You are on page 1of 16

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32 (2008) 1087–1102

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neubiorev

Review

Behavioural and neurochemical effects of post-weaning social isolation in


rodents—Relevance to developmental neuropsychiatric disorders
Kevin C.F. Fone *, M. Veronica Porkess
Institute of Neuroscience, School of Biomedical Sciences, Medical School, Queen’s Medical Centre, University of Nottingham, Nottingham NG7 2UH, UK

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Exposing mammals to early-life adverse events, including maternal separation or social isolation,
Social isolation profoundly affects brain development and adult behaviour and may contribute to the occurrence of
Isolation rearing psychiatric disorders, such as depression and schizophrenia in genetically predisposed humans. The
Schizophrenia molecular mechanisms underlying these environmentally induced developmental adaptations are
Serotonin
unclear and best evaluated in animal paradigms with translational salience. Rearing rat pups from
Dopamine
Cognition
weaning in isolation, to prevent social contact with conspecifics, produces reproducible, long-term
Aggression changes including; neophobia, impaired sensorimotor gating, aggression, cognitive rigidity, reduced
Prepulse inhibition prefrontal cortical volume and decreased cortical and hippocampal synaptic plasticity. These alterations
Neophobia are associated with hyperfunction of mesolimbic dopaminergic systems, enhanced presynaptic
Nucleus accumbens dopamine (DA) and serotonergic (5-HT) function in the nucleus accumbens (NAcc), hypofunction of
Hippocampus mesocortical DA and attenuated 5-HT function in the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus. These
Frontal cortex behavioural, morphological and neurochemical abnormalities, as reviewed herein, strongly resemble
core features of schizophrenia. Therefore unravelling the mechanisms that trigger these sequelae will
improve our knowledge of the aetiology of neurodevelopmental psychiatric disorders, enable
identification of longitudinal biomarkers of dysfunction and permit predictive screening for novel
compounds with potential antipsychotic efficacy.
ß 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Contents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ................... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1088


2. Isolation procedure . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ................... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1088
3. Behavioural effects of isolation rearing. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ................... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1089
3.1. Hyper-reactivity to a novel environment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ................... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1089
3.2. Prepulse inhibition of startle . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ................... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1090
3.3. Latent inhibition . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ................... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1091
3.4. Response to rewarding stimuli . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ................... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1092
3.5. Social interaction and aggressive behaviour . . . . . . . . . . . . ................... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1092
3.6. Pain sensitivity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ................... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1093
3.7. Cognitive deficits . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ................... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1093
3.7.1. Conditioned learning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ................... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1093
3.7.2. Novel object discrimination . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ................... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1093
3.7.3. Morris water maze . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ................... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1093
3.7.4. Rule learning, extra-dimensional set shifting and behavioural flexibility. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1093
3.7.5. Visuospatial attention . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ................... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1094
3.8. Anxiety-like behaviour. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ................... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1094

* Corresponding author. Tel.: +44 115 82 30313.


E-mail address: kevin.fone@nottingham.ac.uk (Kevin C.F. Fone).
Abbreviations: DA, dopamine; ED, extra-dimensional; GABA, gamma-amino butyric acid; 5-HT, 5-hydroxytryptamine; ID, intra-dimensional; LH, lister hooded; LI, latent
inhibition; NOD, novel object discrimination; NAcc, nucleus accumbens; PFC, prefrontal cortex; PPI, prepulse inhibition; THC, D9-tetrahydrocannabinol; mGluR, metabotropic
glutamate receptor.

0149-7634/$ – see front matter ß 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.03.003
1088 K.C.F. Fone, M.V. Porkess / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32 (2008) 1087–1102

4. Influence of gender on the isolation rearing behavioural syndrome . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1094


5. Neurochemical effects of isolation rearing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1095
5.1. Neurotransmitter alterations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1095
5.1.1. Dopamine. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1095
5.1.2. 5-Hydroxytryptamine . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1095
5.1.3. Glutamate . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1097
5.2. Other neurochemical changes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1097
6. Social isolation as a model of core symptoms of developmental neuropsychological disorders . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1098
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1098

1. Introduction such studies have been performed with male rats but where
another species or gender has been examined this will be stated.
A wealth of information shows that exposure of mammals to
early-life adversity, such as maternal separation or social isolation 2. Isolation procedure
from conspecifics, adversely affects brain development and adult
behaviour (Harlow et al., 1965; Heim et al., 2004; Rapoport et al., The isolation rearing procedure adopted by most laboratories
2005). Although the molecular mechanism involved in producing involves housing rat (or mouse) pups in individual cages from the
these developmental adaptations is unclear, similar environmental first day of weaning from the dam, normally on PND21 to PND28
interventions during early-life in humans may contribute to the for rats. From that point isolation housed rats are not handled more
development of common psychiatric disorders, such as depression than once a week (to change bedding material). Isolation-reared
and schizophrenia in genetically predisposed individuals. The rats thus have visual, auditory and olfactory contact with other
availability of a non-pharmacological animal paradigm which isolation reared and group housed rats kept in the same husbandry
elicits robust, reproducible, developmental alterations by pre- conditions. They are however unable to have any form of social
venting social contact during the critical early post-natal brain interaction with littermates. Noise in the housing facility should be
development period may provide an ideal scenario to gain a better carefully controlled and minimised. To avoid any confounding
understanding of the genetic and molecular associations and the affect of litter, pups from the same dam should be divided equally
neurobiological aetiology of developmental psychiatric disorders into isolation and group housed conditions and litters should be
and enable discovery and evaluation of novel therapeutic agents. selected for equal size.
Rearing rodents in persistent social isolation from weaning, to The full behavioural change associated with social isolation of
deprive them of social play, produces a large array of consistent rodents from weaning (discussed by topic later) is only observed if
long-lasting behavioural alterations compared with group housed this intervention is commenced (from PND20 to PND30) in a critical
controls (Einon and Morgan, 1977; Heidbreder et al., 2000; Lapiz period around the time of puberty (Einon and Morgan, 1977;
et al., 2003; Valzelli, 1973) without causing any consistent Robbins et al., 1996). For instance, the reduction in; social interaction
alteration in body weight from age-matched controls. Collectively (Ferdman et al., 2007) and prepulse inhibition (PPI) of acoustic
the behavioural changes observed are consistent with the proposal startle (Wilkinson et al., 1994), or the increase in; self-administra-
that rats reared in social isolation are unable to appropriately tion of ethanol (Schenk et al., 1990) and defensive shock probe
process environmental stimuli. Even from early experiments burying (Arakawa, 2007), only occur when rat pups are isolated and
performed in the 1960s and 1970s rats reared in social isolation not when the same procedure is applied to adults. Although the
were reported to be excessively reactive to handling, anxiogenic, actual style of the cage appears to be unimportant (Einon and
and overly emotional (Hatch et al., 1965; Koch and Arnold, 1972; Morgan, 1977), the housing environment may well affect the precise
Morgan, 1973; Sahakian et al., 1977) which led to the description of nature of the outcome (Weiss et al., 1999) and no environmental
the ‘isolation-induced stress syndrome’ (Holson et al., 1991; enrichment should be provided. For instance, basal corticosterone
Valzelli, 1973). levels may only be elevated by more austere housing conditions,
Some of the behavioural and neurochemical alterations seen in such as wire floored cages where no handling is performed during
isolation-reared rats have translational relevance to developmen- the isolation period (Heidbreder et al., 2000; Holson et al., 1991) or
tal neuropsychological disorders, in particular to several core housing in sound proof cages which also prevent auditory and visual
symptoms of schizophrenia but also to changes seen in depression, contact with littermates (Greco et al., 1989). Furthermore, it is clear
which has led to isolation rearing being proposed as an animal that any form of contact with a conspecific, or excessive handling by
model of these disorders. This review will examine the impact of the experimenter (such as daily drug injection), will readily negate
post-weaning social isolation on brain development and behaviour any long-term changes and strict adherence to the protocol is
and discuss potential correlations with symptoms of neuropsy- required if robust changes are to be reproduced in the laboratory (as
chological disorders, in particular schizophrenia. The majority of discussed later). As isolation rearing leads to an array of behavioural

Table 1
Behavioural characterisation of 13 separate isolation rearing studies

Isolation duration (weeks to Hyperactivity in Novel object discrimination Prepulse inhibition


first behavioural test) a novel arena impairment of startle deficit

4 (5 studies, PPI examined in 3) 100 100 66


5 (8 studies) 88 50 88
Overall 92 69 82

Numbers given indicate the percentage of experiments where a significant change in behaviour was recorded from that in group housed controls. Hyperactivity = 60 min test
in a novel arena; novel object discrimination used a 3 min test with the choice trial 2 h after familiarisation to two identical objects. Prepulse inhibition = 120 dB pulse, 72, 76,
80 and 84 dB prepulses over 65 dB background noise, 100 ms inter-stimulus interval, 40 pseudo-random trials with a 10–20 s variable inter-trial interval.
K.C.F. Fone, M.V. Porkess / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32 (2008) 1087–1102 1089

changes it is important that when these animals are being 3. Behavioural effects of isolation rearing
investigated, or used as a research tool (e.g. for antipsychotic drug
discovery), more than one behavioural phenotype with strong 3.1. Hyper-reactivity to a novel environment
translational relevance to the core domain defects seen in the
disease being modelled should be examined (Powell and Miyakawa, One of the earliest onset and probably the most robust
2006). During the last 3 years the behavioural profile of 13 batches of observations reported in isolation-reared rats is a lack of the
male isolation-reared Lister hooded rats, has been characterised in normal habituation following placement in a novel arena,
our laboratory and the outcome is summarised in Table 1 characterised by motor hyperactivity (horizontal ambulation
demonstrating that reproducible alterations are produced when a and rearing, Table 2) compared with group housed controls
stringent protocol is applied to the same strain of rat. (Domeney and Feldon, 1998; Gentsch et al., 1981b, 1982a, 1983;

Table 2
Summary of the long-term behavioural phenotype changes associated with isolation rearing of rat pups from weaning

Behaviour Consequence of isolation rearing

Body weight Males may be slightly heavier (Fiala et al., 1977) but most groups find little change
and equal food consumption to social housed rats (Hellemans et al., 2004)
Increased food hoarding behaviour (Heidbreder et al., 2000)

Exploratory behaviour Hyper-reactivity to a novel environment (Gentsch et al., 1988, 1982a,b; Sahakian et al.,
1977; Varty et al., 2000), decreased in SI mice (Valzelli et al., 1974)

Prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle Impairment in PPI without change in control startle amplitude, reversed by
antipsychotic treatment (Bakshi et al., 1998; Cilia et al., 2001; Day-Wilson et al.,
2006; Varty et al., 2000) but not iloperidone (Barr et al., 2006)
Neurotoxic DA depletion in the nucleus accumbens reduces the isolation-induced
PPI deficit (Powell et al., 2003)
Isolation-induced PPI deficits reversed by a7 nicotinic agonist (Cilia et al., 2005a,b)
and 5-HT2A antagonist (Geyer et al., 1999)

Latent inhibition No effects (Weiss et al., 2001a,b; Wilkinson et al., 1994)

Anhedonia Increased sucrose consumption (increased motivation) (Hall et al., 1997a,b,c)


Hyperphagia (Shelley, 1965)
Increased reward sensitivity (Jones et al., 1990)
Increased anticipatory behaviour (Morgan et al., 1975)
Increased self-administration with low but impaired acquisition with high
doses of cocaine (Howes et al., 2000)
Icreased ethanol preference in mice (Advani et al., 2007) and Long–Evans
rats (Deehan et al., 2007)

Social interaction Increased interactions and aggression, decreased by diazepam


(Wongwitdecha and Marsden, 1996b)
Progressive increase in muricidal behaviour in isolation-reared rats
(Valzelli and Garattin, 1972)
Increase in defensive burying of a shock probe (Arakawa, 2007)
SI mice-increased aggression (Valzelli, 1973)

Pain Hypoalgesia (Gentsch et al., 1988)


No changes (Hellemans et al., 2004)
Increased oral response to tail pinch (Sahakian and Robbins, 1977)

Passive avoidance SI mice-deficits (reduced avoidance latency) (Valzelli, 1973)


Rats-deficit (Del Arco et al., 2004)
Fear conditioning reduced in male and female isolates (Weiss et al., 2004)

Visual learning and memory Impaired novel object discrimination (Bianchi et al., 2006)
Attention/vigilence No impairments seen in five choice serial reaction time (Dalley et al., 2002)

Spatial learning Water maze- improvements (Wongwitdecha and Marsden, 1996a)


No change (Schrijver et al., 2004)
Impairments (Hellemans et al., 2004; Lu et al., 2003a,b)

Reasoning and problem solving (attentional set shifting) Extradimensional shift impairment in spatial/non-spatial ID-ED task (Schrijver and
Wurbel, 2001)
Reversal impairment, but no ED impairment in bowl-digging (Schrijver et al., 2004)
Impairments in reversal learning (Krech et al., 1962)
Improved Morris water maze reversal performance (Wongwitdecha and Marsden, 1996a)
Clozapine reversal of impaired learning of a visual discrimination stimulus-reward
contingency rule change (Li et al., 2007)

Conditioned place preference No aversion to saline and no preference for amphetamine treated area
(Wongwitdecha and Marsden, 1995)
Anxiety Anxiogenic on elevated plus-maze (Parker and Morinan, 1986; Weiss et al., 2004;
Wright et al., 1991a,b)
Hypo-neophagia (Parker and Morinan, 1986), reduced cage escape latency
(Parker and Morinan, 1986)

Depression Little change in immobility or struggling time in the Porsolt forced swim test in mice
(Hilakivi et al., 1989) or rats (Hall et al., 1998a, 2001)
Desipramine more effective in isolates (Wongwitdecha et al., 2006)
1090 K.C.F. Fone, M.V. Porkess / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32 (2008) 1087–1102

