You are on page 1of 13

Neuroscience Research 156 (2020) 117–129

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Neuroscience Research
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neures

Review article

Neuromagnetic oscillations in the human sensory systems: A mini


review of our series and literature
Shozo Tobimatsu
Department of Clinical Neurophysiology, Neurological Institute, Graduate School of Medical Sciences, Kyushu University, 3-1-1 Maidashi, Higashi-ku,
Fukuoka, 812-8582, Japan

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Oscillatory neuronal (electrical) activity in defined frequency ranges supports synchronous interactions
Received 8 July 2019 between anatomically distinct regions of the human brain during cognitive tasks. Here, the author reviews
Received in revised form 6 November 2019 our previous studies that focused on the neuromagnetic oscillations in the sensory systems in response
Accepted 27 November 2019
to the external stimuli in normal healthy subjects and neurological disorders. A magnetoencephalog-
Available online 24 December 2019
raphy was applied to evaluate the neuromagnetic oscillations in humans. We have demonstrated that
the oscillatory gamma synchronization binds the primary and secondary somatosensory areas (S1 and
Keywords:
S2) in humans. This functional coupling is modulated by aging. In people who stutter, functional and
Magnetoencephalography
Time-frequency analysis structural reorganization of the right auditory cortex appears to be a compensatory mechanism for
Sensory systems impaired left auditory cortex function. This may be partly caused by increased right hemispheric local
Neurological disorders phase synchronization and increased inter-hemispheric phase synchronization. We have also found that
Oscillology the hippocampus modulates auditory processing differently under normal conditions and in epileptic
patients with hippocampal sclerosis. This indicates that altered neural synchronization may provide
useful information about possible functional deterioration in patients with unilateral mesial tempo-
ral lobe epilepsy. Finally, supraspinal (cortical) mechanism is responsible for pain perception and pain
relief via neural oscillations. Together, neuronal synchronization plays an important role in distributed
cortico-cortical processing.
© 2019 Elsevier B.V. and Japan Neuroscience Society. All rights reserved.

Contents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 118
2. Principal approach to studying event-related neuronal oscillations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 118
2.1. Time-frequency analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 118
2.2. Continuous wavelet transforms . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 118
3. Evoked vs. induced oscillations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 119
3.1. Evoked power . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 119
3.2. Induced power . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 119
3.3. Event-related phase locking across trials . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 120
4. Roles of the oscillatory gamma synchronization in the primary (SI) and secondary somatosensory (SII) areas . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 121
4.1. Induced gamma oscillations between SI and SII . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 121
4.2. Aging effects on the induced oscillations in SII . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 121
5. Mechanisms of altered auditory system in stuttering . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 121
5.1. Abnormal auditory system in stuttering . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 122
5.2. Abnormal auditory synchronization in stuttering . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 122
6. Hippocampal modulation of auditory processing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 123
6.1. Effects of hippocampal sclerosis on central auditory processing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 123
6.2. Effects of HS on auditory neural synchronization . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 125

E-mail address: tobi@neurophy.med.kyushu-u.ac.jp

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2019.12.007
0168-0102/© 2019 Elsevier B.V. and Japan Neuroscience Society. All rights reserved.
118 S. Tobimatsu / Neuroscience Research 156 (2020) 117–129

6.3. Effects of HS on auditory gamma band oscillations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 125


7. Pain perception and neural oscillations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 126
7.1. Cortical pain relief by tactile stimulation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 127
7.2. Frequency-dependent changes in sensorimotor and pain affective systems induced by empathy for pain . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 127
8. Conclusion and future perspectives . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 128
Acknowledgement . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 128
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 128

1. Introduction Recently, neural oscillations and coordination dynamics have


been paid attention to understand the normal brain function as well
The aim of the systems neuroscience is to understand how as the neuropsychiatric disorders (Uhlhaas and Singer, 2015). It is
billions of neurons in the human brain work synchronously to well recognized that rhythmic synchronization of neural discharges
proceed the real-world information (Knight, 2007). Oscillatory neu- may provide the necessary spatial and temporal links that bind
ronal (electrical) activity in defined frequency ranges supports together the processing in different brain areas to build a coherent
synchronous interactions between anatomically distinct regions percept (Tallon-Baudry and Bertrand, 1999). To visualize such oscil-
of the human brain during cognitive tasks that require voluntary lations, event-related EEG/MEG time frequency analysis provide a
or automatic attention, memory, or visual processing (Tallon- view of the serial or sequential events in the brain’s information
Baudry and Bertrand, 1999). It is now widely accepted that defining processing stream.
network interactions is key to understanding normal cognition.
In our laboratory, we have been studying the neuromagnetic
2.1. Time-frequency analysis
oscillations in the sensory systems in response to the external stim-
uli in normal healthy subjects and neurological disorders using
Neural oscillations appear with a jitter in latency from one trial
magnetoencephalography (MEG). The magnetic field passes unaf-
to another, centered around a given latency (Figs. 1 and 2). There-
fected through brain tissue and the skull. Although the magnetic
fore, it tends to cancel out completely in the averaged evoked
field is extremely small, it can be detected by superconducting
potential. Specific methods must be used in order to character-
quantum interference detectors that are based on superconduc-
ize induced activities. The time–frequency power of the signals by
tivity. By analyzing the spatial distributions of magnetic fields,
means of a complex Morlet’s wavelet transform which provides a
it is possible, by using a model such as a single equivalent
better compromise between time and frequency resolutions (Roach
current dipole, to estimate the intracranial localization of the
and Mathalon, 2008). When this analysis is applied to the aver-
generator source and superimpose it on an MRI (Tobimatsu and
aged response, the phase-locked activity can be clearly identified.
Kakigi, 2016). Thus, MEG has extremely high temporal and spa-
When this method is applied to each single trial, followed by an
tial resolution. Other functional modalities such as functional MRI,
averaging of the powers across trials, it becomes possible to iden-
except invasive EEG, have either poor temporal or spatial resolu-
tify non-phase-locked activity as long as the signal-to-noise ratio
tion.
is high enough and the jitter does not exceed the wavelet duration
Overall, MEG is nowadays a well-established, non-invasive tech-
(Fig. 2).
nique, which can be used for basic science research and clinical
As the above mentioned, event-related EEG/MEG signals are
applications (Hari et al., 2018). This review focuses on the neu-
decomposed into magnitude and phase information for each fre-
rophysiological implications of the neuromagnetic oscillations in
quency present in the EEG/EMG (Roach and Mathalon, 2008). This
the human brain. For this purpose, the author introduces our find-
approach characterizes their changes over time on a millisecond
ings on the induced oscillations produced by the sensory system in
time scale with respect to task events (Fig. 1). Broadly speaking,
response to the external stimuli. Then, the author will discuss the
this approach is referred to as “time-frequency analysis”. Time-
roles of the neural oscillation in the sensory system in the normal
frequency analyses of EEG/MEG provide additional information
human brain as well as neurological disorders.
about neural synchrony not apparent in the ongoing EEG/MEG.
They can tell us which frequencies have the most power at specific
points in time and space and how their phase angles synchronize
2. Principal approach to studying event-related neuronal
across time and space.
oscillations

