You are on page 1of 11

Proprioceptive Acuity Assessment Via Joint Position

Matching: From Basic Science to General Practice


Daniel J. Goble
PHYS THER. 2010; 90:1176-1184.
Originally published online June 3, 2010
doi: 10.2522/ptj.20090399

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, can be
found online at: http://ptjournal.apta.org/content/90/8/1176

Online-Only Material http://ptjournal.apta.org/content/suppl/2010/07/22/90.8.11


76.DC1.html
Collections This article, along with others on similar topics, appears
in the following collection(s):
Neurology/Neuromuscular System: Other
Perspectives
Tests and Measurements
e-Letters To submit an e-Letter on this article, click here or click on
"Submit a response" in the right-hand menu under
"Responses" in the online version of this article.
E-mail alerts Sign up here to receive free e-mail alerts

Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on March 18, 2013


Perspective

Proprioceptive Acuity Assessment Via


Joint Position Matching: From Basic
Science to General Practice
Daniel J. Goble
D.J. Goble, PhD, is Fellow for the
Canadian Institutes for Health Re- Over the past several decades, studies of use-dependent plasticity have demonstrated
search: Institutes of Aging, housed
in the Department of Physical a critical role for proprioceptive feedback in the reorganization, and subsequent
Medicine and Rehabilitation, Uni- recovery, of neuromotor systems. As such, an increasing emphasis has been placed
versity of Michigan, 325 E. Eisen- on tests of proprioceptive acuity in both the clinic and the laboratory. One test that
hower, Suite 100, Ann Arbor, MI has garnered particular interest is joint position matching, whereby individuals must
48108 (USA). Address all corre- replicate a reference joint angle in the absence of vision (ie, using proprioceptive
spondence to Dr Goble at:
dgoble@umich.edu. information). On the surface, this test might seem straightforward in nature. How-
ever, the present perspective article informs therapists and researchers alike of
[Goble DJ. Proprioceptive acuity multiple insights gained from a recent series of position matching studies by the
assessment via joint position
matching: from basic science to author and colleagues. In particular, 5 factors are outlined that can assist clinicians in
general practice. Phys Ther. developing well-informed opinions regarding the outcomes of tests of position
2010;90:1176 –1184.] matching abilities. This information should allow for enhanced diagnosis of propri-
© 2010 American Physical Therapy oceptive deficits within clinical settings in the future.
Association

Post a Rapid Response to


this article at:
ptjournal.apta.org

1176 f Physical Therapy Volume 90 Number 8 August 2010


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on March 18, 2013
Factors Affecting Position Matching