Gentsch et al., 1982b; Heidbreder et al., 2000; Hughes and Syme, startle-eliciting stimulus (the pulse). In animals, PPI is typically
1972; Phillips et al., 1994b; Sahakian and Robbins, 1977; Sahakian measured as the inhibition of a motor startle response to a loud
et al., 1977; Silva-Gomez et al., 2003; Syme, 1973) due to less time (approximately 120 dB) acoustic startle eliciting stimulus by a
spent resting. Careful analysis of this behaviour with photocells or preceding weaker (typically 70–85 dB) prepulse delivered over a
computerised tracking shows that the response is strain depen- background white noise (usually 65–68 dB). PPI is a useful
dent. For instance the hyperactive locomotor response appears to operational index of pre-attentive sensorimotor gating mechanisms
be much more marked in Lister than in Sprague–Dawley rats which essential for the integration of cognitive and sensory information
have undergone an identical isolation protocol (Geyer et al., 1993; (Geyer et al., 2001) and shows a similar neurobiology and
Weiss et al., 2000) in both male and female rats (Powell et al., 2002; neuropharmacology in rat and man. Although it is not a unique
Weiss et al., 2001a). In addition, while the initial activity level is nor diagnostic feature of the disorder, impairments in PPI are often
comparable to group housed controls in Lister hooded (Fone et al., reported in patients with schizophrenia (Braff et al., 2001, 1992) as
1996) and Long–Evans (Powell et al., 2002) rats the former strain well as other psychiatric disorders such as depression, and it may
show a slower rate of decline such that activity levels are typically reflect stimulus overload-induced cognitive fragmentation. Isola-
elevated after 15 min of exploration. In contrast, the basal level of tion-reared mice (Sakaue et al., 2003; Varty et al., 2006) and rats
activity may be elevated from group housed controls from the show a robust reduction in PPI of acoustic startle (Geyer et al., 1993).
outset of placing isolated Wistar rats in a novel arena (Domeney This finding has been replicated in a large variety of laboratories in
and Feldon, 1998; Heidbreder et al., 2000). This also demonstrates both male and female Lister hooded (Cilia et al., 2005b, 2001; Day-
the need to record the response to novelty over a sufficient time Wilson et al., 2006; Varty et al., 1999b), Long–Evans (Binder et al.,
period to enable the temporal profile to be recorded and could 2001; Powell et al., 2003, 2002) and Sprague–Dawley rats (Geyer
explain the few reports where the response to a novel arena has et al., 1993; Varty et al., 1999a), with less consistent and smaller
appeared unaltered (Rosa et al., 2005) or even reduced during the effects seen in Wistar (Domeney and Feldon, 1998; Rosa et al., 2005;
first 5 min due to initial freezing (Holson et al., 1991). Furthermore, Weiss et al., 2000, 1999; Weiss and Feldon, 2001) and Lewis strains
even brief periods of handling twice a week are sufficient to but it may not occur in isolated Fischer (Varty and Geyer, 1998) rats
prevent this inappropriate habituation to a novel environment (Table 2). As previously discussed in Section 2 above the
(Holson et al., 1991). This hyperactive response of isolation-reared development of PPI may also be dependent on the housing
rats is also reduced in bright light (Hall et al., 1998c) and absent environment and is not seen in Wistar rats raised on a grid rather
when the environment becomes familiar (Phillips et al., 1994b). than a sawdust floor (Weiss et al., 1999). By using comparable
Regardless of strain this decreased habituation to novelty is isolation procedures (8 weeks of isolation from PND 28) Cilia et al.
comprised of elevated horizontal locomotion and vertical rears (2005b, 2001) showed that 23 of 27 cohorts of isolated male Lister
(Gentsch et al., 1981a, 1982b) suggestive of an increased hooded rats exhibited 30–50% reductions in the PPI produced by a
propensity to escape and consistent with neophobia (Fone et al., single prepulse either 5 or 10 dB above baseline, demonstrating the
1996). One recent study using microarray analysis found abnormal robust, reproducible nature of this behavioural alteration which is
expression of immediate early genes and genes that regulate independent of any change in basal startle (Domeney and Feldon,
apoptotic genes and cell differentiation in the medial prefrontal 1998). A comparable reproducibility of this PPI deficit (82%
cortex (PFC) which correlated with the extent of hyperactivity in a compared with 85% of isolation cohorts, Table 1) has been recorded
novel arena after 26 days isolation in a small group of six Sprague– in our laboratory also using male Lister hooded rats.
Dawley rats (Levine et al., 2007). Thus abnormal PFC activity may Once established the deficit in PPI persists consistently with
be related to this behavioural alteration and such behaviour is weekly testing (Cilia et al., 2001) is not reversed by re-socialisation of
potentially relevant to the positive symptoms seen in schizo- Lister hooded rats for 8 weeks (Cilia et al., 2001) or by restricting
phrenia or ‘anxiety’ accompanying depression. In an analogous water access to 30 min per day early in the isolation period (used as
fashion isolation-reared rats are slower to emerge from a confined an additional early-life adverse effect) on PND 24–26 (Binder et al.,
space to a larger novel environment (Arakawa, 2005; Einon and 2001; Powell et al., 2002). Weiss and Feldon (Weiss and Feldon,
Morgan, 1977). Interestingly isolation-reared rats appear to show a 2001) performed a similar analysis of the effect of social isolation
preference for a novel environment compared with group housed measured on repeated occasions in multiple cohorts of male
rats (Sahakian et al., 1977), although another study only found this Sprague–Dawley rats following 12–24 weeks of continuous isola-
to be the case under red-light conditions (Hall et al., 1997b). tion and found that for each cohort the extent of any impairment was
The consistency of the exaggerated response to a novel arena consistent across the isolation period. However, the PPI deficit is
and the ease with which this can be measured makes this a suitable developmentally specific and does not occur when adult rats are
marker with which to confirm the development of the ‘isolation isolated for an 8-week period (Wilkinson et al., 1994). Other groups
syndrome’ prior to performing more complex behavioural or have also found that the PPI deficit can be lost by assessment of
neurochemical analyses (see Table 1). Few groups have system- locomotor activity less than 1 week prior to PPI (Domeney and
atically examined the time course of the development of the hyper- Feldon, 1998) or handling during the isolation period (Krebs-
reactivity to a novel arena but it is clear that this is also one of the Thomson et al., 2001) even briefly (grasping the tail three times a
earliest behavioural features to appear, being evident even after 2 week) will prevent the development of PPI deficits (Rosa et al., 2005).
weeks of isolation (Bakshi and Geyer, 1999; Einon and Morgan, So an adequate recovery period of at least 7 days should be left
1977). In contrast, other behavioural changes such as the reduction between test procedures or repeated PPI testing if this is to be
in prepulse inhibition of startle (discussed later) are only apparent performed (Domeney and Feldon, 1998; Varty and Higgins, 1995).
after at least 4 weeks isolation in Sprague–Dawley rats (Varty et al., Early experiments showed that the deficit in PPI produced by
1999a) and even longer periods of isolation in Lister hooded rats social isolation could be partially reversed by acute treatment with
(Bakshi and Geyer, 1999). the DA D2 receptor antagonist, raclopride (Geyer et al., 1993),
suggesting that it may involve enhanced dopaminergic neuronal
3.2. Prepulse inhibition of startle activity. Subsequent studies found that bilateral injections of 6-
hydroxydopamine into the nucleus accumbens (NAcc), depleting
Prepulse inhibition refers to the inhibitory influence of a weak DA levels by 83%, reduced the deficit in PPI seen in female Long–
sensory stimulus (the prepulse) on the reaction to a subsequent Evans rats produced by 8 weeks isolation rearing (Powell et al.,
K.C.F. Fone, M.V. Porkess / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32 (2008) 1087–1102 1091

Fig. 1. Schematic overview of the most consistent behavioural and neurochemical observations reported following at least 6 weeks social isolation from weaning of rat pups
compared with control responses in group housed conspecifics. Single headed lines reflect a decrease (dotted lines) or and increase (solid bold) in the neurotransmitter
function identified. DA: dopamine; 5-HT: 5-hydroxytryptamine; Glu: glutamate; BDNF: brain derived neurotrophic factor; a2 ADR: a2 adrenoreceptor; HPA: hypothalamic
pituitary adrenal axis and 5-HT receptors are named according to the IUPHAR nomenclature. *Impairment in recognition memory may involve deficits in the entorhinal and
perirhinal cortex and hippocampus not represented on this figure.

2003). This supports the view that dopamine (DA) hyperfunction in Braff, 1994). The strong similarity between the neurobiology of PPI
the NAcc may account for the deficit in PPI seen in isolates, in rodents and man (Swerdlow and Geyer, 1998) together with its
probably by enhancing GABAergic output of the ventral pallidum reversal by most antipsychotic drugs has resulted in several groups
and subsequently the pedunculopontine pathway (Sipes and using this as a screen for novel antipsychotics (Cilia et al., 2005a;
Geyer, 1997; Swerdlow et al., 1990) which modulates PPI Geyer et al., 1993, 1999; Varty and Higgins, 1995). For instance the
(Fig. 1). Recently it has been shown that the isolation-induced selective 5-HT2A receptor antagonist, M100907, partially reversed
PPI deficit is reversed by pre-treatment with an a7 nicotinic the PPI deficit produced by 8 weeks of isolation rearing of Sprague–
receptor agonist (Cilia et al., 2005a). However, the same authors Dawley rats (Geyer et al., 1999), but clinical trials have failed to
showed that isolation rearing did not alter expression of a7 replicate any similar substantial benefit from the use of this drug in
receptor RNA and protein in the hippocampus or frontal cortex, schizophrenia (Gray and Roth, 2007). In addition, acute admin-
suggesting that alteration of this receptor is not associated with the istration of the 5-HT1A agonist, MKC-242 (ozemozotan), reversed
phenotypic changes resulting from isolation rearing. Treatment the PPI deficit produced by 6 weeks isolation of male ddY mice
with D9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) exacerbates the isolation- without affecting PPI in group-housed controls (Sakaue et al.,
induced PPI impairment, but has no effect on PPI in socially housed 2003) but this has yet to be studied in rats. However, it is obvious
animals. This THC effect is blocked by SR 141716 (an antagonist at that no single phenotypic component of the isolation-induce
the CB1 receptor), although SR141716 has no effect on PPI when syndrome will be sufficient to predict antipsychotic potential nor
given alone (Malone and Taylor, 2006). This is of particular interest indeed that a single animal paradigm could be used as a safe
given the reported links between adolescent cannabis exposure mechanism for predicting therapeutic potential in such a complex
and schizophrenia (D’Souza et al., 2005; Zammit et al., 2002) human disorder.
supported by a recent meta-analysis which found that cannabis
use increased the risk of psychotic outcome by a factor of 1.4-fold 3.3. Latent inhibition
(Moore et al., 2007).
The impairment of PPI seen in isolation-reared rats is reversed Latent inhibition refers to the constraining affects of prior
by atypical antipsychotics, such as quetiapine, olanzapine, stimulus exposure on sequential stimulus-response learning
clozapine and risperidol, provided that a variable inter-stimulus (Feldon and Weiner, 1992) and reflects the ability to ignore or
interval is used (Bakshi et al., 1998; Cilia et al., 2001; Varty and suppress irrelevant stimuli to focus on biologically salient input.
Higgins, 1995; Wilkinson et al., 1994). However, a recent study Latent inhibition represents the interaction of associative and non-
found that the novel atypical antipsychotic, iloperidone, failed to associative learning for a specific stimulus, modelling attentional
reverse the PPI deficit in isolation-reared Sprague–Dawley rats processes which are disrupted in schizophrenia. Several studies
(Barr et al., 2006), so negative results should be interpreted with have shown that latent inhibition is reduced in schizophrenic
care. Furthermore, even acute administration of an antipsychotic is patients (Baruch et al., 1988; Gray et al., 1995). Baruch et al.
sufficient to reverse the isolation-induced PPI deficit while the demonstrated that latent inhibition was absent in recently
clinical benefit of antipsychotic medication takes several weeks to diagnosed schizophrenic patients, while chronically ill schizo-
manifest, so the predictive validity to the chronic effect of phrenic patients showed distinct latent inhibition (Gray et al.,
treatment in schizophrenia is unclear. Although it is not unique 1995). This led to the hypothesis that latent inhibition rodent
to schizophrenia, PPI impairments are believed to reflect the loss of paradigms may represent a relevant translational model of the
sensory inhibition resulting in cognitive fragmentation (Perry and positive symptoms seen in acute schizophrenics. Latent inhibition
1092 K.C.F. Fone, M.V. Porkess / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32 (2008) 1087–1102

tasks, therefore, fulfil the criteria for face and construct validity as comprehensive review concluded that isolation rearing causes a
an animal behavioural model of the specific attentional impair- modest increase in the initiation of psychostimulant self-admin-
ments associated with schizophrenia (Table 2). However, it has istration, particularly following low doses of stimulant, but that
been postulated that disrupted latent inhibition seen in schizo- there is little change in the maintenance of the behaviour once it is
phrenia may result from the effects of antipsychotic medication established (Lu et al., 2003b). Such early changes in response to
(Williams et al., 1998). In the few studies performed, isolation pyschostimulants could be the consequence of alteration in the
rearing of Sprague–Dawley rats failed to produce any effect on activity of mesolimbic dopaminergic neurones or DA receptor
latent inhibition (Weiss et al., 2001b; Wilkinson et al., 1994). function in their terminal areas, as discussed later in the section on
neurochemistry.
3.4. Response to rewarding stimuli Similar discrepancies occur in the literature using the condi-
tioned place preference paradigm. Isolation in Lister hooded rats
Isolation-reared rats tend to consume more food than group prevents the development of a conditioned place preference to
housed controls and may weigh slightly more than aged matched either amphetamine (Wongwitdecha and Marsden, 1995) or
controls (Fiala et al., 1977). Yet other groups have reported morphine (Wongwitdecha and Marsden, 1996a). In Long–Evans
isolation-reared rats consume an equal amount of food to socially rats isolated immediately post-weaning (Schenk et al., 1985, 1983)
housed controls under both normal conditions and when food- heroin conditioning was impaired when a biased training
deprived (Hellemans et al., 2004) but may show an altered procedure which initially paired drug treatment with the non-
preference for different types of food (Hall et al., 1997a). preferred compartment was used (similar to that used by
Nonetheless, isolates show a higher response rate in food-reward Wongwitdecha and Marsden (1996a)). Notably this impairment
motivated procedures consistent with them having enhanced was only observed when rats were isolated immediately post-
incentive motivation (Table 2). When given access to increasing weaning and not at 3 months of age, confirming the importance of
concentrations of sucrose solution, isolation-reared Lister hooded ‘early-life’ intervention on this behaviour. Interestingly isolation
rats show increased fluid consumption consistent with increased rearing has been shown to alter opioid receptor binding. While 7
motivation (Hall et al., 1997c). Similarly rearing in isolation days isolation increases PFC [3H]diprenorphine binding measured
enhances the rate of acquisition of a discriminative approach task, by autoradiography (Vanderschuren et al., 1995), a marked
which measures association of a stimulus with a sucrose reward decrease in whole brain naloxone binding occurred following 43
(Harmer and Phillips, 1998). Isolates also show an increased days isolation (Schenk et al., 1982) more comparable with the
response to sucrose reward in a conditioned reward paradigm; an duration used in most conditioning paradigms above, which could
effect further enhanced by intra-accumbens amphetamine, con- account for the decreased behaviours reported. Consistent with
sistent with alteration in the mesolimbic DA system contributing this proposal isolation rearing has also been found to attenuate
to this change (Jones et al., 1990). In contrast, no difference was both the locomotor (Katz and Steinber, 1970) and conditioned
found in conditioned taste avoidance (Hellemans et al., 2004) in taste aversion (Schenk et al., 1987a) response to morphine.
isolation-reared rats. However, Morgan (1973) and Jones et al. Ethanol preference was found to be increased following 6 weeks
(1991) both found that isolation-reared rats will continue to isolation of male C57BL/6J mice (Advani et al., 2007). Similarly
perform in food motivated learning tasks, even after satiation. voluntary ethanol consumption was increased following 8 weeks
Several groups have investigated the effect that rearing rats in isolation of Wistar rats (Hall et al., 1998b; Wolffgramm, 1990) and
isolation has on the response to psychostimulant drugs but the operant responding to obtain ethanol was increased following 90
results appear to be inconsistent. Isolation rearing has been found days of isolation of male Long–Evans rats (Deehan et al., 2007). Like
to increase both the locomotor (Jones et al., 1992, 1990) and/or the most other phenotype changes discussed, this increase preference
stereotype (Einon and Sahakian, 1979; Sahakian et al., 1975) for ethanol does not occur when rats are isolated for 12 weeks at
behavioural response elicited by amphetamine or apomorphine or adulthood (Schenk et al., 1990) and this may explain why some early
to systemic cocaine administration (Phillips et al., 1994b). While studies, which did not state the age at the time of isolation housing,
other groups found no change in either the locomotor or stereotype failed to show this effect (Deatherage, 1972). Thus early-life isolation
response to acute amphetamine administration (Weiss et al., in the rodent at most produces a modest increase in the
2001a). Furthermore both a complete absence of locomotor susceptibility to the reinforcing properties of a variety of drugs
sensitisation to repeated amphetamine administration (Weiss and a very inconsistent effect in paradigms involving motivation and
et al., 2001a) and a marked increase compared to controls (Bardo reward, which is at odds the general picture of anhedonia usually
et al., 1995) have both been recorded in the same strain (Sprague– experienced by schizophrenic or depressed patients (Table 2).
Dawley) of rats; although in the latter study group-housed rats
were raised in a highly enriched condition which could account for 3.5. Social interaction and aggressive behaviour
the difference. Isolation-reared rats have been shown to display an
increased propensity to self-administer cocaine (Howes et al., Social withdrawal is one of the inherent negative symptoms of
2000; Schenk et al., 1987b) or morphine (Alexander et al., 1978; schizophrenia, and several researchers have postulated that it is also
Hadaway et al., 1979), no enhancement following d-amphetamine one of the first symptoms to manifest (Strous et al., 2004). An
(Schenk et al., 1988) or cocaine (Bozarth et al., 1989) or impaired increase in social interaction occurs in male but not female Wistar
reinforcement to intravenous cocaine (Phillips et al., 1994a,b) or rats following isolation rearing which is more evident when
intra accumbens amphetamine (Phillips et al., 1994a). Unlike other isolation begins on PND21 than PND30 (Ferdman et al., 2007). This
studies cited the isolation procedure utilised by Bozarth et al. increase in social interaction is particularly evident under conditions
(1989) did not begin until PND 61 (i.e. at adulthood) which may of bright light and may be largely due to an increase in aggressive
explain the apparent discrepancy of this particular work, but why behaviours (Vale and Montgomery, 1997; Wongwitdecha and
such inconsistency in other studies? Overall, most studies which Marsden, 1996c). A similar increase in aggressive behaviour is a
have reported enhanced responding to stimulant drugs have begun prominent feature in mice reared in isolation (Fig. 1 and Table 2)
with a series of non-contingent priming infusions (unlike the where it was one of the first phenotypical effects of isolation rearing
studies by Phillips et al. (1994a,b)) which could favour detection of to be identified (Valzelli, 1973). An increase in defensive burying of a
‘drug driven’ rather than ‘drug seeking’ behaviour. A recent shock probe occurs in both adult male and female Wistar rats
K.C.F. Fone, M.V. Porkess / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32 (2008) 1087–1102 1093

