for treatment was controlled across experimental conditions;
at the conclusion of this research. Youth 2 had tokens were delivered following every third
also acquired a limited productive vocabulary trial, with exchange for back-up items when a
since program entry, consisting primarily of one- child earned five tokens.
word utterances and a few short phrases. Severe This research was conducted during regular
articulation deficits and extremely low voice parts of the daily activity schedule: school lunch
volume were characteristic of his speech. preparation and 1: 1 language sessions. Teaching
of object labels during lunch preparation oc-
Setting curred in the home's kitchen; generalization was
The research was conducted in a Teaching- assessed in sit-down sessions in the dining room.
Family Model group home, where the two par- The furnishings and materials used in the study
ticipants and three other autistic youth resided are typically available in home settings; these
with their teaching-parents in a suburban com- included food items and lunch packaging sup-
munity. The children's activity schedule began plies, as well as a kitchen counter and a dining
daily at 3:30 p.m., when they arrived home from room table and chairs.
school, and remained in effect until bedtime at
9:30 p.m. Regularly scheduled activities em- Response Definitions
phasized systematic teaching of skills in self- Object identifications were scored as correct
help, home maintenance, social, and academic when the youth responded to the teaching-par-
332 GAIL G. McGEE et al.
ent's request to "Give me _," by selecting from The data sheet indicated the order of requests
an array of five items and handing the requested for target objects, as well as the order in which
object to the teaching-parent. Only unprompted target and distractor items should be displayed
object identifications that occurred within 5 sec on the counter or table. Additional randomiza-
of the teaching-parent's request were scored as tion of stimulus conditions was accomplished by
correct; merely touching an object, or self-cor- arbitrarily varying five versions of the data sheet
recting by handing a second object to the teach- across sessions. Independent observers recorded
ing-parent were scored as incorrect. the label of the objects selected by the youth, and
Receptive-labeling skills were taught for four scored each selection as a correct or incorrect
sets of items used in lunch preparation; each set response.
included three target objects and two distractor Interobserver agreement between two inde-
objects. For example, Set 1 for Youth 1 was pendent observers was assessed during at least
comprised of sandwich-making materials, in- 33% of the sessions in each condition, and was
cluding three target objects (relish, lettuce, bag- calculated using the formula: total number of
gie) and two distractors (aluminum foil and agreements divided by total number of agree-
tuna). The order of presentation of sets was de- ments plus disagreements X 100. For Youth 1,
termined by the typical progression of the lunch mean interobserver agreement during lunch
preparation routine (i.e., Set 1, sandwich-making preparation was 100%, 99%, 97%, and 96%
materials, was presented first; vegetables in Set for Sets 1 through 4, respectively; mean agree-
2 were next; snacks were in Set 3; and, Set 4 ment for each of the four sets in generalization
consisted of items used in packing the lunches). sessions was 97%, 100%, 99%, and 99%.
Data were collected daily in lunch preparation Mean interobserver agreement scores for Youth
sessions in the kitchen and later in daily gen- 2 were 97%, 99%, 98%, and 100% for the
eralization sessions in the dining room. Receptive four sets during lunch preparation sessions, with
labels for the target objects in all sets were as- mean percent agreement during generalization
sessed three times per session. On each trial the assessment of 97%, 98%, 99%, and 97%. In
five objects in the set (three target objects and summary, overall means for interobserver agree-
two distractors) were presented in randomized ment were 98% for Youth 1 (range 94% to
arrangements on the kitchen counter or dining 100%) and 98% for Youth 2 (range 94% to
room table, in order to ensure that the youth 100%).
were accurately discriminating rather than re-
sponding on the basis of item placement. Also Teaching Procedure
randomized was the order of the teaching-
parent's requests for target objects within each Incidental teaching occurred daily in a 45-min
set. The order of requested objects varied across session in the kitchen, during preparation of
three randomized blocks (e.g., lettuce, baggie, lunches that the children would take to school
relish; baggie, relish, lettuce; relish, lettuce bag- the next day. Each youth received 1: 1 instruc-
gie). This further ensured object discrimination, tion and assistance from a teaching-parent in
while permitting "breaks" every third trial, dur- completing this task; the teaching-parent had
ing which the lunch preparation routine or rein- previous experience in implementing standard
forcement for attending behaviors could take research procedures. Both youth had partici-
place. This procedure continued until the youth pated in the lunch preparation routine on a
completed three blocks of trials on the three daily basis since program entry. Prior to the
target objects in all four sets, yielding a total study, pretests were conducted to rule out ob-
of 36 trials per youth per session. ject labels that had been previously acquired via
MODIFIED INCIDENTAL TEACHING 33 :