Electroencephalographic (EEG)/MEG recordings have been used 2.2. Continuous wavelet transforms
to study the sensory and cognitive information processing. When
a large number of parallel-oriented cortical neurons receive the Wavelets are waveforms of limited duration that have an aver-
same repetitive synaptic input and/or generate the same repetitive age value of zero. The waveforms contained in the wavelet must
sequence of outputs, their synchronous activity produces extracel- provide a biologically reasonable fit to the signal being modeled.
lular rhythmic field potentials. These open rhythmic field potentials One common type of biologically plausible wavelet, the Morlet
can be recorded as EEG from the scalp if they are strong enough wavelet, is a Gaussian-windowed sinusoidal wave segment com-
and have the right orientation (i.e., perpendicular or radially ori- prising several cycles (Roach and Mathalon, 2008; Tallon-Baudry
ented fields with respect to the scalp surface produce stronger scalp et al., 1999). Wavelet analyses utilize a different time window
potentials than parallel or tangentially oriented fields) (Nunez et al., length for each frequency, with the longest windows applied to the
2019). In contrast, MEG preferentially picks up the tangentially ori- lowest frequencies and the shortest windows applied to the high-
ented fields. Thus, if the neural activity recorded by EEG/MEG were est frequencies. For example, assuming a wavelet family contains
not already synchronized and not already powerful, it would not be 6 cycles of a sinusoidal oscillation, the wavelet for the 10-Hz fre-
evident at the scalp. Therefore, even before it is spectrally decom- quency spans a time window of 600 ms, whereas the wavelet for the
posed, EEG/MEG at the scalp is clear evidence of neural synchrony 40 Hz frequency spans a time window of 150 ms. This variation in
of cortical activity. the wavelet from coarser to finer temporal resolution with increas-
S. Tobimatsu / Neuroscience Research 156 (2020) 117–129 119

Fig. 1. An individual EEG trial (A) is convolved with a complex Morlet wavelet (B), containing both real (solid line) and imaginary (dotted line) wave components, to produce
a single, complex time-frequency data point (C) consisting of both real (x-axis, denoted r) and imaginary (y-axis, denoted i) parts. This operation is known as “convolution”
that can be defined as the multiplication of one series or “vector” of numbers by another (*). By sliding this windowed function across the EEG time series one point at a time,
a complex number at the window’s center point is estimated for each time point in the EEG (C). The length of the vector from the origin (0, 0) to the complex data point (real,
imaginary) depicts the signal magnitude, and the angle this vector forms with the x-axis depicts the phase angle (␪), for the 100-ms time point. The wavelet’s center time
point (hatched vertical line, B) is overlaid on the 100-ms time point in the EEG epoch (hatched vertical line, A) to perform this convolution. Single subject’s complex data
points from 5 trials (each shown in different color) for the 40-Hz frequency and 100-ms poststimulus time point, plotted on the real (x) and imaginary (y) axes (D(a)). After
removing the phase angles from the complex numbers, remaining magnitude values are squared and then averaged (black line), providing an estimate of total power at 40 Hz
and 100 ms (D(b)). After removing the magnitude values from the complex numbers, remaining equal length vectors, which retain phase angle information, are averaged to
obtain the phase-locking factor (PLF) (length of vector shown in black) (D(c)). Repeating steps D(b) and D(c) for every trial, time, and frequency point in the dataset yields a
time-frequency matrix of total power values (D(d)) and PLF values (D(e)) (with permission, from Roach and Mathalon, 2008, Oxford Press).

ing frequency is accomplished at the cost of diminishing frequency across trials (Fig. 2A–C). The phase-synchronized oscillations in the
resolution as frequency increases. The sinusoidal waves contained EEG/MEG across trials are isolated by first time domain averag-
in a wavelet are typically shaped by an envelope function (e.g., a ing the event-locked EEG/MEG epochs to derive the event-related
Gaussian function), such that the wavelet has its largest magnitude potentials (ERPs)/event-related magnetic fields (ERFs). Frequencies
at the center time point and tapers off toward the edges of the time that are phase synchronized with respect to stimulus onset across
window. Fig. 1 shows that single-trial EEG epochs (Fig. 1A) can be repeated trials survive the averaging process and can be seen in
spectrally decomposed into complex numbers for each EEG time the average ERP/ERF. This is not the case for oscillations that are
point, providing estimates of the magnitude and phase angles of the out of phase with respect to stimulus onset across trials, which can-
oscillations (Fig. 1C) at any given frequency. This is accomplished cel out toward zero during the averaging used to generate ERP/ERF.
through multiplication of the EEG with a windowed transforma- Accordingly, evoked power is calculated by spectral decomposition
tion function (e.g., Morlet wavelet transform, as shown in Fig. 1B) of an individual’s ERP/ERF, squaring the magnitude values associ-
centered on a segment of the EEG epoch. When this is done for each ated with each time and frequency point in the time-frequency
trial, the complex number values for a specific time point relative matrix. Evoked power in specific frequencies, such as the gamma
to an event’s onset (e.g., stimulus onset) are collected across trials band, have been linked to sensory registration (Tallon-Baudry et al.,
(Fig. 1D). 1996; Fries et al., 2001) as well as to top-down cognitive processing
of stimulus events (Spencer et al., 2007; Debener et al., 2003) and
generally occur within the first 200 ms following stimulus onset.
3. Evoked vs. induced oscillations

3.1. Evoked power 3.2. Induced power

Evoked power refers to event-related changes in EEG/MEG Induced power refers to event-related changes in EEG/MEG
power that are phase-locked with respect to the event onset power that are time-locked, but not phase-locked, with respect to
120 S. Tobimatsu / Neuroscience Research 156 (2020) 117–129

Fig. 2. Algorithm for visualization of the induced gamma response. (A) Successive EEG trials (simulated data) with a small amplitude gamma response phase-locked to stimulus
onset (blue boxes) and a gamma burst jittering in latency (green boxes). (B) Averaging across single trials leads to the conventional evoked potential. (C) Time–frequency
power representation of the evoked gamma response. The abscissa represents time, and the ordinate, frequency. The color scale codes the variations of power (positive or
negative) with respect to a pre-stimulus baseline. The non-phase-locked activity is cancelled out. When the time frequency power is computed for each singe trial (D), and
then averaged across trials (E), the induced gamma response is revealed (with permission, from Tallon-Baudry and Bertrand, 1999, Elsevier Science).

the event onset across trials (Fig. 2D, E). Induced power, also known magnitude information has been unit normalized (i.e., transformed
as “asynchronous power” or “phase-invariant power”, is contained to 1) is used. By averaging these normalized complex numbers
within, and is sometimes confused with, total power because the across trials for each time point and frequency bin, a 2-dimensional
latter is calculated from time-frequency decomposition of single matrix of time-frequency values describing the consistency of the
trial EEG/MEG epochs using only the squared magnitude informa- phase angles with respect to an event’s onset is obtained. Specif-
tion without regard to the phase of the signal. It is important to ically, each value in this time-frequency matrix is a real number
recognize that evoked power is explicitly removed from measures between zero and one, with zero reflecting a completely uniform
of total power because the latter actually contain both phase-locked random distribution of phase angles between trials and with one
and non–phase-locked power. Thus, to isolate pure induced power, reflecting identical, or perfectly synchronized, phase angles across
evoked power must be removed from the single trial–based total trials. The measure defined by these values has been called phase-
power estimate. locking factor (PLF) (Tallon-Baudry et al., 1997) and it represents
one minus the circular variance of phases (i.e., phase variance) for
each time-frequency point examined. Event-related phase locking
3.3. Event-related phase locking across trials is an important complement to total power because the complex
number magnitude values on which power calculations depend
Event-related phase consistency, or phase locking with respect have no influence on the phase angles used to calculate phase lock-
to an event’s onset, across trials can be calculated within one elec- ing. Importantly, the term “PLF” is not analogous to one of the terms
trode, complementing the total power measure described above. commonly used to describe the consistency of phase differences
To this end, the phase information shown in Fig. 1D(c) in which
S. Tobimatsu / Neuroscience Research 156 (2020) 117–129 121