following as little as 2 weeks of social isolation (PND 26–40), a recognition could also reflect cognitive inflexibility preventing
behaviour which is highly correlated with individual aggressive attention of the new stimulus. However, no selective impairment in
behaviour and social dominance (Arakawa, 2007). Furthermore, novel object discrimination occurs after isolation when short inter-
isolation of male, but not female, Wistar rats causes the develop- trial intervals (such as 1 min) are used (Lapiz et al., 2000). Although
ment of one of a spectrum of three different behavioural responses to this is not surprising as rats will be very proficient at the task using
a mouse; indifferent, friendly (attempting to play with it) or this protocol (Table 2) it is consistent with isolation impairing
muricide (breaking its neck). The proportion of rats displaying cognitive rather than attention processes. Deficits in novel object
muricidal behaviour increases with the duration of isolation recognition may have construct validity to the visual recognition
(Valzelli and Garattin, 1972). This muricidal activity is not reduced defects seen in schizophrenia (McClure et al., 2007; Nestor et al.,
by treatment with benzodiazepines but is abolished by tricyclic 2007). Our group has performed some studies to determine whether
antidepressants (Valzelli and Bernasconi, 1976) but no studies have potential antipsychotic drugs can reverse the recognition memory
attempted to reverse this behaviour with antipsychotics. impairments produced by social isolation. Five to ten days treatment
with either the ampakine, aniracetam, or the 5-HT6 receptor
3.6. Pain sensitivity antagonist, PRX-07037 (Porkess et al., 2006; King et al., 2007) both
restored novel object recognition to levels seen in social housed
Social isolation appeared to increase the oral response to a mild controls, suggesting that this behavioural deficit may be a valuable
tail pinch, a behaviour which has been linked to striatal tool with which to examine novel therapeutic treatments for
dopaminergic activity (Sahakian and Robbins, 1977), without cognitive impairments relevant to those seen in schizophrenia.
causing any difference in sensitivity in response to formalin
injection. Conflicting results have also been found in the tail flick 3.7.3. Morris water maze
test of heat sensitivity, with either no difference (Hellemans et al., Several groups have examined the impact of isolation on
2004) or a hypoalgesic effect being found (Gentsch et al., 1988) in acquisition and retention of spatial learning in the Morris water
isolates. Recent evidence suggests that schizophrenics show a maze but results are inconsistent (Table 2). The majority of groups
reduced sensitivity to warmth perception and have a higher onset of find no alteration in acquisition using a fixed platform position in
thermal pain sensation (Jochum et al., 2006), so further translational the task (Lapiz et al., 2003; Schrijver et al., 2004), although some
studies in isolation-reared rats are required. The limited data groups have reported modest improvements (Wongwitdecha and
available thus suggest that social isolation does not cause any major Marsden, 1996b) or impairments in learning (Hellemans et al.,
change in nociception. This is important since a major change in pain 2004) accompanied by reduction in long-term potentiation in the
sensitivity would severely confound interpretation of the impact of CA1 area of rat hippocampus (Lu et al., 2003a). However, the
this early-life intervention on many of the paradigms utilised to apparent impairment in acquisition compared to group housed
assess reward-related or conditioned behavioural responses. controls may be due to the well documented affect of environ-
mental enrichment in the group housed cage rather than any affect
3.7. Cognitive deficits of isolation per se (Schrijver et al., 2002). One group has shown that
isolation rearing enhanced reversal performance in the water maze
3.7.1. Conditioned learning over social housed controls (Wongwitdecha and Marsden, 1996b).
Early cognitive analysis in the isolation-reared mouse showed a However, isolation rearing has been shown to improve retention
deficit in avoidance conditioning in the passive avoidance test (Lapiz et al., 2001) and impair reversal learning (Krech et al., 1962;
(Valzelli, 1973) and a similar deficit in retention has been Schrijver et al., 2004) consistent with the induction of behavioural
demonstrated after 8 weeks isolation of Sprague–Dawley rats rigidity or impaired learning of a new rule, as discussed in more
(Del Arco et al., 2004). Reductions in contextual fear conditioning detail for other tasks below. Taken together these results suggest
have also been found in isolation-reared Sprague–Dawley rats that isolation rearing may primarily affect PFC cortico-striatal
irrespective of gender (Weiss et al., 2004) consistent with pathways involved in reversal learning rather than hippocampal
impairment in associative learning. pathways involved in spatial learning.
Schizophrenics show impaired performance in tests of visuo-
3.7.2. Novel object discrimination spatial ability (Bozikas et al., 2006). Recently 22 schizophrenic
Rodents have an innate curiosity for novelty and will direct patients were found to be impaired in both acquisition and
attention to a novel rather than a familiar object. This forms the basis retention of a hippocampal-dependent version of computerised
of the two trial novel object discrimination test (Ennaceur and virtual water maze task compared to healthy controls but normal
Delacour, 1988) which requires no prior training and in the simplest, in a visible platform and non-hippocampal dependent version of
non-spatial, context-independent format measures episodic mem- the task (Hanlon et al., 2006). Importantly this study confirms the
ory which is impaired in schizophrenic patients (Nestor et al., 2007). translational relevance of performing rodent water maze tasks to
In the test a rat is placed in a familiar chamber with two unseen examining human cognitive deficits.
identical objects and investigates them (biting, sniffing, licking, etc.).
The rat is removed from the chamber and one of the objects is 3.7.4. Rule learning, extra-dimensional set shifting and behavioural
replaced with a ‘novel’ object. If the rat remembers the original flexibility
‘familiar’ object it will spend more time exploring the novel object. Isolation rearing appears to cause impairment in many rule
However, isolation-reared rats show impaired recognition memory learning tasks, including reversal learning (Krech et al., 1962). In
when an inter-trial time delay is used in the novel object 1973 Morgan reared rats in isolation with no environmental
discrimination paradigm (Bianchi et al., 2006). This enhanced enrichment and tested them in food motivated learning tasks
natural forgetting is probably due to cognitive impairment rather (Morgan, 1973). Isolates showed no impairment in acquisition of
than hyperactivity as the time spent exploring both objects in the the task but were impaired when required to adopt a different
first trial is unaltered by isolation. In agreement with this proposal strategy (Morgan, 1973) and continued to perform under
and in an acute object contact task isolation-reared rats explored the conditions of satiation and extinction which was interpreted as
same number of novel objects as social reared animals (Einon and a tendency to continue with a previously rewarded behaviour in
Morgan, 1977; File, 1978). The apparent reduction in object the absence of an alternative (Morgan et al., 1977).
1094 K.C.F. Fone, M.V. Porkess / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32 (2008) 1087–1102

Jones et al. (1991) also demonstrated altered rule learning in the 5-CSRTT (Dalley et al., 2004). This is consistent with the previous
isolates. In a conditional visual discrimination task isolation- suggestion that isolation-induced impairment in cognitive tasks is
reared rats achieved the same level of accuracy as group housed not solely the result of impaired attention but this change could in
controls. However, when distracting stimuli were introduced the part reflect the documented affect of isolation on food reward
accuracy of socially reared animals was reduced while that of motivated behaviour discussed earlier.
isolates was unaffected. Isolates also continued to perform the task
when satiated. In a second experiment to test serial reversal 3.8. Anxiety-like behaviour
learning in a simple discrimination task, isolation-reared rats did
not improve their performance after several reversals (Jones et al., Some groups have reported modest increases in anxiety-related
1991). Similarly Sprague–Dawley rats isolated for 8 weeks then behaviours on the elevated plus maze in isolation-reared rats
trained to retrieve a food pellet reward by a light cue in a rotating T (Bickerdike et al., 1993; Hellemans et al., 2004; Parker and
maze in dim light showed no impairment of acquisition (Li et al., Morinan, 1986; Wright et al., 1991b) while others have found no
2007). However, isolates were impaired upon reversal of the visual effect using a similar protocol and strain of rats (Fone et al., 1996).
discrimination stimulus-reward contingency (i.e. from light = food Similarly following 90 days isolation of Long–Evans rats there was
to no light = food) and this effect was attenuated by chronic no effect on the behavioural profile in the black/white box
clozapine treatment. (Hellemans et al., 2004) and although these workers reported an
Attentional set-shifting measures behavioural flexibility, testing anxiogenic-like profile in the elevated plus maze test an error in
the ability to learn rules and then adapt to a change. The Wisconsin data collection meant that some control groups were not reported
Card Sort Test measures this in humans, and schizophrenics have in their study. Isolation-reared rats also show reduced consump-
been shown to be particularly impaired in this paradigm (Bozikas tion of new food and a reduced latency to escape from a novel open
et al., 2006; Elliott et al., 1995; Haut et al., 1996; Ruiz et al., 2007). A top cage (Parker and Morinan, 1986), both of which are behaviours
rodent version of this task has been developed (Birrell and Brown, consistent with a small increase in anxiety in tasks involving mild
2000) in which food deprived rats dig in scented bowls for food aversion (Table 2). Similarly male ddY mice reared in isolation for 6
rewards hidden in different digging mediums. The rats must learn to weeks show a small but significant increase in the number of steps
follow one of the cues (either scent or digging medium) to find the climbed in the staircase test, similar to the pattern of behaviour
reward. The rats then undergo a series of discriminations, in which reported following anxiogenic drugs such as benzodiazepine
they always follow the same type of cue (e.g. a new scent), known as inverse agonists (Ago et al., 2007). Interestingly both mice and
intra-dimensional (ID) or affective shifts. However, in the extra- rats reared in social isolation for 2 months showed a reduction in
dimensional (ED) or attentional shift the rat must now learn to sodium pentobarbital-induced sleep time compared to group
follow the other cue (e.g. digging medium), this transition can be reared controls (Watanabe et al., 1992). However, this is primarily
selectively impaired by lesions of the medial PFC in monkeys (Dias due to a change in drug metabolism rather than altered drug
et al., 1996) and rats (Birrell and Brown, 2000). Schizophrenics also binding to the GABA A receptor complex and therefore not likely to
find this ED shift particularly difficult (Elliott et al., 1995). account for any change in ‘anxiety-related’ behaviour in isolates
Recently one group has found reversal learning to be impaired (see the further discussion of neurotransmitters in Section 5.2).
in isolation-reared rats (Table 2) using a bowl-digging paradigm As loss of social contact and behavioural withdrawal are
(Schrijver et al., 2004). Furthermore, although isolation-reared rats associated with the aetiology of depression in man. Therefore some
were able to perform ID shifts (reversals), they were impaired in ED groups have examined whether rearing rodents in isolation may
shifts when tested in a radial arm maze ED/ID paradigm, which result in the development of behavioural despair, such as the
involves switching between spatial and non-spatial cues (Schrijver immobility adopted by rats placed in a small chamber containing
and Wurbel, 2001). water, known as the Porsolt Forced swim test (Porsolt et al., 1978,
While spatial learning and acquisition (which appear to be 1977). In mice although short periods of social isolation (for
largely unaltered in isolates) is dependent on hippocampal- between 24 h and 5 days) near the age of weaning (Hilakivi et al.,
neocortical pathways, reversal learning depends primarily on 1989) may reduce immobility time in the forced swim test
PFC cortico-striatal pathways which would appear to be prefer- (although see Yates et al., 1991) longer periods of isolation for 10–
entially affected by social isolation from weaning. This would 20 days saw this behaviour return to that seen in group housed
account for the observation that isolation rearing particularly controls. Similarly in a modified version of the Porsolt forced swim
disrupts spatial or discrimination learning in paradigms which test, 12 weeks isolation rearing from PND 21 failed to alter either
require reversal learning (Jones et al., 1991; Li et al., 2007; Schrijver immobility or escape behaviour in either Wistar or Fawn-hooded
et al., 2004), an alteration in the modality of the response rule rats strains (Hall et al., 1998a, 2001). This suggests that prolonged
(Morgan, 1973; Schrijver and Wurbel, 2001), or tasks based on social isolation does not affect the ability of the rodent to cope with
extinction (Jones et al., 1991) rather than impairing simple a short period of severe stress. Furthermore, taken together, these
learning of a new rule (Table 2). data do not support the early view that isolation-reared rats may
have a ‘depressed-like’ phenotype that could be useful to
3.7.5. Visuospatial attention investigate the neurobiology of depressive disorders.
The rat 5-choice serial reaction time task (5-CSRTT) measures
sustained visuospatial attention and aspects of executive function, 4. Influence of gender on the isolation rearing behavioural
including perseverence and impulsivity (Chudasama and Robbins, syndrome
2004). Rats placed into a chamber with five operant response holes
in the wall and are trained by food reward to make appropriate There is a wealth of evidence shows that gender can affect both
operant noses pokes in response to brief visual stimuli. Once training the degree and direction of the resultant long-term changes in
has achieved stable criteria premature, perseverative and omission behaviour and hormones produced by exposure to early-life
responses are recorded to probe attentional defects. Apart from adversity in the rat, such as maternal separation (Slotten et al.,
being slower to collect food rewards and making more perseverative 2006) and chronic mild stress (Westenbroek et al., 2003). In
responses to an auditory distractor, isolation-reared rats were not general isolation rearing appears to have a greater long-term
found to be impaired in accuracy, impulsivity or correct latency in impact on the ‘emotional state’ of male than female rats, as
K.C.F. Fone, M.V. Porkess / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32 (2008) 1087–1102 1095

