You are on page 1of 12

Learning and Individual Differences 67 (2018) 41–52

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Learning and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/lindif

Understanding the academic motivations of students with a history of T


reading difficulty: An expectancy-value-cost approach

Bradley W. Bergeya, , Rauno K. Parrilab, S. Hélène Deaconc
a
Queens College, City University of New York, Secondary Education and Youth Services Department, 65-30 Kissena Blvd., Flushing, NY 11367, United States
b
Macquarie University, Department of Educational Studies, NSW 2109, Australia
c
Dalhousie University, Psychology and Neuroscience Department, Life Science Centre, P.O. Box 15000, Halifax, Nova Scotia B3H 4R2, Canada

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: We examined the academic self-efficacy, positive subjective task values, and perceived effort cost of first-year
Motivation undergraduates with (n = 168) and without (n = 314) a self-reported history of reading difficulty, and further
Academic achievement their relations with academic achievement and satisfaction. Students with a self-reported history of reading
Expectancy-value theory difficulty described lower academic self-efficacy, earned lower grades, and accrued fewer credits. The groups did
Cost
not differ significantly in their positive task values, effort cost, academic satisfaction, or institutional retention.
Reading difficulty
Path analyses indicated that for both groups, academic self-efficacy and effort cost were predictive of first-year
academic performance while intrinsic value was predictive of academic satisfaction and institutional retention.
Multi-group analyses indicated a significant group difference: academic self-efficacy explained unique variance
in academic satisfaction for students without a self-reported history of reading difficulty, but not for those who
reported such a history. We discuss implications of the relations between difficulties in reading acquisition and
motivations.

1. Introduction task values for obtaining a university degree. We also examine how
these motivational perceptions are associated with first-year academic
The academic transition from high school to university is challen- performance, academic satisfaction, and institutional retention for
ging for many types of students, but particularly so for students with students with and without a self-reported history of reading difficulty.
learning difficulties (Eckes & Ochoa, 2005; Madaus, 2005). One group Understanding how motivational perceptions are linked to academic
that is academically vulnerable during this transition is university stu- outcomes for students with a history of reading difficulties illuminates
dents with a history of reading difficulty. While these students achieve how universities can support the academic success of this at-risk po-
academic standing sufficient for admission to university, they tend to pulation.
have below-average reading abilities (Deacon, Cook, & Parrila, 2012).
As a result, they experience greater academic difficulty at university 1.1. Expectancy-value theory
and may face above-average risk of dropping out (Bergey, Deacon, &
Parrila, 2017; Chevalier, Parrila, Ritchie, & Deacon, 2017). Among Expectancy-value theory offers a broad theoretical framework for
general university populations, motivational factors have been found to understanding the development of motivation and its influence on
have strong influences on academic outcomes (Schneider & Preckel, choices, persistence, and achievement in academic settings. Eccles and
2017), though little is known about the academic motivations of stu- her colleagues (Eccles et al., 1983; Eccles & Wigfield, 1995; Wigfield &
dents with a history of reading difficulty. In the current study, we ex- Eccles, 2000) developed the most prominent recent articulation of ex-
amine these motivations through the lens of expectancy-value theory pectancy-value theory for academic contexts. According to Eccles et al.,
(Eccles et al., 1983), which posits that motivation for academic tasks is motivation is a function of one's expectation for success and values for a
driven by expectations for success (e.g., Will I be successful?) and the task (Wigfield & Eccles, 2000). When individuals perceive themselves
perceived value for the task (e.g., Do I care?). We investigate whether to be capable of a task and expect to be successful, they are more likely
students who self-report a history of reading difficulties differ from to choose to engage in it, persist at it longer, and achieve better out-
students with no such history in academic self-efficacy and subjective comes. Subjective task values refer to the extent to which an individual


Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: bradley.bergey@qc.cuny.edu (B.W. Bergey), rauno.parrila@mq.edu.au (R.K. Parrila), helene.deacon@dal.ca (S.H. Deacon).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2018.06.008
Received 2 May 2017; Received in revised form 21 June 2018; Accepted 23 June 2018
1041-6080/ © 2018 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
B.W. Bergey et al. Learning and Individual Differences 67 (2018) 41–52

cares about or values the task at hand. Within the Eccles et al.'s model, fulfillment in the role or experiences of being a student. There has been
subjective task values are comprised of four interrelated values: in- a growing interest in academic satisfaction as an important outcome in
trinsic value, utility value, attainment value, and cost. Intrinsic value educational experiences (e.g., Garriott, Hudyma, Keene, & Santiago,
refers to the enjoyment experienced while engaging in the task. Utility 2015; Lent, 2004; Ojeda, Flores, & Navarro, 2011; Sheu, Mejia, Rigali-
value refers to how useful the task is for reaching immediate and future Oiler, Primé, & Chong, 2016). This trend follows the long-standing at-
goals. Attainment value refers to the extent to which the task is deemed tention that job satisfaction has received in motivational research in
important to individual identity. Cost refers to what is suffered or given employment settings. Lent and colleagues (Lent et al., 2005; Lent,
up as a result of engaging in the task (Eccles, 2009; Wigfield & Eccles, Singley, Sheu, Schmidt, & Schmidt, 2007) have identified self-efficacy
2000; Wigfield, Tonks, & Eccles, 2004). Intrinsic, utility and attainment and interest as positive predictors of academic satisfaction. Satisfaction
values are theorized to motivate task initiation and persistence while has also been found to be shaped by acting in alignment with personal
costs exert the opposite influence. values and goals (Ryan & Deci, 2001) and therefore tasks that are
Self-efficacy is the belief that one can organize and execute actions perceived to be personally relevant, aligned with life goals, and im-
to achieve designated results (Bandura, 1997). Applied to educational portant to one's sense of self are likely to support satisfaction percep-
contexts, academic self-efficacy refers to the belief that one can ac- tions.
complish academic tasks at designated levels (Pajares, 1996; Schunk,
1991). Individuals who believe they are capable of success in a task are 1.4. Associations between motivational beliefs and retention via academic
more likely to initiate and persist at a challenging task. Academic self- performance and satisfaction
efficacy is a type of expectancy belief and is closely related to the
construct of expectancy for success, which Eccles et al. defined as the In a seminal paper on student departure from higher education and
belief in how well one will perform on future tasks (Wigfield & Eccles, in subsequent work, Tinto (1975, 2007) developed a sociological model
2000). The construct of academic self-efficacy is grounded in social- of the decision to drop out of college. The model posits that the decision
cognitive theory (Bandura, 1977, 1986) rather than expectancy-value to leave university prior to graduation is ultimately influenced by the
theory; nevertheless, academic self-efficacy and expectancy for success extent to which an individual is integrated into the academic and social
share substantial conceptual overlap and may be empirically indis- systems within an institution. The aspect of Tinto's theory that is most
tinguishable, especially when the task is operationalized broadly (Bong, relevant to questions taken up by the current study relate to academic
2001; Pajares, 1996; Wigfield & Eccles, 2000), as it is in the current integration. According to Tinto's model, being and feeling academically
study. integrated is the product of a student's academic performance, such as
grades or credit accrual, and his or her intellectual development. The
1.2. Associations between academic self-efficacy, subjective task values, model suggests that students whose academic performance and in-
academic performance and persistence tellectual development aligns with normative standards in the uni-
versity will feel more academically integrated and satisfied, and be less
A compelling body of research has demonstrated that expectancy likely to depart prematurely. Empirical studies have supported predic-
beliefs, such as academic self-efficacy, and subjective task values are tions from Tinto's model. Low grades and failing courses are indicators
correlates of academic performance and persistence in post-secondary of the decision to leave university (Ishitani & DesJardins, 2002). Stu-
education settings. Students who report high levels of self-efficacy are dents who feel satisfied with or integrated into academic life have been
more willing to initiate and persist at challenging tasks, are more likely found to be more likely to be engaged during learning (Wefald &
to achieve higher academic performance, and express greater intentions Downey, 2009) and more likely to be retained (Schertzer & Schertzer,
to persist in a program of study than are students who report low self- 2004; Starr, Betz, & Menne, 1972; Tinto, 1993, 2007). A similar pattern
efficacy (Bong, 2001; Devonport & Lane, 2006; Klassen & Usher, 2010; has been found for students with learning difficulties, with academic
Pajares, 1996). A recent review of meta-analytic studies examining integration explaining unique variance in the intention to persist at
correlates of achievement in higher education found that academic self- university among first- and second-year university students with
efficacy demonstrated consistently large effects on achievement learning disabilities (DeDeppo, 2009).
(Schneider & Preckel, 2017). In an earlier meta-analysis, Multon, While Tinto's (1975) model does not explicitly address the role of
Brown, and Lent (1991) found that self-efficacy explained 14% of motivational beliefs, Bean and Eaton (2000, 2001) expanded on Tinto's
variance in academic performance and 12% of variance in persistence; model to focus on the psychological processes that lead to institutional
ability level and age were significant moderators, with larger effects integration. In their psychological model of retention, Bean and Eaton
found for high school and college students (vs. younger students) and posited that motivational beliefs such as self-efficacy were proximal
larger effects for students with low academic performance (vs. students psychological predictors of academic performance and integration.
with normative academic performance). Rodgers and Summers (2008) have further expanded Bean and Eaton's
The subjective task values held by university students have been model to include subjective values. Taken together, these models sug-
found to be positively associated with academic performance and the gest that academic self-efficacy and subjective task values are asso-
intention to persist in programs of study and the intention to persist at ciated with academic satisfaction and performance (e.g., accruing
university (Bong, 2001; Neuville et al., 2007; Perez, Cromley, & Kaplan, credits), and in turn, the decision to re-enroll in university, though to
2014). When both expectancy beliefs and task values are examined our knowledge these indirect relations have not been empirically
simultaneously, expectancy beliefs tend to be the strongest predictors of tested.
performance while subjective task values tend to be the strongest pre-
dictors of task choice (Wigfield & Cambria, 2010). Although the con- 1.5. Reading skills and academic achievement of students with a self-
struct of effort cost has received less empirical attention, research reported history of reading difficulty
suggests that costs can be important negative predictors of academic
choices, such as the intention to choose or persist in a major (Battle & Children who struggle in the acquisition of reading skills typically
Wigfield, 2003; Perez et al., 2014). continue to experience reading difficulties into adulthood (Lefly &
Pennington, 1991). Some of these individuals learn to compensate for
1.3. Associations between academic self-efficacy, subjective task values and their reading difficulties to the point of earning admission to university
academic satisfaction (Parrila, Georgiou, & Corkett, 2007). Since many individuals who have
early reading or learning difficulty do not receive a diagnosis of a
Academic satisfaction refers to the perceived enjoyment and learning disability (e.g., Brown, 2013; Kaplan & Shachter, 1991;