between two electrodes across trials, “phase-locking value (PLV)” cSII and between cSII and iSII occurred in the early post-stimulus
(Lachaux et al., 1999). stage. Therefore, the oscillatory gamma synchronization binds the
early-stage tactile information processing within the somatosen-
sory cortical network in normal subjects. We also demonstrated
4. Roles of the oscillatory gamma synchronization in the
the decrease in PLVs between SI and SII in MS patients compared
primary (SI) and secondary somatosensory (SII) areas
with healthy controls (HCs). Because subcortical U-fiber lesions and
periventricular white matter lesions in MS patients may frequently
In the somatosensory system, SI and SII comprise the early
involve the intra- and interhemispheric associative pathways, the
stage of the hierarchical organization. Electrophysiological studies
lesion load might significantly contribute to the decreased neu-
of evoked responses have often reported differences in activation
ronal synchronization within the somatosensory cortical network
timing between SI and SII, with the SI response occurring earlier
(Tecchio et al., 2008). Overall, it is notable that this is the first MEG
than the SII responses. The typical characteristics of SII include
study to demonstrate impaired functional connectivity between SI
sensitivity towards higher order functions, such as sensorimotor
and SII in MS.
integrations, attention, unitary body image, and integration of noci-
ceptive and non-nociceptive information (for a review, see Lin and
Forss, 2002). Anatomically, it has been shown that the neurons in 4.2. Aging effects on the induced oscillations in SII
SII have larger and more complex receptive fields than those in SI
(for a review, see Iwamura, 1998). Therefore, SII is often considered Age-related changes are well documented in SI in which the
to be a hierarchically higher cortical area than SI. amplitude of N20 of somatosensory evoked potentials, which is
the initial cortical response following electrical stimulation of
the median nerve or fingers. The age-related electrophysiological
4.1. Induced gamma oscillations between SI and SII
changes in SI were also confirmed by MEG studies, which demon-
strated that the N20 m component, an MEG counterpart of the N20
Neuronal synchronization in the gamma-frequency band has
component, exhibited increased amplitude in the old (Huttunen
received increasing attention as the salient mechanism for cortico-
et al., 1999). Although the precise physiological mechanism has not
cortical information processing (Tallon-Baudry and Bertrand, 1999;
been fully elucidated, the increased amplitude is probably caused
Roach and Mathalon, 2008). Recently, the temporal binding
by cortical disinhibition in the old (Huttunen et al., 1999; Stephen
mechanismin various gamma-frequency bands has been well rec-
et al., 2006). In addition to the amplitude difference, the N20 latency
ognized in several studies of top–down and bottom–up information
is also prolonged in the old because of the slowing of conduction
processing among anatomically distributed cortical areas (Tallon-
velocity in the peripheral nerves and spinal cord. So far, less is
Baudry et al., 1997; Engel et al., 2001). However, the significance
known about age-related change in SII. Therefore, we quantified
of temporal binding theory in the functional relationship between
age-related changes across SI and SII mainly based on oscillatory
SI and SII has not yet been established. To elucidate the neural
activity indices measured with MEG (Hagiwara et al., 2014).
synchronization in the early-stage somatosensory processing, we
We recorded SEFs to right median nerve stimulation in healthy
studied the functional connectivity between SI and SII in healthy
young and old subjects and assessed major SEF components. Fif-
subjects using MEG (Hagiwara et al., 2008).
teen young volunteers (29.0 ± 4.1 years) and fifteen old volunteers
Twenty-three healthy volunteers (18 women and 5 men, mean
(63.7 ± 3.7 years) participated in this study. We evaluated the PLF
age 37.3 ± 10.6 years) and 23 patients with clinically definite
for local field synchrony on neural oscillations and the weighted
multiple sclerosis (MS) patinets (18 women and 5 men, mean
phase-lag index (wPLI) for cortico-cortical synchrony between SI
age 38.8 ± 8.1 years) according to the revised McDonald criteria
and SII. wPLI measures the consistency of the phase relationship
(Polman et al., 2005) participated in this study. Somatosensory
between signals in separate areas while diminishing the effect
evoked magnetic fields (SEFs) to right and left median nerve stim-
of volume conduction (Vinck et al., 2011). PLF was significantly
ulation were recorded to determine the locations of each cortical
increased in SI along with the increased amplitude of N20 m in the
activity. As a result, five major deflections of the SEF waveforms in
old subjects. PLF was also increased in SII associated with a short-
the contralateral SI (cSI) and bilateral SII areas (cSII and iSII) were
ened peak latency of SEFs. wPLI analysis revealed the increased
identified. Short-latency deflections, N20 m, P35 m, and P60 m,
coherent activity between SI and SII in the old subjects (Fig. 4).
which were recorded over the cSI whereas middle-latency deflec-
The most novel finding of this study is that the age-related
tions were observed over the cSII and iSII. The peak latency of cSII
changes occur outside SI, namely in SII. PLF measures local field
was earlier than that of iSII. All cSI deflections were clearly identi-
synchrony, while wPLI reflects functional connectivity between
fied in all normal subjects, whereas N20 m and P35 m deflections
the two cortical areas. Thus, age-related changes are profound
were not always recorded in MS patients. Then, we analyzed the
across the somatosensory areas with interaction between each
PLVs of the induced gamma activity to assess neural synchrony
other. Our results suggest that the functional coupling between
within the somatosensory cortical network. We also assessed PLVs
SI and SII is influenced by the cortical disinhibition due to normal
in patients with MS to validate our PLV analysis in evaluating the
aging. Therefore, we provide the first electrophysiological evidence
inter-areal functional connectivity, which can often be impaired in
for age-related changes in oscillatory neural activities across the
MS. The PLVs of the induced gamma activity were calculated for
somatosensory areas.
each pair of unaveraged MEG signals that represented the activi-
ties of the contralateral SI and bilateral SII areas. Analysis of PLVs
between the SI and SII areas showed significantly increased PLVs 5. Mechanisms of altered auditory system in stuttering
for gamma-band activities, starting at an early post-stimulus stage
in NCs, whereas this increase in PLVs was apparently diminished Stuttering is a developmental disorder that affects speech flu-
in MS (Fig. 3), especially in high gamma band (50−70 Hz). ency. The mechanism of stuttering is still a matter of debate. People
The temporal binding mechanism in various gamma-frequency who stutter (PWS) can reduce their stuttering rates under mask-
bands among anatomically distributed cortical areas is responsible ing noise and altered auditory feedback; such a response can be
for top–down and bottom–up information processing (for a review, attributed to altered auditory input (for a review, see Lincoln et al.,
see Engel et al., 2001). To our knowledge, we first demonstrated 2006). Postma and Kolk (1992) proposed “auditory feedback defect
that synchronized gamma-band activities in both between cSI and theories in PWS,” in which PWS have deviant error monitoring of
122 S. Tobimatsu / Neuroscience Research 156 (2020) 117–129

