You are on page 1of 10

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/269154992

Advertising Memory: The Power of Mirror Neurons

Article  in  Journal of Neuroscience Psychology and Economics · August 2014


DOI: 10.1037/npe0000025

CITATIONS READS
4 1,943

2 authors:

Sophie Lacoste-Badie Olivier Droulers


University of Lille (France) Université de Rennes 1
44 PUBLICATIONS   40 CITATIONS    87 PUBLICATIONS   203 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

IMAJ ; 2015-2018 View project

ASAFE; 2017-2020 View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Sophie Lacoste-Badie on 06 December 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics © 2014 American Psychological Association
2014, Vol. 7, No. 4, 195–202 1937-321X/14/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/npe0000025

Advertising Memory: The Power of Mirror Neurons


Sophie Lacoste-Badie and Olivier Droulers
University of Rennes

As part of the insights of neuroscience available for the study of advertising, the concept of
mirror neurons provides opportunities for new research. Mirror neurons are a particular
class of visuomotor neurons in the brain that show activity both when an individual
performs an action and when he observes another individual performing the same action.
According to researchers in cognitive neuroscience, mirror neurons are the brain basis for
learning by imitation. The observation of 2 types of movement specifically causes the
mirror neurons activation: “grasping with the hand” and “bringing to the mouth.” This
article seeks to show that TV commercials, in which a character grabs a food product and
brings it to his mouth, are more effective. To investigate this research question we designed
a between-subjects experiment (n ⫽ 130) to compare the memorization of 2 versions of the
same ad (featuring an unfamiliar mineral water brand). In the first ad version, the character
grabs the product, while there is no contact between the product and the character in the
second version. The article outlines that memory was higher in the grasping and drinking
condition. Although we cannot conclusively say that the mirror neurons were activated
when people saw the character grabbing the product in the ad, the literature on mirror
neurons provides an explanatory framework for the observed results. In other words, this
study points out that the discovery of mirror neurons is a very new concept for marketing,
which can enrich our understanding of advertising processing.

Keywords: advertising, character, hand, memorization, mirror neurons

For many researchers, one of the biggest ad- sumer behavior (Bagozzi et al., 2012; Dijksterhuis
vances in neuroscience in recent years is certainly et al., 2005; Janiszewski & Wyer, 2014). Since
the discovery of mirror neurons. In this regard, Weber (2007) discussed their implication for mar-
Ramachandran (Director of the Center for Brain keting practices (e.g., in the domains of new prod-
and Cognition, University of California, San Di- uct launch, creative strategy for advertising, sales
ego) predicts that “mirror neurons will do for promotion strategies, personal selling strategies)
psychology what DNA did for biology” (Ram- and underlined that “mirror neurons allow the
achandran, 2000). Mirror neurons are a particular consumer to learn vicariously in a passive envi-
class of visuomotor neurons in the brain that show ronment, which is an ideal scenario as it represents
activity both when an individual performs an ac- most situations in which the consumer is exposed
tion and when he observes another individual per- to a marketing message,” surprisingly very few
forming the same action. research have been conducted in the field of ad-
From a marketing perspective, the study of mir- vertising.
ror neurons can enrich our understanding of con- In this article we propose to import knowledge
from mirror neurons in the field of advertising and
test the influence of the product grabbing process
This article was published Online First September 22, performed by the ad main character. In other
2014. words, we want to see whether having the main
Sophie Lacoste-Badie and Olivier Droulers, CNRS character in an ad grabbing a product and bringing
(UMR 6211) CREM - Center for Research in Economics
and Management, University of Rennes, Graduate School of it to his mouth (for consumption) does improve
Management (IGR). the ad learning by the viewer.
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-
dressed to Sophie Lacoste-Badie, CNRS (UMR 6211) Literature Review
CREM - Center for Research in Economics and Manage-
ment, University of Rennes 1, Institute of Technology (IUT
GEA), Campus de Beaulieu, Avenue du général Leclerc - Mirror neurons were originally discovered in
CS 44202, 35042 Rennes CEDEX, France. E-mail: sophie the inferior frontal gyrus (F5 area) and inferior
.lacoste-badie@univ-rennes1.fr parietal lobule of the macaque monkey
195
196 LACOSTE-BADIE AND DROULERS