42
B.W. Bergey et al. Learning and Individual Differences 67 (2018) 41–52

Orenstein, 2000), researchers have developed self-report measures of academic self-efficacy compared to non-disabled peers. Similarly,
reading history to identify university students with reading difficulties Klassen, Krawchuk, and Rajani (2008) found that university students
(Lefly & Pennington, 2000; Parrila, Corkett, Kirby, & Hein, 2003). We with diagnosed learning disabilities reported greater procrastination
refer to individuals who report moderate to severe reading difficulties and less confidence in their ability to self-regulate their studying be-
on such measures as students who self-report a history of reading dif- haviors.
ficulty. There is now ample evidence that these self-reports of early In a review of motivation literature on students with diagnosed
reading difficulties identify students who have poorer word reading and learning disabilities, Sideridis (2009) emphasized the potential of using
timed reading comprehension skills than their peers who report no such an expectancy-value framework to guide research on achievement
history (Deacon et al., 2012; McGonnell, Parrila, & Deacon, 2007; motivations. Eccles et al. (1983) model (Wigfield & Eccles, 2000) posits
Parrila et al., 2007). For example, Deacon et al. (2012) found that that expectancies and values for academic tasks are influenced by an
university students who self-reported a history of reading difficulties array of social-cognitive factors, including perceptions of individual
had performance on standardized measures of word reading and aptitudes, prior achievement experiences, and socializing influences.
reading comprehension equivalent to four grade levels below the per- From this perspective, a student with low reading abilities who reg-
formance of students who reported no reading acquisition difficulties. ularly experiences academic tasks as arduous and frustrating may come
Critically, students who self-reported reading difficulty did not differ in to perceive himself as less academically capable, may perceive aca-
their scores on standardized measures of word reading and reading demic endeavors as less enjoyable, or may question whether the effort
comprehension compared to students with diagnosed learning dis- necessary to achieve in academic settings is worthwhile.
abilities. Similar patterns were found in Parrila et al. (2007) and Two studies on the academic motivation of university students with
McGonnell et al. (2007), providing further validity evidence towards a a history of reading difficulty point to unanswered questions about the
claim that self-reports of early reading difficulty accurately reflect effects of such a history on motivational variables and their relation
challenges in this experience. with academic achievement. Corkett et al. (2008) interviewed 10 uni-
Research has suggested that the population of students who self- versity students or recent graduates who reported a history of reading
report a history of reading difficulty in post-secondary institutions is difficulty about what contributed to their academic success. High mo-
sizable. Deacon, Tucker, and Bergey (2015), summarizing research tivation and a belief in one's abilities were central themes in students'
from multiple cohorts across multiple universities in Canada, reported reflections. These qualitative findings suggested that, at least among
that approximately 20% of in-coming first-year students reported at academically successful students with reading difficulties, motivation in
least moderate reading acquisition difficulty. The authors noted that general, and confidence in one's abilities in particular, were important
approximately one-third of those who self-reported a history of reading factors in supporting their achievement. It remains an open question
difficulty also reported a history of a diagnosis of a reading-related whether confidence beliefs are linked with academic achievement in a
learning disability. Therefore, the population of university students larger sample of students with a history of reading difficulty that en-
who self-report a history of reading difficulty is likely to include both compasses a broader range of levels of academic achievement.
students with and without a formal diagnosis of reading-related Bergey et al. (2017) compared the self-reported academic motiva-
learning disabilities. tion and academic achievement of first-year university students with
Students who self-report early reading difficulty have been found to and without a self-reported history of reading difficulty. Students who
have below-average academic performance. For students with low reported a history of reading difficulty described significantly lower
reading skills, the reading-intensive academic tasks that are common at levels of academic motivation compared to students with no such his-
university are likely to be particularly onerous. Students who self-re- tory. Intriguingly, motivation was not significantly correlated with first-
ported a history of reading difficulty have been found to earn, on year GPA among students with a history of reading difficulties, but was
average, lower GPAs in each of their first four years at university for students with no history of reading difficulties. This pattern suggests
(Bergey et al., 2017; also see Chevalier et al., 2017). They also tend to that motivational variables may relate to academic outcomes differ-
earn fewer of the credits they attempt. Bergey et al. (2017) found that ently for students with versus without a history of reading difficulty.
students who self-reported a history of reading difficulty earned a Taken together, the results of these two studies raise questions about
median of three fewer credits each year compared to students with no how having a history of reading difficulty may impact specific moti-
history of reading difficulty, despite attempting the same amount. vational perceptions, such as confidence in one's abilities and percep-
Academic difficulty may translate into reduced institutional retention. tions about the values of academic tasks, and further, whether and how
In order to support this academically vulnerable group, it is important motivational perceptions support academic achievement and well-
to identify potential strengths and weakness as well as pinpoint corre- being for students with a history of reading difficulty.
lates of academic performance and retention.
1.7. Current study
1.6. Motivation of Students with a History of Reading Difficulty
In the current study, we sought to identify motivational correlates of
Learning difficulties influence the psychological development of academic performance, academic satisfaction, and institutional reten-
children and adolescents and can shape academic motivation in a tion for students with a self-reported history of reading difficulty. To
variety of ways (Cohen, 1986; Sideridis, 2009; Wigfield & Eccles, 2000). assess whether and how these motivational perceptions operated dis-
Some individuals can become highly motivated to achieve in the face of tinctly for students with a self-reported history of reading difficulty, we
their unique learning difficulties, especially when they frame their compared them with students with no such history. We pursued the
difficulty as a personal challenge and receive support from others following two research aims:
(Cohen, 1986). An individual with reading difficulties, for example,
might use his personal motivation to achieve as a way of compensating 1.7.1. Research aim 1
and overcoming cognitive weaknesses (Corkett, Hein, & Parrila, 2008; Our first aim was to examine whether students who self-reported a
Łockiewicz, Bogdanowicz, & Bogdanowicz, 2014). Yet for other stu- history of reading difficulty differed from those who reported no such
dents, persistent learning difficulties can lead to self-beliefs and per- history in their motivational self-perceptions, academic satisfaction,
ceptions that lower academic motivation, reflecting less enjoyment and academic achievement. In terms of motivational self-perceptions,
with academic tasks and doubt about one's ability to succeed. For ex- we examined whether the groups differed in mean levels of academic
ample, Baird, Scott, Dearing, and Hamill (2009) found that secondary self-efficacy, positive subjective task values, and effort cost for
students with diagnosed learning disabilities tended to report lower achieving a university degree. Within the expectancy-value framework,

