You are on page 1of 10

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/7450441

Mental, Physical and Social Components in Leisure Activities Equally Contribute


to Decrease Dementia Risk

Article  in  Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders · February 2006


DOI: 10.1159/000089919 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

381 2,310

6 authors, including:

Anita Karp Stéphanie Paillard-Borg


Karolinska Institutet Karolinska Institutet
31 PUBLICATIONS   3,430 CITATIONS    7 PUBLICATIONS   1,665 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Hui-Xin Wang Merril Silverstein


Stockholm University Syracuse University
142 PUBLICATIONS   6,963 CITATIONS    108 PUBLICATIONS   8,298 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Design and development of molecular chaperone modulators for the treatment of neuro-degenerative disorders. View project

The MOPEAD Project: sEvolving models of patient engagement and access for earlier identification of Alzheimer’s disease View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Hui-Xin Wang on 05 November 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Original Research Article

Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2006;21:65–73 Accepted after revision: September 3, 2005
Published online: November 23, 2005
DOI: 10.1159/000089919

Mental, Physical and Social Components in


Leisure Activities Equally Contribute to Decrease
Dementia Risk

Anita Karp a Stephanie Paillard-Borg a Hui-Xin Wang a Merril Silverstein a, b


Bengt Winblad a Laura Fratiglioni a
a
Aging Research Center, Division of Geriatric Epidemiology, Department of Neurotec, Karolinska Institutet, and
the Stockholm Gerontology Research Center, Stockholm, Sweden; b Andrus Gerontology Center,
University of Southern California, Los Angeles, Calif., USA

Key Words subjects with high scores in all or in two of the compo-
Dementia risk  Leisure activities, mental, physical and nents (RR of dementia = 0.53; 95% CI: 0.36–0.78). Conclu-
social components  Elderly population sions: These findings suggest that a broad spectrum of
activities containing more than one of the components
seems to be more beneficial than to be engaged in only
Abstract one type of activity.
Background: There is accumulating evidence in the lit- Copyright © 2006 S. Karger AG, Basel

erature that leisure engagement has a beneficial effect


on dementia. Most studies have grouped activities ac-
cording to whether they were predominantly mental, Introduction
physical or social. Since many activities contain more
than one component, we aimed to verify the effect of all After retirement, leisure activities usually constitute a
three major components on the dementia risk, as well as relatively larger part of the daily life and may take on the
their combined effect. Methods: A mental, social and role of providing mental stimulation, social engagement
physical component score was estimated for each activ- and physical activity that was earlier offered by school or
ity by the researchers and a sample of elderly persons. employment. Consequently, leisure activities in old age
The correlation between the ratings of the authors and have been a common focus for studies regarding several
the means of the elderly subjects’ ratings was 0.86. The outcomes like mortality [1–3], cognition [4–8], morbidity
study population consisted of 776 nondemented sub- [9] and well-being [10–12].
jects, aged 75 years and above, living in Stockholm, Swe- In recent years, prospective studies have found an as-
den, who were still nondemented after 3 years and were sociation between leisure activities and decreased risk
followed for 3 more years to detect incident dementia of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and dementia [13]. It has
cases. Results: Multi-adjusted relative risks (RRs) of de- been suggested that engagement in leisure activities may
mentia for subjects with higher mental, physical and so- result in functionally more efficient cognitive networks,
cial component score sums were 0.71 (95% CI: 0.49–1.03), hereby providing a cognitive reserve that could delay the
0.61 (95% CI: 0.42–0.87) and 0.68 (95% CI: 0.47–0.99), re- onset of clinical manifestations of dementia [14]. Men-
spectively. The most beneficial effect was present for tally stimulating activities, in particular, have been as-