ongoing teaching (e.g., wheat bread, apple, with lunch preparation activities, such as making
chocolate chip cookie). a sandwich or putting food in the lunch bag.
Teaching sessions began when the youth stood Training was terminated on one set when the
quietly by the kitchen counter and faced the data indicated acquisition as determined by no
teaching-parent, who suggested, "We're going overlapping data points and minimal perfor-
to make lunch now. O.K.?" Each of the four mance level of 70% correct.
sets of objects was introduced with an inquiry
related to readiness to prepare the objects for Generalization Assessment Procedure
school lunches (e.g., "Are you ready to make Throughout baseline and teaching conditions,
sandwiches?" for Set 1; "Are you ready to pre- daily 30-min, 1:1 discrete-trial sessions were
pare vegetables/snacks?" for Sets 2 and 3, re- held in the dining room to permit assessment of
spectively; and "'Are you ready to put lunches generalization across settings. These generaliza-
together for school?" for Set 4). When the child tion sessions were separated from teaching ses-
responded affirmatively, looked at the teaching- sions by at least one intervening activity, and
parent and ceased any off-task or self-stimulatory were conducted by the same teaching-parent
behavior, the teaching procedure was initated. who assisted the youth with lunch preparation.
Incidental teaching consisted of the teaching- Teaching-parent and child were seated at per-
parent making a request for the youth to "Give pendicular corners of the dining room table, and
me ." The youth's task was to select from an the same four sets of stimulus objects used dur-
array of three target objects plus two distractor ing lunch preparation were displayed on the
objects that were displayed in random order on dining room table in randomized arrangements.
the kitchen counter. The teaching-parent made Thirty-six trials (9 trials for each set) were again
eye contact with the youth during and following presented, and the youth were again requested to
each request, simultaneously extending her hand "Give me ." The teaching-parent maintained
toward the youth. If the youth was unable to eye contact with the youth during and following
select the correct object, a gestural prompt was each request, extending her hand as the request
delivered (i.e., the teaching-parent repeated the was made. Both correct and incorrect selections
request and simultaneously pointed to the ob- were received by the teaching-parent, who then
ject). After each trial, the teaching-parent took replaced the object in the set. Token reinforce-
the object selected by the youth and returned it ment and praise were provided for attending be-
to the counter. Three lunches were prepared, so havior. No prompts, tokens, praise, or feedback
that every target object could be requested three were delivered for either correct or incorrect
times, in random order, for each session. Tokens object identifications.
and behavior-specific praise statements were de-
livered contingent on cooperative participation Experimental Design
in lunch preparation; however, tokens were not Receptive-labeling skills for four sets of ob-
delivered for correct object identifications. In jects were assessed in baseline and teaching con-
order to control for the frequency of verbal ditions during school lunch preparation, and
stimuli presented, object labels were used in generalization to a different time and setting
one praise statement following each correct ob- was assessed throughout experimental condi-
ject identification (either prompted or un- tions. For both youth, teaching was begun on Set
prompted) during the lunch preparation session 1 after five baseline sessions in the 1: 1 lunch
(e.g., "Good, you gave me the "). Completing preparation activity; baseline conditions were
the receptive-labeling tasks for a set of objects identical to teaching conditions, except that ges-
enabled the youth to use those objects to proceed tural prompts and praise for correct object iden-
334 GAIL G. McGEE et al.
