You are on page 1of 13

Guide to observational vs.

experimental studies
Observational studies Experimental studies Systematic reviews & meta-analyses

Convincing evidence When to trust observational studies Pros and cons Take home points

Although findings from the latest nutrition studies often make news headlines
and are shared widely on social media, many aren’t based on strong scientific
evidence.
You’ve no doubt noticed that there are conflicting reports about whether a
food is good or bad for you. One day headlines will say drinking coffee is
overwhelmingly beneficial, but the following day new headlines shout that
coffee increases risk of heart attacks.

Indeed, most foods and diets have been both positively and negatively
associated with health in various studies. According to a 2012 article, eggs,
carrots, cheese and several other foods have been shown to both increase
and decrease risk of cancer, depending on the study! 1

To say that this can be confusing and frustrating is an understatement. Many


of us do our best to make food choices that will improve our health and quality
of life. How can we know if the latest research being reported is reliable?

Generally speaking, the media fail to evaluate the evidence; instead, studies
with “exciting” conclusions are turned into click-worthy headlines, no matter
how weak the evidence is.

Most of the studies used to connect foods to chronic diseases are


observational studies, not experimental ones. While these are far cheaper and
easier to do, they can very rarely prove anything at all. 2

In this guide, we discuss the differences between observational and


experimental studies, the advantages and disadvantages of each, and why in
nearly all cases observational research shouldn’t be used when making
decisions about your diet. After reading this guide, you may be able to identify
media reports about nutritional science that you can safely ignore, i.e. most of
them.

In our evidence-based guides at Diet Doctor, we make it simple by using a


color code to show how strong evidence a study provides: strong, moderate,
weak or very weak evidence. 3 After reading this guide, you’ll
understand much more about what that means.
What is an observational study?
In an observational study (also known as an epidemiological study),
researchers observe a group of people to see what happens to them over
time. Although study participants may answer questions and fill out
questionnaires, researchers don’t conduct any experiments and have no
control over the participants.

An observational study is basically an exercise in statistics. Researchers try to


find correlations between certain behaviours and certain outcomes. For
example, do people who eat more vegetables have a larger or smaller risk of
developing a certain disease?

Although the statistics from observational studies can show associations


between certain behaviors and the development of a disease or condition,
these associations may or may not be cause-effect relationships. 4 In
most cases, an observational study is not enough to be able to tell. An
observational study can often just provide very weak evidence. 5 A
different kind of study, usually an experimental one, is needed to prove that
something causes something else, for example that drinking coffee can make
people lose weight.

There are good reasons for the famous quote stating that “there are three
kinds of lies: lies, damned lies, and statistics.”

Observational studies – i.e. statistics – can in most cases just provide weak
clues, and they can rarely prove anything. Of course, the study is not a lie – it’s
just data. But the way these studies are often used in the media, to “prove” a
thing one day and the exact opposite the next day, can bring that quote to
mind.

What is an experimental study?


In a nutrition-related experimental study (also known as a clinical trial or
interventional study), researchers provide participants with a diet, nutrition
education, or other kind of intervention and evaluate its effects.
Experimental evidence is considered stronger than observational evidence.
Randomized, controlled trials (RCTs) are often referred to as the “gold
standard” for evidence. They are designed to test an intervention against a
different intervention (i.e. low carb vs. low fat), or against a control group that
does not change its behaviors (i.e low carb vs standard American diet), under
tightly monitored conditions.

Assigning participants randomly to either the experimental or the control


group helps to ensure that both groups are similar in ways that are not being
tested (such as income, education, level of exercise, etc.). This makes these
studies (in best case) a fair comparison, and makes the evidence they provide
far stronger: often moderately strong evidence.

The best RCTs use the actual development of the disease being studied or
death of the participant as the outcome being measured. Because medical
conditions may take many years to develop, decades-long RCTs are very
expensive, making them impractical in most cases. Therefore, many RCTs are
much shorter, and instead of measuring health outcomes, they measure
changes in health markers that reflect disease risk, such as changes in blood
sugar, insulin, or inflammation levels.

Unfortunately, this assumes the changes in a surrogate marker reflect a


positive or negative impact on one’s health. As we have seen in many studies,
this may not always be the case.