(Macaca nemestrina) (Di Pellegrino et al., contrast to previous findings (Decety et al.,
1992; Gallese et al., 1996; Rizzolatti et al., 1994; Tai et al., 2004), Gazzola et al. (2007)
1996). Recently Kilner et al. (2009), then Mu- then Peeters et al. (2009) show that the mirror
kamel et al. (2010) demonstrated their existence neurons system can respond to robotic actions
in the human brain. When an individual ob- as well as human actions.
serves the actions of another individual, con-
stantly active areas are the anterior portion of
the inferior parietal lobule (IPL; BA 40 area), . . . To Understanding Intention
the ventral premotor cortex, and the posterior
area of the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG; BA 44 Since 2005 several studies have suggested
area; considered as homologous human of mon- that the role of mirror neurons would not only
key area F5). Human mirror system presents a be limited to understanding observed actions
somatotopic organization: observation of motor but would also enable the comprehension of
acts performed by others with their leg, hand, other people’s intentions, while watching their
and mouth activates the IFG in a medial to actions. Iacoboni et al. (2005) in an fMRI ex-
lateral direction, as in the classical homunculus periment have exposed participants to three dif-
of Penfield and Rasmussen (1950). In IPL, ferent types of movie clips: Context, Action,
mouth motor acts are represented rostrally, and Intention (see Figure 1). In the context
hand/arm motor acts caudally, and leg motor condition, participants saw various objects (jam
acts even more caudally, and dorsally extending pot, teapot, cup, sugar bowl, dishes with cook-
into the superior parietal lobule (Rizzolatti & ies, milk jug) arranged either as if someone was
Fabbri-Destro, 2010). about to eat breakfast (context before tea) or as
if someone had already taken his breakfast (con-
From Understanding Actions . . . text after tea). In the action condition partici-
pants could see a hand grasping a cup of tea
The attribute of mirror neurons has been first with a precision grip (the fingers grasping the
to allow immediate recognition and understand- cup handle), or with a whole-hand prehension
ing of others’ actions through the congruence (the hand grasping the cup body). In the inten-
between motor and perceptual properties of mir- tion condition participants would see one of the
ror neurons: “we understand actions when we two action conditions embedded either in the
map the visual representation of the observed “before tea” context or in the “after tea” con-
action onto our motor representation of the text. Thus, one of the conditions would suggest
same action. According to this view, an action is that fingers are grasping the cup of tea to drink.
understood when its observation causes the mo- Then the other would suggest that the hand is
tor system of the observer to ‘resonate’” (Riz- grasping the cup to clean it. Compared with the
zolatti et al., 2001). Thereby, for example when action condition, the intention condition yielded
the observer sees someone grabbing a cup of significant signal increases in the right inferior
tea, the same population of neurons involved in frontal cortex (an area known to have mirror
execution of grasping movements becomes ac- neuron properties). Thus the authors conclude
tive in the observer’s motor areas. that this brain area is not only involved in the
Moreover, researchers found that the ma- recognition of a specific action but also for
caque monkey area F5 contains a population of understanding the intentions behind others’ ac-
neurons—audio-visual mirror neurons—that tions. They suggest that “a subset of mirror
discharge not only during the execution or ob- neurons in the inferior frontal cortex discharge
servation of a specific action but also when this in response to the motor acts that are most likely
action can only be heard (Kohler et al., 2002). to follow the observed one” (Iacoboni et al.,
Some neurons in F5 area responded to the vi- 2005). This proposal is shared by Rizzolatti and
sion and sound of a tearing action (paper rip- Craighero (2005): “These neurons discriminate
ping) but also to the sound of the same action one motor act from another, thus activating a
performed out of the monkey’s sight. These motor act chain that codes the final goal of the
audiovisual mirror neurons code actions inde- action. In this way the observing individual may
pendently of whether those actions are per- reenact internally the observed action and thus
formed, heard, or seen (Kohler et al., 2002). In predict the goal of the observed action. In this
ADVERTISING MEMORY: THE POWER OF MIRROR NEURONS 197