O
ver the past several decades, sessment of proprioceptive acuity in cidated through numerous studies
a relative shift has occurred clinical situations is the joint position conducted in the same laboratory
in the field of physical ther- matching task.17–33 In this test, a pa- (see acknowledgments) over the
apy and rehabilitation medicine. In tient is asked to re-create (ie, match) past 5 years. For each experiment,
contrast to traditional assessments a reference joint angle (ie, position) an elbow position matching para-
of physical well-being, which have in the absence of vision. The accu- digm was used involving a custom-
focused largely on the ability of in- racy of matching is subsequently de- made motorized manipulanda sys-
dividuals to generate motor output, termined in a variety of ways, ranging tem (Fig. 1; see video available
a greater emphasis is now being from the use of specialized biome- online at ptjournal.apta.org). Partici-
placed on sensory feedback acuity. chanical equipment to more qualita- pants were blindfolded (to prevent
This fundamental change is likely tive visual inspection by a trained the use of vision) and seated with
the result of our growing under- observer. head supported and forearms resting
standing regarding the role sensory on 2 aluminum levers. The levers
information plays in promoting neu- Although the validity and reliability were attached below the pivot point
ral plasticity through use-dependent of position matching tests have rarely of the elbow to a ball bearing system
mechanisms. Indeed, it is now well been evaluated,17,34,35 it is generally that allowed near-frictionless move-
accepted that a clear relationship ex- well accepted that the magnitude of ment of the forearm about the elbow
ists between massed physical prac- matching errors can be a useful indi- joint in the horizontal plane. Refer-
tice and the expression of neural rep- cator of proprioceptive acuity. More ence elbow positions were estab-
resentations in primary sensorimotor specifically, those individuals who are lished either by a torque motor sys-
regions of the brain.1,2 prone to making large position match- tem or through movement of the
ing errors are thought to be, at least lever by the experimenter via a han-
Arguably the most important source in some way, proprioceptively defi- dle attached to its distal end.
of feedback for promoting neural cient. The purpose of this perspective
plasticity is our sense of propriocep- article is to express to clinicians and Elbow position (ie, angle) data were
tion.3– 6 Proprioception can be de- clinical researchers alike that, despite continuously transduced into a volt-
fined as the ability of an individual to their relative efficacy, some caution age signal via potentiometers located
determine body segment positions is necessary when designing and below the ball bearing system (ie,
and movements in space and is interpreting the results of position elbow joint). This analog signal was
based on sensory signals provided to matching studies. The foundation digitally sampled by a laptop com-
the brain from muscle,7–9 joint,10 for this assertion lies in a recent puter and displayed on the comput-
and skin11,12 receptors. Studies of in- series of studies conducted by me er’s screen in real time. The online
dividuals who lack proprioceptive and my colleagues that assessed, in display gave the experimenter a con-
sense (due mainly to large fiber neu- detail, the position matching abilities tinuous estimate of joint position
ropathy) have taught us important of individuals with and without dis- throughout the experimental ses-
lessons regarding the role of propri- abilities.36 – 42 Taken together, the re- sion. Testing sessions consisted of 30
oceptive feedback during sensorimo- sults of these studies emphasize that to 75 matching trials presented in a
tor performance. Indeed, despite the position matching tasks are not as pseudo-randomized order for the
fact that these individuals have mo- straightforward as they might appear condition of interest. Data were
tor systems that remain fully intact,13 initially and that matching errors are saved to the computer’s hard drive,
“deafferented” people show pro- significantly influenced by a number which allowed for more precise cal-
found deficits in motor control. of experimental factors. In this arti-
These deficits include, but are not cle, a summary of 5 of these factors
limited to, poor endpoint accura- and their clinical relevance is pro- Available With
cy,14 reduced control of multiseg- vided. It is hoped that this informa- This Article at
mental dynamics,15,16 and an inabil- tion will aid therapists in future as- ptjournal.apta.org
ity to perform extended movement sessments of proprioceptive acuity,
sequences.13 both in the clinic and in the • Video: Animation of basic
laboratory. experimental setup.
Although other methods (eg, identi- • Audio Abstracts Podcast
fication of imposed movement direc- An Overview of the Basic
This article was published ahead of
tion or measurement of movement Experimental Setup print on June 3, 2010, at
detection threshold) are available, an The experimental factors described ptjournal.apta.org.
increasingly utilized tool for the as- in this perspective article were elu-

August 2010 Volume 90 Number 8 Physical Therapy f 1177


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on March 18, 2013
Factors Affecting Position Matching