43
B.W. Bergey et al. Learning and Individual Differences 67 (2018) 41–52

we operationalized expectancy as academic self-efficacy. The concept of We hypothesized (Hypothesis H2a) that academic self-efficacy and
psychological costs has received increased attention recently (Barron & subjective task values would be positively associated with academic
Hulleman, 2015) and several types of costs have been proposed (e.g., performance, academic satisfaction, and institutional retention. We
Flake, Barron, Hulleman, McCoach, & Welsh, 2015). In the current further hypothesized (Hypothesis H2b) that subjective task values would
study, we focused on effort expenditure (which we refer to as effort explain unique variance in institutional retention, beyond academic
cost) because low reading abilities of students who self-report a history performance and satisfaction. Hypotheses H2a and H2b were based on
of reading difficulty may require greater level of effort expenditures for prior research with general university populations that has suggested
success. that expectancy beliefs, such as self-efficacy, and task values predict
We first hypothesized (Hypothesis H1a) that students who self-re- academic performance (Bong, 2001; Garriott et al., 2015; Sheu et al.,
ported a history of reading difficulties would report lower academic 2016). Among students with learning difficulties, expectancy beliefs
self-efficacy beliefs than students with no such history. This expectation have been identified as important factors supporting academic
was based on extrapolating findings from studies on students with di- achievement (Margolis & McCabe, 2004); subjective task values, on the
agnosed learning disabilities (Baird et al., 2009; Klassen et al., 2008). other hand, have received little empirical attention.
Second, we hypothesized (Hypothesis H1b) that students with a We also hypothesized (Hypothesis H2c) that academic self-efficacy
history of reading difficulty would report lower intrinsic value, greater would have an indirect effect on institutional retention via its effect on
effort cost, and lower academic satisfaction. This expectation was based academic performance. This expectation was based on the fact that self-
on the presumption that repeated experiences with academic difficul- efficacy has been found to be a robust predictor of academic perfor-
ties may lead students with a history of reading difficulty to view mance, as noted above, and academic performance has been shown to
academic tasks as less enjoyable or requiring more extensive effort predict institutional retention (Allen, Robbins, Casillas, & Oh, 2008).
compared to how academic tasks are viewed by peers with no such We did not make specific hypotheses regarding group differences.
history. Further, prior research with students with a self-reported his- Prior research has found the relations between general academic mo-
tory of reading difficulty observed lower motivation, less positive atti- tivation and academic performance were less robust for university
tude, and higher anxiety compared to students with no history of students with a history of reading difficulty than for those with no such
reading difficulty (Bergey et al., 2017). However, our explorations in history (Bergey et al., 2017). Yet, among academically successfully
this area are novel as we are not aware of prior literature that has ex- individuals, self-efficacy and task values, such as interest, have been
amined subjective task values and academic satisfaction among uni- identified as particularly potent motivators of performance and per-
versity populations with learning difficulties or disabilities. sistence (Corkett et al., 2008; Fink, 1995).
Third, we hypothesized (Hypothesis H1c) that students with a self-
reported history of reading difficulty would earn lower first-year GPA, 2. Method
accrue fewer first-year credits, and be retained at the institution at a
lower rate than students with no such history. This expectation was 2.1. Participants
based on prior research with this population that has found below-
average first-year academic performance (Bergey et al., 2017; Chevalier Participants were 482 university students at a research-intensive
et al., 2017). We extrapolated from these findings that students with a Canadian university: 168 students self-reported a history of reading
history of reading difficulty may be less likely to be retained in their difficulty (M age = 18 years 5 months; SD = 14 months; 117 females)
second year relative to students with no history of reading difficulty. and 314 students self-reported no history of reading difficulty (M
age = 18 years 5 months; SD = 13 months; 248 females). Students had
1.7.2. Research aim 2 a wide range of majors: 56% of students with a self-reported history of
Our second aim was to evaluate the extent to which academic self- reading difficulty and 57% of those with no such history were enrolled
efficacy and subjective task values uniquely and collectively predicted in faculties of Science, Technology, Mathematics or Engineering; re-
first-year academic performance, academic satisfaction and institu- spectively, 17% and 21% were enrolled in the Arts and Social Science
tional retention for each group. To examine these relations, we tested a faculty, and 27% and 22% were enrolled in professional programs, such
model shown in Fig. 1, and we examined whether relations differed for as business management, journalism, and nursing. As a proxy for socio-
the two groups. We examined direct effects of academic self-efficacy economic status, we report parental educational attainment. A bache-
and subjective task values on academic performance and satisfaction. lor's degree was the highest median degree earned by fathers (28% and
We also examined direct effects of academic self-efficacy and subjective 29%) and mothers (31% and 38%) of students with and without a self-
task values on institutional retention as well as their indirect relations reported history of reading difficulty, respectively. Race and ethnicity
mediated by academic performance and satisfaction. information were not available for the sample. Inclusion criteria were

Fig. 1. Conceptual model of expectancy-value variables and academic outcomes.


Note. No additional controls were included in the model.

44
B.W. Bergey et al. Learning and Individual Differences 67 (2018) 41–52

(1) being in the first year of university, (2) reporting English as one's used a 5-point Likert response scale, anchored with descriptors at 1 (not
first written and spoken language, and (3) consenting to tracking aca- at all true of me) and 5 (very true of me). Intrinsic value was assessed
demic progress. Among students who reported a history of reading with five items (α = 0.92). A sample item is What I am learning in
difficulty, 18% also reported a current or prior diagnosis of a learning university is exciting to me. Attainment value was assessed with five items
disability; among students who reported no history of reading diffi- (α = 0.91). A sample item is Doing well in university is an important part
culties, 5% reported a current or prior diagnosis of a learning disability. of who I am. Utility value was assessed with five items (α = 0.77). A
sample item is A university degree will be useful for me later in life. Effort
2.2. Procedure cost items were modified from Battle and Wigfield (2003), Conley
(2012), and Perez et al. (2014). Effort cost was assessed with four items
All first-year students at the university were invited by the registrar (α = 0.78). A sample item is I worry that I will waste a lot of time and
to complete an online questionnaire about their reading history along effort by going to university.
with other measures not reported on in the current study. Consenting
students provided a non-anonymous university identification number, 2.3.4. Academic satisfaction
which was used to link survey responses to registrar data. Initial Academic satisfaction was assessed with the Academic subscale of
questionnaires were completed by 2027 students (response rate: ap- the Extended Satisfaction with Life Scale (Alfonso, Allison, Rader, &
proximately 41%) within three weeks of the start of students' first se- Gorman, 1996). The academic satisfaction scale consisted of five items
mester at university. In the middle of the second semester of students' (α = 0.82), which assessed satisfaction with one's current education,
first year, students who completed the initial questionnaire, gave per- including satisfaction with instructors, classmates, curriculum, and
mission to track academic progress, verified they were in their first year schooling in general. A sample item is The education I get at school is
of university, and reported English as their first language (n = 1229) great. Responses were recorded on a 7-point Likert scale with de-
were invited to complete a follow-up questionnaire. The follow-up scriptors at each point ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly
questionnaire assessed academic self-efficacy beliefs, subjective task agree).
values, and academic satisfaction, along with other measures of first-
year perceptions and experiences not analyzed here. The follow-up 2.3.5. Academic performance and institutional retention
questionnaire was completed by 546 students (response rate: 44%) with First-year academic performance was assessed with two variables:
all questionnaires completed prior to the final exam period of the cumulative first-year GPA and total credits accrued during the first
second semester. Following the academic year, GPA, credits accrued, year. GPA was a continuous variable representing the cumulative
and institutional retention status were retrieved from the registrar. The average grade using a scale from 0 to 4.3. Total credits accrued indicate
research was conducted with the approval of the University's the total number of credits that were successfully completed (i.e.,
Institutional Review Board and in accordance with American achieving a passing grade) during the first year. A typical full load for
Psychological Association's guidelines for ethical research with human two semesters was 30 credits. Institutional retention was oper-
subjects. ationalized as having attempted at least one course in the first semester
of the second year. All achievement data was retrieved from the
2.3. Measures University Registrar.