measured by the ability to cope with aversive contexts. As justified the NAcc contributing to components of the isolation behavioural
below, the more aversive the context to which the rat is exposed syndrome.
the greater the disparity between genders. Thus both male and Hall et al. showed that the inhibitory effect of the DA D2
female isolates show a similar decrease in habituation when receptor on DA D1 receptor function in the striatum was also
exposed to a mildly aversive novel arena in Long–Evans (Powell attenuated in isolates, suggesting functional down-regulation of
et al., 2002) and Wistar (Ferdman et al., 2007) rats. However, DA D2 receptors (Hall et al., 1998d). However, other groups have
isolation-reared male Sprague–Dawley rats show a greater found no change in either the density or affinity of either DA D1 or
anxiogenic profile in the slightly more aversive elevated plus D2 receptors using autoradiography (and the radioligands
maze, together with a higher ACTH and corticosterone elevation in [H-3]SCH 23390 and [H-3]spiroperidol, respectively) in the
response to a stressor, than females (Weiss et al., 2004). Following mesolimbic or nigrostriatal systems (Bardo and Hammer, 1991)
11 weeks of isolation only male and not female rats also made or striatum (Del Arco et al., 2004) following 30 days isolation of
more social contact in their interaction with a partner (Ferdman Sprague–Dawley rats. In contrast, some groups (Djouma et al.,
et al., 2007) tending to switch to more aggressive behaviours. With 2006) have found a selective elevation in DA D2 but not D1 receptor
an acute stress sufficient to induce a startle reflex, isolation rearing binding measured using autoradiography with [125I]-NCQ 298 in
produces a greater deficit in prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle the NAcc and amygdala of isolation reared Fawn hooded rats and
in male than in female rats (see review Weiss and Feldon, 2001). elevation in striatal [3H]spiroperidone DA D2 receptor binding in
The reason for the apparently greater susceptibility of male rats to Wistar rats. Part of this discrepancy may be due to the inability of
isolation rearing is unclear but one suggestion is that oestrogen DA D2 ligands to differentiate between presynaptic autoreceptors
acts as a protective factor against some of these early environment- and postsynaptic D2 receptors which may be differentially
induced developmental changes (Weiss and Feldon, 2001). This regulated by isolation. In addition there may be an increase in
gender-dependent sensitivity also suggests that male rats are more the ratio of high:low affinity states of the DA D2 receptors in
useful to investigate the isolation-induced behavioural syndrome. isolation rearing (which may be a common feature of psychosis
It is interesting to note that the onset, rate and severity of (Seeman et al., 2006)) but this has not yet been fully investigated.
symptoms are greater in male than female patients with The level of immunoreactivity of the presynaptic protein CDCrel-
schizophrenia (Aleman et al., 2003; Nicole et al., 1992). In contrast, 1 was reduced in the striatum and increased in the hippocampus of
women are almost twice as likely to suffer from depressive isolated rats (Barr et al., 2004). This protein is normally co-localised
disorders as men (Hirschfeld and Weissmann, 2002). Furthermore, with syntaxin and modulates DA neurotransmission, but this
in several putative animal models of depression (Anisman and relationship appears to be lost in isolation-reared rats (Barr et al.,
Matheson, 2005; Kennett et al., 1986), female rather than male rats 2004). The overall picture from these studies suggests an enhanced
show a greater response to stressors in contrast to the dimorphic dopaminergic activity in the NAcc and ventral striatum and reduced
response of rats reared in isolation. It is possible however that DA function in the PFC of isolation-reared rats, but the exact DA
isolation-induced behavioural changes takes longer to become receptor alterations are not yet clear (Fig. 1).
apparent in female than male rats, but this has not yet been
examined systematically. Furthermore, as yet no studies have 5.1.2. 5-Hydroxytryptamine
reported sexual dimorphism of any neurochemical consequences Similar discrepancy is found in studies examining 5-hydro-
of isolation rearing. xytryptamine (5-HT) function following isolation rearing probably
because it is also affected in a brain region specific manner
5. Neurochemical effects of isolation rearing (Table 3). Isolation rearing has been shown to decrease basal 5-HT
turnover in NAcc (Heidbreder et al., 2000), but not in the PFC or
5.1. Neurotransmitter alterations caudate putamen (Jones et al., 1992). Furthermore, exposure to
inescapable foot shock increased 5-HT release from the medial
5.1.1. Dopamine NAcc in isolation but not social reared rats measured by
Overall, post-mortem analysis has found few consistent altera- microdialysis (Fulford and Marsden, 1998a, 2007). Repeated
tions in tissue levels of DA, DOPAC (Jones et al., 1992; Leng et al., exposure to the contextual stimulus also caused 5-HT release in
2004), 5-HT or 5-HIAA or acetylcholine (Leng et al., 2004) in the NAcc the isolated and not in social housed rats. Thus isolation rearing
or striatum of isolation compared with group housed rats, although appears to increase presynaptic serotonergic function in the NAcc.
basal PFC DA levels may be elevated (Jones et al., 1992). While This may represent an adaptation to preceding early-life stress or
isolation rearing does cause a variety of changes to brain be secondary to changes in the function of other neurotransmitters
neurochemistry, evidence is often contradictory, as will be discussed such as DA. In a recent follow-up to earlier experiments this group
in detail below (Fig. 1 and Table 3). Some groups have reported that (Fulford and Marsden, 2007) has also shown that on re-exposure to
isolation rearing increases basal DA turnover in amgydala (Heid- the context where the shock was received the significant elevation
breder et al., 2000) and NAcc (Hall et al., 1998d). Isolation-reared rats in n. accumbens 5-HT and DA release seen only in isolation and not
have an enhanced locomotor response to amphetamine and show a group reared rats, are both blocked by depletion of DA with a-
potentiation of amphetamine-induced DA release in the NAcc (Hall methyl-p-tyrosine suggesting that the enhanced presynaptic
et al., 1998d), suggesting a possible sensitisation of the mesoac- dopaminergic input may contribute to the augmented serotonergic
cumbens DA projection (Jones et al., 1990). Isolation-reared rats are neuronal function in this brain region.
impaired in the development of schedule-induced polydipsia, which In contrast, Lister hooded rats reared in social isolation from
has been shown to be dependent on optimal mesolimbic DA activity, weaning for 6 weeks show no change in hippocampal post-
specifically in the NAcc (Jones et al., 1989). Interestingly isolation synaptic 5-HT1A receptor function in vivo using electrophysiology
rearing decreases DA turnover in medial PFC (Heidbreder et al., but a decrease in 5-HT1B mediated inhibition of endogenous 5-HT
2000), which is similar to the hypofrontality seen in schizophrenia. release, consistent with impaired presynaptic 5-HT function in this
In a conditioned emotional response (CER) paradigm foot shock area (Muchimapura et al., 2002, 2003). Isolation rearing signifi-
increased DA levels in the shell of the NAcc to a greater magnitude cantly increased 5-HT2A receptor binding in the prelimbic, motor
and for a more prolonged period in isolation-reared rats (Fulford and and cingulate cortex, whereas 5-HT1A receptor binding was
Marsden, 1998b) consistent with enhanced dopaminergic activity in significantly reduced in the prelimbic cortex and increased in
1096 K.C.F. Fone, M.V. Porkess / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32 (2008) 1087–1102

Table 3
Summary of the long-term neurochemical changes associated with rearing rat pups social isolation

Neurotransmitter Consequence of isolation rearing

Dopamine Basal DA: in NAcc unchanged (Fulford and Marsden, 1998b; Hall et al., 1998a; Howes et al., 2000; Jones et al., 1992)
or increased (Heidbreder et al., 2000) in isolates which was reversed by handling (Hall et al., 1998b)
Foot shock induced DA release in NAcc increased and prolonged in isolates (Fulford and Marsden, 1998b)
Increased amphetamine-induced DA release in NAcc (Jones et al., 1992). No change in amphetamine-induced
release of DOPAC or 5HIAA (Hall et al., 1998c). Amphetamine induced LMA time course altered in
isolation (Jones et al., 1992)
Increased sensitivity to apomorphine induced sedation (Jones et al., 1992)
K+-induced DA release decreased in isolates; D2 receptors down-regulated in NAcc (Hall et al., 1998d)
Cocaine-induced DA efflux in NAcc potentiated by isolation (Howes et al., 2000)
Isolation rearing interacts with DA-mechanisms in NAcc to enhance reward-related stimuli (Jones et al., 1990)
DA depletion in NAcc reversed isolation induced PPI deficits (female Long–Evans) (Powell et al., 2003)
Number, affinity and efficacy of D2Rs in dorsal/ventral striatum unaltered by isolation (Del Arco et al., 2004)
Basal DA unchanged in PFC (Dalley et al., 2002), but turnover decreased in FC (Heidbreder et al., 2000)
In mPFC clozapine and olanzapine-induced DA release is increased in isolates (Heidbreder et al., 2001)

5-HT No effect on basal extracellular 5-HT in PFC (Dalley et al., 2002) or NAcc (Howes et al., 2000)
Amphetamaine-induced 5-HT release in PFC attenuated in isolated animals (Dalley et al., 2002)
KCl and novel environment induced FC 5-HT release absent in isolates (Bickerdike et al., 1993)
Decreased basal 5-HT turnover in NAcc (Heidbreder et al., 2000)
Footshock and subsequent exposure to footshock-context only increase 5-HT release in NAcc isolated
rats (Fulford and Marsden, 1998a)
Parachloroamphetamine and footshock increase 5-HT release in dorsal hippocampus of social, but not
isolated rats (Muchimapura et al., 2002)
Stress reduced 5-HT release in isolates only (Muchimapura et al., 2002)
Isolation-rearing in mice increased 5-HT1A binding. Males—in CA1, septum, amygdala and periaqueductal grey.
Females—dentate gyrus, supramammillary nucleus and ventromedial hypothalamus (Schiller et al., 2006)
Isolation alters presysnaptic 5-HT1B not post-synaptic hippocampal 5-HT1A receptors (Muchimapura et al., 2003).
Increased 5-HT2 receptor responsiveness in isolates (Wright et al., 1991a,b)
Isolation rearing increases 5-HT2A binding in prelimbic, motor and cingulate cortices. 5-HT1A binding is reduced
in prelimbic cortex and increased in motor & somatosensory cortices and dentate gyrus and CA of
hippocampus (Preece et al., 2004)
Increased 5-HT fibre density in dorsal caudate putamen, NAcc and amygdala in gerbils (Lehmann et al., 2003)
Circadian rhythms altered by isolation- 5-HT peak later, tryptophan and 5-HIAA earlier (Greco et al., 1989)

Noradrenaline Isolation has no effect on basal or K+-induced NA release in hippocampus or hypothalamus (in vitro slices).
Idazoxan-induced NA release greater in isolates hippocampus- suggests altered sensitivity of
presynaptic a2 autoreceptors (Fulford and Marsden, 1997a)
Unaltered basal but enhanced K+-stimulated dorsal hippocampal NA release. Isolation enhances
presynaptic a2 autoreceptor function of NA nerve terminals (Fulford and Marsden, 1997b)
Isolates show improved retention in the water maze normalised by NA depletion (Lapiz et al., 2001)
Hypothalamic synaptosomal NA uptake increased in isolates, decreased pons-medulla NA sensitive cAMP
production. Decreased b-adrenoceptors and increased a-adrenoceptors is in pons-medulla (Kraeuchi et al., 1981)
Post-synaptic a2 adrenoceptor activity unaltered but clonidine-induced hypoactivity increased suggesting
enhanced presynaptic a2 function with increased a2 receptors in isolates hippocampus (Fulford et al., 1994)

HPA-axis and corticosterone Basal plasma corticosterone (Scaccianoce et al., 2006) and ACTH unaffected by isolation (Schrijver et al., 2002)
Corticosterone increased in isolates house on grid floors only (Heidbreder et al., 2000)
Isolation decreases plasma ACTH. CRF icv augments increased plasma corticosterone in isolates and dexamethasone
suppression is attenuated, suggesting impaired negative feedback of HPA axis (Serra et al., 2005)
Male isolates exhibit increased basal ACTH and enhanced stress-induced release of ACTH and corticosterone.
Females isolates had unaltered ACTH and corticosterone, normal adrenal weight and unaltered hippocampal
MR and GR mRNA (Weiss et al., 2004)

GABA Isolated mice have reduced allopregnanolone (+modulator of GABA) and increased susceptibility
to picrotoxin (GABA A antagonist) seizures (Matsumoto et al., 2003)
In isolation-reared rats, reduced number of parvalbumin and calbindin positive hippocampal
GABAergic interneurones (Harte et al., 2006, 2007)

Glutamate NMDAR1A mRNA unaltered in striatum, hippocampus and FC (in fawn-hooded and Wistars)
(Hall et al., 2002), decreased mGluR1 and mGluR5 protein in the dorsal PFC (Melendez et al., 2004),
increased mGluR6 and AMPA3 inotropic glutamate receptor subunits in the
medial PFC of Sprague–Dawley rats (Levine et al., 2007)

Opiate receptors Isolates do not show morphine induced place-preference (Wongwitdecha and Marsden, 1996a,b,c)
mPFC Opioid binding increased in isolates (only 7 days isolation in adult rats) (Vanderschuren et al., 1995)

Histamine Isolated H1 KO mice fail to exhibit PPI impairments (Dai et al., 2005)
Electrophysiology LTP in the CA1 - subiculum pathway reduced in isolates (Roberts and Greene, 2003)
Pyramidal neurones in PFC show abnormal firing and short hyperpolarisation (Peters and O’Donnell, 2005)