The use of contralateral matching


tasks eliminates the need for memory-
based matching because the reference
joint angle is available throughout the
task from the opposing limb. Despite
this advantage, however, contralateral
position matching tasks are subject to
limitations of their own. First, due to
the involvement of both arms in the
task, it is difficult to ascertain whether
the measured error arises from the ref-
erence arm, the matching arm, or
both. Additionally, based on the ana-
tomical pathways involved in the
transmission of peripheral proprio-
ceptive input to the brain, it is al-
most certain that matching using the
Figure 1. opposite limb requires greater inter-
Animated version of the basic setup used for determining the experimental factors hemispheric communication (or
influencing proprioceptive position matching (see video available online at ptjournal.
apta.org).
transfer) compared with ipsilateral
matching. Specifically, propriocep-
tive information from the reference
limb is directed initially to the pri-
culation of matching error using lished target joint angle remains at the mary somatosensory region of the
computer algorithms off-line. The reference position throughout match- cerebral cortex in the contralateral
preferred measure of matching accu- ing and can serve as an “on-line” refer- hemisphere (eg, right arm reference
racy was absolute error, defined as ence to aid in participant matching. information first received by the left
the absolute difference between the hemisphere).43,44 Before this infor-
reference and matching elbow joint Although the choice between ipsilat- mation can be used to direct action
angles in degrees. eral and contralateral limb matching of the opposite limb, which also is
tasks may seem trivial, this is most controlled by the opposing hemi-
Factor 1: Type of Position certainly not the case. For example, sphere, it must cross the hemi-
Matching Task in ipsilateral matching tasks, where spheric divide. This crossing likely
There are 2 types of position match- the same arm serves to establish both occurs via the transcallosal pathways
ing tasks that have commonly been the reference and matching loca- of the corpus callosum.45 The en-
used for the assessment of proprio- tions, it is inherently necessary for hanced need for interhemispheric
ceptive acuity in clinical and re- participants to use memory in order transfer in contralateral matching
search situations. In the first task to accurately match the target posi- tasks, therefore, represents another
type, a reference joint angle initially tion. This reliance on memory is due significant cognitive factor that may
is provided to the participant for a to the fact that no single limb can influence position matching perfor-
short period of time (usually several concurrently provide a reference mance in certain clinical populations.
seconds). Then, the joint is brought joint angle while performing match- Indeed, individuals with asymmetric
back to the “start” angle, and the ing of that angle. Given the memory brain injuries or individuals with tran-
participant is asked to replicate the component to this type of task, cli- scallosal degeneration would seem to
previously experienced reference nicians should exercise caution be particularly disadvantaged for con-
position using the same (ie, ipsilat- when ipsilateral matching is used to tralateral matching tasks.
eral) arm. This so-called “ipsilateral assess proprioceptive acuity in indi-
matching” task is contrasted by the viduals who are prone to having The influence of task type on posi-
second commonly used task type, memory issues. Indeed, it is likely tion matching has been tested di-
known as “contralateral matching.” that, in this situation, some portion rectly by the author and colleagues
Contralateral matching involves of the matching error measured ac- in several groups of young individu-
matching of a reference joint angle tually reflects cognitive or memory als who were healthy.37,39 – 41 These
with the opposite (ie, contralateral) deficits, rather than any decrease in studies compared both the ipsilateral
limb. In this case, the previously estab- proprioception itself. and contralateral position matching

1178 f Physical Therapy Volume 90 Number 8 August 2010


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on March 18, 2013
Factors Affecting Position Matching

tasks described above, as well as a


third type of matching task that com-
bines elements of ipsilateral and con-
tralateral matching. In the third task,
participants are given a reference
(elbow) joint angle on one side of
the body and required to memorize
this position over the course of sev-
eral seconds. The reference joint po-
sition then is taken away from the
participants by changing the joint
angle just prior to their making a
mirror image match of the reference
angle with the opposite arm (ie, re-
quiring interhemispheric transfer).
Compared with the ipsilateral and con-
tralateral tasks, which involve only
memory or interhemispheric commu-
nication demands, respectively, the
third task (also known as “contralat-
eral remembered” matching) is con-
sidered to be more challenging, as it
requires both memory and interhemi-
spheric transfer components.
Figure 2.
Figure 2 shows exemplary data from Average (SE) absolute errors for various types of position matching tasks.
previously unpublished work dem- IHT⫽interhemispheric transfer.
onstrating the effects of task type on
absolute elbow position matching er-
rors in 10 individuals who were
healthy. Overall, clear differences in mands of the particular type of Contrary to the above hypothesis,
matching error are evident among all matching task being performed. however, one of the more intriguing
3 task conditions, with the smallest and consistent findings to arise from
errors evident in the ipsilateral Factor 2: Limb Preference the work of my colleagues and me is
matching task involving memory de- Handedness is the tendency to pre- a nonpreferred arm advantage for
mand. In contrast, absolute match- fer one arm versus the other arm for proprioceptive position matching
ing errors in the contralateral task, performing various activities of daily tasks. Inspired by previous research
requiring more interhemispheric in- living and is one of the most obvious within the realm of psychology,51–54
teraction, are greater than those in examples of lateralized brain func- my colleagues and I have explored in
the ipsilateral case, despite not hav- tion in humans.46 Based largely on some depth the extent to which el-
ing a memory component. In the questionnaires such as the Edin- bow position matching errors differ
contralateral remembered condition, burgh Handedness Inventory of Old- for the left and right arms of left-
where cognitive demands included field,47 it has been estimated that handed42 and right-handed37– 40,55 in-
both memory and interhemispheric 90% of the population is right- dividuals. In initial studies,37–39 the 3
communication, absolute errors are handed for tasks such as handwrit- matching tasks described in the pre-
approximately twice as large as in ing, throwing a ball, or eating with a vious section (ie, ipsilateral, con-
the ipsilateral condition and about spoon.48 –50 Given the robust and en- tralateral, and contralateral remem-
one third greater than in the con- during nature of this phenomenon, a bered) were used to manipulate the
tralateral matching task. Taken to- logical prediction might be that the proprioceptive demands (ie, the
gether, these results provide strong preferred right arm of right-handed need for memory or interhemi-
evidence that tests of position people would demonstrate a posi- spheric transfer of proprioceptive in-
matching are not simple measures of tion matching advantage over the formation) placed on the partici-
proprioceptive acuity alone. Rather, nonpreferred left arm. pants. The results of this work
they also reflect the cognitive de-