© 2006 S. Karger AG, Basel Anita Karp


1420–8008/06/0212–0065$23.50/0 ARC-Äldrecentrum
Fax +41 61 306 12 34 Box 6401 (Olivecronas väg 4)
E-Mail karger@karger.ch Accessible online at: SE–113 82 Stockholm (Sweden)
www.karger.com www.karger.com/dem Tel. +46 8 6906856, Fax +46 8 6906889, E-Mail anita.karp@neurotec.ki.se
sociated with significantly reduced risk of incident de- first follow-up (1991–1993) and second follow-up (1994–1996) ex-
mentia [15–19]. Being engaged in social recreational ac- aminations.
Of the 1,810 eligible participants who underwent the baseline
tivities [18, 20] and having a rich social network [21]
examination, 1,473 were diagnosed as nondemented. The detailed
have also been found to have a protective effect against procedures are available elsewhere [24, 25]. Since institutionaliza-
dementia. In addition, engaging in regular physical ac- tion or impaired cognition may limit subjects’ activity [4], 98 sub-
tivity has been suggested as an important component of jects whose Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores [26]
a preventive strategy against AD [22] and, in women, were less or equal to 23 or who were living in an institution were
also excluded from the present study; thus, 1,375 persons remained
against cognitive decline at 6–8 years of follow-up [23]. for analysis. By the first follow-up examination, 269 subjects had
However, other reports did not confirm such an associa- died, 172 refused participation and 934 participated, of whom 158
tion [16, 18]. were diagnosed as demented. Thus, the population for the present
Most studies of leisure activities and dementia have study was composed of those 776 subjects participating and still
grouped activities according to whether they were pre- nondemented at the first follow-up examination. Out of these, 732
subjects were followed for another 3 years (second follow-up) to
dominantly mental, physical or social. Indeed, activities detect incident dementia cases (44 subjects refused to participate
may simultaneously embody one or more of these three to the second follow-up).
aspects. As an example, dancing [19] is a physical activ-
ity that contains social interaction, as well as a certain Diagnosis of Dementia
degree of cognitive involvement, at least if the dance re- Incident dementia cases examined in this study were those
subjects who developed dementia during the second follow-up
quires learning new steps and turns. Hence, identifying period. At each follow-up examination, all cohort subjects were
the activities according to their predominant component clinically examined following a standardized protocol, which in-
may lead to an underestimation of other components. For cluded personal and family history collected by nurses, clinical
instance, several common leisure activities classified as examination conducted by physicians and psychological tests ad-
social or mental in nature also contain light physical ac- ministered by trained personnel. If a participant was not able to
answer, an informant, usually the subject’s next of kin, was inter-
tivity which, although modest, may be of great impor- viewed. The study design has been described in detail elsewhere
tance for the health of elderly people. [24, 25]. Time of dementia onset was assumed to be the midpoint
For these reasons, the present study considers activi- between the first and second follow-up examinations or the time
ties as having a multidimensional profile, each manifest- of death.
ing various combinations of mental, social and physical Dementia diagnoses were made according to the Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Third Edition-Revised cri-
involvement. Using data from subjects in the Kungshol- teria [26] following the same three-step procedure used at baseline
men Project, we hypothesize that all three components examination [24]. First, a preliminary diagnosis was made by the
are relevant protective factors against dementia, and that examining physician. Second, all cases were independently re-
a combined beneficial effect may be present. Because re- viewed by a specialized clinician and a second diagnosis was made.
duced participation in leisure activities may very well be If the diagnoses were in agreement, they were accepted as the final
decision. In case of disagreement, a third opinion was obtained and
a consequence and not a cause of cognitive decline in the the concordant diagnosis was accepted. When only one item of the
preclinical phase of dementia [16, 18, 19], the present diagnostic criteria was not fulfilled, the subject was classified as af-
study lags predictors relative to dementia diagnosis by an fected by ‘questionable dementia’, which is in contrast with ‘clini-
average of 6 years through excluding subjects diagnosed cally definite dementia’. Both groups were included in the analyses.
as demented at first follow-up. Furthermore, we excluded For those participants (n = 172) who had died between the first and
second follow-up examinations, a preliminary diagnosis was made
subjects with low baseline cognition, and adjusted for by a physician through consulting medical records and death cer-
cognitive function at baseline. tificates, and then reviewed by a senior clinician. When only dis-
charge diagnoses from hospitals or death certificates were available,
the reported diagnosis was accepted.

Materials and Methods Assessment of Activity


Information on leisure activities was obtained from the subjects
Study Population by means of a personal interview carried out by trained nurses at
Subjects were derived from the Kungsholmen Project, a longi- baseline (1987–1989) with open questions. Subjects were asked:
tudinal population-based study of aging and dementia. All resi- whether they regularly engaged in any particular activities, the type
dents of the Kungsholmen district of Stockholm, Sweden, aged 75 of activities and the frequency of participation. The leisure activi-
years or older in October 1987 (n = 2,368), were asked to participate ties were grouped into 29 main types of activities. A mental, social
in the initial examination. Subjects who agreed and signed an in- and physical component score was assigned to each of the 29 ac-
formed consent were interviewed by nurses, examined by physi- tivities. Two authors of this study (A.K. and S.P.-B.) independent-
cians, and tested by psychologists during the baseline (1987–1989), ly assigned scores to each activity based on their own evaluations

66 Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2006;21:65–73 Karp /Paillard-Borg /Wang /Silverstein /


Winblad /Fratiglioni
Table 1. List of activities, scorings (0 = none; 1 = low; 2 = moderate; 3 = high) and frequency of participation