2.3.1. Reading history 2.4. Data analysis


Reading history was assessed with the Elementary scale of the Adult
Reading History Questionnaire-Revised (ARHQ-R; Parrila et al., 2003). 2.4.1. Factor structure of academic self-efficacy and subjective task value
The ARHQ-R scale consisted of eight items (α = 0.90) assessing the measures
extent to which individuals reported having experienced reading ac- We first established the factor structure of the expectancy-value
quisition difficulty as children. An example item is How much difficulty constructs. There is little prior research examining the factor structure
did you have learning to read in elementary school? Responses were re- of academic self-efficacy and subjective task values when effort cost is
corded on a 5-point Likert scale with descriptors for each point on the measured. Therefore, we conducted exploratory factor analysis in SPSS
scale. Summed scores were transformed to a scale ranging from 0 (no (Vers. 24) using principle axis factoring with an Oblimin rotation on
early reading difficulty) to 1 (widespread difficulty). Following cutoff academic self-efficacy and subjective task value items. Examination of
points described in prior research (Bergey et al., 2017; Deacon et al., the scree plot and factor structure matrix suggested a four-factor solu-
2012), students with scores over 0.37 on the ARQH-R were identified as tion with academic self-efficacy beliefs, intrinsic value, and costs items
having a history of moderate to severe difficulty. Those with scores loading on separate factors and with utility and attainment value items
below 0.26 were identified as reporting no history of reading difficulty. loading together. To evaluate the adequacy of this four-factor model,
Following prior research (Chevalier et al., 2017; Parrila et al., 2007), we conducted a Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) in Mplus (Vers. 7,
students whose scores fell between 0.26 and 0.37 (n = 64) were ex- Muthén & Muthén, 2012) using Maximum Likelihood estimator, spe-
cluded from analyses to ensure clear separation between groups; sample cifying a model in which items from academic self-efficacy, intrinsic
characteristics presented in Section 2.1 do not include these excluded value, attainment/utility value, and effort costs scales loaded on four
students. factors. Modification indices were used to improve the model fit by
correlating error terms among items within the same scale. In addition,
2.3.2. Academic self-efficacy two utility value items were removed due to low factor loading (< 0.5).
Academic self-efficacy was measured with a five-item scale The resulting model showed acceptable model fit (χ2 [182] = 387.367,
(α = 0.85; Klassen et al., 2008). An example item is I'm confident I can p < .001, CFI = 0.972, RMSEA = 0.048, SRMR = 0.055). Model fit
understand the most complex material presented by my instructors. Re- was evaluated against the following criteria suggested by Kline (2010):
sponses were recorded using a 5-point Likert scale with anchored de- chi-square p value ≥ .05; CFI ≥ 0.95; RMSEA ≤ 0.10; SRMR ≤ 0.08.
scriptors at 1 (Not at all true of me) and 5 (Very true of me). An alpha of p < .05 was adopted for all analyses.

2.3.3. Subjective task values 2.4.2. Group differences in measurement and structural models
Positive subjective task values were measured with three scales We evaluated group differences following the procedures outlined
adapted from Conley (2012). Items were adapted to focus on the task of in Kline (2010). First, we assessed configural invariance by fitting the
achieving a university degree rather than math achievement. All scales same four-factor model (described in Section 2.4.1) to both groups,

45
B.W. Bergey et al. Learning and Individual Differences 67 (2018) 41–52

with no equality constraints (an unconstrained model). If the un- 3.2. Mean group differences
constrained model had acceptable fit, we assessed construct-level me-
tric invariance by testing a model in which all parameters were con- Descriptive statistics for academic self-efficacy, subjective task va-
strained to be equal for the two groups (a fully constrained model). We lues, academic satisfaction, GPA, credits accrued, and institutional re-
then tested the difference in chi-square between the constrained and tention rates are shown in Table 2. Descriptively, students who self-
unconstrained models to determine whether the less parsimonious un- reported a history of reading difficulty reported lower academic self-
constrained model resulted in significantly improved fit. Due to the efficacy and subjective task values, higher costs, and lower academic
sample size of students who self-reported a history of reading difficul- satisfaction; earned lower first-year GPA, accrued fewer first-year
ties, we lacked the statistical power to include the measurement model credits, and had a lower institutional retention rate.
in the structural regression. Therefore, we saved factor scores from the A significant MANOVA (F[8, 473] = 3.445, p = .001, partial
final CFA model and used these in subsequent analyses. Given that GPA μ2 = 0.055) indicated that students who self-reported a history of
and credits accrued were relatively strongly correlated with each other reading difficulty reported significantly lower academic self-efficacy (F
(see Table 3), we opted to include only the latter in the model, as shown [1, 480] = 16.482, p < .001, d = 0.39), lower first-year GPA (F[1,
in Figs. 2 and 3. Analyses resulting from models with GPA instead of 480] = 4.840, p = .028, d = 0.21), and fewer accrued credits (F[1,
credits accrued revealed similar results to the ones reported below.1 480] = 4.512, p = .034, d = 0.20). Groups did not significantly differ
We assessed structural invariance by fitting the model illustrated in in attainment-utility value (F[1, 480] = 1.724, p = .190), effort cost (F
Figs. 2 and 3 to both groups, with no equality constraints. If this model [1, 480] = 0.930, p = .335), intrinsic value (F[1, 480] = 2.912,
had acceptable fit, we compared it to a nested model in which all model p = .089), or academic satisfaction (F[1, 480] = 0.627, p = .429). That
parameters were constrained to be equal for the two groups (the fully is, students with a self-reported history of reading difficulty had sig-
constrained model). If the constrained model resulted in significantly nificantly lower first-year performance and reported feeling less cap-
poorer model fit, a series of models were tested in which individual able of handling academic challenges than students with no such his-
loadings were freed while all other were constrained to be equal to tory. Yet, the two groups did not significantly differ in the perceived
determine which did not significantly improve model fit. In the final value of attaining a university degree or their satisfaction with their
model, we estimated indirect effects of expectancy-value constructs on academic experience. A chi-square test indicated that the two groups
institutional retention via academic performance and satisfaction. did not differ in their institutional retention rate (χ2 [1] = 0.596,
p = .440).

2.4.3. Additional analyses 3.3. Bivariate correlations


We assessed mean group differences with a MANOVA in which self-
reported reading history status was the independent variable and aca- Turning to our second aim, we examined the relations among aca-
demic self-efficacy, intrinsic value, attainment-utility value, effort cost, demic self-efficacy beliefs, subjective task values, academic satisfaction,
academic satisfaction, first-year GPA, and credits accrued were de- academic performance, and institutional retention. Bivariate correla-
pendent variables. For significant effects, Cohen's ds were calculated to tion for students with and without a history of reading difficulties is
evaluate effect size, with small, medium, and large effect sizes indicated shown in Table 3. Both groups had the same pattern of relations in
by value of 0.02, 0.05, and 0.08, respectively (Cohen, 1988). We used a terms of valence. All correlations were in the expected directions. For
2 (history of reading difficulty, no difficulty) × 2 (retained, not re- both groups, GPA and credits accrued had significant positive correla-
tained) chi-square test to assess whether groups differed in their in- tions with academic self-efficacy and intrinsic value, and significant
stitutional retention rate. Bivariate Pearson correlations were calcu- negative correlations with effort cost. For both groups, institutional
lated for all variables to estimate zero-order correlations and identify retention had significant positive correlations with intrinsic and at-
variables that might be multicollinear or redundant. tainment/utility values and significant negative correlations with effort
cost. For students with a history of reading difficulties only, institu-
tional retention was significantly correlated with academic self-effi-
3. Results cacy.

3.1. Group differences in the factor structure of academic self-efficacy and 3.4. Model modification and fitting
subjective task value scales
Invariance testing of the constrained and unconstrained structural
As shown in Table 1, both the fully constrained and fully un- models is presented in Table 1. The unconstrained structural model
constrained models demonstrated adequate fit. The differences in fit (shown in Figs. 2 and 3) demonstrated good fit, suggesting the ade-
indices between these models were minimal: the change in chi-square quacy of the structural model for both groups. The fully constrained
was non-significant and changes in CFI and RMSEA were slight (Chen, model also demonstrated adequate fit. However, the change in chi-
2007). This suggested no significant differences in the factor structure square was significant, suggesting that one or more model parameters
for the two groups. As a result, a model in which loadings were the were significantly different for the two groups. Invariance testing for
same for both group was more parsimonious and therefore preferable. individual paths revealed that one structural coefficient differed sig-
nificantly for the two groups: academic self-efficacy had a significant
positive relation with academic satisfaction for students with no self-
reported history of reading difficulty (β = 0.121) while this relation-
1
We examined whether gender and parental education merited being in- ship was negative and non-significant for students with a self-reported
cluded as statistical controls in the model. For both HRD and NRD samples, history of reading difficulty (β = −0.127). Academic self-efficacy did
bivariate correlations among gender and parental education with credits
not explain unique variance in academic satisfaction among students
earned, academic satisfaction, and retention were not statistically significant
who self-reported a history of reading difficulty, as it did among stu-
(p > .10). We also tested the model shown in Figs. 2 and 3 controlling for
gender and parental education. Results indicated that gender and parental dents with no such history. Fit statistics and coefficients for the final
education had non-significant effects on all endogenous variables in the model, model are presented in Table 4 and illustrated in Figs. 2 and 3.
and all model estimates were similar to those of the reported model. Therefore, Indirect effects of academic self-efficacy and subjective task values
in the interest of parsimony, our final model does not include gender and on institutional retention via credits accrued were then calculated. For
parental education as control variables. students with a self-reported history of reading difficulty, academic self-

46
B.W. Bergey et al. Learning and Individual Differences 67 (2018) 41–52

Fig. 2. Model showing significant standardized path coefficient for students with a self-reported history of reading difficulty.
Note. Solid lines denote significant paths; dashed lines denoted tested paths with non-significant coefficients.