The ultimate solution in the future – for very important questions regarding
human diet and health – may be funding far fewer but much larger
experimental studies. Studies that are large and long enough to measure real
health outcomes. For example, studies that test a strict low-carb diet vs. a
conventional diet on hundreds or thousands of people with diabetes type 2 for
5-10 years, and measure outcomes like disease and death. A high-quality
study like this could easily cost hundreds of millions of dollars, yet the results
could be worth orders of magnitude more for humanity. 6

Systematic reviews and meta-analyses


A single study on its own is often not enough to provide clear answers about
the relationship between food and health. Systematic reviews and meta-
analyses are both ways of putting together multiple studies in an attempt to
clarify what the evidence says.

A systematic review is a detailed, standardized process of gathering,


assessing and synthesizing a collection of relevant studies on a particular
topic.

A meta-analysis is a statistical procedure for combining data from the studies


used in a systematic review.

Systematic reviews and meta-analyses may consist of observational research,


experimental research, or a combination of both. They have historically been
considered the strongest type of evidence; however, this is not always the
case.

Systematic reviews and meta-analyses are sometimes seen as ways to


“strengthen” the weak findings of observational studies. The thinking is that if
a number of observational studies show the same effect, this must indicate a
cause-effect relationship even if the effect is very small in all cases. But
systematic reviews and meta-analyses made up of observational studies
cannot override the fundamental principle that association is not causation. If
you took a placebo pill that had no effect on a condition you wanted to treat, it
wouldn’t work better if you took more of them! In the same way, weak
observational studies do not develop rigor by combining many of them.

Systematic reviews and meta-analyses can be flawed in other ways as well,


including cherry-picking studies for inclusion that support a researcher’s own
position and excluding those that do not. 7

Only RCTs (experimental studies) can come close to establishing that a


certain food or way of eating causes a particular outcome. Systematic
reviews and meta-analyses based on experimental studies have a much
greater chance of providing good evidence on which to base decisions about
your own health. We grade these as strong evidence.
Why many observational studies don’t provide
convincing evidence
Observational studies can only give us information about how certain
behaviors and diseases are associated or correlated. An association must be
very strong in order to indicate a potential cause-effect relationship, and even
strong associations do not necessarily show this. For example, skirt-wearing
is strongly associated with the likelihood of developing breast cancer (since
they are mostly worn by women!), but it would be silly to suggest that wearing
a skirt causes breast cancer.

Typically, the strength of associations in observational studies about nutrition


and chronic diseases is small, as reflected by the low relative risks that are
found. A relative risk of 1.0 means there is no association. In most
observational studies about nutrition, the relative risk is close to 1.0, with a
range of 0.8 to 1.5, indicating a weak association. 8 Weak associations
are likely to be due to other factors such as random chance or confounding
variables, and not likely to be a cause-effect relationship.

The reasons for such weak associations are often built into the design of
observational studies. Because scientists are only observing a selected
population, they cannot take into account all the possible factors that might
affect how diet appears to be related to a disease.

For example, people who are concerned about their health are likely to choose
foods they think will help prevent disease. But they are also more likely to do
many other things they think will promote and protect their health, such as
exercising regularly, avoiding smoking, and taking a multivitamin. It is hard to
know which of these factors are responsible for outcomes found in an
observational study.

Professor John Ioannidis is a highly-regarded expert in meta-research, the


study of research practices and how to improve them. In September of 2018,
he wrote an opinion piece for the Journal of the American Medical
Association stating that nutrition observational studies are hopelessly flawed
and in need of “radical reform.” 9 In the article, he points out that hidden
factors that may bias the outcomes of an observational study are not
accounted for (for instance, people who eat a lot of meat may also drink a lot
of beer and get little exercise) and that findings are routinely influenced by
researcher bias.
He also points out the absurdity of claiming that certain foods will increase
lifespan for a specific length of time. As an example, various studies show
that consuming hazelnuts, coffee, oranges, and other foods and beverages on
a daily basis may each help extend life by several years.

“If you were to gain all the benefit speculated by each one of these studies, we
would be able to live for 5,000 years,” says Ioannidis.

When can the results of observational research be


trusted?
Findings from observational studies usually need to be confirmed by higher-
quality research, such as an experimental study, to be considered reliable.
Unfortunately, this often do not happen. Instead, experimental trials in
nutrition are likely to contradict the findings of earlier observational studies. 10

In other words, findings from observational studies can usually not be trusted
on their own.

