You are on page 1of 4

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/5779411

Reactive agility of rugby league players

Article  in  Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport · January 2008


DOI: 10.1016/j.jsams.2007.08.011 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

106 1,495

2 authors, including:

Tim Gabbett
Gabbett Performance Solutions
323 PUBLICATIONS   18,044 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

MOST DEMANDING PASSAGES OF PLAY - FOOTBALL View project

Sprinting Performance in Elite Rugby Sevens View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Tim Gabbett on 27 August 2020.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport (2009) 12, 212—214

SHORT REPORT

Reactive agility of rugby league players


Tim Gabbett a,∗, Dean Benton b

a Queensland Academy of Sport, Australia


b Brisbane Broncos Rugby League Club, Australia

Received 24 July 2007 ; received in revised form 16 August 2007; accepted 18 August 2007

KEYWORDS Summary While studies have investigated change of direction speed in rugby
Anticipation; league players, no study has investigated the reactive agility of these athletes.
Decision-making; The purpose of this study was to investigate the reactive agility of rugby league
Speed; players, to determine if this quality discriminated higher and lesser skilled players.
Agility; Twenty-four elite (mean ± S.D. age, 24.5 ± 4.2 years) and 42 sub-elite (23.6 ± 5.3
Change of direction years) rugby league players completed a game-specific test of reactive agility. Elite
players had better response accuracy (93.2 ± 1.9% vs. 85.5 ± 2.5%; p < 0.05, effect
speed
size = 0.58) and faster decision (89.5 ± 5.8 ms vs. 111.5 ± 6.4 ms; p < 0.05, effect
size = 0.62) and movement times (2.35 ± 0.03 s vs. 2.56 ± 0.03 s; p < 0.05, effect
size = 1.39) on the reactive agility test than sub-elite players. The reactive agility test
was able to distinguish four distinct classifications. Specifically, players were classi-
fied as requiring either (1) decision-making and change of direction speed training
to further consolidate good physical and perceptual abilities, (2) decision-making
training to develop below average perceptual abilities, (3) change of direction
speed training to develop below average physical attributes or (4) a combination
of decision-making and change of direction speed training to develop below average
physical and perceptual abilities. The results of this study demonstrate that a test
of reactive agility discriminates higher and lesser skilled rugby league players. In
addition, these findings highlight the important contribution of perceptual skill to
agility in rugby league players.
© 2007 Sports Medicine Australia. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Introduction change of direction speed tests that rely on pre-


planned movements. Although most practitioners
Given the importance of agility to rugby league would classify agility as any movement involving
players,1 several investigators have assessed this rapid change(s) of direction, agility has recently
quality using a wide range of performance tests been defined as a rapid change of direction in
(e.g. ‘L’ run and 505 test).2,3 However, a limi- response to a sport-specific stimulus.4 While the
tation of these tests is that they are all simple majority of agility research has been devoted
to pre-planned change of direction speed tests,
∗ Corresponding author. investigators have recently begun to study the per-
E-mail address: tim.gabbett@srq.qld.gov.au (T. Gabbett). ceptual components of agility.5,6 In this respect,

1440-2440/$ — see front matter © 2007 Sports Medicine Australia. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jsams.2007.08.011
Reactive agility in rugby league 213

the ability of team sport athletes to ‘read and ferent for each player.5 The test protocol involved
react’ to a game-specific stimulus has been tested. randomised presentation of four different cues, for
Sheppard et al.5 demonstrated that a test of reac- a total of eight trials (i.e. two trials on each of
tive agility was able to successfully discriminate the four different cues). These cues created varying
higher and lesser skilled Australian football play- demands on the participants (as in a game setting).
ers, where pre-planned change of direction speed The participants sprinted forward prior to any
tests did not. Farrow et al.6 reported similar results change of direction, in reaction to the forward
for netball players. Given the important contribu- movement of the investigator. The participant was
tion reactive agility tests have made to other team instructed to recognise the cues as soon as possi-
sports, it is likely that a test of reactive agility could ble, and react by changing direction and sprinting
significantly advance the understanding of agility in through the gates on the left or right in response.
rugby league. The participants were instructed to emphasise
While investigators have studied speed and decision-making accuracy and speed of movement.
change of direction speed in rugby league play- A high-speed video camera (HSC-200 PM, Peak
ers, no study has investigated the reactive agility Performance Technologies, Inc.) interfaced with
of these athletes. With this in mind, the purpose of a video recorder (Panasonic AG-5700) was posi-
this study was to investigate the reactive agility of tioned 5 m behind the player in order to record the
rugby league players, to determine if this quality player’s change of movement direction relative to
discriminated higher and lesser skilled players. the investigator. Video was sampled at 200 Hz so
that the number of frames between the investiga-
tor’s and the player’s movement initiation enabled
Methods the player’s decision-making time to be recorded to
within ±5 ms for each trial.
Twenty-four elite (mean ± S.D. age, 24.5 ± 4.2 Movement time was defined as the total time
years) and 42 sub-elite (23.6 ± 5.3 years) rugby taken to complete the test, from the moment the
league players participated in this study. Elite play- investigator initiated the test and began the tim-
ers were members of a National Rugby League ing, to the moment the player triggered the timing
club while sub-elite players were registered with beam on either side of the investigator. Decision
a recreational rugby league club. time was defined as the time difference between
The reactive agility test has been described in the first definitive foot contact initiating the move-
detail elsewhere.5 The player began on a marked ment of the investigator in the final direction he
line with timing gates placed 5 m to the left and moved his body, and the first definitive foot con-
right and 2 m forward of the start line. tact initiating the response of the player. Response
The investigator stood opposite, and facing, the accuracy was expressed in terms of whether the
player. The investigator stood behind a set of timing player moved to the correct finish gate relative to
lights. Each test trial involved the investigator initi- the movement of the investigator.
ating movement, and thereby beginning the timing. Differences in performances between elite and
The player reacted to the movements of the inves- sub-elite players were compared using an indepen-
tigator by moving forward, then to the left or right dent t-test.
in response to, and in the same direction as, the left
or right movement of the investigator. The timing
stopped when the player triggered the timing beam Results
on either side.
The investigator displayed one of four possible Significant differences (p < 0.05) were detected
scenarios to the player. The four possible scenar- between elite and sub-elite players for response
ios all involved steps of approximately 1.5 m, and accuracy, decision and movement times on the
were presented in a random order that was dif- reactive agility test (Table 1).