Figure 1. Images taken from the Context, Action, and Intention clips. From “Grasping the
Intentions of Others With One’s Own Mirror Neuron System,” by M. Iacoboni, I. Molnar-
Szakacs, V. Gallese, G. Buccino, J. C. Mazziotta, and G. Rizzolatti, 2005, PLoS Biology, 3,
e79, p. 530. Copyright 2005 by the Iacoboni et al. Reprinted with permission.

way, the observer can ‘read’ the intention of the and anterior inferior parietal lobule. The authors
acting individual.” concluded that the functioning of the mirror
neurons system differs across people systemat-
Mirror Neurons in Marketing Research ically and that the ability of the customer ori-
entation salespeople could be based on an effi-
Dijksterhuis et al. (2005) raise the role of the cient mirror neurons network.
mirror neuron system in the imitation behavior Curiously, although for many researchers
(« low road to imitation »), Henderson et al. mirror neurons constitute the neural substrate
(2011) discuss their role in a review about loy- for learning by imitation, research in advertising
alty programs, Janiszewski and Wyer (2014) communication based on mirror neurons knowl-
mention the role of mirrors neurons as support edge are rare. Ohme et al. (2009) published a
of mimicry behavior, and Weber (2007) in a study in which the authors reported that two
paper on mirror neurons and consumer behavior versions of the same advertising generated dif-
emphasizes that “mirror neurons allow the con- ferentiated electrophysiological responses
sumer to learn vicariously in a passive environ- (electroencephalography [EEG]; facial electro-
ment, which is an ideal scenario as it represents myography [EMGf]; skin conductance [SC]).
most situations in which the consumer is ex- Although the authors do not suggest it, it is
posed to a marketing message.” possible that mirror neurons of subjects exposed
However, experimental studies based on mir- to the ads are mobilized very differently, de-
ror neurons in marketing are still uncommon. pending on the version of the ad being watched:
Bagozzi et al. (2012) did explore with fMRI “in version 1, only the model’s face was pre-
neurological bases for customer orientation sented; conversely, in version 2, the viewers
(CO) and contrast them with sales orientation saw her face from a slightly different angle and
(SO). They show that CO is related positively to then she made a particular manual gesture. First,
activation of the mirror neuron system: the she touched her face with the back of her right
greater the CO, the greater the activation of the hand, and then when viewers could see her
posterior inferior frontal cortex, Broca’s area, whole body, she made a delicate hand move-
198 LACOSTE-BADIE AND DROULERS

ment and touched her stomach” (Ohme et al., study was part of a project designed to assess
2009). Thus in version 2, the observer sees the reactions to TV programs.
model making a gesture (putting one’s hand on
one’s face) with a particular intent (to caress). Materials
This could cause differentiation into neurons
mirror activation of observers and could explain One food product advertisement was selected
differentiated results. for this experiment. Ad selection meets several
Because the same neurons fire when we move criteria. The tested product had to be a product
to grasp an object and bring it to our mouth, intended for consumption by younger adults
when we look at someone grasping and bringing both male and female. Respondents had to be
an object to his mouth, because even some unfamiliar with the brand to neutralize potential
mirror neurons enable to anticipate upcoming prior exposure based effects and in the selected
actions (based on intention understanding), the advertisement the main character had to handle
discovery of mirror neurons, applied to an ad- the product. Finally, we selected a mineral wa-
vertising context, allows us to hypothesize that ter brand not marketed in the country hosting
advertisements, in which products are handled the experiment. In the original ad (version 1) the
by ad characters, are better learnt and memo- main character grasps the product and brings it
rized than advertisements in which products are to the mouth to drink. Using professional video
not handled. The proposed experiment was de- editing software we realized a modified version
signed to test this hypothesis. of the ad (version 2) in which the product is not
handled by the character (that is to say no con-
Method tact between the character and the object). The
original passages containing “grasping” and
Participants “drinking” scenes were replaced by a fixed
product shot (see Table 1).
One hundred thirty under- and postgraduate Several points were checked to ensure the
students were recruited (60% women), ranging effectiveness of the video versions. First, mo-
in age from 19 to 28 years (M ⫽ 21.22; SD ⫽ ment and duration of product presentation were
1.90). The recruitment of participants took the same in both versions. The advertising
place within university libraries. Participants soundtrack was also identical. Second, we con-
were instructed by the experimenter that the ducted several pretests to check the consistency