Morphology mPFC volume decreased, no change in neurone number (Day-Wilson et al., 2006)
Decreased dendritic length of pyramidal calles from CA1. Density of dendritic spines decreased in pyramidal
cells from mPFC and hippocampus (Silva-Gomez et al., 2003)
Decreased spine density in dorsolateral striatum compared to rats reared in enriched
environment (Comery et al., 1995; Comery et al., 1996)
Hippocampal cytoskeletal alterations suggestive of microtubule stabilization. Decreased MAP2
expression (Bianchi et al., 2006)
Isolation (PND18-32) decreased neuronal dendritic arborisation and increased VIP neurons, Not reversed
by resocialisation (Pascual et al., 2006)
K.C.F. Fone, M.V. Porkess / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32 (2008) 1087–1102 1097

Table 3 (Continued )
Neurotransmitter Consequence of isolation rearing

N-Acetyl aspartate Decreased NAA in temporal cortex, but no changes in hippocampus, FC or striatum (Harte et al., 2004)
CDCrel-1 Presynaptic protein modulating DA transmission. Protein decreased in striatum and increased in hippocampus.
Isolation also caused loss of relationship between CDCrel-1 and syntaxin (Barr et al., 2004)

Synaptophysin No changes seen (Barr et al., 2004)


Decreased in dentate gyrus (Varty et al., 1999a,b)

BDNF Decreased hippocampal BDNF (rats adult when isolated) (Scaccianoce et al., 2006)

the motor cortex and hippocampus (Preece et al., 2004). In addition, rearing Sprague–Dawley rats in isolation from PND21 on
agreement with this proposal 30 days isolation rearing in Lister wire grids without handling produced a specific decrease in
hooded rats elevated the wet-dog shakes and back muscle mGluR1 and mGluR5 levels in the dorsal PFC but not in the ventral
contractions elicited by systemic administration of a 5-HT2A PFC or striatum (Melendez et al., 2004). Furthermore, while there
receptor agonist and enhanced 5-HT1A receptor mediated flat body was no change in basal glutamate release in the PFC of isolates,
posture and reciprocal forepaw treading (Wright et al., 1991a). A measured by microdialysis, there was a blunted elevation in
possible cause of this apparent post-synaptic 5-HT receptor release associated with administration of the mGluR1 agonist
supersensitivity could be reduced 5-HT release. Indeed, Bickerdike (DHPG) or mGluR2 antagonist (LY341495) (Melendez et al., 2004).
et al. (1993) found evidence of attenuated 5-HT release in cortical This reduction in PFC glutamate was accompanied by impaired
and hippocampal microdialysates evoked by either potassium or performance of isolation-reared rats in a T-maze delayed alter-
elevated plus maze exposure, respectively, in isolation compared nation task, showing a deficit in working memory performance.
with social housed Lister hooded rats. Similarly, although basal 5- Although this evidence is at a preliminary stage it would appear
HT release in the frontal cortex measured using in vivo micro- that isolation rearing is associated with glutamatergic hypofunc-
dialysis in the conscious Lister hooded rat was unaltered, the tion in the PFC analogous to that reported in schizophrenia.
normal elevation in 5-HT release induced by systemic ampheta-
mine was attenuated in isolation compared with group reared rats 5.2. Other neurochemical changes
(Dalley et al., 2002). Furthermore, 5-HT1A function (measured by
[35S]GTPgammaS binding) was increased in the dorsal raphe Since the development of synaptic connectivity and the
nucleus of isolation-reared CB57BL/6J mice (Advani et al., 2007), neurochemical phenotype of neurones in the rodent brain are
consistent with heightened presynaptic autoreceptor inhibition of incomplete until well after weaning, it is not surprising that early
serotonergic neuronal function. Isolation rearing caused an post-natal social deprivation alters brain development. For
increased anxiogenic response to the 5-HT agonist, meta- instance, most of the monoamine neurotransmitters and their
chlorophenylpiperazine (mCPP), on the elevated plus maze associated receptor compliment continue to change, only reaching
compared to socially reared animals, which has been attributed the adult composition between PND 30 and 50 of age in the rat. The
to increased 5-HT2C receptor responsiveness, possibly in the hippocampus and PFC show a number of alterations indicative of
hippocampus (Fone et al., 1996). change in plasticity as a consequence of social isolation at weaning
Although there have been fewer studies on noradrenergic (Fig. 1 and Table 3). Comery et al. found reduced spine density and
function in isolation-reared rats (Table 3), early studies found an dendritic branching measured by Golgi-Cox staining on medium
increase in noradrenaline turnover in the hippocampus, cerebel- sized spiny neurones in the corpus striatum and Sila-Gomez
lum and cortex of Wistar rats (Miachon et al., 1993). Consistent reported a similar reduction in pyramidal neurones of the PFC and
with this observation isolation-reared rats also appear to have hippocampus (Comery et al., 1995, 1996; Silva-Gomez et al., 2003).
enhanced presynaptic a2-adrenoceptor autoreceptor function in These changes are consistent with isolation rearing producing
the hippocampus (Fulford and Marsden, 1997a,b). persistent morphological alterations in rats (Comery et al., 1995,
1996; Silva-Gomez et al., 2003). Just like the isolation behavioural
5.1.3. Glutamate syndrome these reductions in dendritic branching in the PFC
Very few studies have examined the effect of isolation on (Pascual et al., 2007) are not reversed by 30 days of re-socialisation
glutamatergic neurotransmission in the brain, which is clearly of in Sprague–Dawley rats (Pascual et al., 2006), but appear to
great value to clarify the translational relevance of the neurobio- partially recover following chronic administration with deprenyl
logical changes induced by isolation to those seen in schizophrenia (Pascual and Zamora-Leon, 2007). Consistent with this rearing-
(Table 3). However, studies have found a reduction in NMDAR1A induced loss of neuropil, Lister hooded rats reared in isolation for 8
mRNA expression by in situ hybridization in Fawn hooded rats weeks show a 7% decrease in medial PFC volume (without any
which is potentiated by isolation rearing in some areas of the change in neuronal number measured by stereology) which is
striatum and PFC (Hall et al., 2002), and could contribute to the accompanied by deficits in PPI (Day-Wilson et al., 2006). Our group
observed enhanced DA release in the NAcc. Indeed sub-chronic has also recently reported a small but significant decrease in PFC
administration of the non-competitive NMDA receptor antagonist, volume measure by MRI in isolation-reared Lister hooded rats
phencyclidine, appears to enhance the isolation-induced locomo- accompanied by the characteristic isolation behavioural syndrome
tor hyperactivity (Lapiz et al., 2003) supporting the proposal that a (Porkess et al., 2007). Furthermore, a reduction in cortical
deficit in glutamatergic innervation of the NAcc may contribute to thickness has also been found in isolation-reared rats (Hellemans
the enhanced presynaptic DA release seen in this brain area. et al., 2004).
One recent study using microarray analysis reported an Numerous markers of synaptic function appear to be altered by
increase in mGluR6 and AMPA3 inotropic glutamate receptor isolation rearing reminiscent of alteration in synaptic plasticity
subunits in the medial PFC following 26 days isolation of Sprague– (Table 3). Synaptophysin, a synapse specific protein associated
Dawley rats (Levine et al., 2007). However, these finding require with presynatic release of neurotransmitters, was reduced in the
replication as only six rats were used in each rearing condition. In molecular layer of the dentate gyrus after 8 weeks of isolation in
1098 K.C.F. Fone, M.V. Porkess / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32 (2008) 1087–1102

Lister hooded rats (Varty et al., 1999b). Although this technique 10 min open-field test isolation-reared rats have lower plasma
does not permit analysis of the neurotransmitter phenotype of corticosterone than socially housed rats (Gentsch et al., 1981a).
affected neurones, it suggests that isolation also reduces the gross In contrast, the ACTH and corticosterone response to an acoustic
synaptic density in this hippocampal subfield, which receives a startle stress was enhanced in both male and female Sprague–
dense innervation from the entorhinal cortex. Consistent with this Dawley rats, consistent with a hyper-responsivity to stress
suggestion, isolation rearing appears to induce dynamic instability accompanied by a mild hyperfunction of the HPA-axis, which
of microtubules in the hippocampus, as suggested by a decrease in again may depend on strain, duration of isolation and severity of
hippocampal tyrosinated a-tubulin (Tyr-Tub) and a parallel the stress experienced.
increase of detyrosinated a-tubulin (Glu-Tub) together with a
decrease in the Tyr/Glu-Tub ratio without any gross change in total 6. Social isolation as a model of core symptoms of
a-tubulin content (Bianchi et al., 2006). Furthermore, this was developmental neuropsychological disorders
accompanied by a decrease in the neuronal-specific dendritic
marker, microtubule associated protein-2 (Bianchi et al., 2006), As extensively reviewed elsewhere, no single animal model can
suggesting impaired dendritic development, which could account possibly replicate all the myriad of symptoms associated with
for the cognitive inflexibility seen in isolation-reared rats. schizophrenia, which in itself is a heterogeneous, polygenetic,
Following 3 months of isolation of Wistar rats Miachon et al. disorder influenced by early-life adverse environment (Gainetdi-
(1990) reported a 70% increase in [3H]flunitrazepam binding nov et al., 2001; Lipska and Weinberger, 2000). Furthermore, no
affinity in hippocampal tissue which they speculated might be due single behavioural paradigm is uniquely pertinent to schizophre-
to alteration in endogenous levels of allosteric modulators of the nia. Therefore it is clearly inappropriate to describe isolation-
GABA receptor. Indeed a similar isolation-induced change in reared rats as a model for schizophrenia. Nonetheless, isolation
hippocampal benzodiazepine binding was reported by Petkov and rearing of rats from weaning produces reproducible, long-term
Yanev (1982). More recently 11 weeks of isolation rearing in the changes in behaviour including; neophobia, impaired sensorimo-
female Sprague–Dawley rat has been found to produce reduced tor gating, social withdrawal and cognitive inflexibility which
numbers of the parvalbumin and calbindin (but not calretinin) cover the three domains affected in schizophrenia and is a desired
positive subsets of GABAergic interneurones within the hippo- feature of a suitable rodent paradigm to investigate the neurobiol-
campus (Harte et al., 2006, 2007) indicative of a developmental ogy of the disorder (Powell and Miyakawa, 2006). In addition the
alteration in local hippocampal ciruits. These changes in isolation- isolation-induced behavioural syndrome is accompanied by
reared rats agree well with substantial indirect evidence of reductions in PFC volume and hippocampal synaptic plasticity,
functional impairment of inhibitory GABAergic interneurones in hyperfunction of mesolimbic dopaminergic systems and hypo-
the hippocampus seen in post-mortem schizophrenic brains. function of mesocortical DA which also resemble abnormalities
Eight weeks isolation of 2-month-old Sprague–Dawley rats also seen in schizophrenia (Fig. 1). This favours the characterisation of a
results in a selective elevation of hippocampal BDNF measured by battery of behavioural, neuroanatomical and neurochemical
ELISA with no concomitant alteration in the PFC or striatum measures with robust translational relevance to core defects in
(Scaccianoce et al., 2006). In contrast, in Fawn hooded rats, BDNF schizophrenia (positive, negative and cognitive impairments) in
mRNA is decreased in the dentate gyrus but increased in the rats reared in social isolation to investigate the neurodevelopment
retrosplenial cortex (Djouma et al., 2006). Thus changes in aetiology of this disorder to identify longitudinal biomarkers of
hippocampal neuronal morphology may result from accompany- dysfunction and serve as a predictive screen for novel compounds
ing changes in neurotrophic factors induced by the isolation with potential antipsychotic efficacy.
procedure. Consistent with these findings, neurogenesis appears to
References
be significantly reduced in isolation-reared rats, as was LTP in the
CA1 region of the hippocampus (Lu et al., 2003a). As predicted Advani, T., Hensler, J.G., Koek, W., 2007. Effect of early rearing conditions on alcohol
these rats were impaired at spatial learning in the water maze, drinking and 5-HT1A receptor function in C57BL/6J mice. Int. J. Neuropsycho-
even though this conflicts with results from other groups pharmacol. 10, 595–607.
Ago, Y., Takahashi, K., Nakamura, S., Hashimoto, H., Baba, A., Matsuda, T., 2007.
(Wongwitdecha and Marsden, 1996b). Anxiety-like and exploratory behaviors of isolation-reared mice in the staircase
The level of N-acetyl aspartic acid (NAA), a marker of neuronal test. J. Pharmacol. Sci. 104, 153–158.
integrity, is reduced in the temporal cortex of isolated rats, Aleman, A., Kahn, R.S., Selten, J.P., 2003. Sex differences in the risk of schizophrenia—
evidence from meta-analysis. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 60, 565–571.
indicating possible neuronal loss or dysfunction, while no similar
Alexander, B.K., Coambs, R.B., Hadaway, P.F., 1978. Effect of housing and gender on
change has been reported in the hippocampus, striatum or frontal morphine self-administration in rats. Psychopharmacology 58, 175–179.
cortex (Harte et al., 2004). This closely mimics the change seen in Anisman, H., Matheson, K., 2005. Stress, depression, and anhedonia: caveats con-
schizophrenia where post-mortem NAA reductions occur in the cerning animal models. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 525–546.
Arakawa, H., 2005. Interaction between isolation rearing and social development on
temporal but not frontal cortex. exploratory behavior in male rats. Behav. Process. 70, 223–234.
Finally, the electrophysiological properties of some neurones Arakawa, H., 2007. Ontogenetic interaction between social relationships and
are altered in isolation-reared rats. For instance, hippocampal defensive burying behavior in the rat. Physiol. Behav. 90, 751–759.
Bakshi, V.P., Geyer, M.A., 1999. Ontogeny of isolation rearing-induced deficits in
pyramidal neurones show a reduced action potential height and sensorimotor gating in rats. Physiol. Behav. 67, 385–392.
increased action potential threshold, with no changes in resting Bakshi, V.P., Swerdlow, N.R., Braff, D.L., Geyer, M.A., 1998. Reversal of isolation
membrane potential (Greene et al., 2001). Altered long-term rearing-induced deficits in prepulse inhibition by seroquel and olanzapine. Biol.
Psychiatry 43, 436–445.
potentiation has also been seen in the CA1 to subiculum pathway Bardo, M.T., Hammer, R.P., 1991. Autoradiographic localization of dopamine-D1 and
of isolation-reared rats (Roberts and Greene, 2003). D2-receptors in rat nucleus-accumbens—resistance to differential rearing con-
Basal corticosterone levels are not altered by social isolation, ditions. Neuroscience 45, 281–290.
Bardo, M.T., Bowling, S.L., Rowlett, J.K., Manderscheid, P., Buxton, S.T., Dwoskin, L.P.,
if the animals are housed in standard cages (Schrijver et al., 2002) 1995. Environmental enrichment attenuates locomotor sensitization, but not
but are only elevated by more aversive isolation in wire floor in-vitro dopamine release, induced by amphetamine. Pharmacol. Biochem.
cages (Heidbreder et al., 2000). Neither is there any effect of Behav. 51, 397–405.
Barr, A.M., Young, C.E., Sawada, K., Trimble, W.S., Phillips, A.G., Honer, W.G., 2004.
isolation on hippocampal glucocorticoid or mineralocorticoid
Abnormalities of presynaptic protein CDCrel-1 in striatum of rats reared in
receptor mRNA or adrenal gland weight but levels of ACTH may social isolation: relevance to neural connectivity in schizophrenia. Eur. J.
be elevated (Miachon et al., 1993; Weiss et al., 2004). After a Neurosci. 20, 303–307.
K.C.F. Fone, M.V. Porkess / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32 (2008) 1087–1102 1099