August 2010 Volume 90 Number 8 Physical Therapy f 1179


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on March 18, 2013
Factors Affecting Position Matching

showed that not only was matching outside of visual attention, where it important factor to consider in tests
more accurate by the nonpreferred must rely on proprioceptive feed- of position matching ability. This
arm across all task types but the mag- back to perform tasks such as hold- consideration includes not only the
nitude of arm asymmetry favoring ing objects in a particular position.56 magnitude of the reference joint an-
the nonpreferred arm also was rela- gle but also the characteristics of
tively increased with greater cogni- The nonpreferred arm position how the joint was displaced to pro-
tive demands. Specifically, although matching advantages seen in adults duce that angle. Studies by me and my
preferred arm errors were progres- who are healthy also appear to have colleagues have regularly made com-
sively larger when comparing the meaning for position matching as- parisons of matching errors at dif-
contralateral condition with the ipsi- sessments of proprioceptive acuity ferent reference joint angles.38 – 40,42
lateral and contralateral remembered in clinical populations. There is cur- Based on this work, we found that a
conditions, nonpreferred arm match- rently evidence in the literature that clear relationship exists between the
ing errors were similar in magnitude enhanced nonpreferred left arm target amplitude and the magnitude
across all tasks. matching is linked to a right- of matching error, with further tar-
hemisphere specialization for the gets inducing greater matching er-
To ensure that the handedness ef- processing of proprioceptive inputs. rors (ie, poor performance). For ex-
fects seen in initial studies of posi- The most convincing work done in ample, in one study,38 20 young
tion matching truly were due to pro- this area to date is that of Naito and adults who were healthy were asked
prioceptive processing, rather than a colleagues,44,57 who used a tendon to match targets that were either 20
generally greater ability of the non- vibration paradigm in combination or 40 degrees from the start elbow
preferred arm to match elbow posi- with neuroimaging to map regions of joint position. Regardless of whether
tions, a follow-up experiment was the brain responsible for processing the preferred or nonpreferred arm
conducted. In this study,40 young input from key proprioceptors—the served as the reference, absolute er-
participants who were healthy were muscle spindles. Given that the basis rors were found to be, on average,
asked to match 15- and 30-degree for proprioceptive performance lies 66% greater for the larger target am-
elbow extension positions according more directly within the right hemi- plitude. One hypothesis that has
to both the ipsilateral and contralat- sphere, it may be expected that in- been put forth to explain this finding
eral remembered task types. In one dividuals with right-hemisphere is that the increased errors seen with
set of conditions, target elbow an- damage would be more prone to larger target amplitudes reflect in-
gles were matched visually by having proprioceptive deficits than individ- creased sensorimotor noise when
participants point with a laser on a uals with left-hemisphere injury. My movement requires an increased
projection screen to targets that re- colleagues and I36 recently tested neural control signal.58 Nevertheless,
quired the target amount of elbow this hypothesis for the ipsilateral el- clinicians and researchers are en-
extension. In another set of condi- bow matching abilities of children couraged to take great care when
tions, matching relied solely on pro- with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. In conducting position matching stud-
prioceptive information. Based on agreement with our hypothesis, we ies, ensuring that target amplitudes
this design, it was shown that non- found that, compared with children are matched across all populations of
preferred arm position matching ad- who were developing typically, individuals tested and within each
vantages were evident only in the only those individuals with right- particular experimental condition.
proprioceptive version of the match- hemisphere injuries showed in-
ing experiment. Indeed, in the visual creased position matching errors of Beyond the influence of target ampli-
version, the reverse trend was iden- the affected arm and, to a lesser ex- tude on position matching perfor-
tified (ie, a preferred arm advantage tent, the unaffected arm. These pre- mance, experiments from as early as
was seen for matching). These re- liminary results suggest the need for the 1960s demonstrated that partici-
sults suggest that the preferred and differential treatment of lateralized pants make smaller errors when
nonpreferred arms rely to a different brain injuries in the clinic, with a matching a reference position that is
extent on visual versus propriocep- greater emphasis placed on proprio- established through their own active
tive information, as might be benefi- ception in those individuals with movement, rather than the same tar-
cial for performing activities of daily right-hemisphere damage. get position determined passively by
living requiring both hands. Typi- the experimenter.59 – 61 This effect is
cally, the preferred arm requires vi- Factor 3: Reference Position thought to be the consequence of 2
sual information for the fine manip- Establishment movement-related features. First, the
ulation of objects. In contrast, the The means by which the reference sensitivity of muscle spindles (key
nonpreferred arm more often is held joint angle is established also is an proprioceptors) is enhanced during