Table 1
Storyboards That Illustrate Each Experimental Advertisement
Audio (voice-over) Visual sequences
The modified version of the ad
Version 1 & 2: Identical ad The original ad “grasping and drinking” « no interaction » condition
soundtrack condition (version 1) (version 2)
“When you take care of yourself, Young adults gather after work
you need more”
« to move more » The characters play sports
« to laugh more » The characters have fun with a garden hose
« to drink » The characters gather in a kitchen
« and eat » the main character grasps the product a fixed product shot
« more balanced meals » The characters prepare a meal together
« BRAND » the main character grasps the product a fixed product shot
“0% ⫹ This is mineral water, fruit, Mineral water visual
fine bubbles,”
« calcium, magnesium » the main character grasps the product a fixed product shot
« and 0% of calories » the main character brings a glass of mineral a fixed product shot
water to his mouth and drinks
“BRAND, taking care of yourself
is a pleasure” Visual packshot
ADVERTISING MEMORY: THE POWER OF MIRROR NEURONS 199

of the story. Tests to measure the advertising Table 2


aesthetics and narrative continuity showed no Means for Product and Brand Recall, and Product,
significant difference between the two versions. Packaging, and Brand Recognition (Standard
A pod has been developed with this target ad- Deviation in Parentheses)
vertisement and 6 distractive advertisements Condition 1 Condition 2
(20 seconds each). This pod was inserted in a Response (n ⫽ 65) (n ⫽ 65) t p
neutral TV program (documentary film about Recall
mass consumer computing). Product .44 (.50) .22 (.41) ⫺2.74 .007
Brand .28 (.45) .06 (.24) ⫺3.42 .001
Experimental Procedures Recognition
Product .50 (.50) .23 (.42) ⫺3.27 .001
Participants were randomly assigned to the Packaging .84 (.36) .60 (.49) ⫺3.18 .002
Condition 1 (“grasping and drinking”; n ⫽ 65) Brand .72 (.45) .58 (.49) ⫺1.60 .111
or to the Condition 2 (“no interaction”; n ⫽ 65;
between-subjects design). They watched a 15-
minute TV program (the documentary in which .50) than in the “no interaction” condition (M ⫽
a pod was inserted at the 7th minute). The .23), t(122) ⫽ ⫺3.27, p ⫽ .001, r ⫽ .28.
experimenter asked them to “relax and watch Packaging recognition was also higher in the
the TV program as if they were at home” and “grasping and drinking” condition (M ⫽ .84)
made no reference to the advertisements. than in the “no interaction” condition (M ⫽
After seeing the program, participants com- .60), t(118) ⫽ ⫺3.18, p ⫽ .002, r ⫽ .28.
pleted a questionnaire. First they performed However, we found no significant differences
free-recall product and brand tests, then product in brand recognition between the two condi-
brand and packaging recognition tests. Finally, tions, t(126) ⫽ ⫺1.60, p ⫽ .111.
they answered brand awareness, evaluation pro- In conclusion, the results of the experiment
gram, TV use, product category involvement, provided support for the hypothesis in terms of
and demographic questions. recall and recognition (except for brand recog-
nition). Findings represent a medium-sized ef-
Results fect (r ⬇ .30).