Fig. 3. Time-frequency analysis with the continuous wavelet transform in a representative subject. (A) Induced gamma-band activities (30–70 Hz) recorded by the sensors
showing the SI and SII activities. (B) Topography of the induced gamma activities around the SI and SII areas in the same subject. (C) The grand average of PLVs in normal
subjects shows increased PLVs in the entire gamma-frequency band (30–70 Hz) (upper row). The high PLVs in the high-frequency gamma-band (50–70 Hz) are observed in
the earlier latencies (mostly in the post-stimulus period of 20–50 ms), whereas those in the low-frequency gamma-band (30–40 Hz) last up to 100 ms. There are also some
phase-locking activities of various frequencies during the later post-stimulus period (> 100 ms). In MS patients, decreased PLVs are noted in the entire frequency range (lower
row) (with permission, from Hagiwara et al., 2010, Elsevier Science).

speech production, namely, PWS detect errors more than people expansion. Taken together, we suggest that PWS have impaired left
who do not stutter. This suggests that auditory input processing auditory sensory gating during basic auditory input processing and
could be different in PWS compared with non-PWS. that some error signals in the auditory cortex could result in abnor-
mal speech processing. Functional and structural reorganization of
the right auditory cortex appears to be a compensatory mechanism
5.1. Abnormal auditory system in stuttering
for impaired left auditory cortex function in PWS.
The details of abnormal processing of basic auditory information
remain unclear in PWS. In order to characterize such abnormal- 5.2. Abnormal auditory synchronization in stuttering
ities, we examined the functional and structural changes in the
auditory cortices of PWS by using a 306-channel MEG system We demonstrated both functional and structural reorganization
to assess auditory sensory gating (P50 m suppression) and tono- of the right auditory cortex and impaired left auditory cortex func-
topic organization (Kikuchi et al., 2011). Additionally, we employed tion in PWS (Kikuchi et al., 2011). It was interesting to explore the
voxel-based morphometry to compare cortical gray matter (GM) underlying mechanism(s) of the functional alteration of the audi-
volumes on structural MR images. tory cortices in PDS. Therefore, we reevaluated the same dataset to
Two successive clicks evoked a well-defined P50 m response in further investigate how the right and left auditory cortices interact
the hemisphere contralateral to the stimulated ear in PWS (n = 18) to compensate for stuttering (Kikuchi et al., 2017). We evaluated
and HCs (n = 18). The amplitude of S2-P50 m was apparently smaller bilateral N100 m latencies as well as indices of local and inter-
than that of S1-P50 m in the control subjects. However, no such hemispheric phase synchronization of the auditory cortices.
amplitude difference between S1-P50 m and S2-P50 m was evident The left N100 m latency was significantly prolonged relative to
in the left hemisphere in PWS. This suggests that PWS has impaired the right N100 m latency in PWS, while healthy control partici-
left auditory sensory gating. The tonotopic organization in the right pants did not show any inter-hemispheric differences in latency.
hemisphere of PWS (n = 16) is expanded compared with that of A PLF analysis, which indicates the degree of local phase syn-
the HCs (n = 16). Furthermore, PWS (n = 15) showed a significant chronization, demonstrated enhanced alpha-band synchrony in
increase in the GM volume of the right superior temporal gyrus the right auditory area of PWS (Fig. 5A). A PLV analysis of inter-
compared with NSs (n = 15), consistent with the right tonotopic hemispheric synchronization demonstrated significant elevations
S. Tobimatsu / Neuroscience Research 156 (2020) 117–129 123

Fig. 4. Grand-means for wPLIs in young (left column) and old (middle column) subjects, and point-by-point t-test results for wPLI (right column). Since the significant points
were distributed in a contiguous manner ranging from alpha to gamma band in both areas (right column), ROIs were defined as follows: 8–120 Hz and 0–200 ms for SI, and
8–60 Hz and 0–200 ms for cSII (red rectangles). Then, the group comparison using mean values of a contiguous region within the ROIs was performed. Only statistically
significant points (p < 0.05) are displayed in the t-maps. The old subjects show significantly elevated wPLIs in each comparison (with permission, from Hagiwara et al., 2,
Elsevier Science).

in the beta band between the right and left auditory cortices in Thus, the electrophysiological process by which unilateral hip-
PWS (Fig. 5B). In addition, right PLF and PLVs were positively cor- pocampal lesions, such as hippocampal sclerosis (HS), modulate
related with stuttering frequency in PWS. Taken together, our data the auditory processing remains unknown. Auditory-evoked mag-
suggest that increased right hemispheric local phase synchroniza- netic fields (AEFs) are valuable for evaluating auditory functions,
tion and increased inter-hemispheric phase synchronization are because M100, a major component of AEFs, originates from audi-
electrophysiological correlates of a compensatory mechanism for tory areas. HS is the primary pathology of mesial temporal lobe
impaired left auditory processing in PWS. epilepsy (mTLE). Therefore, AEFs of mTLE (n = 17) with unilateral
HS were compared with HCs (n = 17) and disease controls (n = 9),
thereby determining whether AEFs were indicative of hippocampal
6. Hippocampal modulation of auditory processing
influences on the auditory processing (Chatani et al., 2016).
Monaural tone-burst stimuli were presented for each side,
The hippocampus is an integral component of the medial tempo-
followed by analysis of M100 and a previously less character-
ral lobe memory system and recently was thought to be involved in
ized exogenous component (M400: 300–500 ms). The frequency
sensory (particularly visual) perception (Graham et al., 2010). With
of acceptable M100 dipoles was significantly decreased in the HS
respect to auditory perception, the hippocampus receives afferent
side. Beam-forming-based source localization analysis also showed
inputs from the auditory association cortices, and in turn, it projects
decreased activity of the auditory area, which corresponded to the
back to the primary auditory cortex and auditory association areas
inadequately estimated dipoles. M400 was found to be related to
(Mehta et al., 2009). Within this reciprocal network, the hippocam-
the medial temporal structure on the HS side. Volumetric anal-
pus has been shown to inhibit redundant auditory inputs, as well
ysis was also performed, focusing on the auditory-related areas
as detect novel auditory information (Kraus and Canlon, 2012).
(planum temporale, Heschl’s gyrus, and superior temporal gyrus),
as well as the hippocampus. M100 amplitudes positively corre-
6.1. Effects of hippocampal sclerosis on central auditory lated with hippocampal and planum temporale volumes in the HC
processing group, whereas they negatively correlated with Heschl’s gyrus vol-
ume in the mTLE group. Interestingly, significantly enhanced M400
It is likely that hippocampus plays a critical role in relaying component was observed in the HS side of the mTLE patients. In
auditory information to later perceptual and cognitive processes. addition, the M400 component positively correlated with Heschl’s
However, little attention has been paid to the electrophysiologi- gyrus volume and tended to positively correlate with disease dura-
cal signatures of hippocampal contribution to auditory processing.
124 S. Tobimatsu / Neuroscience Research 156 (2020) 117–129

Fig. 5. (A) PLFs in healthy control participants (HCs) versus people who stutter (PWS) in both hemispheres. Black rectangles indicate alpha band (8–13 Hz) synchronization
between 80 and 160 ms after stimulus onset. Auditory tone-burst stimulation of 1000 Hz was monaurally presented for 300 ms (10-ms and 20-ms rise and fall times,
respectively). Right local phase synchronization was significantly higher than left local phase synchronization in PWS when a repeated measure analysis of variance (ANOVA)
performed (p < 0.05). Right local phase synchronization was also higher than that of right local phase synchronization in HCs. Then, time frequency analysis was applied
to the auditory magnetic fields. (B) PLVs in HCs and PWS in 1000 Hz stimulation. White rectangles indicate beta band (14–30 Hz) synchronization between 80 and 160 ms
after stimulus onset. Synchronization between the right and left auditory cortices in PWS was significantly higher than that in HCs when a repeated measures ANOVA was
performed (p < 0.05) (with permission, from Kikuchi et al., 2017, Elsevier Science).