Type of activity Scores Subjects


mental physical social n %

Reading literature 3 0 0 152 20.8


Handicraft: needlework, weaving, knitting 2 1 1 146 19.9
Doing crossword puzzles 3 0 0 94 12.8
Political or cultural interests 3 1 3 75 10.2
Playing cards or chess 3 0 3 71 9.7
Visiting the summerhouse 2 2 2 70 9.6
Attending courses 3 1 2 59 8.1
Watching TV 2 0 0 55 7.5
Going to theatres or concerts 3 1 2 55 7.5
Doing sport 1 3 2 49 6.7
Going to exhibitions or museums 3 1 1 47 6.4
Meeting friends, participating in groups 2 1 3 47 6.4
Walking 1 3 1 37 5.1
Listening to radio 2 0 0 33 4.5
Travelling 3 2 2 29 4.0
Gardening and flowers 2 2 1 29 4.0
Painting, drawing, photo 3 0 0 26 3.6
Engaging in family or charity 2 2 3 23 3.1
Doing outdoor activities 2 3 2 23 3.1
Collecting stamps or other items 3 0 0 18 2.5
Cooking food 2 1 1 17 2.3
Writing 3 0 1 16 2.2
Housekeeping 1 2 0 15 2.0
Attending church activities 2 1 3 15 2.0
Playing music 2 1 2 14 1.9
Doing solitaire 2 0 0 9 1.2
Following the stock market 3 0 0 8 1.1
Playing bingo 2 1 3 7 1.0
Singing 2 1 2 7 1.0
No activity at all 0 0 0 153 20.9

and then discussed with a third author (L.F.) in order to reach con- The three components were highly correlated. The Pearson cor-
sensus. The grading of the three components was coded as: 0 = none, relation coefficients were 0.76 between the mental and social com-
1 = low, 2 = moderate, 3 = high (table 1). ponent score sums, 0.77 between the physical and social and 0.58
To validate the scoring, 13 cognitively intact, elderly raters (7 between the physical and mental component score sums.
men, 6 women), aged 75 years or more, but not participants in the
Kungsholmen study, were asked to individually fill in a small ques- Covariates
tionnaire containing a list of all 29 activities together with scoring We used all covariates with available baseline information from
instructions. The overall correlation between the consensus ratings our database that may have acted as possible confounders: age, sex,
of the authors and the means of the elderly subjects’ ratings was education, cognitive functioning, comorbidity, depressive symp-
0.86. Reliability analyses were performed. For all three components toms and physical function at baseline examination. Information
taken together, Cronbach’s  was high; 0.90 indicating high inter- about age and sex was derived from the National Population Reg-
rater consistency. Cronbach’s  values for each of the components ister. Information about highest level of formal education was col-
were: 0.89 for the mental component, 0.95 for the physical compo- lected through personal interview. Cognitive function was mea-
nent and 0.82 for the social component. Since the disagreements sured by using MMSE [27] since there is a global impairment of
between researchers and elderly raters mainly concerned the social cognitive functioning in the preclinical phase of AD [28]. Depres-
component of activities, four final ratings (handicraft, travels, sing- sive symptoms were assessed by two self-reported symptoms: being
ing and collections) were changed to better fit the elderly raters’ in a low mood and/or often feeling lonely. Both of these symptoms
opinion. Table 1 shows the final decision regarding the component are included in the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression
scores for the 29 activities and the frequency of each activity. Scale [29]. Physical function was defined as disability in at least