Fig. 3. Model showing significant standardized path coefficient for students without a self-reported history of reading difficulty
Note. Solid lines denote significant paths; dashed lines denoted tested paths with non-significant coefficients.

Table 1 Table 2
Evaluation of group differences in measurement model. Descriptive statistics for academic self-efficacy, task values, satisfaction, and
academic achievement by reading history status.
χ2 df p CFI RMSEA SRMR
History of reading No history of reading
Measurement model difficulty difficulty
Fully constrained 662.828 421 < 0.001 0.967 0.049 0.078
Fully unconstrained 614.306 383 < 0.001 0.968 0.050 0.059 M SD M SD
Difference between 48.522 38 0.119 −0.001 −0.001 0.019
models Academic self-efficacy −0.13 0.58 0.07 0.50
Structural model Intrinsic value −0.08 0.68 0.04 0.74
Fully constrained 28.759 16 0.026 0.976 0.058 –a Utility-attainment value −0.05 0.56 0.02 0.53
Fully unconstrained 2.950 2 0.229 0.998 0.044 –a Effort cost 0.04 0.65 −0.02 0.72
Difference between 25.809 14 0.027 −0.022 0.014 –a Academic satisfaction 5.32 0.91 5.39 1.00
models GPA 3.05 0.76 3.21 0.80
Credits accrued 27.31 4.60 28.28 4.85
Mplus does not calculate SRMR for models involving categorical outcomes. Institutional retention 0.88 0.33 0.90 0.31

efficacy had a significant positive indirect effect on institutional re- Academic self-efficacy and subjective task values are standardized factor
loading scores from the measurement model. M = Mean; SD = Standard
tention (β = 0.060) and effort cost had a significant negative indirect
Deviation.
effect on institutional retention (β = −0.048). Similar indirect effects
were found for students with no self-reported history of reading diffi-
culty, as shown in Table 4.

47
B.W. Bergey et al. Learning and Individual Differences 67 (2018) 41–52

Table 3
Correlations among modelled variables for students with (above diagonal) and without (below diagonal) a self-reported history of reading difficulty.
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

1. Academic self-efficacy 0.744 0.252 −0.598 0.376 0.317 0.487 0.185


2. Intrinsic value 0.676 0.390 −0.726 0.567 0.303 0.386 0.342
3. Attainment-utility value 0.343 0.376 −0.420 0.279 0.128 0.108 0.164
4. Effort cost −0.622 −0.710 −0.567 −0.494 −0.344 −0.399 −0.294
5. Academic satisfaction 0.521 0.643 0.230 −0.444 0.242 0.260 0.155
6. Year 1 GPA 0.329 0.300 0.267 −0.324 0.226 0.603 0.356
7. Year 1 credits accrued 0.503 0.374 0.257 −0.389 0.306 0.611 0.213
8. Retained to year 2 0.069 0.213 0.148 −0.165 0.152 0.188 0.162

Correlations of 0.155 or greater were significant (p < .05) for students with a self-reported history of reading difficulty (above diagonal). Correlations of 0.148 or
greater were significant (p < .05) for students without a self-reported history of reading difficulty (below diagonal).

Table 4
Direct and indirect model estimates for students with and without a self-reported history of reading difficulties.
History of readings difficulty No history of reading difficulty

Unst SE Std Unst SE Std

Institutional retention on
Credits accrued 0.076 0.012 0.313⁎⁎⁎ 0.076 0.012 0.318⁎⁎⁎
Academic satisfaction 0.053 0.097 0.041 0.053 0.097 0.046
Academic self-efficacy −0.425 0.246 −0.220 −0.425 0.246 −0.184
Intrinsic value 0.530 0.163 0.320⁎⁎⁎ 0.530 0.163 0.337⁎⁎⁎
Utility-attainment 0.179 0.154 0.090 0.179 0.154 0.082
Effort cost −0.215 0.151 −0.124 −0.215 0.151 −0.134

Credits accrued on
Academic self-efficacy 1.530 0.548 0.191⁎⁎ 1.530 0.548 0.158⁎⁎
Intrinsic value 0.439 0.396 0.064 0.439 0.396 0.067
Utility-attainment 0.490 0.380 0.059 0.490 0.380 0.054
Effort cost −1.100 0.313 −0.153⁎⁎⁎ −1.100 0.313 −0.163⁎⁎⁎

Academic satisfaction on
Academic self-efficacy −0.114 0.099 −0.076 0.252 0.098 0.125⁎⁎
Intrinsic value 0.766 0.066 0.596⁎⁎⁎ 0.766 0.066 0.562⁎⁎⁎
Utility-attainment −0.010 0.074 −0.006 −0.010 0.074 −0.005
Effort cost −0.020 0.069 −0.015 −0.020 0.069 −0.015

Indirect effects on institutional retention


Via self-efficacy → credits accrued 0.116 0.046 0.060⁎ 0.116 0.046 0.050⁎
Via effort cost → credits accrued −0.083 0.028 −0.048⁎⁎ −0.083 0.028 −0.052⁎⁎

Unst = Unstandardized coefficient; SE = Standard error; Std = Standardized coefficient.



p < .05.
⁎⁎
p ≤ .01.
⁎⁎⁎
p ≤ .001.

4. Discussion that when compared to typically reading peers, students with a history
of reading difficulty reported lower academic self-efficacy, but similar
In our study, we applied an expectancy-value framework to un- subjective task values and academic satisfaction, despite lower aca-
derstand the motivations and academic outcomes of university students demic performance. These results confirm hypotheses H1a and H1c, but
who reported a history of reading difficulty. Our results indicated that not H1b. This pattern of motivational beliefs clarifies prior research on
academic self-efficacy beliefs and subjective task values were sig- this population. Bergey et al. (2017) found that students with a self-
nificant correlates of academic satisfaction, academic performance, and reported history of reading difficulty reported below average levels of
second-year institutional retention. We found several similarities and motivation, less positive attitudes, and greater anxiety. Our findings
differences between students with and without a self-reported history of suggest that low motivation may be the result of lower academic self-
reading difficulty in motivational variables and their relations with efficacy. Since students with a history of reading difficulties have been
academic outcomes, which we discuss below. Together, these findings found to have lower reading skills and have greater academic difficul-
underscore the importance of motivational variables in understanding ties (Chevalier et al., 2017; Deacon et al., 2012), it is understandable
the academic satisfaction, achievement and persistence of students who that students who report a history of reading difficulty also report less
report a history of reading difficulty. confidence in their academic abilities. Yet regardless of one's current
abilities, self-efficacy is an important motivational resource for per-
sisting in the face of challenge (Bandura, 1997; Bouffard-Bouchard,
4.1. Group differences in motivational self-perceptions and academic Parent, & Larivee, 1991).
satisfaction, performance, and institutional retention At the same time, comparable reported levels of subjective task
values and academic satisfaction suggest that a history of reading dif-
Our first aim was to examine whether students with and without a ficulties did not diminish the perceived enjoyment, utility and im-
self-reported history of reading difficulty differed in their motivational portance of attaining a university degree or academic satisfaction with
self-perceptions, academic satisfaction, academic performance, and the process. Also, students with a self-reported history of reading
institutional retention. A novel and important finding of the study is