tion. M400 was markedly diminished after hippocampal resection. tion (Han et al., 2011). In auditory-evoked potential studies, right
Although volumetric analysis showed decreased hippocampal vol- HS caused left lateralized topographical changes of N100 (Rosburg
ume in the HS side, the planum temporale and Heschl’s gyrus, the et al., 2008), a major peak evoked 100 ms after the onset of audi-
two major sources of M100, were preserved. These results sug- tory stimulation, and reduced N100 amplitudes were also found in
gested that HS significantly influenced AEFs. mTLE (Bougeard and Fischer, 2002). Based on the results of previ-
With respect to auditory-related dysfunctions, mTLE patients ous works and our AEF findings, we conclud that the hippocampus
exhibit deficits in precise detection of changes in frequency and modulates auditory processing differently under normal conditions
duration of auditory stimuli, despite intact primary hearing func- and in HS.
S. Tobimatsu / Neuroscience Research 156 (2020) 117–129 125

Fig. 6. (A) Time-frequency plots of PLVs in HCs, patients with left mTLE, and patients with right mTLE as grand-averaged data. White rectangles represent neural synchro-
nization between the right and left Heschl’s gyri in the theta, alpha, and beta bands, respectively. (B) PLVs in both patients with left and right mTLE were substantially reduced
compared with those of HCs in the theta and alpha frequency bands, irrespective of the side of ear stimulation. In the alpha band, PLVs in response to left ear stimulation
exhibited larger values compared with right ear stimulation in all groups (with permission, from Matsubara et al., 2018, Elsevier Science).

6.2. Effects of HS on auditory neural synchronization was reduced in both patients with right and left mTLE compared
with that of HCs (Fig. 6). The accuracy of diagnosis and lateralization
Our previous study of monaural AEFs demonstrated that HS sig- was 80 %–90 %.
nificantly modulated auditory processing in patients with mTLE We confirmed that auditory processing was disrupted in
(Chatani et al., 2016). However, the small sample size (n = 17) and patients with mTLE (Chatani et al., 2016). Therefore, the current
focus on the M100 response were insufficient to elucidate the later- results indicated that monaural AEFs provide useful information for
alization of the epileptic focus. Therefore, we increased the number detecting mTLE pathologies. In addition, we found that AC subnor-
of patients with mTLE (n = 39) to examine whether neural syn- mal function was more pronounced in patients with right mTLE.
chronization induced by monaural pure tone stimulation provides Neural synchronization highlighted lateralization of the epileptic
useful diagnostic information about epileptic foci in patients with focus in patients with unilateral mTLE. Our non-invasive simple
unilateral mTLE (Matsubara et al., 2018). task was useful for localizing the epileptic focus in patients with
Twenty-five patients with left mTLE, 14 patients with right mTLE, as well as for diagnosing patients with mTLE. Finally, our
mTLE, and 32 HCs were recruited. Auditory stimuli of 500-Hz tone findings suggest that altered neural synchronization is exhibited
burst were monaurally presented to subjects. The AEF data were by patients with mTLE, providing evidence for altered AC function
analyzed with source estimation of M100 responses in bilateral in mTLE.
auditory cortices (ACs). Neural synchronization within ACs and
between ACs was evaluated with PLF and PLV, respectively. Linear 6.3. Effects of HS on auditory gamma band oscillations
discriminant analysis was performed for diagnosis and lateraliza-
tion of epileptic focus. The M100 amplitude revealed that patients Patients with mTLE often exhibit central auditory processing
with right mTLE exhibited smaller M100 amplitude than patients (CAP) dysfunction. Various studies have reported that some mTLE
with left mTLE and HCs. Interestingly, PLF was able to differenti- patients show decreased performance in irregularity discrimina-
ate the groups with mTLE, with decreased PLFs in the alpha band tion of rapid auditory sequences (Ehrle et al., 2001), decreased
observed in patients with right mTLE compared with those (PLFs) temporal processing in the Gaps-In-Noise test (Aravindkumar et al.,
in patients with left mTLE (data not shown). Right hemispheric pre- 2012) and the duration pattern sequence test (Lavasani et al., 2016).
dominance was confirmed in both HCs and patients with left mTLE In accord with these results, a recent MEG study in our laboratory
while patients with right mTLE showed a lack of right hemispheric provided neuromagnetic evidence for hippocampal modulation of
predominance. Functional connectivity between bilateral ACs (PLV) CAP (Chatani et al., 2016; Matsubara et al., 2018). However, the
126 S. Tobimatsu / Neuroscience Research 156 (2020) 117–129

Fig. 7. Induced gamma responses of PLFs in HCs, left mTLE and right mTLE patients obtained from contralateral and ipsilateral ACs. Time-frequency plots are shown in three
groups as grand-averaged data (upper row). 40-Hz ASSR elicits temporal frequency dynamics at approximately 40 Hz in bilateral ACs irrespective of ear stimulation during
sound presentation. Note that more prominent responses in the contralateral AC were evident, compared with those of ipsilateral AC. Time courses of PLFs as the average of
35–45 Hz oscillations are shown in each group by each ear stimulation (middle row). Black lines represent the mean activity of contralateral AC, while gray lines represent
the mean activity of ipsilateral AC. More prominent responses were found in the contralateral AC in HCs, while mTLE patients showed less marked contralaterality. Contra-
laterality index (cLI) scores are presented for each group (lower row). HCs (blue lines) exhibited symmetrical contralaterality in response to left and right ear stimulation
during all time courses, while mTLE patients exhibited different response patterns. Left mTLE patients (red lines) exhibited decreased contralaterality in response to right
ear stimulation, whereas right mTLE patients (green lines) exhibited reduced contralaterality in either right or left ear stimulation, with more prominent reductions in left
ear stimulation. Shaded areas represent the standard error of the mean (with permission, from Matsubara et al., 2019, Elsevier Science).

functional impact of CAP dysfunction on unilateral HS remains to tion revealed by gamma band oscillations can be used to reveal the
be fully elucidated. Monaural 40-Hz auditory steady-state mag- pathophysiology of mTLE.
netic responses (ASSRs) were recorded to study whether gamma
band neural synchronization induced by 40-Hz monaural stimula-
tion provides useful information on the pathophysiology of mTLE 7. Pain perception and neural oscillations
(Matsubara et al., 2019). Eighteen left mTLE patients, 11 right mTLE
patients and 16 HCs were examined. Monaural clicks were pre- Nociceptive stimuli induce responses in an extensive cortical
sented at a rate of 40 Hz. PLF and power values were analyzed network including mainly SI and SII somatosensory, insular and
within bilateral Heschl’s gyri. Monaural 40-Hz ASSR demonstrated anterior cingulate areas. The activity of this network, often referred
temporal frequency dynamics in both PLF and power data. Symmet- to as the “pain matrix”, is thought to reflect the mechanisms by
rical hemispheric contralaterality was revealed in HCs. However, which a nociceptive input is transformed into a conscious per-
predominant contralaterality was absent in mTLE patients (Fig. 7). cept of pain (for a review, see Tracey and Mantyh, 2007). The SI
Specifically, right mTLE patients exhibited a lack of contralaterality and SII cortices have been variably implicated in acute nociception.
in response to left ear but not right ear stimulation, and vice versa Several neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that the insular-
in left mTLE patients. opercular region (IOR) is consistently involved in pain processing
This is the first study to use monaural 40-Hz ASSR with unilateral (Apkarian et al., 2005; Peyron et al., 2002). Some reports have also
mTLE patients to clarify the relationship between CAP and epileptic suggested that this region encodes quantitative as well as quali-
focus. CAP dysfunction was characterized by a lack of contralater- tative aspects of pain (Oertel et al., 2012). Furthermore, the IOR is
ality corresponding to epileptic focus. Monaural 40-Hz ASSR can known to be involved in multisensory processing, including tac-
provide useful information for localizing epileptic focus in mTLE tile sensation, pain, proprioception, temperature, and vestibular
patients. These results link neuromagnetic assessment and differ- information (Mazzola et al., 2012). The anterior cingulate may be
ences in clinical findings of cognitive processing (i.e., CAP) between concerned with the affective and attentional components of pain
HCs and mTLE patients, and also between left mTLE patients and experience, and experimental evidence lends some support to this
right mTLE patients. Overall, our results indicate that CAP dysfunc- concept (Rainville et al., 1997).
S. Tobimatsu / Neuroscience Research 156 (2020) 117–129 127