Mental, Physical and Social Components Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2006;21:65–73 67
in Leisure Activities and Dementia Risk
one of the basic activities of daily living: bathing, dressing, toilet- Table 2. Baseline characteristics of the study population (n = 732)
ing, continence, feeding or transfer [30]. and of the incident dementia cases detected at second follow-up
Data of previous diseases were obtained by reviewing hospital (1994–1996)
discharge diagnoses through the Stockholm Computerized Inpa-
tient Register System. These diagnoses were made according to Characteristic Participants Incident dementia
the International Classification of Disease, 8th edition [31]: coro- (n = 732) cases (n = 123)
nary heart disease (ICD-8: 410–414), cerebrovascular disease
(ICD-8: 430–438), diabetes mellitus (ICD-8: 250), malignancy n n %
(ICD-8: 140–208 and 230–239) and hip fracture (ICD-8: 820).
Comorbidity was defined as subjects who had any of these five Age groups
diseases. 75–79 364 36 9.9
80–84 232 55 23.7
Statistical Analyses 85+ 136 32 23.5
To evaluate the differences in baseline characteristics between Sex
participants and drop-outs, logistic regression was used. Cox pro- Female 543 93 17.1
portional hazards regressions were performed to estimate the rela- Male 189 30 15.9
tive risk (RR) of incident AD and dementia associated with mental,
physical and social components in the different leisure activities. Education1
The mental, physical and social components were analysed in ^7 years 386 70 18.1
the following ways: 68 years 343 53 15.5
(1) We first intended to tap the intensity of involvement in each MMSE score
component by using a threshold to signify moderate/high involve- 24–26 178 38 21.3
ment: for each person and each component (mental, physical and 27–30 554 85 15.3
social), the number of moderately/highly (2–3) scored activities
were summed up separately and then divided into three categories. Comorbidity1
The analyses were performed for each component, contrasting sub- Yes 179 46 25.7
jects with one, and subjects with two or more moderately/highly No 550 77 14.0
scored activities with subjects without any moderately/highly
Depressive symptom1
scored activity.
Yes 201 53 26.4
(2) In order to measure the accumulation of each component
No 528 70 13.3
across the range of activities, the rated scores were added to a sum
of scores for each person and each component. This sum score was Physical dependence2
analysed using (a) the continuous variable, (b) four groups with a Yes 132 30 22.7
similar number of cases and (c) two groups dichotomized according No 592 93 15.7
to the median value.
(3) To capture the combined effect of the three components in Number of activities
reducing the dementia risk, combinations of the mental, physical 0 153 37 24.2
and social components were created using the sum score for each 1–2 400 67 16.8
component after dichotomization according to the median value. 3–7 179 19 10.6
All of the associations studied were first assessed using simple 1
models including age and sex, and then adjustments for all covari- For 3 subjects, information was missing.
2
ates described above were carried out. Age and MMSE score were For 8 subjects, information was missing.
entered into the models as continuous variables. Education was
entered as a categorical variable (112 years, 8–12 years and
!8 years of schooling). Sex (female vs. male), comorbidity (the
presence of coronary heart disease, cerebrovascular disease, dia-
betes mellitus, malignancy or hip fracture vs. the presence of none
of them), depressive symptoms (yes vs. no) and physical function- sex, education, cognitive functioning, depressive symp-
ing (dependent vs. independent) were entered as dichotomous toms and physical functioning.
variables. A total of 123 subjects developed clinical dementia
during the second follow-up. Table 2 shows the baseline
characteristics of the study population and incident de-
Results mentia cases.
The multi-adjusted RRs of dementia associated with
The baseline study population consisted of 776 sub- participation in 1–2 activities, and participation in
jects, but 44 persons refused second follow-up examina- 3–7 activities, versus no participation were 0.77 (95%
tion. While non-participants had less comorbidity than CI: 0.51–1.15) and 0.56 (95% CI: 0.31–0.99), respec-
participants, there were no differences with respect to age, tively.

68 Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2006;21:65–73 Karp /Paillard-Borg /Wang /Silverstein /


Winblad /Fratiglioni
Table 3. RRs of dementia in relation to degree of estimated mental, physical and social component score sums
in baseline leisure activities

n Dementia RR (95% CI)1 RR (95% CI)2


cases

Mental component score sum


0 153 37 1.0 1.0
1–3 207 38 0.75 (0.47–1.18) 0.82 (0.52–1.31)
4–7 251 34 0.61 (0.38–0.98) 0.67 (0.42–1.08)
8 or more 121 14 0.49 (0.26–0.93) 0.56 (0.29–1.07)
Higher score (0–3 vs. 4–18) 372 48 0.67 (0.46–0.98) 0.71 (0.49–1.03)
Physical component score sum
0 285 67 1.0 1.0
1 165 22 0.55 (0.34–0.89) 0.58 (0.36–0.95)
2–3 182 24 0.61 (0.38–0.99) 0.64 (0.40–1.04)
4 or more 100 10 0.51 (0.26–0.99) 0.59 (0.30–1.16)
Higher score (0 vs. 1–12) 447 56 0.57 (0.40–0.81) 0.61 (0.42–0.87)
Social component score sum
0 261 62 1.0 1.0
1 103 13 0.49 (0.27–0.90) 0.53 (0.29–0.97)
2–3 179 23 0.48 (0.29–0.78) 0.51 (0.31–0.82)
4 or more 189 25 0.61 (0.38–0.98) 0.69 (0.43–1.13)
Higher score (0–1 vs. 2–13) 368 48 0.64 (0.44–0.93) 0.68 (0.47–0.99)

For each component (mental, physical and social) two Cox regression models were performed: one analysing
the component score sum as a 4-category indicator variable, and the second as a dichotomous variable.
1
Adjusted for age, sex, education, baseline MMSE score, comorbidity and physical functioning.
2
Adjusted for age, sex, education, baseline MMSE score, comorbidity, physical functioning and depressive
symptoms.