48
B.W. Bergey et al. Learning and Individual Differences 67 (2018) 41–52

difficulty did not perceive increased effort cost. These results are variance in academic performance after controlling for academic self-
somewhat surprising, since students with a history of reading difficulty efficacy and positive subjective task values. Our results underscore the
have likely experienced greater academic difficulty and report lower importance of considering perceived drawbacks and costs alongside
confidence in their abilities. Nevertheless, these students have found positive task values when conceptualizing motivation through an ex-
ways to experience typical levels of enjoyment and satisfaction in their pectancy-value framework (Barron & Hulleman, 2015).
academic experience and perceive the endeavor of pursuing a uni- An important finding of the study was that intrinsic value was the
versity degree as worth the effort. The results point to the fact that strongest unique predictor of institutional retention for students with
many students with a self-reported history of reading difficulty held and without a self-reported history of reading difficulty, explaining
motivational beliefs that are likely to support them through challenging more variance in institutional retention than even first-year academic
academic tasks (Wigfield & Cambria, 2010). performance. Students who reported enjoying their first-year academic
Our results regarding academic performance confirmed and ex- experience were more likely to return the following year. Our results
tended prior research with this population. We found that students with extend prior research with general populations that show that values
(vs. without) a self-reported history of reading difficulty had sig- positively predict college students' course enrollment intentions or ac-
nificantly lower first-year GPAs and accrued fewer credits, consistent tions (Bong, 2001; Perez et al., 2014; Wigfield & Cambria, 2010). We
with our hypothesis (H1c). While the pattern and magnitude of aca- extend these results by demonstrating the importance of intrinsic value
demic performance differences are consistent with prior research as it relates to actual institutional retention at university for an aca-
(Bergey et al., 2017; Chevalier et al., 2017), results from our sample did demically vulnerable group.
not bear out predictions that students who reported a history of reading Finally, this study is novel in examining correlates of academic sa-
difficulty would be more likely to leave the institution. We note our tisfaction from an expectancy-value framework. We observed an in-
study was limited to second-year retention; assessing potential differ- teresting group difference in how academic satisfaction related to
ences in institutional retention in subsequent years of study is an im- subjective task values and academic self-efficacy. For both groups,
portant direction for future research. These results are consistent with academic satisfaction was strongly and positively related to intrinsic
prior research on university students with diagnosed learning dis- value. This is perhaps not surprising giving the conceptual similarity
abilities that has found similar institutional retention rates compared to between intrinsic enjoyment and satisfaction. Yet only for students who
those of students with no disabilities (Vogel & Adelman, 1992; see also reported no history of reading difficulty was academic satisfaction also
Wessel, Jones, Markle, & Westfall, 2009). uniquely predicted by academic self-efficacy. That is, for students with
no self-reported history of reading difficulty, academic satisfaction was
4.2. Relations among academic self-efficacy, subjective task values and dependent, in part, on confidence in their ability to achieve academic
academic outcomes success. By contrast, for students who reported a history of reading
difficulty, academic satisfaction was largely independent of such con-
Our second aim was to evaluate the extent to which academic self- fidence. One explanation is that students with a self-reported history of
efficacy and subjective task values uniquely and collectively predicted reading difficulty may become accustomed to experiencing doubt about
first-year academic performance, academic satisfaction, and institu- their academic abilities, yet they have found ways to find satisfaction in
tional retention for each group. An important and novel finding is that academic experiences even when they are less academically confident.
academic self-efficacy, effort cost, and interest value explained unique Future research might examine how students with a history of reading
variance in first-year academic performance or institutional retention, difficulty report comparable levels of academic satisfaction and sub-
consistent with our hypotheses. Multi-group analyses indicated that the jective task values despite experiencing greater academic difficulties
medium-to-large effect of academic self-efficacy and the small effect of and often facing academic endeavors with lower confidence in their
effort cost on academic performance were comparable in magnitude for academic abilities.
the two groups; the same is true for the medium effect of intrinsic value Our results raise questions for future research using Tinto's (1975)
on institutional retention. model of dropout from higher education. As Tinto's model would sug-
Our findings extend prior research on the effects of academic self- gest, academic satisfaction, which we used as a proxy for academic
efficacy on academic performance of university students with learning integration, had significant and positive bivariate correlations with
disabilities (Hampton & Mason, 2003; Hen & Goroshit, 2014) and of retention for both students with and without a self-reported history of
general university populations (Wigfield & Cambria, 2010) to a new reading difficulty. Yet model results indicated that, after controlling for
population. Our study provides support for Corkett et al.'s (2008) credits accrued, academic satisfaction did not explain a statistically
qualitative finding that academically successful students with a history significant amount of unique variance in retention. This finding raises
of reading difficulty credit their success to a belief in themselves. Our the question of whether academic performance has a direct relationship
results suggest that having a history of reading difficulty may diminish with student departure decisions, rather than a mediated relationship
university students' confidence in their academic abilities; such beliefs through academic integration, as Tinto's model suggests. An alternative
may, in turn, exacerbate the effects of low reading ability on academic interpretation is that perceptions of academic integration such as aca-
performance and persistence, and indirectly on institutional retention. demic satisfaction are less predictive of student departure than are
Low self-efficacy beliefs may result in students avoiding initiating dif- actual, objective measures of academic performance, such as credit
ficult tasks, exerting less effort during challenging tasks, using less ef- accrual and grades. Future research should explore these issues in the
fective study strategies, and giving up more easily in the face of diffi- context of a model that more closely reflects the complexity of Tinto's
culties (Bandura, 1986; Pajares, 1996). model.
With regard to effort cost, our results extend the limited empirical
research on the relations among effort cost and academic outcomes. 4.3. Limitations
Our factor analyses suggested effort cost was empirically distinct from
positive task values, consistent with prior research (Chiang, Byrd, & Our results should be considered alongside the following study
Molin, 2011; Conley, 2012; Kosovich, Hulleman, Barron, & Getty, 2015; limitations. Our study design did not allow us to make causal claims
Perez et al., 2014). While prior research has linked effort cost to the about the effect of academic self-efficacy and subjective task values on
intention to persist (e.g., Perez et al., 2014), our study extends this academic performance or institutional retention. Academic self-efficacy
research by demonstrating that—for both students with and without a and subjective task values were assessed at a single time point and in
self-reported history of reading difficulty—effort cost was negatively relatively close proximity to first-year academic performance outcomes.
correlated with actual institutional retention and explained unique While expectancy-value constructs may lead to higher achievement, the