Table 1 Reactive agility of elite and sub-elite rugby league players


Reactive agility Elite Sub-elite Effect size
Movement time (s) 2.35 (2.30—2.40)* 2.56 (2.51—2.61) 1.39
Decision time (ms) 89.5 (78.2—100.8)* 111.5 (99.0—124.0) 0.62
Response accuracy (%) 93.2 (89.5—96.8)* 85.5 (80.6—90.5) 0.58
Data are mean (and 95% confidence intervals).
* Denotes significant differences (p < 0.05) between groups.
214 T. Gabbett, D. Benton

Discussion Among a squad of elite and sub-elite rugby


league players, the reactive agility test was able
The purpose of this study was to investigate the to distinguish four distinct classifications. Specif-
reactive agility of rugby league players, to deter- ically, players were classified as requiring either
mine if this quality discriminated higher and lesser (1) decision-making and change of direction speed
skilled players. The results of this study demon- training to further consolidate good physical and
strate that decision and movement times on the perceptual abilities, (2) decision-making training
reactive agility test were faster in higher skilled to develop below average perceptual abilities, (3)
players, without compromising response accuracy. change of direction speed training to develop below
The finding of superior anticipatory skill in the elite average physical attributes or (4) a combination
players of the present study suggests that these of decision-making and change of direction speed
players had a greater ability to extract relevant training to develop below average physical and per-
information earlier in the visual display, by identify- ceptual abilities.
ing relevant postural cues presented by the tester, While this study assessed the relatively recent
and disregarding irrelevant sources of information.6 concept of reactive agility in rugby league play-
These findings demonstrate the practical utility of ers, this quality has been shown to be relevant to
the reactive agility test for assessing the perceptual a wide range of team sport athletes.5,6 In combi-
components of agility in rugby league players. nation with traditional change of direction speed
The inclusion of decision time and response accu- tests, coaches may use these results to objectively
racy information using high-speed video footage determine the individual strengths and weaknesses
provides additional insight into the anticipatory of players, so that the most appropriate agility
strengths and weaknesses of rugby league play- training stimulus (i.e. either pre-planned change
ers. The analysis of the high-speed video footage of direction movements, or reactive agility drills)
allows for a delineation of the performance com- can be prescribed for an individual player’s needs.
ponents of the reactive agility test, by determining However, despite the importance of reactive agility
whether the test was limited by decision time or for team sports, few studies have investigated
movement speed. While the study by Sheppard et whether this quality can be improved through train-
al.5 provided important information on the reactive ing. Consequently, the present results should be
agility of Australian football players, it did not mea- considered a starting point for further research in
sure decision time or response accuracy of players. this area.
Consequently, their results are likely to be heav-
ily influenced by the movement speed of athletes.
Indeed, in the present study, if movement time References
alone was used to classify reactive agility, some
players would be incorrectly classified as having 1. Meir R, Arthur D, Forrest M. Time and motion analysis of
superior anticipatory skill. professional rugby league: a case study. Strength Cond Coach
1993;1:24—9.
It should be noted that a speed-accuracy trade- 2. Meir R, Newton R, Curtis E, et al. Physical fitness qualities
off exists in the majority of players, so that of professional rugby league players: determination of posi-
reductions in decision time (i.e. faster decision- tional differences. J Strength Cond Res 2001;15:450—8.
making ability) result in reductions in response 3. Gabbett T, Kelly J, Ralph S, et al. Physiological and anthro-
accuracy. While the instructions to emphasise pometric characteristics of junior elite and sub-elite rugby
league players, with special reference to starters and non-
decision-making accuracy and speed of movement starters. J Sci Med Sport, in press.
were identical to those recommended by previ- 4. Sheppard JM, Young WB. Agility literature review: classi-
ous authors,5 it is possible that this instructional fications, training and testing. J Sports Sci 2006;24:919—
set did contribute to the speed-accuracy trade-off 32.
observed in the present study. It is possible that 5. Sheppard JM, Young WB, Doyle TA, et al. An evaluation
of a new test of reactive agility, and its relationship to
requesting players to ‘react to the tester as you sprint speed and change of direction speed. J Sci Med Sport
would in a game situation by moving as quickly 2006;9:342—9.
as possible to intercept the tester’ may provide 6. Farrow D, Young W, Bruce L. The development of a test of
more relevant instructions to minimise the speed- reactive agility for netball: a new methodology. J Sci Med
accuracy trade-off. Sport 2005;8:52—60.

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

View publication stats

You might also like