Barr, A.M., Powell, S.B., Markou, A., Geyer, M.A., 2006. Iloperidone reduces sensor- Einon, D.F., Morgan, M.J., 1977. A critical period for social isolation in the rat. Dev.
imotor gating deficits in pharmacological models, but not a developmental Psychobiol. 10, 123–132.
model, of disrupted prepulse inhibition in rats. Neuropharmacology 51, 457– Einon, D.F., Sahakian, B.J., 1979. Environmentally induced differences in suscept-
465. ibility of rats to CNS stimulants and CNS depressants—evidence against a
Baruch, I., Hemsley, D.R., Gray, J.A., 1988. Differential performance of acute and unitary explanation. Psychopharmacology 61, 299–307.
chronic-schizophrenics in a latent inhibition task. J. Nerv. Men. Dis. 176, 598– Elliott, R., McKenna, P.J., Robbins, T.W., Sahakian, B.J., 1995. Neuropsychological
606. evidence for frontostriatal dysfunction in schizophrenia. Psychol. Med. 25, 619–
Bianchi, M., Fone, K.F.C., Azmi, N., Heidbreder, C.A., Hagan, J.J., Marsden, C.A., 2006. 630.
Isolation rearing induces recognition memory deficits accompanied by cytos- Ennaceur, A., Delacour, J., 1988. A new one-trial test for neurobiological studies of
keletal alterations in rat hippocampus. Eur. J. Neurosci. 24, 2894–2902. memory in rats. 1. Behavioral data. Behav. Brain Res. 31, 47–59.
Bickerdike, M.J., Wright, I.K., Marsden, C.A., 1993. Social isolation attenuates rat Feldon, J., Weiner, I., 1992. From an animal-model of an attentional deficit towards
forebrain 5-HT release induced by KCI stimulation and exposure to a novel new insights into the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. J. Psychiat. Res. 26,
environment. Behav. Pharmacol. 4, 231–236. 345–366.
Binder, E.B., Kinkead, B., Owens, M.J., Kilts, C.D., Nemeroff, C.B., 2001. Enhanced Ferdman, N., Murmu, R.P., Bock, J., Braun, K., Leshem, M., 2007. Weaning age, social
neurotensin neurotransmission is involved in the clinically relevant behavioral isolation, and gender, interact to determine adult explorative and social beha-
effects of antipsychotic drugs: evidence from animal models of sensorimotor vior, and dendritic and spine morphology in prefrontal cortex of rats. Behav.
gating. J. Neurosci. 21, 601–608. Brain Res. 180, 174–182.
Birrell, J.M., Brown, V.J., 2000. Medial frontal cortex mediates perceptual attentional Fiala, B., Snow, F.M., Greenough, W.T., 1977. Impoverished rats weigh more than
set shifting in the rat. J. Neurosci. 20, 4320–4324. enriched rats because they eat more. Dev. Psychobiol. 10, 537–541.
Bozarth, M.A., Murray, A., Wise, R.A., 1989. Influence of housing conditions on the File, S.E., 1978. Exploration, distraction, and habituation in rats reared in isolation.
acquisition of intravenous heroin and cocaine self-administration in rats. Dev. Psychobiol. 11, 73–81.
Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 33, 903–907. Fone, K.C., Shalders, K., Fox, Z.D., Arthur, R., Marsden, C.A., 1996. Increased 5-HT2C
Bozikas, V.P., Kosmidis, M.H., Kiosseoglou, G., Karavatos, A., 2006. Neuropsycholo- receptor responsiveness occurs on rearing rats in social isolation. Psychophar-
gical profile of cognitively impaired patients with schizophrenia. Comp. Psy- macology (Berl.) 123, 346–352.
chiatry 47, 136–143. Fulford, A.J., Butler, S., Heal, D.J., Kendall, D.A., Marsden, C.A., 1994. Evidence for
Braff, D.L., Grillon, C., Geyer, M.A., 1992. Gating and habituation of the startle reflex altered alpha 2-adrenoceptor function following isolation-rearing in the rat.
in schizophrenic-patients. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 49, 206–215. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 116, 183–190.
Braff, D.L., Geyer, M.A., Swerdlow, N.R., 2001. Human studies of prepulse inhibition Fulford, A.J., Marsden, C.A., 1997a. Effect of isolation-rearing on noradrenaline
of startle: normal subjects, patient groups, and pharmacological studies. Psy- release in rat hypothalamus and hippocampus in vitro. Brain Res. 748, 93–
chopharmacology 156, 234–258. 99.
Chudasama, Y., Robbins, T.W., 2004. Psychopharmacological approaches to mod- Fulford, A.J., Marsden, C.A., 1997b. Social isolation in the rat enhances alpha(2)-
ulating attention in the five-choice serial reaction time task: implications for autoreceptor function in the hippocampus in vivo. Neuroscience 77, 57–64.
schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology 174, 86–98. Fulford, A.J., Marsden, C.A., 1998a. Conditioned release of 5-hydroxytryptamine in
Cilia, J., Reavill, C., Hagan, J.J., Jones, D.N.C., 2001. Long-term evaluation of isolation- vivo in the nucleus accumbens following isolation-rearing in the rat. Neu-
rearing induced prepulse inhibition deficits in rats. Psychopharmacology 156, roscience 83, 481–487.
327–337. Fulford, A.J., Marsden, C.A., 1998b. Effect of isolation-rearing on conditioned dopa-
Cilia, J., Cluderay, J.E., Robbins, M.J., Reavill, C., Southam, E., Kew, J.N., Jones, D.N., mine release in vivo in the nucleus accumbens of the rat. J. Neurochem. 70, 384–
2005a. Reversal of isolation-rearing-induced PPI deficits by an alpha7 nicotinic 390.
receptor agonist. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 182, 214–219. Fulford, A.J., Marsden, C.A., 2007. An intact dopaminergic system is required for
Cilia, J., Hatcher, P.D., Reavill, C., Jones, D.N.C., 2005b. Long-term evaluation of context-conditioned release of 5-HT in the nucleus accumbens of postweaning
isolation-rearing induced prepulse inhibition deficits in rats: an update. Psy- isolation-reared rats. Neuroscience 149, 392–400.
chopharmacology 180, 57–62. Gainetdinov, R.R., Mohn, A.R., Caron, M.G., 2001. Genetic animal models: focus on
Comery, T.A., Shah, R., Greenough, W.T., 1995. Differential rearing alters spine schizophrenia. Trends Neurosci. 24, 527–533.
density on medium-sized spiny neurons in the rat corpus striatum—evidence Gentsch, C., Lichtsteiner, M., Feer, H., 1981a. Locomotor activity, defecation score
for association of morphological plasticity with early response gene-expression. and corticosterone levels during an openfield exposure: a comparison among
Neurobiol. Learn. Memory 63, 217–219. individually and group-housed rats, and genetically selected rat lines. Physiol.
Comery, T.A., Stamoudis, C.X., Irwin, S.A., Greenough, W.T., 1996. Increased density Behav. 27, 183–186.
of multiple-head dendritic spines on medium-sized spiny neurons of the Gentsch, C., Lichtsteiner, M., Feer, H., 1981b. Taste neophobia in individually and
striatum in rats reared in a complex environment. Neurobiol. Learn. Memory socially reared male-rats. Physiol. Behav. 27, 199–202.
66, 93–96. Gentsch, C., Lichtsteiner, M., Feer, H., 1982a. Behavioural comparisons between
Dai, H., Okuda, T., Sakurai, E., Kuramasu, A., Kato, M., Jia, F., Xu, A.J., Iinuma, K., Sato, individually- and group-housed male rats: effects of novel environments and
I., Yanai, K., 2005. Blockage of histamine H1 receptor attenuates social isolation- diurnal rhythm. Behav. Brain Res. 6, 93–100.
induced disruption of prepulse inhibition: a study in H1 receptor gene knockout Gentsch, C., Lichtsteiner, M., Kraeuchi, K., Feer, H., 1982b. Different reaction patterns
mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 183, 285–293. in individually and socially reared rats during exposures to novel environments.
Dalley, J.W., Theobald, D.E., Pereira, E.A.C., Li, P., Robbins, T.W., 2002. Specific Behav. Brain Res. 4, 45–54.
abnormalities in serotonin release in the prefrontal cortex of isolation-reared Gentsch, C., Lichtsteiner, M., Feer, H., 1983. Individually housed rats exceed group-
rats measured during behavioural performance of a task assessing visuospatial housed animals in rotational movements when exposed to a novel environ-
attention and impulsivity. Psychopharmacology 164, 329–340. ment. Experientia 39, 1412–1414.
Dalley, J.W., Cardinal, R.N., Robbins, T.W., 2004. Prefrontal executive and cognitive Gentsch, C., Lichtsteiner, M., Frischknecht, H.R., Feer, H., Siegfried, B., 1988. Isola-
functions in rodents: neural and neurochemical substrates. Neurosci. Biobehav. tion-induced locomotor hyperactivity and hypoalgesia in rats are prevented by
Rev. 28, 771–784. handling and reversed by resocialization. Physiol. Behav. 43, 13–16.
Day-Wilson, K.M., Jones, D.N.C., Southam, E., Cilia, J., Totterdell, S., 2006. Medial Geyer, M.A., Wilkinson, L.S., Humby, T., Robbins, T.W., 1993. Isolation rearing of rats
prefrontal cortex volume loss in rats with isolation rearing-induced deficits in produces a deficit in prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle similar to that in
prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle. Neuroscience 141, 1113–1121. schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 34, 361–372.
Deatherage, G., 1972. Effect of housing density on alcohol intake in the rat. Physiol. Geyer, M.A., Krebs-Thomson, K., Varty, G.B., 1999. The effects of M100907 in
Behav. 9, 55–57. pharmacological and developmental animal models of prepulse inhibition
Deehan, G.A., Cain, M.E., Kiefer, S.W., 2007. Differential rearing conditions alter deficits in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 21, S134–S142.
operant responding for ethanol in outbred rats. Alcohol.-Clin. Exp. Res. 31, Geyer, M.A., Krebs-Thomson, K., Braff, D.L., Swerdlow, N.R., 2001. Pharmacological
1692–1698. studies of prepulse inhibition models of sensorimotor gating deficits in schizo-
Del Arco, A., Zhu, S.W., Terasmaa, A., Mohammed, A.H., Fuxe, K., 2004. Hyperactivity phrenia: a decade in review. Psychopharmacology 156, 117–154.
to novelty induced by social isolation is not correlated with changes in D2 Gray, J.A., Roth, B.L., 2007. Molecular targets for treating cognitive dysfunction in
receptor function and binding in striatum. Psychopharmacology 171, 148–155. schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 33, 1100–1119.
Dias, R., Robbins, T.W., Roberts, A.C., 1996. Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of Gray, N.S., Pilowsky, L.S., Gray, J.A., Kerwin, R.W., 1995. Latent inhibition in drug-
affective and attentional shifts. Nature 380, 69–72. naive schizophrenics—relationship to duration of illness and dopamine D2
Djouma, E., Card, K., Lodge, D.J., Lawrence, A.J., 2006. The CRF1 receptor antagonist, binding using SPET. Schizophr. Res. 17, 95–107.
antalarmin, reverses isolation-induced up-regulation of dopamine D-2 recep- Greco, A.M., Gambardella, P., Sticchi, R., D’Aponte, D., Di Renzo, G., De Franciscis, P.,
tors in the amygdala and nucleus accumbens of Fawn–Hooded rats. Eur. J. 1989. Effects of individual housing on circadian rhythms of adult rats. Physiol.
Neurosci. 23, 3319–3327. Behav. 45, 363–366.
Domeney, A., Feldon, J., 1998. The disruption of prepulse inhibition by social Greene, J.R.T., Kerkhoff, J.E., Guiver, L., Totterdell, S., 2001. Structural and functional
isolation in the Wistar rat: how robust is the effect? Pharmacol. Biochem. abnormalities of the hippocampal formation in rats with environmentally
Behav. 59, 883–890. induced reductions in prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle. Neuroscience
D’Souza, D.C., Abi-Saab, W.M., Madonick, S., Forselius-Bielen, K., Doersch, A., Braley, 103, 315–323.
G., Gueorguieva, R., Cooper, T.B., Krystal, J.H., 2005. Delta-9-tetrahydrocanna- Hadaway, P.F., Alexander, B.K., Coambs, R.B., Beyerstein, B., 1979. Effect of housing
binol effects in schizophrenia: implications for cognition, psychosis, and addic- and gender on preference for morphine-sucrose solutions in rats. Psychophar-
tion. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 594–608. macology 66, 87–91.
1100 K.C.F. Fone, M.V. Porkess / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32 (2008) 1087–1102