To examine if moderate/high-scored activities were 18, the physical score sums from 0 to 12 and the social
most beneficial, the number of moderately/highly scored score sums from 0 to 13. The mental component differed
activities were added up and then divided into three cat- from the physical and social components in that it had no
egories. In comparison to subjects with none or low (0–1) subjects with a 0 score, aside from the group of subjects
involvement in the mental component, persons who had who participated in no activities at all. All of the three
one moderately/highly (2–3) rated activity had a multi- components’ score sum distributions were positively
adjusted RR of 0.90 (95% CI: 0.57–1.41), and persons skewed.
with two or more moderately/highly rated activities had When first analysed as continuous variables, multi-ad-
an RR of 0.67 (95% CI: 0.42–1.05). Regarding the phys- justed RRs indicated that greater score sums in the men-
ical component, the multi-adjusted RR of dementia was tal component (RR = 0.94, 95% CI: 0.87–1.00) and phys-
0.87 (95% CI: 0.55–1.39) for subjects engaged in one ical component (RR = 0.89, 95% CI: 0.77–0.99) appeared
moderately/highly rated activity and 0.44 (95% CI: 0.14– to lower the risk of dementia. Higher score sums in the
1.39) for being engaged in two or more moderately/high- social component were not significantly associated with
ly rated activities. For the social component, the multi- lower risk of dementia when analysed continuously
adjusted RR associated with engagement in one moder- (RR = 0.95, 95% CI: 0.87–1.04).
ately/highly rated activity was 0.75 (95% CI: 0.48–1.18) All of the components’ score sum distributions were
and the RR associated with two or more moderately/high- then categorized into approximate quartiles (table 3).
ly rated activities was 0.94 (95% CI: 0.55–1.61). Having mental component score sums above 3 was sig-
Next, the total scores of each of the three components nificantly associated with a decreased risk of dementia,
were examined. The mental score sums ranged from 0 to when controlled for age, sex, education, baseline MMSE

Mental, Physical and Social Components Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2006;21:65–73 69
in Leisure Activities and Dementia Risk
Table 4. RRs of dementia associated with combinations of higher or lower mental, physical and social score
sums

Subjects Cases RR (95% CI)1 RR (95% CI)2

Lower score in all of the mental,


physical and social components 228 57 1.0 1.0
Higher score in one of the mental,
physical or social components 92 15 0.63 (0.36–1.12) 0.69 (0.39–1.22)
Higher score in two of the mental,
physical or social components 141 16 0.37 (0.21–0.65) 0.39 (0.22–0.69)
Higher score in all of the mental,
physical and social components 271 35 0.57 (0.37–0.89) 0.63 (0.41–0.98)
1
Adjusted for age, sex, education, baseline MMSE score, comorbidity and physical functioning.
2
Adjusted for age, sex, education, baseline MMSE score, comorbidity, physical functioning and depressive
symptoms.

score, comorbidity and physical functioning. When the only two of the three components (mental and physical;
depressive symptoms (feeling alone and in a low mood) physical and social) were also significantly protected
were added to the model, the association did not reach against dementia. Due to the small number of cases in
the 0.05 level of significance. The mental component some of the combinations, subjects were merged into four
score sum, categorized in four grades, showed a signifi- categories: (1) low in all three components, (2) one high,
cant trend (p = 0.043) in relation to dementia risk. Having (3) two high, or (4) three high. The results are shown in
a physical and social component score larger than or table 4. Having high scores in two or three of the compo-
equal to 1 was significantly related to a lower risk of de- nents was associated with a significant reduction in risk
mentia after adjustments for all covariates. There was no of dementia. When those having higher scores on two or
trend with regard to the four score sum categories for the more of the components were combined (category 3 and
physical or the social components. The basic adjusted 4 together), the RR ratio fell to almost half (RR = 0.53,
(only age and sex adjustments) results were similar. 95% CI: 0.36–0.78).
Table 3 also shows the RRs of dementia in relation to The following additional analyses were performed to
the sum of scores of mental, physical and social compo- verify the results: although we had excluded subjects with
nents using the approximate medians for dichotomiza- MMSE scores ^23, scores above that level could still in-
tion. The median for the mental component score was 4, fluence the studied relation. We therefore performed ad-
for the physical component 1 and for the social compo- ditional analyses for subjects whose MMSE scores were
nent 2. When analysed one by one, all three components more than 26 at baseline examination. The results were
were protective against dementia, although the mental largely identical for the mental and physical component
score was not entirely significant when adjusted for all score sums and showed a similar tendency for the social
covariates. component score sum. Finally, since there were quite a
We further combined the dichotomized score sums of few subjects (n = 153) who did not report any activity at
the three components into eight groups, with the purpose all and might have been qualitatively different from the
of investigating if some specific activity combinations other combinations, additional analyses were performed
were more beneficial. The dichotomizations were based treating these subjects as a separate category. The results
on the median values described above. These results (not and overall pattern did not change, and the ‘no activity
shown) indicated that having high overall scores on all group’ had similar RRs as the group who had a lower
three components was associated with significantly lower score in all three components. We also performed extra
risks of dementia when all covariates were taken into ac- analyses excluding the subjects who did not report any
count. In two combinations, subjects who scored high on activities. Similar results were observed.