tifications were not delivered, and access to the tion sessions, mean percent correct responding
lunch preparation routine was provided noncon- again increased as a function of incidental teach-
tingently after every third trial. Baseline assess- ing during lunch preparation, with baseline to
ment continued on the sets not targeted for teaching changes of 13% to 60% for Set l,
teaching until the data indicated acquisition on 26% to 65% for Set 2, 18% to 53% for Set 3,
the first set, at which time the youth began and 25% to 74% for Set 4.
receiving teaching on Set 2, and so on. In sum- Almost all incorrect responses were errors of
mary, a multiple-baseline design across sets of commission; the only exceptions were for Youth
objects was used, with replication across a 2, who on two trials responded to the cue "Give
second child. me" after the 5-sec limit for correct responding.
Although errors were random across items on
most sets, confusion between two target items
RESULTS predominated on Set 1 for Youth 1 and on Set 2
Figure 1 shows the effects of incidental teach- for both youth. Selections of distractor items
ing on the acquisition and generalization of re- decreased as training proceeded, but distractors
ceptive object labels. For Youth 1 a mean of 4% continued to remain functional (i.e., they repre-
of correct, unprompted responses for objects in sented a portion of errors) throughout the study.
Set 1 occurred during baseline in the lunch Although acquisition of receptive object la-
preparation activity; following the initiation of bels generalized across settings and times of day,
incidental-teaching procedures, a mean of 72% generalization across sets of objects did not occur.
correct, unprompted responses was achieved. Furthermore, introduction of incidental teaching
Correct responses during lunch preparation for of new sets of objects did not produce retroactive
Sets 2, 3, and 4 also increased as a function of interference in previously acquired responses.
teaching. Changes in mean percent correct from
baseline to teaching conditions were 29% to
DISCUSSION
70% for Set 2, 29% to 70% for Set 3, and
27% to 71% for Set 4. Similar increases oc- In one of the first systematic investigations
curred during generalization assessment sessions of incidental teaching with severely language-
as a function of the introduction of incidental delayed autistic youth, incidental-teaching pro-
teaching during the lunch preparation activity. cedures were adapted to teach receptive language
Mean percent correct responses during generali- skills to autistic youth who did not initiate inter-
zation sessions when baseline conditions were in actions with others. The modified procedure ap-
effect in the lunch preparation activity were 7 %, pears to yield benefits similar to standard inci-
19%, 28%, and 26% for Sets 1-4, respectively, dental-teaching procedures, in terms of facili-
with increases to means of 61%, 61%, 57%, tating relatively rapid acquisition and promoting
and 55 % for Sets 1-4 following the introduction generalization. Training in naturalistic settings
of teaching during lunch preparation. substantially increases the amount of instruction
These results were replicated across a second that can be provided to autistic children, since
youth; although slightly more sessions were re- language skills may be trained concurrently with
quired for acquisition, similar increases occurred other adaptive skills such as meal preparation,
from baseline to teaching conditions. For Youth leisure activities, or self-care skills. This economy
2 during lunch preparation, mean percent cor- of effort is particularly beneficial in group home
rect responding in baseline for Sets 1-4 was settings, since teaching-parents can teach lan-
11%, 37%, 10%, and 23%, respectively; guage skills while completing other necessary
means in teaching conditions for Sets 1-4 were home maintenance and child care responsibil-
69%, 77%, 68%, and 76%. During generaliza- ities.
MODIFIED INCIDENTAL TEACHING 335
o-4 LUNCH PREPARATION
YOUTH 1 o.....GENERALIZATION I