49
B.W. Bergey et al. Learning and Individual Differences 67 (2018) 41–52

reverse pattern of relations, that achievement affects expectancy-va- environments that support student interest (e.g., Bergin, 1999). Stu-
lues, may also be true. In addition, we assessed academic self-efficacy dents with a self-reported history of reading difficulty might also ben-
and subjective task values at a domain-general level (e.g., values for efit from messages that make the case for why the effort it takes to
achieving a university degree), rather than for domain-specific tasks, succeed and persist at university is worthwhile. In addition, it may be
such as achieving in science or humanities. While our approach allowed important to help students who report a history of reading difficulty
us to examine relations across academic contexts, this may have masked identify personal goals and values that may make persistence at uni-
different relations that occur within specific academic domains (e.g., versity seem worth the effort. Recent work on self-reflective writing to
Selkirk, Bouchey, & Eccles, 2011; Watt, 2004). For example, ex- increase perceived relevance provides one promising way this might be
pectancies and values may operate differently for students reporting a accomplished (Hartwell & Kaplan, 2018).
history of reading difficulty within domains in which more or less Students with a self-reported history of reading difficulty are not the
emphasis is placed on reading. Similarly, future research might examine only students who are likely to benefit from motivational interventions.
relations controlling for known predictors, such as prior achievement, We found similar relations among motivational perceptions and aca-
which were not included in the current analyses due to data avail- demic achievement, satisfaction, and retention for students with and
ability. Further, the study did not include some information about without a self-reported history of reading difficulty. Given that students
students with reading difficulties that may have affected their motiva- with no history of reading difficulty may also benefit from efforts to
tional beliefs, academic achievement, and their relations; these include support motivational beliefs, interventions might target any student
students' actual disability status, comorbidity of other diagnoses or who experiences academic difficulty. Alternatively, educators could ask
academic difficulties (e.g., ADHD), or the academic support or accom- students to self-report on their past and current reading difficultie-
modations some may have received. s—using the ARHQ-R or a similar instrument—to identify and recruit
In addition, results on institutional retention should be considered students who are academically vulnerable due to their reading abilities.
in light of the research design and operational definitions. Since we did
not follow-up with students who did not re-enroll in their second year, Acknowledgements
we do not know their reason for leaving or whether they enrolled in
another institution; therefore, we caution against interpreting lack of This research was funded by Employment and Social Development
retention as a measure of dropout. That said, leaving the institution Canada's Office of Literacy and Essential Skills (Agreement
certainly reflects a change in course of academic study. We also note #011684222) and by Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council
that the retention rates of the samples were likely lower than actual Partnership Development Grant 890-2011-0072. We thank Language
institutional retention rates, since students who had already left the and Literacy Lab manager Annie Laroche for her help with study lo-
institution prior to data collection in the second semester could not gistics.
participate in the study. In addition, institutional retention rates and
relations to motivational beliefs and academic performance and sa- References
tisfaction may differ across contexts (by country, institution type, etc.),
highlighting the importance of replicating the patterns observed here. Alfonso, V. C., Allison, D. B., Rader, D. E., & Gorman, B. S. (1996). The extended sa-
Finally, we reported on indirect effects of academic self-efficacy and tisfaction with life scale: Development and psychometric properties. Social Indicators
Research, 38(3), 275–301. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00292049.
subjective task values on institutional retention. While our model fol- Allen, J., Robbins, S. B., Casillas, A., & Oh, I. S. (2008). Third-year college retention and
lows the temporal ordering of achievement variables (i.e., first-year transfer: Effects of academic performance, motivation, and social connectedness.
academic performance preceded the decision to re-enroll in year two), Research in Higher Education, 49(7), 647–664.
Baird, G. L., Scott, W. D., Dearing, E., & Hamill, S. K. (2009). Cognitive self-regulation in
conclusions about the indirect effects should be viewed as tentative, in youth with and without learning disabilities: Academic self-efficacy, theories of in-
light of concerns that have been raised about using cross-sectional data telligence, learning vs. performance goal preferences, and effort attributions. Journal
to evaluate indirect relations (Jose, 2013). of Social and Clinical Psychology, 28(7), 881–908. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2009.
28.7.881.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change.
4.4. Implications Psychological Review, 84(2), 191. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.191.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Our findings have practical implications. Prior research has in-
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. London: Macmillan.
dicated that self-reports of reading difficulty identify university stu- Barron, K. E., & Hulleman, C. S. (2015). Expectancy-value-cost model of motivation.
dents with actual reading deficits (Deacon et al., 2012; McGonnell Psychology, 84, 261–271. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.26099-6.
et al., 2007; Parrila et al., 2007) and below-average academic Battle, A., & Wigfield, A. (2003). College women's value orientations toward family, ca-
reer, and graduate school. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 62(1), 56–75. https://doi.
achievement (Bergey et al., 2017; Chevalier et al., 2017). Given the org/10.1016/S0001-8791(02)00037-4.
academic vulnerability of this group, institutions may wish to intervene Bean, J., & Eaton, S. B. (2001). The psychology underlying successful retention practices.
to support the academic success of students with a self-reported history Journal of College Student Retention, 3(1), 73–89.
Bean, J. P., & Eaton, S. B. (2000). A psychological model of college student retention. In J.
of reading difficulty (e.g., Deacon, Tucker, Bergey, Laroche, & Parrila, M. Braxton (Ed.). Reworking the student departure puzzle (pp. 48–61). Nashville, TN:
2017). Results of the current study suggest that motivational percep- Vanderbilt University Press.
tions might be directly addressed when attempting to support uni- Bergey, B. W., Deacon, S. H., & Parrila, R. K. (2017). Metacognitive reading and study
strategies and academic achievement of university students with and without a his-
versity students with a self-reported history of reading difficulty. Given tory of reading difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 50(1), 81–94. https://doi.
that such students tend to report lower academic self-efficacy, they may org/10.1177/0022219415597020.
benefit from interventions that support confidence in their academic Bergin, D. A. (1999). Influences on classroom interest. Educational Psychologist, 34(2),
87–98.
abilities. For example, interventions that assist these students in de-
Bong, M. (2001). Role of self-efficacy and task-value in predicting college students' course
veloping effective study strategies, scaffold initial academic experi- performance and future enrollment intentions. Contemporary Educational Psychology,
ences, and provide encouragement may bolster self-efficacy beliefs. 26(4), 553–570. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.2000.1048.
Bouffard-Bouchard, T., Parent, S., & Larivee, S. (1991). Influence of self-efficacy on self-
While support for academic self-efficacy is important, approaches to
regulation and performance among junior and senior high-school age students.
motivate students with a self-reported history of reading difficulty that International Journal of Behavioral Development, 14(2), 153–164. https://doi.org/10.
address a range of motivational perceptions, beliefs, and goals are likely 1177/016502549101400203.
to be the most successful (Hidi & Harackiewicz, 2000). The strong re- Brown, K. J. (2013). The experiences of individuals who are diagnosed with learning dis-
abilities in adulthood: A qualitative study (Doctoral dissertation)New York University.
lations among intrinsic value, academic satisfaction, and retention for Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement in-
both students with and without a self-reported history of reading dif- variance. Structural Equation Modeling, 14(3), 464–504. https://doi.org/10.1080/
ficulty underscore the importance of designing learning activities and 10705510701301834.