1180 f Physical Therapy Volume 90 Number 8 August 2010


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on March 18, 2013
Factors Affecting Position Matching

young participants who were


healthy.67 Using a motorized system,
each participant’s elbow joint was dis-
placed to the same reference position,
but at speeds that were faster or
slower than or the same as the speed
at which the elbow was subsequently
moved back to the target location for
matching. It was shown that matching
error was systematically biased by the
speed at which the reference position
was established. Taken together, the
results described in this section under-
score the need for clinicians to take
great care when establishing reference
joint positions. Keeping potential ex-
perimental biases in mind when per-
forming proprioceptive assessments
via position matching will improve
proprioceptive sensibility estimates.

Factor 4: Participant Age


Figure 3. Age-related changes in the periph-
Average (SE) absolute errors in the ipsilateral matching of 30-degree targets for different
eral and central nervous system are a
cross sections of the human life span.
natural consequence of human
growth and development. Peripheral
alterations of proprioceptor size
active movements via the gamma match actively determined reference and number vary across the life span
motor system.62 Second, an “efferent positions. and affect the quality of the limb posi-
copy” of the motor command used tion signal provided to the brain. On
to get to the target position may be Given that most position matching the other hand, central feedback pro-
called upon to inform subsequent assessments conducted in clinical cessing of limb position signals is re-
matching movements.63,64 In addi- environments use passive displace- fined throughout a lifetime of experi-
tion to improvements in matching ment of the joint to the target angle ence. Given that clinical populations
error with actively determined tar- (usually by the experimenter), an- often occupy certain spectra of the life
gets, work by Goble and Brown37 other point of concern for clinical span (eg, cerebral palsy in children,
suggests that matching strategy also researchers is the “tau effect.” The stroke in older people), it is important
is altered based on the means by tau effect, initially described in stud- for clinicians to take into account nat-
which the reference angle is estab- ies of movement extent estima- ural shifts in acuity with age when
lished (ie, active versus passive). In- tion,65,66 manifests as a strong interde- determining the existence of any pro-
deed, regardless of matching task (ie, pendence between time and space, prioceptive deficit.
ipsilateral, contralateral, or contralat- such that movements of longer dura-
eral remembered), elbow joint refer- tion are perceived as traveling farther In light of the task-related factors in-
ence positions determined actively than those of shorter duration. For po- fluencing position matching error
by movement of the participant sition matching experiments, this find- described in the previous sections,
(rather than passively by the experi- ing implies that, if during the establish- the true relationship between propri-
menter) appear to lead to target ment of the target joint angle an oceptive matching ability and age is
movements that are faster and experimenter takes a long versus a difficult to ascertain from a simple
smoother. Although these increases short amount of time, the participants meta-analysis of the existing literature.
in speed and smoothness did not re- will perceive the target as being fur- Rather, comparisons of position
sult in fewer errors in their study, it ther from the starting joint angle than matching performance among various
seems likely that this change in strat- it actually is. The tau effect was con- age groups must be made under ex-
egy reflected the higher confidence firmed recently in a set of elbow posi- perimental conditions that are very
participants had in their ability to tion matching trials conducted with closely matched. In this case, a com-

August 2010 Volume 90 Number 8 Physical Therapy f 1181


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on March 18, 2013
Factors Affecting Position Matching

centered to the right of body mid-


line. In light of this seeming “right
shift” of workspace preference for
visual tasks, a question of interest for
me was whether a similar or differ-
ent dependence on workspace ex-
isted for tests of positions matching.