Hall, F.S., Humby, T., Wilkinson, L.S., Robbins, T.W., 1997a. The effects of isolation- Jones, G.H., Marsden, C.A., Robbins, T.W., 1990. Increased sensitivity to ampheta-
rearing of rats on behavioural responses to food and environmental novelty. mine and reward-related stimuli following social isolation in rats: possible
Physiol. Behav. 62, 281–290. disruption of dopamine-dependent mechanisms of the nucleus accumbens.
Hall, F.S., Humby, T., Wilkinson, L.S., Robbins, T.W., 1997b. The effects of isolation- Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 102, 364–372.
rearing on preference by rats for a novel environment. Physiol. Behav. 62, 299– Jones, G.H., Marsden, C.A., Robbins, T.W., 1991. Behavioural rigidity and rule-
303. learning deficits following isolation-rearing in the rat: neurochemical corre-
Hall, F.S., Humby, T., Wilkinson, L.S., Robbins, T.W., 1997c. The effects of isolation- lates. Behav. Brain Res. 43, 35–50.
rearing on sucrose consumption in rats. Physiol. Behav. 62, 291–297. Jones, G.H., Hernandez, T.D., Kendall, D.A., Marsden, C.A., Robbins, T.W., 1992.
Hall, F.S., Huang, S., Fong, G.F., Pert, A., 1998a. The effects of social isolation on the Dopaminergic and serotonergic function following isolation rearing in rats:
forced swim test in Fawn hooded and Wistar rats. J. Neurosci. Methods 79, 47– study of behavioural responses and postmortem and in vivo neurochemistry.
51. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 43, 17–35.
Hall, F.S., Huang, S., Fong, G.W., Pert, A., Linnoila, M., 1998b. Effects of isolation- Katz, D.M., Steinber, H., 1970. Long-term isolation in rats reduces morphine
rearing on voluntary consumption of ethanol, sucrose and saccharin solutions response. Nature 228, 469–471.
in Fawn Hooded and Wistar rats. Psychopharmacology 139, 210–216. Kennett, G.A., Chaouloff, F., Marcou, M., Curzon, G., 1986. Female rats are more
Hall, F.S., Huang, S., Fong, G.W., Pert, A., Linnoila, M., 1998c. Effects of isolation vulnerable than males in an animal-model of depression—the possible role of
rearing on locomotion, anxiety and responses to ethanol in Fawn Hooded and serotonin. Brain Res. 382, 416–421.
Wistar rats. Psychopharmacology 139, 203–209. King, M.V., Fone, K.C.F., Shacham, S., Gannon, K.S., 2007. The novel 5-HT6 receptor
Hall, F.S., Wilkinson, L.S., Humby, T., Inglis, W., Kendall, D.A., Marsden, C.A., Robbins, antagonist, PRX-07037, enhances memory and reduces food intake in a neu-
T.W., 1998d. Isolation rearing in rats: pre- and postsynaptic changes in striatal rodevelopmental model of schizophrenia. J. Psychopharmacol. 21, A57.
dopaminergic systems. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 59, 859–872. Koch, M.D., Arnold, W.J., 1972. Effects of early social deprivation on emotionality in
Hall, F.S., Sundstrom, J.M., Lerner, J., Perth, A., 2001. Enhanced corticosterone release rats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 78, 391–399.
after a modified forced swim test in Fawn hooded rats is independent of rearing Kraeuchi, K., Gentsch, C., Feer, H., 1981. Individually reared rats: alteration in
experience. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 69, 629–634. noradrenergic brain functions. J. Neural. Transm. 50, 103–112.
Hall, F.S., Ghaed, S., Pert, A., Xing, G., 2002. The effects of isolation rearing on Krebs-Thomson, K., Giracello, D., Solis, A., Geyer, M.A., 2001. Post-weaning handling
glutamate receptor NMDAR1A mRNA expression determined by in situ hybri- attenuates isolation-rearing induced disruptions of prepulse inhibition in rats.
dization in Fawn hooded and Wistar rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 73, 185– Behav. Brain Res. 120, 221–224.
191. Krech, D., Rosenzweig, M.R., Bennett, E.L., 1962. Relations between chemistry and
Hanlon, F.M., Weisend, M.P., Hamilton, D.A., Jones, A.P., Thoma, R.J., Huang, M.X., problem-solving among rats raised in enriched and impoverished environ-
Martin, K., Yeo, R.A., Miller, G.A., Canive, J.M., 2006. Impairment on the hippo- ments. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 55, 801–807.
campal-dependent virtual Morris water task in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. Lapiz, M.D., Mateo, Y., Parker, T., Marsden, C., 2000. Effects of noradrenaline
87, 67–80. depletion in the brain on response on novelty in isolation-reared rats. Psycho-
Harlow, H.F., Dodswort.Ro, Harlow, M.K., 1965. Total social isolation in monkeys. pharmacology (Berl.) 152, 312–320.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 54, 90–97. Lapiz, M.D.S., Mateo, Y., Durkin, S., Parker, T., Marsden, C.A., 2001. Effects of central
Harmer, C.J., Phillips, G.D., 1998. Isolation rearing enhances acquisition in a con- noradrenaline depletion by the selective neurotoxin DSP-4 on the behaviour of
ditioned inhibition paradigm. Physiol. Behav. 65, 525–533. the isolated rat in the elevated plus maze and water maze. Psychopharmacology
Harte, M.K., Powell, S.B., Reynolds, L.M., Swerdlow, N.R., Geyer, M.A., Reynolds, G.P., 155, 251–259.
2004. Reduced N-acetylaspartate in the temporal cortex of rats reared in Lapiz, M.D., Fulford, A., Muchimapura, S., Mason, R., Parker, T., Marsden, C.A., 2003.
isolation. Biol. Psychiatry 56, 296–299. Influence of postweaning social isolation in the rat on brain development,
Harte, M.K., Powell, S.B., Swerdlow, N.R., Geyer, M.A., Reynolds, G.P., 2006. Deficits conditioned behavior, and neurotransmission. Neurosci. Behav. Physiol. 33, 13–
in parvalbumin and calbindin immunoreactivity in the hippocampus of isola- 29.
tion reared rats. Schizophr. Res. 81, 210–211. Lehmann, K., Lesting, J., Polascheck, D., Teuchert-Noodt, G., 2003. Serotonin fibre
Harte, M.K., Powell, S.B., Swerdlow, N.R., Geyer, M.A., Reynolds, G.P., 2007. Deficits densities in subcortical areas: differential effects of isolated rearing and
in parvalbumin and calbindin immunoreactive cells in the hippocampus of methamphetamine. Brain Res. Dev. Brain Res. 147, 143–152.
isolation reared rats. J. Neural Transm. 114, 893–898. Leng, A., Feldon, J., Ferger, B., 2004. Long-term social isolation and medial prefrontal
Hatch, A.M., Wiberg, G.S., Zawidzka, Z., Cann, M., Airth, J.M., Grice, H.C., 1965. cortex: dopaminergic and cholinergic neurotransmission. Pharmacol. Biochem.
Isolation syndrome in rat. Toxicol. Appl. Pharmacol. 7, 737–745. Behav. 77, 371–379.
Haut, M.W., Cahill, J., Cutlip, W.D., Stevenson, J.M., Makela, E.H., Bloomfield, S.M., Levine, J.B., Youngs, R.M., MacDonald, M.L., Chu, M., Leeder, A.D., Berthiaume, F.,
1996. On the nature of Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in schizo- Konradi, C., 2007. Isolation rearing and hyperlocomotion are associated with
phrenia. Psychiatry Res. 65, 15–22. reduced immediate early gene expression levels in the medial prefrontal cortex.
Heidbreder, C.A., Weiss, I.C., Domeney, A.M., Pryce, C., Homberg, J., Hedou, G., Neuroscience 145, 42–55.
Feldon, J., Moran, M.C., Nelson, P., 2000. Behavioral, neurochemical and endo- Li, N.X., Wu, X.H., Li, L., 2007. Chronic administration of clozapine alleviates
crinological characterization of the early social isolation syndrome. Neu- reversal-learning impairment in isolation-reared rats. Behav. Pharmacol. 18,
roscience 100, 749–768. 135–145.
Heidbreder, C.A., Foxton, R., Cilia, J., Hughes, Z.A., Shah, A.J., Atkins, A., Hunter, A.J., Lipska, B.K., Weinberger, D.R., 2000. To model a psychiatric disorder in animals:
Hagan, J.J., Jones, D.N., 2001. Increased responsiveness of dopamine to atypical, Schizophrenia as a reality test. Neuropsychopharmacology 23, 223–239.
but not typical antipsychotics in the medial prefrontal cortex of rats reared in Lu, L., Bao, G.B., Chen, H., Xia, P., Fan, X.L., Zhang, J.S., Pei, G., Ma, L., 2003a.
isolation. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 156, 338–351. Modification of hippocampal neurogenesis and neuroplasticity by social envir-
Heim, C., Plotsky, P.M., Nemeroff, C.B., 2004. Importance of studying the contribu- onments. Exp. Neurol. 183, 600–609.
tions of early adverse experience to neurobiological findings in depression. Lu, L., Shepard, J.D., Hall, F.S., Shaham, Y., 2003b. Effect of environmental stressors
Neuropsychopharmacology 29, 641–648. on opiate and psychostimulant reinforcement, reinstatement and discrimina-
Hellemans, K.G.C., Benge, L.C., Olmstead, M.C., 2004. Adolescent enrichment par- tion in rats: a review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 27, 457–491.
tially reverses the social isolation syndrome. Dev. Brain Res. 150, 103–115. Malone, D.T., Taylor, D.A., 2006. The effect of Delta(9)-tetrahydrocannabinol on
Hilakivi, L.A., Ota, M., Lister, R.G., 1989. Effect of isolation on brain monoamines and sensorimotor gating in socially isolated rats. Behav. Brain Res. 166, 101–109.
the behavior of mice in tests of exploration, locomotion, anxiety and behavioral Matsumoto, K., Nomura, H., Murakami, Y., Taki, K., Takahata, H., Watanabe, H., 2003.
despair. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 33, 371–374. Long-term social isolation enhances picrotoxin seizure susceptibility in mice:
Hirschfeld, R., Weissmann, M., 2002. Secondary risk factors for major depression up-regulatory role of endogenous brain allopregnanolone in GABAergic sys-
and bipolar disorder. In: Davis, K.L., Charney, D., Coyle, J.T., Nemeroff, C.B. tems. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 75, 831–835.
(Eds.), Neuropsychopharmacology: The Fifth Generation of Progress. Lippincott McClure, M.M., Romero, M.J., Bowie, C.R., Reichenberg, A., Harvey, P.D., Siever, L.J.,
Williams & Wilkin, Philadelphia, PA, pp. 1018–1025. 2007. Visual-spatial learning and memory in schizotypal personality disorder:
Holson, R.R., Scallet, A.C., Ali, S.F., Turner, B.B., 1991. Isolation stress revisited— continued evidence for the importance of working memory in the schizophre-
isolation-rearing effects depend on animal care methods. Physiol. Behav. 49, nia spectrum. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 22, 109–116.
1107–1118. Melendez, R.I., Gregory, M.L., Bardo, M.T., Kalivas, P.W., 2004. Impoverished rearing
Howes, S.R., Dalley, J.W., Morrison, C.H., Robbins, T.W., Everitt, B.J., 2000. Leftward environment alters metabotropic glutamate receptor expression and function
shift in the acquisition of cocaine self-administration in isolation-reared rats: in the prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology 1–8.
relationship to extracellular levels of dopamine, serotonin and glutamate in the Miachon, S., Manchon, M., Fromentin, J.R., Buda, M., 1990. Isolation-induced
nucleus accumbens and amygdala-striatal FOS expression. Psychopharmacol- changes in radioligand binding to benzodiazepine binding sites. Neurosci. Lett.
ogy (Berl.) 151, 55–63. 111, 246–251.
Hughes, R.N., Syme, L.A., 1972. The role of social isolation and sex in determining Miachon, S., Rochet, T., Mathian, B., Barbagli, B., Claustrat, B., 1993. Long-term
effects of chlordiazepoxide and methylphenidate on exploratory behaviour. isolation of Wistar rats alters brain monoamine turnover, blood corticosterone,
Psychopharmacologia 27, 359–366. and ACTH. Brain Res. Bull. 32, 611–614.
Jochum, T., Letzsch, A., Greiner, W., Wagner, G., Sauer, H., Bar, K.J., 2006. Influence of Moore, T.H.M., Zammit, S., Lingford-Hughes, A., Barnes, T.R.E., Jones, P.B., Burke, M.,
antipsychotic medication on pain perception in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. Lewis, G., 2007. Cannabis use and risk of psychotic or affective mental health
142, 151–156. outcomes: a systematic review. Lancet 370, 319–328.
Jones, G.H., Robbins, T.W., Marsden, C.A., 1989. Isolation-rearing retards the acqui- Morgan, M.J., 1973. Effects of post-weaning environment on learning in rat. Anim.
sition of schedule-induced polydipsia in rats. Physiol. Behav. 45, 71–77. Behav. 21, 429–442.
K.C.F. Fone, M.V. Porkess / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32 (2008) 1087–1102 1101