Fig. 8. t-Values (needle condition vs. cotton swab condition) for the ␥ band. (A) A significant cluster of significant electrodes is outlined by a black oval line. Induced ␥
band activities (30–60 Hz) were increased in the needle condition over the right hemisphere. (B) Power values and t-values of a representative electrode. The degree of
power suppression was decreased between 1000 and 1500 ms after movie onset. (C) A significant cluster was observed over the right centroparietal region, as indicated by
a multiplication sign (×). This was confirmed by a cluster-based permutation test. (D) Source estimation of the time and frequency window on the standard brain indicates
that the decreased ␥ band suppression is centered at the right motor area (with permission, from Motoyama et al., 2017, Dove Press).

7.1. Cortical pain relief by tactile stimulation a 60-ms ISI for cortical modulation and a 0-ms ISI for spinal level
modulation. Source localization using minimum norm estimates
The analgesic effect of vibrotactile stimuli has been acknowl- demonstrated that pain-related activity was located in the poste-
edged as an important mechanism that can modulate pain rior insula, whereas tactile-related activity was estimated in the
perception (Melzack and Wall, 1965). Practically, people uncon- parietal operculum. We also found significant inhibition of pain-
sciously rub a painful part of their body to reduce pain. Several related activity in the posterior insula due to cortical modulation.
studies have shown the analgesic effect of both tactile and vibra- In contrast, spinal modulation was observed both in the poste-
tory stimuli in psychophysiological evaluation in healthy subjects rior insula and parietal operculum. Subjective pain, as evaluated by
(Mancini et al., 2014) and in chronic pain conditions (Staud et al., the visual analog scale, also showed significant reduction in both
2011). Because these studies presented nociceptive and non- conditions.
nociceptive stimuli simultaneously, cross-modal interaction most Pain–tactile interaction at the cortical level was investigated
likely occurred in the spinal cord. However, Inui et al. (2006) using carefully timed sensory modality-specific stimuli, in com-
demonstrated a supraspinal mechanism in tactile-induced pain bination with cortical source-based neuromagnetic analysis. We
relief via experimental paradigms with concurrent bimodal inputs were able to clearly distinguish source location from pain and tac-
at the cortical level. However, the precise spatiotemporal profile for tile inputs associated with natural-touch like sensation. Our study
centrally mediated tactile-induced analgesia is largely unknown. clearly demonstrates that the spatial feature of cortical modulation
The IOR integrates multiple sensory inputs, and nociceptive modu- by nociceptive input was different compared with spinal modula-
lation by other sensory inputs occurs in this area. In this study, we tion. The current study also provides new spatiotemporal evidence
focused on the IOR to characterize the spatiotemporal signature of that the posterior insula is a critical area of the cortical mechanisms
tactile-induced analgesia using MEG (Hayamizu et al., 2016). underlying tactile-induced analgesia in humans.
Eleven healthy right-handed volunteers participated in this
study. A␦ (intra-epidermal electrical stimulation) inputs were 7.2. Frequency-dependent changes in sensorimotor and pain
modified by A␤ (mechanical tactile stimulation) selective stimu- affective systems induced by empathy for pain
lation (Inui et al., 2002), either independently or concurrently, to
the right forearm. The optimal inter-stimulus interval (ISI) for corti- Understanding or having empathy for others while observ-
cal level modulation was determined after comparing the 40-, 60-, ing their circumstances is crucial for making fundamental
and 80-ms ISI conditions, and the calculated cortical arrival time social ties. Witnessing another person in a painful situation
difference between A␦ and A␤ inputs. Subsequently, we adopted can cause “empathy for pain” in the observer. Empathy for
128 S. Tobimatsu / Neuroscience Research 156 (2020) 117–129