However, there are occasional exceptions. Under exceptional and rare


circumstances with very, very strong and plausible correlations (e.g. smoking
and lung cancer), observational data can provide moderate evidence. 11

Pros and cons of observational studies


Pros:
Are much less expensive than clinical trials

Can last for several years or even decades

Can include tens of thousands of study participants


Can look at development of a disease or death as an outcome

Cons:
Rely on self-reported data that often can’t be confirmed

Have no control group for comparison

Can’t take into account all of the factors that can influence the results

Are not cause-effect relationships

Pros and cons of experimental studies


Pros:
Are tightly controlled and monitored

Compare outcomes between those who receive an intervention and those who
don’t

Can use randomization to deal with unknown factors that might influence
outcomes

Cons:
Are expensive and time consuming

Use health markers rather than development of disease or death as endpoints

Are typically smaller than most observational studies

Are usually shorter than most observational studies

Take home points


Observational research usually produces unreliable results, and these results
are often given more attention in the media than they deserve.

Before changing your diet based on the most recent news story, find out a few
things about the study being discussed. Is the study observational or
experimental? Are the findings consistent with previous research, especially
with higher-quality studies like experimental ones? If the study is
observational, how strong were the associations between the outcome and
the behavior, food, or diet being studied?

Most importantly, remember that observational studies usually can’t show that
a specific food, diet or lifestyle caused a particular outcome. This normally
requires an experimental study.

The bottom line is that most observational studies, and all the media
headlines generated by them, can safely be ignored.

Franziska Spritzler, RD

DD+ MEMBERSHIP

Start your FREE 30-day trial!


Get instant access to healthy low-carb and keto meal plans, fast and easy
recipes, weight loss advice from medical experts, and so much more. A
healthier life starts now with your free trial!

Start FREE trial!


All articles and guides by Franziska Spritzler
Franziska Spritzler is a registered dietitian, author and certified diabetes edu‐
cator who takes a low-carb, real-food approach to diabetes, weight manage‐
ment and overall health.
Adele Hite, RD
Adele came to rhetoric and communication from a Ph.D. program in nutritional
epidemiology and a background in nutrition, dietetics, and public health. She’s
animated by questions and concerns, many of which boil down to this: Why is
nutrition [science, policy, discourse] the way it is?

Understanding absolute and relative risk


GUIDE Although it seems as if numbers should be objective and trust‐
worthy, there are many ways that they can be used to distort the truth. Entire
books have been written about this subject. Let’s take a look at the differences
between absolute risk and relative risk.
More
The Diet Doctor policy for grading scientific evidence 

The Diet Doctor policy for evidence-based guides 


1. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 2013: Is everything we eat associated with
cancer? A systematic cookbook review

2. Advances in Nutrition 2018: Limiting dependence on nonrandomized studies and


improving randomized trials in human nutrition research: why and how

JAMA 2018: The challenge of reforming nutritional epidemiologic research

PLoS Medicine 2005: Why most published research findings are false ↩

3. For the full details about our evidence-grading policy, see this page:

The Diet Doctor policy for grading scientific evidence ↩

4. A confounding variable is one that is not taken into consideration in the study.


Confounding variables can introduce bias and indicate a relationship between a food
or diet and a health outcome when there isn’t one.

5. Though there are exceptions:

The Diet Doctor policy for grading scientific evidence ↩


6. Advances in Nutrition 2018: Limiting dependence on nonrandomized studies and
improving randomized trials in human nutrition research: why and how

7. The Milbank Quarterly 2016: The mass production of redundant, misleading, and
conflicted systematic reviews and meta-analyses

8. There is some discussion about what is considered a “weak” versus a “strong”


association and how strong an association must be to potentially indicate a cause-
effect relationship.

A helpful comparision is that relative risks found in assocations between smoking and
lung cancer were around 10.0 for moderate smokers and 20.0 for heavy smokers. This
level of relative risk was strong enough for experts to argue for a cause-effect
relationship.


American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 1999: Causal criteria in nutritional epidemiology


9. Journal of the American Medical Association 2018: The challenge of reforming
nutritional epidemiologic research

10. In the past few decades, there have many instances where the results of observational
nutrition studies have been contradicted in RCTs.

Significance 2011: Deming, data and observational studies: A process out of control
and needing fixing

Seminars in Oncology 2010: Epidemiological and clinical studies of nutrition ↩

11. For us to use this evidence grade, HR needs to be consistently > 5 in several high-
quality observational studies, with biological plausibility, no other obvious explanation
and generally following the classic Bradford Hill criteria.


Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine 1965: The environment and disease:
association or causation? By Sir Austin Bradford Hill

You might also like