Manipulation Check Discussion

As expected, the brand was unknown to all As outlined previously, mirror neurons are
participants. We also checked several variables considered as one of the most interesting dis-
to verify the comparability of the two samples: coveries in human and social sciences in recent
the difference was not significant for evaluation years. Mirror neurons are involved in both the
program (p ⬎ .70), for TV use (p ⬎ .65), for understanding of motor acts performed by oth-
product category involvement (p ⬎ .65), and ers and the understanding of intention behind
will not be discussed further. the acts (Rizzolatti et al., 2009). Today, the
contribution of neuroscience to marketing and
Advertising Memory in particular to the study of consumer behavior
is essential. In line with advances in neurosci-
As shown in Table 2, Product recall was ence research, some researchers are beginning
significantly higher in the “grasping and drink- to take an interest in the study of mirror neurons
ing” condition (M ⫽ .44) than in the “no inter- in marketing contexts (Bagozzi et al., 2012).
action” condition (M ⫽ .22), t(122) ⫽ ⫺2.74, The present study proposed to examine the
p ⫽ .007, r ⫽ .24. marketing practices such as advertising design
Brand recall was significantly higher in the mobilizing neuroscientific knowledge in mirror
“grasping and drinking” condition (M ⫽ .28) neurons. The results lent some support to the
than in the “no interaction” condition (M ⫽ hypothesis according to which commercials
.06), t(95) ⫽ ⫺3.42, p ⫽ .001, r ⫽ .33. featuring a character grabbing a product and
Product recognition was significantly higher bringing it to his mouth to eat (or drink) benefit
in the “grasping and drinking” condition (M ⫽ from better learning— here measured in terms
200 LACOSTE-BADIE AND DROULERS

of advertising recall and recognition—than only grasping, only drinking, or grasping and
commercials in which the products are not han- drinking together.
dled. Although we cannot conclusively say that Second, it seems that the functioning of neu-
the mirror neurons were activated when people ron mirror system is modulated by the expertise
saw the character handle the product in the ad, and motor repertoire of the observer. Using
the literature on mirror neurons provides an functional MRI, Calvo-Merino et al. (2005) ex-
explanatory framework for the observed results. posed experts in classical ballet, experts in ca-
This result is consistent with Ohme et al. (2009) poeira, and nonexperts in ballet and capoeira
study that indicated that the brain can register (for control purpose) to videos of ballet or ca-
even small differences between the ads. poeira moves. They observed greater bilateral
To our knowledge no work based on mirror activations in classical mirror areas—premotor
neuron system has been conducted in the con- cortex, parietal cortex, superior temporal sul-
text of advertising persuasion and memoriza- cus—when expert dancers viewed movements
tion. This first study was conducted among a that they had been trained to perform compared
population of young adults (130 individuals). It with movements they had not. The authors con-
would be interesting to stretch this study to a clude that results show that this ‘mirror system’
much older and larger population. Our results integrates observed actions of others with an
show that advertisers should design TV com- individual’s personal motor repertoire, and sug-
mercials in which characters handled the prod- gest that the human brain understands actions
ucts. This may seem obvious, but in a prelimi- by motor simulation. Because action observa-
nary study conducted on more than 800 TV tion may recruit such mirror areas to the extent
commercials in Europe we observed that the that the observed action is represented in the
product was handled by the main character in subject’s personal motor repertoire, it would be
only 44% of ads. Although further work is interesting to test the effect of seen actions in
required to gain a more complete understanding advertisements that the observer can more or
of the processing of advertising that involve less easily transforms into inner representation.
handling the product, our findings indicate that Third, this research calls into question the
the observation of actions performed by others possible part of neuron mirrors in triggering in
may well be a significant lever for improving the viewer a behavior. It could be interesting to
advertisements. find out whether or not the fact of observing in
The exploratory nature of the study can lay an ad someone bringing food (product) to his
the foundation for future research. First, it mouth for consumption can trigger the same
seems that there are motor sequences that cause behavior in the viewer in a short or medium
preferential activation of neurons mirrors. Iaco- term to a point of potential overconsumption.
boni et al. (2005) stressed that in both action This question regarding the potential influence
and intention conditions there was an activation of (an ad character’s) behavior observation on
of the mirror neurons system and that, com- our own behavior remains open to discussion,
pared with the action condition, intention con- as one of the key attributes of neuron mirrors is
dition yielded significant signal increases in the their involvement in the understanding of the
mirror neurons area (right inferior frontal action of someone we observe or the under-
gyrus). But another finding of this work lays in standing of his intention, without it automati-
the observation of the tea-cup-grasping-to-drink cally causing us to adopt the same behavior.
movement, which produced stronger activation Researching more in depth the question of neu-
than did the observation of the cup-grasping-to- ron mirrors influence upon imitation-based be-
clean movement. Rizzolatti et al. (2009) suggest haviors—in the context of food, alcohol or to-
that this result is similar to findings of studies bacco consumption— could potentially lead to
conducted on monkeys, which show that the improving the design of prevention campaign.
number of neurons that code “grasping for Finally, the authors believe that before mobi-
bringing” to the mouth largely exceeds the num- lizing sophisticated neuroscientific techniques
ber of neurons that code “grasping for putting to visualize activation of brain areas, it is useful
an object into a container.” In an advertising to carry out exploratory studies based on con-
context, it could be interesting to test different ventional methods in marketing. On one hand,
ways to handle the product, and to compare as this study shows, first significant results can
ADVERTISING MEMORY: THE POWER OF MIRROR NEURONS 201