- BASELINE . TEACHING

' *. .* * *a....
co
n : or 1. :

CM

CU
I-
co

I 50
LU
0 CD
25
C:

0
LU
z UTH 2
IASELINE . TEACHING
C:)LUa- 1 00 IB P.....
75 .
p....V~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~.v .0

I 50
LU
co
25
s-a~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~90
lw -.Cy is

II
1 00 A
75 -: ... .
.
. ,..
N *..*..~~~~~~~..
I-
LU
co
50
l.e*0. -
. *. a
.*

25

1 00
U V~~~~~~~
CO
75
t 50
25
R * a e

0.0

10
7
It
5
LU
no 2

5
_ , *' . *0., .*PI
2 4 6 8 1-0 1-2 1'4 1,a 1i8 2-0 22 24 26 28 30

SESSIONS
Fig. 1. Percentage of correct, unprompted object identifications achieved by Youth 1 and Youth 2 on four
sets of objects presented during Lunch Preparation (solid line) and Generalization (dotted line).
336 GAIL G. McGEE et al.
Incidental teaching has typically been used to youth could not discriminate target or distractor
promote the language development of children objects. Careful observation of her behavior dur-
who spontaneously initiate verbal interactions ing the generalization session revealed that she
with adults. In this study, however, the youths' was glancing at the data sheets after each re-
newly developing expressive speech created po- sponse, suggesting that she was modifying her
tential measurement problems, since clarity of behavior on the basis of responses scored plus
enunciations and voice volume were highly or minus. This hypothesis was tested with items
variable. Thus, procedural modifications were unrelated to stimulus Sets 1-4 used in the study
made in order to permit incidental teaching of and, indeed, initially random responding was
receptive language to youth who did not initiate gradually replaced by acquisition trends across
verbal interactions. Revised procedures were randomly presented items. Thereafter, observers
similar to "standard" incidental-teaching proce- took care to prevent visual access to data sheets
dures in that language skills were taught in the and correct responding again decreased, with the
course of a natural home-living activity, and re- exception of her performance on Set 3 in the
ceptive-labeling responses were followed by ac- 15th session, which appeared to represent spon-
cess to activities and events (lunch preparation taneous recovery of the responses that were in-
and verbal praise) that would be expected to advertently taught in session 5. However, the
maintain the behavior in normalized home set- introduction of teaching procedures continued
tings. Although token reinforcement for par- to affect her performance positively by increasing
ticipation in the lunch preparation routine was and stabilizing correct responding. Other pro-
used to increase the salience of relevant environ- cedural considerations viewed as important in
mental cues, for these and many severely delayed preventing stimulus overselectivity are that the
autistic youth, token systems are also naturalistic teacher should not look at the objects when
in that they are part of these children's everyday making a request, the teacher should extend the
routines. The magnitude of effects demonstrates receiving hand toward the child rather than
that incidental-teaching procedures are amenable toward the target object, and the receiving hand
to extensions that accommodate the program- should be extended when the request for an
ming needs of severely delayed autistic children. object is delivered rather than for correct objects
When procedural revisions are accomplished in only. Furthermore, when randomly arranging
a fashion that approximates standard procedures, target and distractor objects, it is important that
so that youth become familiar with the process of target objects not always be the last items
incidental teaching, their future readiness to en- touched by the teacher, since some youth may
gage in self-initated incidental-teaching episodes interpret this as a prompt.
may be enhanced. The youth's high levels of cooperative behav-
As often occurs during highly structured, dis- ior and the virtual absence of disruptive be-
crete-trial teaching with autistic youth (Lovaas, haviors suggest that lunch preparation was an
Schreibman, Koegel, & Rehm, 1971; Wilhelm enjoyable activity. The only exception occurred
& Lovaas, 1976), overselectivity to extraneous for Youth 1, between sessions 20 and 21; on
stimulus cues may also occur during incidental three consecutive trials, the youth picked up ob-
teaching, unless teaching procedures are care- jects after receiving gestural prompts, and threw
fully controlled. For example, during the fifth them vigorously at the teaching-parent. Lunch
baseline session, Youth 1 displayed apparent preparation was discontinued on this day, which
acquisition, particularly on Set 3; this was ini- was consistent with Hart and Risley's (1982)
tially puzzling, in light of the fact that no feed- recommendation that incidental-teaching epi-
back was being provided and earlier pretesting sodes be discontinued if corrective feedback is
and baseline assessment had indicated that the required. The youth resumed cooperative par-
MODIFIED INCIDENTAL TEACHING 337