pain involves complex processes that make it possible for the Acknowledgement
observer to experience sensory and emotional qualities of vicar-
ious pain (Betti and Aglioti, 2016). In addition, empathy for pain This review article was supported in part by JSPS KAKENHI Grant
induces not only affective–motivational representations, but also Number 15H05875.
sensory–discriminatory representations in the brain (Avenanti
et al., 2005). Thus, we can use empathic pain to explore the pain References
system without inducing actual pain.
To better comprehend the processing of empathic pain, we stud- Apkarian, A.V., Bushnell, M.C., Treede, R.D., Zubieta, J.K., 2005. Human brain mech-
anisms of pain perception and regulation in health and disease. Eur. J. Pain 9,
ied the frequency-dependent modulation of cortical oscillations 463–484.
induced by watching movies depicting pain using high-density EEG, Aravindkumar, R., Shivashankar, N., Satishchandra, P., Sinha, S., Saini, J., Sub-
MEG, and motor evoked potentials (MEP) (Motoyama et al., 2017). bakrishna, D.K., 2012. Temporal resolution deficits in patients with refractory
complex partial seizures and mesial temporal sclerosis (MTS). Epilepsy Behav.
Experiment 1 (17 normal subjects) was designed to explore the 24, 126–130, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2012.03.004.
relationship between neuronal oscillations and subjective inner Avenanti, A., Bueti, D., Galati, G., Aglioti, S.M., 2005. Transcranial magnetic stimu-
pain. Experiment 2 (8 normal subjects) was designed to study lation highlights the sensorimotor side of empathy for pain. Nat. Neurosci. 8,
955–960.
the effect of empathy for pain on M1 excitability contralateral
Betti, V., Aglioti, S.M., 2016. Dynamic construction of the neural networks underpin-
to the pricked hand. Event-related desynchronization of EEG and ning empathy for pain. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 63, 191–206, http://dx.doi.org/
MEG was assessed while participants viewed videos of painful 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.02.009.
(needle) or neutral (cotton swab) situations. The amplitudes of Bougeard, R., Fischer, C., 2002. The role of temporal pole in auditory processing.
Epileptic Disord. 4, 29–32.
MEPs were also compared between the needle and cotton swab Chatani, H., Hagiwara, K., Hironaga, N., Ogata, K., Shigeto, H., Morioka, T., Sakata,
conditions. The degree of suppression in ␣/␤ band power was sig- A., Hashiguchi, K., Murakami, N., Uehara, T., Kira, J.I., Tobimatsu, S., 2016.
nificantly increased, whereas that of ␥ band power was significantly Neuromagnetic evidence for hippocampal modulation of auditory process-
ing. NeuroImage 124, 256–266, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.
decreased, in the needle condition compared with the cotton swab 09.006.
condition. EEG revealed that significant differences in ␣/␤ band Chen, A.C., Herrmann, C.S., 2001. Perception of pain coincides with the spatial expan-
were distributed in the right frontocentral and left parietooccipital sion of electroencephalographic dynamics in human subjects. Neurosci. Lett.
297, 183–186, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3940(00)01696-7.
regions, whereas significant ␥ band differences were distributed Debener, S., Herrmann, C.S., Kranczioch, C., Gembris, D., Engel, A.K., 2003. Top-down
predominantly over the right hemisphere, which were confirmed attentional processing enhances auditory evoked gamma band activity. Neu-
by source estimation using MEG (Fig. 8). There was a significant roreport 14, 683–686.
Ehrle, N., Samson, S., Baulac, M., 2001. Processing of rapid auditory information
positive correlation between the difference in ␥ power of the two in epileptic patients with left temporal lobe damage. Neuropsychologia 39,
conditions and the visual analog scale subjective rating of aver- 525–531, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00121-4.
sion, but not in the ␣/␤ band. The amplitude of MEPs decreased in Engel, A.K., Fries, P., Singer, W., 2001. Dynamic predictions: oscillations and Syn-
chrony in top-down processing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 704–716.
the needle condition, which confirmed the inhibition of the pri-
Fries, P., Reynolds, J., Rorie, A., Desimone, R., 2001. Modulation of oscillatory neuronal
mary motor cortex. MEP suppression supports that modulation synchronization by selective visual attention. Science 291, 1560–1563, http://
of cortical oscillations by viewing movies depicting pain involves dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1055465.
sensorimotor processing. Graham, K.S., Barense, M.D., Lee, A.C.H., 2010. Going beyond LTM in the MTL:
a synthesis of neuropsychological and neuroimaging findings on the role of
We explored the frequency-dependent changes of empathy for the medial temporal lobe in memory and perception. Neuropsychologia 48,
pain by viewing movies depicting pain using high-density EEG and 831–853, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.01.001.
MEG with the condition that MEP inhibition was observed. Our Gross, J., Schnitzler, A., Timmermann, L., Ploner, M., 2007. Gamma oscillations in
human primary somatosensory cortex reflect pain perception. PLoS Biol. 5 (5),
results suggest that ␣ /␤ band suppression is involved in the pro- e133, http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0050133.
cessing of somatic aspects of empathy for pain, which in turn results Hagiwara, K., Okamoto, T., Shigeto, H., Ogata, K., Somehara, Y., Matsushita, T., Kira,
in inhibition of M1 excitability. ␥ oscillations most likely reflect the J.-I., Tobimatsu, S., 2010. Oscillatory gamma synchronization binds the pri-
mary and secondary somatosensory areas in humans. NeuroImage 51, 412–420,
cognitive aspects of pain assessment (Chen and Herrmann, 2001; http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.001.
Gross et al., 2007; Hauck et al., 2007). The analyses help advance our Hagiwara, K., Ogata, K., Okamoto, T., Uehara, T., Hironaga, N., Shigeto, H., Kira, J.-
understanding of the processing of empathic pain. Further studies I., Tobimatsu, S., 2014. Age-related changes across the primary and secondary
somatosensory areas: an analysis of neuromagnetic oscillatory activities. Clin.
are needed to study the direct link between ␣ /␤ band suppres- Neurophysiol. 125, 1021–1029, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2013.10.005.
sion and MEP suppression, and the relationship between ␣ /␤ and Han, M.W., Ahn, J.H., Kang, J.K., Lee, E.M., Lee, J.H., Bae, J.H., Chung, J.W., 2011. Cen-
␥ band modulations, to further elucidate the neural substrates of tral auditory processing impairment in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy.
Epilepsy Behav. 20, 370–374, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2010.12.032.
pain observation.
Hauck, M., Lorenz, J., Engel, A.K., 2007. Attention to painful stimulation enhances
gamma-band activity and synchronization in human sensorimotor cortex. J.
8. Conclusion and future perspectives Neurosci. 27, 9270–9277, http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2283-07.
Hari, R., Baillet, S., Barnes, G., Burgess, R., Forss, N., Gross, J., Hämäläinen, M., Jensen,
O., Kakigi, R., Mauguière, F., Nakasato, N., Puce, A., Romani, G.L., Schnitzler,
It is apparent that neuronal synchronization plays an impor- A., Taulu, S., 2018. IFCN-endorsed practical guidelines for clinical magnetoen-
tant role in distributed cortico-cortical processing in the sensory cephalography (MEG). Clin. Neurophysiol. 129, 1720–1747, http://dx.doi.org/
systems in human. Thus, the induced oscillatory activities are 10.1016/j.clinph.2018.03.042.
Hayamizu, M., Hagiwara, K., Hironaga, N., Ogata, K., Hoka, S., Tobimatsu, S., 2016.
responsible for temporal binding of spatially distributed informa- A spatiotemporal signature of cortical pain relief by tactile stimulation: an
tion processing in the human brain. Future studies will be necessary MEG study. NeuroImage 130, 175–183, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.
to facilitate our understanding of the roles of the neural oscillations 2016.01.065.
Huttunen, J., Wikstrom, H., Salonen, O., Ilmoniemi, R.J., 1999. Human somatosen-
in the network interactions. sory cortical activation strengths: comparison between males and females and
age-related changes. Brain Res. 818, 196–203, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0006-
Declaration of Competing Interest 8993(98)01215-3.
Inui, K., Tran, T.D., Hoshiyama, M., Kakigi, R., 2002. Preferential stimulation of A␦
fibers by intra-epidermal needle electrode in humans. Pain 96, 247–252, http://
The authors declare that they have no competing interests. dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(01)00453-5.
S. Tobimatsu / Neuroscience Research 156 (2020) 117–129 129