70 Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2006;21:65–73 Karp /Paillard-Borg /Wang /Silverstein /


Winblad /Fratiglioni
Discussion Mentally stimulating activities may buffer or delay path-
ological development. Persistent engagement in effortful
This study shows that participation in leisure activities mental activities may even promote plastic changes in the
is associated with reduced risk of dementia and that each brain that can circumvent the underlying dementia pa-
of the mental, physical and social components is of im- thology [33]. Physical activity may increase cerebral oxy-
portance. When an intensity threshold was used, compar- genation, leading to improved neurotransmitter metabo-
ing the number of activities that rated moderate to high lism. It also reduces the risk of diseases that may be risk
on the components versus no or low-rated activities, the factors for dementia such as hypertension, diabetes and
results were not statistically significant. However, when cardiovascular disease [8]. Predominantly social leisure
the subjects’ score sums for each component were taken activities may affect the immune system [34], which in
into account, the RRs of dementia were significantly de- turn could influence inflammatory processes in the brain,
creased, showing that even small contributions of the which may be involved in dementia.
mental, physical or social components mattered when ac- When interpreting these findings, one needs to take
cumulated across several activities. This was especially into account some considerations. First, open-ended
relevant for the physical component, since only few el- questions were used to obtain information of engagement
derly had two or more activities with moderate to high in leisure activities. Although these types of questions can
intensity. be of great value because they capture a wide variety of
The most beneficial effect was, however, present for possible and sometimes unexpected answers [35], there
subjects with high scores in all or in two of the compo- is always the risk that those who did not report any ac-
nents. In the latter group, nearly half of the subjects had tivities were subjects who differed systematically in some
reported only one activity. Examples of these single ac- important respect. We performed analyses excluding the
tivities scoring high in more than one component were: subjects who did not report any activities at all (as well as
being engaged in political and cultural activities, visiting treating them as a separate group), and the results showed
the summerhouse, attending courses, going to the theatre a similar tendency as compared to the whole group. Fur-
or concerts, being active in sport or outdoor activities, thermore, we could not determine if any changes in lei-
travelling, being engaged in charity or church activities sure engagement had occurred prior to baseline. Findings
or playing music together with others. from cross-sectional studies indicate that leisure engage-
Although other researchers [8, 19] have acknowledged ment may decrease with age; however, longitudinal, ret-
the fact that most leisure activities consist of several over- rospective studies have reported continuity in overall en-
lapping components, nobody, to our knowledge, has tried gagement [36].
to separate the different components from each other. Secondly, we do not have access to any ‘objective’ mea-
This study used a new approach, where three of the au- surements of the three components, although our assess-
thors (a neurologist, a psychologist and a sociologist), and ment method had good interrater reliability. It is also
13 elderly raters scored the mental, physical and social difficult to compare the importance of each component
components in each of the 29 common leisure activities isolated from the others, since they often occur simulta-
reported by the participants. In addition, participation in neously in the same activities. The fact that each activity
the leisure activities was assessed on average 6 years be- was used to rate all three components meant that the com-
fore dementia diagnosis in an attempt to reduce the influ- ponents were not independent, and therefore it was prob-
ence of preclinical dementia. None of the covariates (age, lematic to definitively adjust for the influence of one com-
sex, education, baseline MMSE score, comorbidity and ponent on the other. Still, we believe that there is an ad-
physical function) could explain the reported associa- vantage in this method reducing all the 29 activities to
tions. ‘Depressive symptoms’ was the only control vari- three main components, instead of merely grouping the
able that marginally changed the RRs of dementia – de- activities in an exclusive manner, not allowing overlap.
spite the crude estimation of depressive symptomatology In spite of the imprecision of the assessments and catego-
in the present study. rizations, we were able to detect risk differences.
There are a number of different hypotheses concerning Thirdly, there is always the possibility that cognitive
the possible effect of each of the mental, physical and so- disturbances in the preclinical stages of dementia might
cial dimensions of common leisure activities on dementia affect the subject’s choice of and engagement in leisure
development. Activation of the nerve cells might prolong activities in general and in activities with a high degree
their optimal function throughout the life-span [32]. of mental components in particular. Although initial cog-