be underlined; on the other hand, these studies in human inferior frontal gyrus. The Journal of
allow to refine the research question and better Neuroscience, 29, 10153–10159. doi:10.1523/
justify a deepening of research mobilizing more JNEUROSCI.2668-09.2009
complex techniques. Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umilta, A., Fogassi, L.,
Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Hearing
sounds, understanding actions: Action representa-
tion in mirror neurons. Science, 297, 846 – 848.
References doi:10.1126/science.1070311
Bagozzi, R. P., Verbeke, W. J., van den Berg, W. E., Mukamel, R., Ekstrom, A. D., Kaplan, J., Iacoboni,
Rietdijk, W. J., Dietvorst, R. C., & Worm, L. M., & Fried, I. (2010). Single-neuron responses in
(2012). Genetic and neurological foundations of humans during execution and observation of ac-
customer orientation: Field and experimental evi- tions. Current Biology, 20, 750 –756. doi:10.1016/
dence. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Sci- j.cub.2010.02.045
ence, 40, 639 – 658. doi:10.1007/s11747-011- Ohme, R., Wiener, D., Reykowska, D., & Choroman-
0271-4 ska, A. (2009). Analysis of neurophysiological re-
Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grèzes, J., Passing- actions to advertising stimuli by means of EEG
ham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2005). Action obser- and galvanic skin response measures. Journal of
vation and acquired motor skills: An fMRI study Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 2, 1,
with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 8, 1243– 21–31. doi:10.1037/a0015462
1249. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi007 Peeters, R., Simone, L., Nelissen, K., Fabbri-Destro,
Decety, J., Perani, D., Jeannerod, M., Bettinardi, V., M., Vanduffel, W., Rizzolatti, G., & Orban, G. A.
Tadary, B., Woods, R., . . . Fazio, F. (1994). (2009). The representation of tool use in humans
Mapping motor representations with positron and monkeys: Common and uniquely human fea-
emission tomography. Nature, 371, 600 – 602. doi: tures. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 11523–
10.1038/371600a0 11539. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2040-09.2009
Dijksterhuis, A., Smith, P. K., van Baaren, R. B., & Penfield, W., & Rasmussen, T. (1950). The cerebral
Wigboldus, D. H. J. (2005). The unconscious con- cortex of man. A clinical study of localization of
sumer: Effects of environment on consumer be- function. New York, NY: Macmillan.
havior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 15, 3, Ramachandran, V. S. (2000). Mirror neurons and imi-
193–202. doi:10.1207/s15327663jcp1503_3 tation learning as the driving force behind “the great
Di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., leap forward” in human evolution. Retrieved March
& Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor 4, 2014, from http://www.edge.org/3rd_culture/
events: A neurophysiological study. Experimental ramachandran/ramachandran_pl.html
Brain Research, 91, 176 –180. doi:10.1007/ Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2005). Mirror neu-
BF00230027 ron: A neurological approach to empathy. In J.-P.
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. Changeux, A. R. Damasio, W. Singer, & Y. Chris-
(1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. ten (Eds.), Neurobiology of human values (pp.
Brain, 119, 593– 609. doi:10.1093/brain/119.2.593 107–123). Berlin, Germany: Springer-Verlag. doi:
Gazzola, V., Rizzolatti, G., Wicker, B., & Keysers, 10.1007/3-540-29803-7_9
C. (2007). The anthropomorphic brain: The mirror Rizzolatti, G., & Fabbri-Destro, M. (2010). Mirror
neuron system responds to human and robotic ac- neurons: From discovery to autism. Experimental
tions. NeuroImage, 35, 1674 –1684. doi:10.1016/j Brain Research, 200, 223–237. doi:10.1007/
.neuroimage.2007.02.003 s00221-009-2002-3
Henderson, C. M., Beck, J. T., & Palmatier, R. W. Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., & Fogassi, L.
(2011). Review of the theoretical underpinnings of (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of
loyalty programs. Journal of Consumer Psychol- motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 131–
ogy, 21, 256 –276. doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2011.02.007 141. doi:10.1016/0926-6410(95)00038-0
Iacoboni, M., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Gallese, V., Buc- Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2001).
cino, G., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the
Grasping the intentions of others with one’s own understanding and imitation of action. Nature Re-
mirror neuron system. PLoS Biology, 3, 3, e79. views Neuroscience, 2, 661– 670. doi:10.1038/
doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0030079 35090060
Janiszewski, C., & Wyer, R. S., Jr. (2014). Content Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2009). The
and process priming: A review. Journal of Con- mirror neuron system: A motor-based mechanism
sumer Psychology, 24, 1, 96 –118. doi:10.1016/j for action and intention understanding. In M. S.
.jcps.2013.05.006 Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences
Kilner, J. M., Neal, A., Weiskopf, N., Friston, K. J., (fourth ed., pp. 625– 640). London, UK: The MIT
& Frith, C. D. (2009). Evidence of mirror neurons Press Cambridge.
202 LACOSTE-BADIE AND DROULERS