50
B.W. Bergey et al. Learning and Individual Differences 67 (2018) 41–52

Chevalier, T. M., Parrila, R., Ritchie, K. C., & Deacon, S. H. (2017). The role of meta- York: Guilford Press.
cognitive reading strategies, metacognitive study and learning strategies, and beha- Kosovich, J. J., Hulleman, C. S., Barron, K. E., & Getty, S. (2015). A practical measure of
vioral study and learning strategies in predicting academic success in students with student motivation: Establishing validity evidence for the expectancy-value-cost scale
and without a history of reading difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 50(1), in middle school. Journal of Early Adolescence, 35(5–6), 790–816. https://doi.org/10.
34–48. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219415588850. 1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00995.x.
Chiang, E. S., Byrd, S. P., & Molin, A. J. (2011). Children's perceived cost for exercise: Lefly, D. L., & Pennington, B. F. (1991). Spelling errors and reading fluency in compen-
Application of an expectancy-value paradigm. Health Education & Behavior, 38(2), sated adult dyslexics. Annals of Dyslexia, 41(1), 141–162. https://doi.org/10.1007/
143–149. https://doi.org/10.1177/1090198110376350. BF02648083.
Cohen, J. (1986). Learning disabilities and psychological development in childhood and Lefly, D. L., & Pennington, B. F. (2000). Reliability and validity of the adult reading
adolescence. Annals of Dyslexia, 36(1), 287–300. https://doi.org/10.1007/ history questionnaire. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(3), 286–296. https://doi.
BF02648035. org/10.1177/002221940003300306.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, Lent, R. W. (2004). Toward a unifying theoretical and practical perspective on well-being
NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates. and psychosocial adjustment. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 51(4), 482–509.
Conley, A. M. (2012). Patterns of motivation beliefs: Combining achievement goal and https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.51.4.482.
expectancy-value perspectives. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(1), 32–47. Lent, R. W., Singley, D., Sheu, H.-B., Gainor, K. A., Brenner, B. R., Treistman, D., & Ades,
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026042. L. (2005). Social cognitive predictors of domain and life satisfaction: Exploring the
Corkett, J. K., Hein, S. F., & Parrila, R. (2008). Compensating for reading difficulties: A theoretical precursors of subjective well-being. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
qualitative investigation of university students' experiences of influential personal 52(3), 429–442. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.52.3.429.
characteristics. Exceptionality Education International, 18(2), 51–68. Lent, R. W., Singley, D., Sheu, H.-B., Schmidt, J. A., & Schmidt, L. C. (2007). Relation of
Davenport, T. J., & Lane, A. M. (2006). Relationships between self-efficacy, coping and social-cognitive factors to academic satisfaction in engineering students. Journal of
student retention. Social Behavior and Personality, 34, 127–138. Career Assessment, 15(1), 87–97. https://doi.org/10.1177/1069072706294518.
Deacon, S. H., Cook, K., & Parrila, R. (2012). Identifying high-functioning dyslexics: Is Łockiewicz, M., Bogdanowicz, K. M., & Bogdanowicz, M. (2014). Psychological resources
self-report of early reading problems enough? Annals of Dyslexia, 62(2), 120–134. of adults with developmental dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 47(6),
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-012-0068-2. 543–555. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413478663.
Deacon, S. H., Tucker, R., & Bergey, B. W. (2015). The role of metalinguistic and socio- Madaus, J. W. (2005). Navigating the college transition maze: A guide for students with
cognitive factors in reading skill. In E. Segers, & P. van den Broek (Eds.). learning disabilities. Teaching Exceptional Children, 37(3), 32–37. https://doi.org/10.
Developmental perspectives in written language and literacy. Amsterdam: John 1177/004005990503700305.
Benjamins. Margolis, H., & McCabe, P. P. (2004). Self-efficacy: A key to improving the motivation of
Deacon, S. H., Tucker, R., Bergey, B. W., Laroche, A., & Parrila, R. (2017). Personalized struggling learners. The Clearing House: A Journal of Educational Strategies, Issues and
outreach to university students with a history of reading difficulties: Early screening Ideas, 77(6), 241–249. https://doi.org/10.1080/10459880309603362.
and outreach to support academically at-risk students. Journal of College Student McGonnell, M., Parrila, R., & Deacon, S. H. (2007). The recruitment and description of
Development, 58(3), 432–450. https://doi.org/10.1353/csd.2017.0032. university students who self-report difficulty acquiring early reading skills.
DeDeppo, L. M. (2009). Integration factors related to the academic success and intent to Exceptionality Education Canada, 17(1/2), 155–174.
persist of college students with learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research and Multon, K. D., Brown, S. D., & Lent, R. W. (1991). Relation of self-efficacy beliefs to
Practice, 24(3), 122–131. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5826.2009.00286.x. academic outcomes: A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
Eccles, J. (2009). Who am I and what am I going to do with my life? Personal and col- 38(10), 30–38. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.38.1.30.
lective identities as motivators of action. Educational Psychologist, 44(2), 78–89. Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2012). Mplus user's guide (Seventh Edition). Los Angeles,
https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520902832368. CA: Muthén & Muthén.
Eccles, J. S., Adler, T. F., Futterman, R., Goff, S. B., Kaczala, C. M., Meece, J. L., & Neuville, S., Frenay, M., Schmitz, J., Boudrenghien, G., Noël, B., & Wertz, V. (2007).
Midgley, C. (1983). Expectancies, values, and academic behaviors. In J. T. Spence Tinto's theoretical perspective and expectancy-value paradigm: A confrontation to
(Ed.). Achievement and achievement motivation (pp. 75–146). San Francisco, CA: W. H. explain freshmen's academic achievement. Psychologica Belgica, 47(1), 31–50.
Freeman. https://doi.org/10.5334/pb-47-1-31.
Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (1995). In the mind of the actor: The structure of adolescents' Ojeda, L., Flores, L. Y., & Navarro, R. L. (2011). Social cognitive predictors of Mexican
achievement task values and expectancy-related beliefs. Personality and Social American college students' academic and life satisfaction. Journal of Counseling
Psychology Bulletin, 21(3), 215–225. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167295213003. Psychology, 58(1), 61–71. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021687.
Eckes, S. E., & Ochoa, T. A. (2005). Students with disabilities: Transitioning from high Orenstein, M. (2000). Picking up the clues: Understanding undiagnosed learning dis-
school to higher education. American Secondary Education, 33(3), 6–20. abilities, shame, and imprisoned intelligence. Journal of College Student Psychotherapy,
Fink, R. P. (1995). Successful dyslexics: A constructivist study of passionate interest 15(2), 35–46.
reading. Journal of Adolescent & Adult Literacy: A Journal from the International Reading Pajares, F. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs in academic settings. Review of Educational
Association, 39(4), 268–280. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-998-0014-5. Research, 66(4), 543–578. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543066004543.
Flake, J. K., Barron, K. E., Hulleman, C., McCoach, B. D., & Welsh, M. E. (2015). Parrila, R., Corkett, J., Kirby, J., & Hein, S. (2003). Adult reading history questionnaire-
Measuring cost: The forgotten component of expectancy-value theory. Contemporary Revised. Unpublished questionnaireUniversity of Alberta.
Educational Psychology, 41, 232–244. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2015.03. Parrila, R., Georgiou, G., & Corkett, J. (2007). University students with a significant
002. history of reading difficulty: What is and is not compensated? Exceptionality Education
Garriott, P. O., Hudyma, A., Keene, C., & Santiago, D. (2015). Social cognitive predictors Canada, 17, 195–220. https://doi.org/10.1037/t52486-000.
of first- and non-first-generation college students' academic and life satisfaction. Perez, T., Cromley, J. G., & Kaplan, A. (2014). The role of identity development, values,
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 62(2), 253–263. https://doi.org/10.1037/ and costs in college STEM retention. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106(1),
cou0000066. 315–329. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034027.
Hampton, N. Z., & Mason, E. (2003). Learning disabilities, gender, sources of efficacy, Rodgers, K. A., & Summers, J. J. (2008). African American students at predominantly
self-efficacy beliefs, and academic achievement in high school students. Journal of white institutions: A motivational and self-systems approach to understanding re-
School Psychology, 41(2), 101–112. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-4405(03) tention. Educational Psychology Review, 20(2), 171–190. https://doi.org/10.1007/
00028-1. s10648-008-9072-9.
Hartwell, M., & Kaplan, A. (2018). Students' personal connection with science: Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A review of re-
Investigating the multidimensional phenomenological structure of self-relevance. The search on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52(1),
Journal of Experimental Education, 86(1), 86–104. 141–166. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141.
Hen, M., & Goroshit, M. (2014). Academic procrastination, emotional intelligence, aca- Schertzer, C. B., & Schertzer, S. M. B. (2004). Student satisfaction and retention: A con-
demic self-efficacy, and GPA: A comparison between students with and without ceptual model. Journal of Marketing for Higher Education, 14(1), 79–91. https://doi.
learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 47(2), 116–124. https://doi.org/ org/10.1300/J050v14n01_05.
10.1177/0022219412439325. Schneider, M., & Preckel, F. (2017). Variables associated with achievement in higher
Hidi, S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2000). Motivating the academically unmotivated: A education: A systematic review of meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 143(6),
critical issue for the 21st century. Review of Educational Research, 70(2), 151–179. 565–600.
https://doi.org/10.2307/1170660. Schunk, D. H. (1991). Self-efficacy and academic motivation. Educational Psychologist,
Ishitani, T. T., & DesJardins, S. L. (2002). A longitudinal investigation of dropout from 26(3–4), 207–231. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.1991.9653133.
college in the United States. Journal of College Student Retention, 4(2), 173–201. Selkirk, L. C., Bouchey, H. A., & Eccles, J. S. (2011). Interactions among domain-specific
Jose, P. E. (2013). Doing statistical mediation and moderation. New York: Guilford Press. expectancies, values, and gender: Predictors of test anxiety during early adolescence.
Kaplan, C. P., & Shachter, E. (1991). Adults with undiagnosed learning disabilities: Journal of Early Adolescence, 31(3), 361–389. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Practice considerations. Families in Society, 72(4), 195–201. 0272431610363156.
Klassen, R. M., Krawchuk, L. L., & Rajani, S. (2008). Academic procrastination of un- Sheu, H. B., Mejia, A., Rigali-Oiler, M., Primé, D. R., & Chong, S. S. (2016). Social cog-
dergraduates: Low self-efficacy to self-regulate predicts higher levels of procrastina- nitive predictors of academic and life satisfaction: Measurement and structural
tion. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 33(4), 915–931. https://doi.org/10.1016/ equivalence across three racial/ethnic groups. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 63(4),
j.cedpsych.2007.07.001. 460–474. https://doi.org/10.1037/cou0000158.
Klassen, R. M., & Usher, E. L. (2010). Self-efficacy in educational settings: Recent research Sideridis, G. D. (2009). Motivation and learning disabilities: Past, present, and future. In
and emerging directions. In S. A. Karabenick, & T. C. Urdan (Eds.). The decade ahead: K. R. Wenzel, A. Wigfield, K. R. Wenzel, & A. Wigfield (Eds.). Handbook of motivation
Theoretical perspectives on motivation and achievement (pp. 1–33). Bingley, England: at school (pp. 605–625). New York, NY, US: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
Emerald Group Publishing Limited. Starr, A., Betz, E. L., & Menne, J. (1972). Differences in college student satisfaction:
Kline, R. B. (2010). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (3rd ed.). New Academic dropouts, nonacademic dropouts and nondropouts. Journal of Counseling

51
B.W. Bergey et al. Learning and Individual Differences 67 (2018) 41–52

Psychology, 19(4), 318–322. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0033083. 10.3200/JRLP.143.1.91-112.


Tinto, V. (1975). Dropout from higher education: A theoretical synthesis of recent re- Wessel, R. D., Jones, J. A., Markle, L., & Westfall, C. (2009). Retention and graduation of
search. Review of Educational Research, 45(1), 89–125. students with disabilities: Facilitating student success. Journal of Postsecondary
Tinto, V. (1993). Leaving college: Rethinking the causes and cures of student attrition (2nd Education and Disability, 21(3), 116–125.
ed.). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Wigfield, A., & Cambria, J. (2010). Students' achievement values, goal orientations, and
Tinto, V. (2007). Taking student retention seriously. Syracuse, NY: Syracuse University. interest: Definitions, development, and relations to achievement outcomes.
Vogel, S. A., & Adelman, P. B. (1992). The success of college students with learning Developmental Review, 30(1), 1–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2009.12.001.
disabilities: Factors related to educational attainment. Journal of Learning Disabilities, Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2000). Expectancy–value theory of achievement motivation.
25(7), 430–441. https://doi.org/10.1177/002221949202500703. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25(1), 68–81. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.
Watt, H. M. (2004). Development of adolescents' self-perceptions, values, and task per- 1999.1015.
ceptions according to gender and domain in 7th-through 11th-grade Australian stu- Wigfield, A., Tonks, S., & Eccles, J. S. (2004). Expectancy-value theory in cross-cultural
dents. Child Development, 75(5), 1556–1574. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624. perspective. In D. M. McInerney, & S. Van Etten (Vol. Eds.), Big theories revisited:
2004.00757.x. Research on sociocultural influences on motivation and learning. Vol. 4. Big theories re-
Wefald, A. J., & Downey, R. G. (2009). Construct dimensionality of engagement and its visited: Research on sociocultural influences on motivation and learning (pp. 165–198).
relation with satisfaction. The Journal of Psychology, 143(1), 91–112. https://doi.org/ Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.

52

You might also like