Mental, Physical and Social Components Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2006;21:65–73 71
in Leisure Activities and Dementia Risk
nitive functioning was adjusted for, and activity data This study confirms earlier results stating that engage-
were collected on average 6 years before dementia diag- ment in leisure activities with mental, social and physical
nosis and a minimum of 3 years before dementia onset, content in late life is associated with decreased risk of
there still exists an uncertainty about how early in the dementia. Furthermore, it was found that small contribu-
preclinical phase of dementia relevant cognitive deficits tions of the mental, physical or social components mat-
appear. It is therefore premature to interpret these results tered when accumulated. Because few elderly engaged in
as causal or even as delaying the disease onset, since less- more vigorous exercise, the benefit of light physical com-
er leisure engagement may be a consequence of early ponents in activities that are not primarily physical is
symptoms. However, a prospective study as extended as especially noteworthy.
21 years has recently reported results similar to ours [19]. The results also indicate that engaging in activities that
Furthermore, Bennett et al. [37] investigated post-mor- cover more than one of the mental, physical and social
tem data in the Religious Order Study and found that the components seems to be more beneficial than to be en-
relation between senile plaques and level of cognitive gaged in only one type of activity. Implications for public
function differed by level of education, suggesting that health and community may very well be that making dif-
differences in lifestyle may affect cognitive reserve by ferent types of activities more accessible to elderly per-
partially mediating the relationship between brain dam- sons could reduce the risk of developing dementia.
age and the clinical manifestation of AD. Additionally,
in the MRC National Survey of Health and Develop-
ment, Richards et al. [8] had the opportunity to control Acknowledgements
for the influence of IQ at 15 years of age, when studying
We thank all members of the Kungsholmen Project study group
active leisure and cognition. This research group drew the
for their cooperation in data collection and management. This re-
conclusion that it was unlikely that the finding of an as- search was supported by a grant from the Swedish Council for
sociation between active leisure and cognition was a re- Working Life and Social Research (No. 2003-0386) and the Amer-
flection of reverse causality. ican Alzheimer foundation.

References

1 Glass TA, de Leon CM, Marotti RA, Berkman 7 Laurin D, Verreault R, Lindsay J, MacPherson 15 Scarmeas N, Levy G, Tang MX, Manly J, Stern
LF: Population-based study of social and pro- K, Rockwood K: Physical activity and risk of Y: Influence of leisure activity on the incidence
ductive activities as predictors of survival cognitive impairment and dementia in elderly of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 2001; 57:
among elderly Americans. BMJ 1999; 319: persons. Arch Neurol 2001;58:498–504. 2236–2242.
478–483. 8 Richards M, Hardy R, Wadsworth ME: Does 16 Wilson RS, Mendes de Leon CF, Barnes LL, et
2 Musick MA, Herzog AR, House JS: Volunteer- active leisure protect cognition? Evidence from al: Participation in cognitively stimulating ac-
ing and mortality among older adults: findings a national birth cohort. Soc Sci Med 2003;56: tivities and risk of incident Alzheimer disease.
from a national sample. J Gerontol B Psychol 785–792. JAMA 2002;287:742–748.
Sci Soc Sci 1999;54:S173–S180. 9 Feskanich D, Willett W, Colditz G: Walking 17 Wilson RS, Bennett DA, Bienias JL, et al: Cog-
3 Lennartsson C, Silverstein M: Does engage- and leisure-time activity and risk of hip frac- nitive activity and incident AD in a popula-
ment with life enhance survival of elderly peo- ture in postmenopausal women. JAMA 2002; tion-based sample of older persons. Neurology
ple in Sweden? The role of social and leisure 288:300–306. 2002;59:1910–1914.
activities. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 10 Baltes MM, Carstensen LL: The process of suc- 18 Wang HX, Karp A, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L:
2001;25:S335–S342. cessful aging. Ageing Soc 1996;16:397–422. Late-life engagement in social and leisure ac-
4 Hultsch DF, Hammer M, Small BJ: Age differ- 11 McAuley E, Blissmer B, Marquez DX, Jerome tivities is associated with a decreased risk of
ences in cognitive performance in later life: re- GJ, Kramer AF, Katula J: Social relations, dementia: a longitudinal study from the
lationships to self-reported health and activity physical activity, and well-being in older Kungsholmen Project. Am J Epidemiol 2002;
life style. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci adults. Prev Med 2000;31:608–617. 155:1081–1087.
1993;48:P1–P11. 12 Silverstein M, Parker MG: Leisure activities 19 Verghese J, Lipton RB, Katz MJ, et al: Leisure
5 Hill RD, Wahlin A, Winblad B, Backman L: and quality of life among the oldest old in Swe- activities and the risk of dementia in the el-
The role of demographic and life style variables den. Res Aging 2002;24:528–547. derly. N Engl J Med 2003;348:2508–2516.
in utilizing cognitive support for episodic re- 13 Fratiglioni L, Paillard-Borg S, Winblad B: An 20 Fabrigoule C, Letenneur L, Dartigues JF, Zar-
membering among very old adults. J Gerontol active and socially integrated lifestyle in late rouk M, Commenges D, Barberger-Gateau P:
B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 1995;50:P219–P227. life might protect against dementia. Lancet Social and leisure activities and risk of demen-
6 Hultsch DF, Hertzog C, Small BJ, Dixon RA: Neurol 2004;3:343–353. tia: a prospective longitudinal study. J Am
Use it or lose it: engaged lifestyle as a buffer of 14 Scarmeas N, Stern Y: Cognitive reserve and Geriatr Soc 1995;43:485–490.
cognitive decline in aging? Psychol Aging lifestyle. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2003; 25:
1999;14:245–263. 625–633.