Tai, Y. F., Scherfler, C., Brooks, D. J., Sawamoto, N., strategies. Academy of Marketing Studies Journal,
& Castiello, U. (2004). The human premotor cor- 11, 2, 57– 68.
tex is ‘mirror’ only for biological actions. Current
Biology, 14, 117–120. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2004.01
.005 Received March 15, 2014
Weber, J. M. (2007). Mirror neurons networks: Im- Revision received July 17, 2014
plications for modeling and consumer behavior Accepted August 5, 2014 䡲

New Editors Appointed, 2016 –2021

The Publications and Communications Board of the American Psychological Association


announces the appointment of 9 new editors for 6-year terms beginning in 2016. As of
January 1, 2015, manuscripts should be directed as follows:

● History of Psychology (http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/hop/), Nadine M.


Weidman, PhD, Harvard University
● Journal of Family Psychology (http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/fam/), Barbara
H. Fiese, PhD, University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign
● JPSP: Personality Processes and Individual Differences (http://www.apa.org/pubs/
journals/psp/), M. Lynne Cooper, PhD, University of Missouri—Columbia
● Psychological Assessment (http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/pas/), Yossef S. Ben-
Porath, PhD, Kent State University
● Psychological Review (http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/rev/), Keith J. Holyoak,
PhD, University of California, Los Angeles
● International Journal of Stress Management (http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/
str/), Oi Ling Siu, PhD, Lingnan University, Tuen Mun, Hong Kong
● Journal of Occupational Health Psychology (http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/
ocp/), Peter Y. Chen, PhD, Auburn University
● Personality Disorders (http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/per/), Thomas A. Widi-
ger, PhD, University of Kentucky
● Psychology of Men & Masculinity (http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/men/), Wil-
liam Ming Liu, PhD, University of Iowa

Electronic manuscript submission: As of January 1, 2015, manuscripts should be


submitted electronically to the new editors via the journal’s Manuscript Submission Portal
(see the website listed above with each journal title).

Current editors Wade E. Pickren, PhD, Nadine J. Kaslow, PhD, Laura A. King, PhD,
Cecil R. Reynolds, PhD, John Anderson, PhD, Sharon Glazer, PhD, Carl W. Lejuez, PhD,
and Ronald F. Levant, EdD, will receive and consider new manuscripts through December
31, 2014.
Copyright of Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, & Economics is the property of
Association for NeuroPsychoEconomics and its content may not be copied or emailed to
multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission.
However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

View publication stats

You might also like