ticipation in the activity the next day and in sub- tion to a different setting, the possibility that the
sequent sessions, with no recurrence of disrup- teaching-parent had acquired stimulus control
tive behavior during the activity. Earlier findings over the autistic youth's correct responding sug-
that preschool children's use of noncontingently gests that it would also be informative for future
available play materials decreased when inci- studies to assess generalization across persons.
dental teaching was provided for other play ma- Rudimentary expressive langauge skills
terials (Hart & Risley, 1974) suggested that the should also be responsive to modified incidental-
incidental-teaching process itself may be a rein- teaching procedures, although experimental in-
forcing activity. These results bear particular vestigations of incidental teaching of expressive
significance in extensions to autistic youth, since speech would most likely require prior language
early in treatment they often display extremely training in discrete-trial or naturalistic training
limited activity preferences (McClannahan & sessions, in order to shape clearly articulated
Krantz, 1981). verbalizations. Additional research is also indi-
A sit-down, discrete-trial session held in a cated to determine whether modified incidental-
different location later in the day provided a teaching procedures produce benefits such as
stringent test of generalization of the effects of increasing autistic children's untrained spon-
the teaching procedure. The dining room repre- taneous speech. Anecdotal reports from teaching-
sented a different natural setting in which the parents and one of their associates suggested that
appearance of the targeted food objects was con- Youth 1 dramatically increased the frequency of
textual. The dining room was also a setting her spontaneous verbal requests during and fol-
where the youth regularly participated in sit- lowing participation in this study, particularly
down teaching sessions that called for receptive during family meals. Although assessment pro-
language (e.g., following multiple-step direc- cedures did not permit identification of the
tions). Prior to this study, although both youth variables contributing to this behavior change,
had received discrete-trial training in object iden- it is possible that participation in incidental-
tification sessions, Youth 1 made extremely slow teaching procedures produced changes in the
progress and Youth 2 demonstrated virtually no youth's responsiveness to environmental cues
acquisition of object labels. However, since the or in the teaching-parent's effectiveness in mak-
youth were able to respond accurately to dinner- ing use of contextually appropriate stimuli avail-
time requests to pass some of the same objects able in the immediate environment. Finally, it
that they failed to discriminate during highly would be useful to extend incidental-teaching
structured object-labeling sessions, it was hy- procedures to skills other than language, such as
pothesized that incidental teaching might pro- academic, self-help, or home-maintenance tasks.
vide a more powerful paradigm for teaching The special significance of this study is the
receptive-labeling skills. Data from this and development of procedures that produce gen-
previous investigations of incidental-teaching eralized language skills for autistic youth in the
procedures indicate that generalization may be course of their participation in naturalistic, fam-
facilitated as a result of teaching in the context ilylike activities. The effectiveness of such
of activities where naturally occurring events modified incidental teaching has important im-
maintain the behavior. Direct comparisons be- plications for maximizing the benefits of com-
tween naturalistic and highly structured training munity-based treatment for autistic children.
situations would clarify whether there are, in Thus, the provision of attractive and stimulating
fact differential procedural effects in promoting home-living environments may do more than
generalization. Further, although this investiga- improve the quality of living for higher func-
tion clearly demonstrated the effectiveness of tioning autistic youth, but may also provide a
the teaching procedures in producing generaliza- viable and efficient mechanism for accelerating
338 GAIL G. McGEE et al.
skill acquisition by severely delayed autistic Koegel, R. L., & Rincover, A. Treatment of psy-
children. chotic children in a classroom environment: I.
Learning in a large group. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 1974, 7, 45-59.
REFERENCES Lovaas, 0. I. The autistic child: Language develop-
ment through behavior modification. New York:
Guess, D., Sailor, W., & Baer, D. M. Functional Irvington Press, 1977.
speech and language training for the severely Lovaas, 0. 1., Schreibman, L., Koegel, R., & Rehm, R.
handicapped: Parts I and 11. Lawrence, Ks.: H & Selective responding by autistic children to multi-
H Enterprises, 1976. ple sensory input. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
Halle, J. W., Baer, D. M., & Spradlin, J. E. Teach- ogy, 1971, 77, 211-222.
ers' generalized use of delay as a stimulus control McClannahan, L. E., & Krantz, P. J. Program ac-
procedure to increase language use in handicapped countability systems as protection of the rights of
children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, autistic children and youth. In J. T. Hannah, M.
1981, 14, 389-409. B. Clark, & W. P. Christian (Eds.), Preservation
Hart, B. M., & Risley, T. R. Establishing use of de- of client rights: Systems for the treatment and
scriptive adjectives in the spontaneous speech of protection of clients receiving psychological and
disadvantaged preschool children. Journal of Ap- educational services. New York: Free Press, 1981.
plied Behavior Analysis, 1968, 1, 109-120. Rogers-Warren, A., & Warren, S. F. Mands for ver-
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. Using preschool materials balization: Facilitating the display of newly
to modify the language of disadvantaged children. trained language in children. Behavior Modifica-
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1974, 7, tion, 1980, 4, 361-382.
243-256. Wilhelm, H., & Lovaas, 0. I. Stimulus overselec-
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. Incidental teaching of lan- tivity: A common feature in autism and mental
guage in the preschool. Journal of Applied Behav- retardation. American Journal of Mental Defi-
ior Analysis, 1975, 8, 411-420. ciency, 1976, 81, 26-31.
Hart, B. M., & Risley, T. R. In vivo language inter-
vention: Unanticipated general effects. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 1980, 13, 407-432. Received August 4, 1982
Hart, B. M., & Risley, T. R. How to use incidental Final acceptance January 19, 1983
teaching for elaborating language. Lawrence, Ks.:
H & H Enterprises, 1982.

You might also like