72 Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2006;21:65–73 Karp /Paillard-Borg /Wang /Silverstein /


Winblad /Fratiglioni
21 Fratiglioni L, Wang HX, Ericsson K, Maytan 26 American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic 32 Swaab DF: Brain aging and Alzheimer’s dis-
M, Winblad B: Influence of social network on and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, ed ease, ‘wear and tear’ versus ‘use it or lose it’.
occurrence of dementia: a community-based 3 Revised. Washington, American Psychiatric Neurobiol Aging 1991;12:317–324.
longitudinal study. Lancet 2000; 355: 1315– Association, 1987. 33 Coyle JT: Use it or lose it – Do effortful mental
1319. 27 Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: ‘Mini- activities protect against dementia? N Engl J
22 Lindsay J, Laurin D, Verreault R, et al: Risk Mental State’: a practical method for grading Med 2003;348:2489–2490.
factors for Alzheimer’s disease: a prospective the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. 34 Seeman TE: Social ties and health: the benefits
analysis from the Canadian Study of Health J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189–198. of social integration. Ann Epidemiol 1996; 6:
and Aging. Am J Epidemiol 2002; 156: 445– 28 Backman L, Jones S, Berger AK, Laukka EJ, 442–451.
453. Small BJ: Cognitive impairment in preclinical 35 Kerlinger FN: Foundations of Behavioral Re-
23 Yaffe K, Barnes D, Nevitt M, Lui LY, Covin- Alzheimer’s disease: a meta-analysis. Neuro- search, ed 2. New York, Holt, Rinehart and
sky K: A prospective study of physical activity psychology 2005;19:520–531. Winston, 1981, p 484.
and cognitive decline in elderly women: wom- 29 Radloff LS, Teri L: Use of the Center for Epi- 36 Cutler J, Hendricks J: Leisure and time use
en who walk. Arch Intern Med 2001; 161: demiological Studies Depression Scale with across the life course; in Binstock RH, George
1703–1708. older adults. Clin Gerontol 1986;5:119–136. LK (eds): Handbook of Aging and the Social
24 Fratiglioni L, Grut M, Forsell Y, et al: Preva- 30 Katz S, Ford AB, Moskowitz RW, Jackson BA, Sciences, ed 3. New York, Academic Press,
lence of Alzheimer’s disease and other demen- Jaffe MW: The index of ADL: a standardized 1990, pp 169–185.
tias in an elderly urban population: relation- measure of biological and psychosocial func- 37 Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Schneider JA, Evans
ship with age, sex, and education. Neurology tion. JAMA 1963;185:914–919. DA, Mendes de Leon CF, Arnold SE, Barnes
1991;41:1886–1892. 31 World Health Organization: International LL, Bienias JL: Education modifies the rela-
25 Fratiglioni L, Viitanen M, von Strauss E, Ton- Classification of Diseases. Manual of the Inter- tion of AD pathology to level of cognitive func-
todonati V, Herlitz A, Winblad B: Very old national Statistical Classification of Diseases, tion in older persons. Neurology 2003; 60:
women at highest risk of dementia and Alz- Injuries, and Causes of Death, ed 8. Geneva, 1909–1915.
heimer’s disease: incidence data from the World Health Organization, 1967, vol 1.
Kungsholmen Project, Stockholm. Neurology
1997;48:132–138.

Mental, Physical and Social Components Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2006;21:65–73 73
in Leisure Activities and Dementia Risk

View publication stats

You might also like