Inui, K., Tsuji, T., Kakigi, R., 2006. Temporal analysis of cortical mechanisms for pain Peyron, R., Frot, M., Schneider, F., Garcia-Larrea, L., Mertens, P., Barral, F.G., Sin-
relief by tactile stimuli in humans. Cereb. Cortex 16, 355–365, http://dx.doi.org/ dou, M., Laurent, B., Mauguière, F., 2002. Role of operculoinsular cortices in
10.1016/S0304-3959(01)00453-5. human pain processing: converging evidence from PET, fMRI, dipole modeling,
Iwamura, Y., 1998. Hierarchical somatosensory processing. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 8, and intracerebral recordings of evoked potentials. NeuroImage 17, 1336–1346,
522–528, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(98)80041-X. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1315.
Kikuchi, Y., Ogata, K., Umesaki, T., Yoshiura, T., Kenjo, M., Hirano, Y., Okamoto, T., Polman, C.H., Reingold, S.C., Edan, G., Filippi, M., Hartung, H.P., Kappos, L., Lublin, F.D.,
Komune, S., Tobimatsu, S., 2011. Spatiotemporal signatures of an abnormal audi- Metz, L.M., McFarland, H.F., O’Connor, P.W., Sandberg-Wollheim, M., Thomp-
tory system in stuttering. NeuroImage 55, 891–899, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ son, A.J., Weinshenker, B.G., Wolinsky, J.S., 2005. Diagnostic criteria for multiple
j.neuroimage.2010.12.083. sclerosis: 2005 revisions to the “McDonald Criteria”. Ann. Neurol. 58, 840–846,
Kikuchi, Y., Okamoto, T., Ogata, K., Hagiwara, K., Umezaki, T., Kenjo, M., Nakagawa, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ana.20703.
T., Tobimatsu, S., 2017. Abnormal auditory synchronization in stuttering: a mag- Postma, A., Kolk, H., 1992. Error monitoring in people who stutter: evidence against
netoencephalographic study. Hear. Res. 344, 82–89, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ auditory feedback defect theories. J. Speech Hear. Res. 35, 1024–1032, http://dx.
j.heares.2016.10.027. doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3505.1024.
Knight, R.T., 2007. Neural networks debunk phrenology. Science 316, 1578–1579, Rainville, P., Duncan, G.H., Price, D.D., Carrier, B., Bushnell, M.C., 1997. Pain affect
http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1144677. encoded in human anterior cingulate but not somatosensory cortex. Science
Kraus, K.S., Canlon, B., 2012. Neuronal connectivity and interactions between the 277, 968–971, http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.277.5328.968.
auditory and limbic systems: effects of noise and tinnitus. Hear. Res. 288, 34–46, Roach, B.J., Mathalon, D.H., 2008. Event-related EEG time-frequency analysis: an
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2012.02.009. overview of measures and an analysis of early gamma band phase locking in
Lachaux, J.P., Rodriguez, E., Martinerie, J., Varela, F.J., 1999. Measuring phase syn- schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 34, 907–926, http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/
chrony in brain signals. Hum. Brain Mapp. 8, 194–208, http://dx.doi.org/10. sbn093.
1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:4<194::AID-HBM4>3.0.CO;2-C. Rosburg, T., Trautner, P., Ludowig, E., Helmstaedter, C., Bien, C.G., Elger, C.E., Boutros,
Lavasani, A.N., Mohammadkhani, G., Motamedi, M., Karimi, L.J., Jalaei, S., Shojaei, N.N., 2008. Sensory gating in epilepsy: effects of the lateralization of hippocam-
F., Danesh, A., Azimi, H., 2016. Auditory temporal processing in patients with pal sclerosis. Clin. Neurophysiol. 119, 1310–1319, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 60, 81–85, https//j.yebeh.2016.04.017. clinph.2008.02.007.
Lin, Y.Y., Forss, N., 2002. Functional characterization of human second somatosen- Spencer, K.M., Niznikiewicz, M.A., Shenton, M.E., McCarley, R.W., 2007. Sensory-
sory cortex by magnetoencephalography. Behav. Brain Res. 135, 141–145, http:// evoked gamma oscillations in chronic schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 63,
dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00143-2. 744–747, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.10.017.
Lincoln, M., Packman, A., Onslow, M., 2006. Altered auditory feedback and the treat- Staud, R., Robinson, M.E., Goldman, C.T., Price, D.D., 2011. Attenuation of exper-
ment of stuttering: a review. J. Fluency Disord. 31, 71–89, http://dx.doi.org/10. imental pain by vibro-tactile stimulation in patients with chronic local or
1016/j.jfludis.2006.04.001. widespread musculoskeletal pain. Eur. J. Pain 15, 836–842, http://dx.doi.org/
Matsubara, T., Ogata, K., Hironaga, N., Kikuchi, Y., Uehara, T., Chatani, H., Mitsudo, T., 10.1016/j.ejpain.2011.01.011.
Shigeto, H., Tobimatsu, S., 2018. Altered neural synchronization to pure tone Stephen, J.M., Ranken, D., Best, E., Adair, J., Knoefel, J., Kovacevic, S., 2006. Aging
stimulation in patients with mesial temporal lobe epilepsy: an MEG study. changes and gender differences in response to median nerve stimulation mea-
Epilepsy Behav. 88, 96–105 https://10.1016/j.yebeh.2018.08.036. sured with MEG. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 131–143, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
Matsubara, T., Ogata, K., Hironaga, N., Uehara, T., Mitsudo, T., Shigeto, H., Maekawa, T., clinph.2005.09.003.
Tobimatsu, S., 2019. Monaural 40-Hz auditory steady-state magnetic responses Tallon-Baudry, C., Bertrand, O., Delpuech, C., Pernier, J., 1996. Stimulus specificity of
can be useful for identifying epileptic focus in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. phase-locked and non-phase-locked 40 Hz visual responses in human. J. Neu-
Clin. Neurophysiol. 130, 341–351, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2018.11. rosci. 16, 4240–4249, http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-13-04240.
026. Tallon-Baudry, C., Bertrand, O., Delpuech, C., Permier, J., 1997. Oscillatory gamma-
Mancini, F., Nash, T., Iannetti, G.D., Haggard, P., 2014. Pain relief by touch: a quan- band (30-70 Hz) activity induced by a visual search task in humans. J. Neurosci.
titative approach. Pain 155, 635–642, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2013.12. 17, 722–734.
024. Tallon-Baudry, C., Bertrand, O., 1999. Oscillatory gamma activity in humans and its
Mazzola, L., Faillenot, I., Barral, F.G., Mauguière, F., Peyron, R., 2012. Spatial segre- role in object representation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 151–162, http://dx.doi.org/10.
gation of somatosensory and pain activations in the human operculo-insular 1016/S1364-6613(99)01299-1.
cortex. NeuroImage 60, 409–418, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011. Tecchio, F., Zito, G., Zappasodi, F., Dell’ Acqua, M.L., Landi, D., Nardo, D., Lupoi, D.,
12.072. Rossini, P.M., Filippi, M.M., 2008. Intra-cortical connectivity in multiple sclero-
Mehta, A.D., Ettinger, A.B., Perrine, K., Dhawan, V., Patil, A., Jain, S.K., Klein, G., Schnei- sis: a neurophysiological approach. Brain 131, 1783–1792, http://dx.doi.org/10.
der, S.J., Eidelberg, D., 2009. Seizure propagation in a patient with musicogenic 1093/brain/awn087.
epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 14, 421–424, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2008. Tobimatsu, S., Kakigi, R., 2016. Clinical Applications of Magnetoencephalography.
11.010. Springer, Germany.
Melzack, R., Wall, P.D., 1965. Pain mechanisms: a new theory. Science 150, 971–979, Tracey, I., Mantyh, P.W., 2007. The cerebral signature for pain perception and its
http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.150.3699.971. modulation. Neuron 55, 377–391, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2007.07.
Motoyama, Y., Ogata, K., Hoka, S., Tobimatsu, S., 2017. Frequency-dependent changes 012.
in sensorimotor and pain affective systems induced by empathy for pain. J. Pain Uhlhaas, P.J., Singer, W., 2015. Oscillations and neuronal dynamics in schizophrenia:
Res. 29 (May 10), 1317–1326, eCollection 2017 https://10.2147/JPR.S129791. the search for basic symptoms and translational opportunities. Biol. Psychiatry
Nunez, P.L., Nunez, M.D., Srinivasan, R., 2019. Multi-scale neural sources of EEG: 77, 1001–1009, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.11.019.
genuine, equivalent, and representative. A tutorial review. Brain Topogr. 32, Vinck, M., Oostenveld, R., van Wingerden, M., Battaglia, F., Pennartz, C.M., 2011. An
193–214, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10548-019-00701-3. improved index of phase-synchronization for electrophysiological data in the
Oertel, B.G., Preibisch, C., Martin, T., Walter, C., Gamer, M., Deichmann, R., Lötsch, J., presence of volume-conduction, noise and sample-size bias. NeuroImage 55,
2012. Separating brain processing of pain from that of stimulus intensity. Hum. 1548–1565, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.055.
Brain Mapp. 33, 883–894, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21256.

You might also like