Morgan, M.J., Einon, D.F., Nicholas, D., 1975. The effects of isolation rearing on derived neurotrophic factor without altering plasma corticosterone. Behav.
behavioural inhibition in the rat. Quart. J. Exp. Psychol. 27, 615–634. Brain Res. 168, 323–325.
Morgan, M.J., Einon, D., Morris, R.G., 1977. Inhibition and isolation rearing in the rat: Schenk, S., Britt, M.D., Atalay, J., Charleson, S., 1982. Isolation rearing decreases
extinction and satiation. Physiol. Behav. 18, 1–5. opiate receptor-binding in rat-brain. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 16, 841–842.
Muchimapura, S., Fulford, A.J., Mason, R., Marsden, C.A., 2002. Isolation rearing in the Schenk, S., Hunt, T., Colle, L., Amit, Z., 1983. Isolation versus grouped housing in
rat disrupts the hippocampal response to stress. Neuroscience 112, 697–705. rats—differential-effects of low-doses of heroin in the place preference para-
Muchimapura, S., Mason, R., Marsden, C.A., 2003. Effect of isolation rearing on pre- digm. Life Sci. 32, 1129–1134.
and post-synaptic serotonergic function in the rat dorsal hippocampus. Synapse Schenk, S., Ellison, F., Hunt, T., Amit, Z., 1985. An examination of heroin conditioning
47, 209–217. in preferred and nonpreferred environments and in differentially housed
Nestor, P.G., Kubicki, M., Kuroki, N., Gurrera, R.J., Nimikiewicz, M., Shenton, M.E., mature and immature rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 22, 215–220.
McCarley, R.W., 2007. Episodic memory and neuroimaging of hippocampus and Schenk, S., Hunt, T., Klukowski, G., Amit, Z., 1987a. Isolation housing decreases the
fornix in chronic schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res.-Neuroimaging 155, 21–28. effectiveness of morphine in the conditioned taste-aversion paradigm. Psycho-
Nicole, L., Lesage, A., Lalonde, P., 1992. Lower incidence and increased male–female pharmacology 92, 48–51.
ratio in schizophrenia. Br. J. Psychiatry 161, 556–557. Schenk, S., Lacelle, G., Gorman, K., Amit, Z., 1987b. Cocaine self-administration in
Parker, V., Morinan, A., 1986. The socially-isolated rat as a model for anxiety. rats influenced by environmental-conditions—implications for the etiology of
Neuropharmacology 25, 663–664. drug-abuse. Neurosci. Lett. 81, 227–231.
Pascual, R., Zamora-Leon, S.P., 2007. Chronic ( )-deprenyl administration attenu- Schenk, S., Robinson, B., Amit, Z., 1988. Housing conditions fail to affect the
ates dendritic developmental impairment induced by early social isolation in intravenous self-administration of amphetamine. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav.
the rat. Dev. Neurosci. 29, 261–267. 31, 59–62.
Pascual, R., Zamora-Leon, S.P., Valero-Cabre, A., 2006. Effects of postweaning social Schenk, S., Gorman, K., Amit, Z., 1990. Age-dependent effects of isolation housing on
isolation and re-socialization on the expression of vasoactive intestinal peptide the self-administration of ethanol in laboratory rats. Alcohol 7, 321–326.
(VIP) and dendritic development in the medial prefrontal cortex of the rat. Acta Schiller, L., Jahkel, M., Oehler, J., 2006. The influence of sex and social isolation
Neurobiol. Exp. (Wars) 7–14. housing on pre- and postsynaptic 5-HT(1A) receptors. Brain Res. 1103, 76–
Pascual, R., Zamora-Leon, P., Catalan-Ahumada, M., Valero-Cabre, A., 2007. Early 87.
social isolation decreases the expression of calbindin D-28k and dendritic Schrijver, N.C.A., Wurbel, H., 2001. Early social deprivation disrupts attentional, but
branching in the medial prefrontal cortex of the rat. Int. J. Neurosci. 117, not affective, shifts in rats. Behav. Neurosci. 115, 437–442.
465–476. Schrijver, N.C.A., Bahr, N.I., Weiss, I.C., Wurbel, H., 2002. Dissociable effects of
Perry, W., Braff, D.L., 1994. Information-processing deficits and thought-disorder in isolation rearing and environmental enrichment on exploration, spatial
schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 151, 363–367. learning and HPA activity in adult rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 73,
Petkov, V.V., Yanev, S., 1982. Brain benzodiazepine receptor changes in rats with 209–224.
isolation syndrome. Pharmacol. Res. Commun. 14, 739–744. Schrijver, N.C.A., Pallier, P.N., Brown, V.J., Wurbel, H., 2004. Double dissociation of
Phillips, G.D., Howes, S.R., Whitelaw, R.B., Robbins, T.W., Everitt, B.J., 1994a. Isola- social and environmental stimulation on spatial learning and reversal learning
tion rearing impairs the reinforcing efficacy of intravenous cocaine or intraac- in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 152, 307–314.
cumbens D-amphetamine—impaired response to intraaccumbens D1 and D2/ Seeman, P., Schwarz, J., Chen, J.F., Szechtman, H., Perreault, M., McKnight, G.S.,
D3 dopamine-receptor antagonists. Psychopharmacology 115, 419–429. Roder, J.C., 2006. Psychosis pathways converge via D2(High) dopamine recep-
Phillips, G.D., Howes, S.R., Whitelaw, R.B., Wilkinson, L.S., Robbins, T.W., Everitt, B.J., tors. Synapse 60, 319–346.
1994b. Isolation rearing enhances the locomotor response to cocaine and a Shelley, H.P., 1965. Eating behavior: social facilitation or social inhibition? Psy-
novel environment, but impairs the intravenous self-administration of cocaine. chonomic Sci. 3, 521–522.
Psychopharmacology 115, 407–418. Serra, M., Pisu, M.G., Floris, I., Biggio, G., 2005. Social isolation-induced changes in
Porkess, M.V., Dhanji, H., Huan, S., Watson, D.J.G., Topham, I.A., Marsden, C.A., Fone, the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in the rat. Stress 8, 259–264.
K.C.F., 2006. Aniracetam has no effect on isolation rearing induced hyperactiv- Silva-Gomez, A.B., Rojas, D., Juarez, I., Flores, G., 2003. Decreased dendritic spine
ity, PPI deficits or passive avoidance impairment in rats, but does improve novel density on prefrontal cortical and hippocampal pyramidal neurons in post-
object recognition. J. Psychopharmacology 20, A46. weaning social isolation rats. Brain Res. 983, 128–136.
Porkess, M.V., Schubert, M.I., Auer, D.P., Fone, K.C.F., 2007. Isolation rearing of rats Sipes, T.E., Geyer, M.A., 1997. DOI disrupts prepulse inhibition of startle in rats via 5-
induces a reduction in prefrontal cortex volume measured by MR volumetry. J. HT2A receptors in the ventral pallidum. Brain Res. 761, 97–104.
Psychopharmacology 21, A57. Slotten, H.A., Kalinichev, M., Hagan, J.J., Marsden, C.A., Fone, K.C.F., 2006. Long-lasting
Porsolt, R.D., Lepichon, M., Jalfre, M., 1977. Depression—new animal-model sensi- changes in behavioural and neuroendocrine indices in the rat following neonatal
tive to antidepressant treatments. Nature 266, 730–732. maternal separation: gender-dependent effects. Brain Res. 1097, 123–132.
Porsolt, R.D., Anton, G., Blavet, N., Jalfre, M., 1978. Behavioral despair in rats—new Strous, R.D., Alvir, J.M.J., Robinson, D., Gal, G., Sheitman, B., Chakos, M., Lieberman, J.A.,
model sensitive to antidepressant treatments. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 47, 379–391. 2004. Premorbid functioning in schizophrenia: relation to baseline symptoms,
Powell, C.M., Miyakawa, T., 2006. Schizophrenia-relevant behavioral testing in treatment response, and medication side effects. Schizophr. Bull. 30, 265–278.
rodent models: a uniquely human disorder? Biol. Psychiatry 59, 1198–1207. Swerdlow, N.R., Geyer, M.A., 1998. Using an animal model of deficient sensorimotor
Powell, S.B., Swerdlow, N.R., Pitcher, L.K., Geyer, M.A., 2002. Isolation rearing- gating to study the pathophysiology and new treatments of schizophrenia.
induced deficits in prepulse inhibition and locomotor habituation are not Schizophr. Bull. 24, 285–301.
potentiated by water deprivation. Physiol. Behav. 77, 55–64. Swerdlow, N.R., Braff, D.L., Geyer, M.A., 1990. GABAergic projection from nucleus-
Powell, S.B., Geyer, M.A., Preece, M.A., Pitcher, L.K., Reynolds, G.P., Swerdlow, N.R., accumbens to ventral pallidum mediates dopamine-induced sensorimotor
2003. Dopamine depletion of the nucleus accumbens reverses isolation- gating deficits of acoustic startle in rats. Brain Res. 532, 146–150.
induced deficits in prepulse inhibition in rats. Neuroscience 119, 233–240. Syme, L.A., 1973. Social isolation at weaning—some effects on 2 measures of
Preece, M.A., Dalley, J.W., Theobald, D.E.H., Robbins, T.W., Reynolds, G.P., 2004. activity. Anim. Learn. Behav. 1, 161–163.
Region specific changes in forebrain 5-hydroxytryptamine(1A) and 5-hydro- Vale, A.L., Montgomery, A.M.J., 1997. Social interaction: responses to chlordiazep-
xytryptamine(2A) receptors in isolation-reared rats: An in vitro autoradiogra- oxide and the loss of isolation reared effects with paired-housing. Psychophar-
phy study. Neuroscience 123, 725–732. macology 133, 127–132.
Rapoport, J.L., Addington, A.M., Frangou, S., Psych, M., 2005. The neurodevelop- Valzelli, L., 1973. Isolation syndrome in mice. Psychopharmacologia 31, 305–320.
mental model of schizophrenia: update 2005. Mol. Psychiatry 10, 434–449. Valzelli, L., Bernasconi, S., 1976. Psychoactive drug effect on behavioral-changes
Robbins, T.W., Jones, G.H., Wilkinson, L.S., 1996. Behavioural and neurochemical induced by prolonged socio-environmental deprivation in rats. Psychol. Med. 6,
effects of early social deprivation in the rat. J. Psychopharmacol. 10, 39–47. 271–276.
Roberts, L., Greene, J.R.T., 2003. Post-weaning social isolation of rats leads to a Valzelli, L., Garattin, S., 1972. Biochemical and behavioral changes induced by
diminution of LTP in the CA1 to subiculum pathway. Brain Res. 991, 271–273. isolation in rats. Neuropharmacology 11, 17–22.
Rosa, M., Silva, R.C.B., Moura-de-Carvalho, F.T., Brandao, M.L., Guimaraes, F.S., Del Valzelli, L., Bernasconi, S., Gomba, P., 1974. Effect of isolation on some behavioral
Bel, E.A., 2005. Routine post-weaning handling of rats prevents isolation rear- characteristics in three strains of mice. Biol. Psychiatry. 9, 329–334.
ing-induced deficit in prepulse inhibition. Br. J. Med. Biol. Res. 38, 1691–1696. Vanderschuren, L.J., Stein, E.A., Wiegant, V.M., Van Ree, J.M., 1995. Social isolation
Ruiz, J.C., Soler, M.J., Fuentes, I., Tomas, P., 2007. Intellectual functioning and and social interaction alter regional brain opioid receptor binding in rats. Eur.
memory deficits in schizophrenia. Comp. Psychiatry 48, 276–282. Neuropsychopharmacol. 5, 119–127.
Sahakian, B.J., Robbins, T.W., 1977. Isolation-rearing enhances tail pinch-induced Varty, G.B., Geyer, M.A., 1998. Effects of isolation rearing on startle reactivity,
oral behavior in rats. Physiol. Behav. 18, 53–58. habituation, and prepulse inhibition in male Lewis, Sprague–Dawley, and
Sahakian, B.J., Robbins, T.W., Morgan, M.J., Iversen, S.D., 1975. The effects of Fischer F344 rats. Behav. Neurosci. 112, 1450–1457.
psychomotor stimulants on stereotypy and locomotor activity in socially- Varty, G.B., Higgins, G.A., 1995. Examination of drug-induced and isolation-induced
deprived and control rats. Brain Res. 84, 195–205. disruptions of prepulse inhibition as models to screen antipychotic drugs.
Sahakian, B.J., Robbins, T.W., Iversen, S.D., 1977. Effects of isolation rearing on Psychopharmacology 112, 15–26.
exploration in rat. Anim. Learn. Behav. 5, 193–198. Varty, G.B., Braff, D.L., Geyer, M.A., 1999a. Is there a critical developmental ‘window’
Sakaue, M., Ago, Y., Baba, A., Matsuda, T., 2003. The 5-HT1A receptor agonist MKC- for isolation rearing-induced changes in prepulse inhibition of the acoustic
242 reverses isolation rearing-induced deficits of prepulse inhibition in mice. startle response? Behav. Brain Res. 100, 177–183.
Psychopharmacology 170, 73–79. Varty, G.B., Marsden, C.A., Higgins, G.A., 1999b. Reduced synaptophysin immunor-
Scaccianoce, S., Del Bianco, P., Paolone, G., Caprioli, D., Modafferi, A.M.E., Nencini, P., eactivity in the dentate gyrus of prepulse inhibition-impaired isolation-reared
Badiani, A., 2006. Social isolation selectively reduces hippocampal brain- rats. Brain Res. 824, 197–203.
1102 K.C.F. Fone, M.V. Porkess / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32 (2008) 1087–1102

Varty, G.B., Paulus, M.P., Braff, D.L., Geyer, M.A., 2000. Environmental enrichment inhibition of the acoustic startle response without disrupting latent inhibition.
and isolation rearing in the rat: effects on locomotor behavior and startle Neuropsychopharmacology 10, 61–72.
response plasticity. Biol. Psychiatry 47, 864–873. Williams, J.H., Wellman, N.A., Geaney, D.P., Cowen, P.J., Feldon, J., Rawlins, J.N.P.,
Varty, G.B., Powell, S.B., Lehmann-Masten, V., Buell, M.R., Geyer, M.A., 2006. Iso- 1998. Reduced latent inhibition in people with schizophrenia: an effect of
lation rearing of mice induces deficits in prepulse inhibition of the startle psychosis or of its treatment. Br. J. Psychiatry 172, 243–249.
response. Behav. Brain Res. 169, 162–167. Wolffgramm, J., 1990. Free choice ethanol intake of laboratory rats under different
Watanabe, H., Ohdo, S., Ishikawa, M., Ogawa, N., 1992. Effects of social-isolation on social conditions. Psychopharmacology 101, 233–239.
pentobarbital activity in mice—relationship to racemate levels and enantiomer Wongwitdecha, N., Marsden, C.A., 1995. Isolation rearing prevents the reinforcing
levels in brain. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 263, 1036–1045. properties of amphetamine in a conditioned place preference paradigm. Eur. J.
Weiss, I.C., Feldon, J., 2001. Environmental animal models for sensorimotor gating Pharmacol. 279, 99–103.
deficiencies in schizophrenia: a review. Psychopharmacology 156, 305–326. Wongwitdecha, N., Marsden, C.A., 1996a. Effect of social isolation on the reinforcing
Weiss, I.C., Feldon, J., Domeney, A.M., 1999. Isolation rearing-induced disruption of properties of morphine in the conditioned place preference test. Pharmacol.
prepulse inhibition: further evidence for fragility of the response. Behav. Biochem. Behav. 53, 531–534.
Pharmacol. 10, 139–149. Wongwitdecha, N., Marsden, C.A., 1996b. Effects of social isolation rearing on
Weiss, I.C., Di Iorio, L., Feldon, J., Domeney, A.M., 2000. Strain differences in the learning in the Morris water maze. Brain Res. 715, 119–124.
isolation-induced effects on prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle response Wongwitdecha, N., Marsden, C.A., 1996c. Social isolation increases aggressive
and on locomotor activity. Behav. Neurosci. 114, 364–373. behaviour and alters the effects of diazepam in the rat social interaction test.
Weiss, I.C., Domeney, A.M., Heidbreder, C.A., Moreau, J.L., Feldon, J., 2001a. Early social Behav. Brain Res. 75, 27–32.
isolation, but not maternal separation, affects behavioral sensitization to amphe- Wongwitdecha, N., Kasemsook, C., Plasen, S., 2006. Social isolation alters the effect
tamine in male and female adult rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 70, 397–409. of desipramine in the rat forced swimming test. Behav. Brain Res. 167, 232–236.
Weiss, I.C., Domeney, A.M., Moreau, J.L., Russig, H., Feldon, J., 2001b. Dissociation Wright, I.K., Ismail, H., Upton, N., Marsden, C.A., 1991a. Effect of isolation rearing
between the effects of pre-weaning and/or post-weaning social isolation on on 5-Ht agonist-induced responses in the rat. Psychopharmacology 105,
prepulse inhibition and latent inhibition in adult Sprague–Dawley rats. Behav. 259–263.
Brain Res. 121, 207–218. Wright, I.K., Upton, N., Marsden, C.A., 1991b. Resocialisation of isolation-reared rats
Weiss, I.C., Pryce, C.R., Jongen-Relo, A.L., Nanz-Bahr, N.I., Feldon, J., 2004. Effect of does not alter their anxiogenic profile on the elevated X-maze model of anxiety.
social isolation on stress-related behavioural and neuroendocrine state in the Physiol. Behav. 50, 1129–1132.
rat. Behav. Brain Res. 152, 279–295. Yates, G., Panksepp, J., Ikemoto, S., Nelson, E., Conner, R., 1991. Social-isolation
Westenbroek, C., Ter Horst, G.J., Roos, M.H., Kuipers, S.D., Trentani, A., den Boer, J.A., effects on the behavioral despair forced swimming test—effect of age and
2003. Gender-specific effects of social housing in rats after chronic mild stress duration of testing. Physiol. Behav. 49, 347–353.
exposure. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 27, 21–30. Zammit, S., Allebeck, P., Andreasson, S., Lundberg, I., Lewis, G., 2002. Self reported
Wilkinson, L.S., Killcross, S.S., Humby, T., Hall, F.S., Geyer, M.A., Robbins, T.W., 1994. cannabis use as a risk factor for schizophrenia in Swedish conscripts of 1969:
Social-isolation in the rat produces developmentally specific deficits in prepulse historical cohort study. Br. Med. J. 325, 1199–1201.

You might also like