Figure 4 presents previously unpub-


lished data from a study of 10 young
adults who were healthy and who
performed 20-degree elbow joint po-
sition matching tasks in various loca-
tions of the workspace relative to
body midline. Workspace was di-
vided into far left, near left, near
right, and far right of body midline.
The elbow on the same side of the
body as the workspace tested per-
formed matching in either an ipsilat-
eral or contralateral fashion. In con-
Figure 4. trast to what typically is seen for
Demonstration of bias in average (SE) absolute error when position matching occurs in visually guided tasks, proprioceptive
different locations of the task workspace. position matching showed greater
performance to the left of body mid-
line, with the smallest errors seen in
the far left workspace. These results,
pilation of the data collected by Goble provided in this (or any) particular although subtle in absolute magni-
and colleagues39,40,68 provides an ideal position matching study should not tude (⬃1°), are in good agreement
opportunity to explore the relation- be considered “norms.” Unless very with those of a previous study69 in
ship between normal aging and pro- similar task parameters (eg, elbow which a similar far-left advantage
prioceptive ability. In Figure 3, the av- joint, ipsilateral matching, preferred was found for reaching to proprio-
erage absolute matching errors are arm) are used, it is recommended ceptive targets (ie, the unseen finger
displayed for different cross-sections that therapists make use of study- of a participant located under a ta-
of the age spectrum that were tested specific normative databases when bletop). In this case, a left work-
using very similar task parameters (ie, making any determination of what space bias in position matching per-
ipsilateral matching with the preferred represents pathological performance. formance also should be accounted
arm of a 30° target). As shown in Fig- Indeed, failure to compare data under for in the design and interpretation
ure 3, the relationship between age similar task conditions may result in a of position matching studies by cli-
and position matching error is non- relative underestimation or overesti- nicians and researchers alike.
monotonic, with an overall refinement mation of a patient’s matching ability
of position matching ability (ie, de- and a subsequent misdiagnosis of pro- Summary and Conclusions
crease in absolute error) from child- prioceptive sensibility. Based on studies of elbow position
hood (ie, age 8 –10 years) through matching, this article sought to in-
adolescence to young adulthood. Ab- Factor 5: Task Workspace form clinicians and clinician re-
solute matching errors then increase Task workspace is the relative area searchers of various insights gained
into middle age and continue to rise wherein most individuals perform a from studies conducted by my col-
with old age. given activity of daily living. For most leagues and me over the past half
visually guided upper-limb tasks (eg, decade. Although these insights may
Although the data provided in Figure writing, opening a jar, cutting food) not be fully generalizable to all joints
3 clearly demonstrate the pattern of the “common” workspace used by in the upper and lower extremities,
proprioceptive performance across right-handed individuals is located at it remains possible to offer several
the life span, it must be emphasized the front of the body at approxi- “best practice” suggestions regard-
the absolute magnitude of the errors mately chest height with the hands ing how clinically based assessments

1182 f Physical Therapy Volume 90 Number 8 August 2010


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on March 18, 2013
Factors Affecting Position Matching

of proprioceptive function should be Fifth, there appears to be an effect of 4 Xerri C, Merzenich MM, Peterson BE, Jen-
kins W. Plasticity of primary somatosen-
performed. task workspace on position match- sory cortex paralleling sensorimotor skill
ing (factor 5), such that matching recovery from stroke in adult monkeys.
J Neurophysiol. 1998;79:2119 –2148.
First, it has been demonstrated that movements are enhanced when
5 Schwenkreis P, Pleger B, Hoffken O, et al.
the choice of ipsilateral versus con- made to the far left of body midline. Repetitive training of a synchronised
tralateral matching task (factor 1) is Clinicians and researchers, there- movement induces short-term plastic
changes in the human primary somatosen-
not trivial, as these tasks have very fore, should perform tests of posi- sory cortex. Neurosci Lett. 2001;312:99 –
different requirements for memory tion matching ability within a com- 102.
and interhemispheric communica- mon workspace. 6 Carel C, Loubinoux I, Boulanouar K, et al.
Neural substrate for the effects of passive
tion, respectively. In this case, pru- training on sensorimotor cortical repre-
dent selection of matching task, Despite the many factors presented, sentation: a study with functional mag-
netic resonance imaging in healthy sub-
based on the cognitive abilities of the it was not the intent of this perspec- jects. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2000;20:
individual being tested, will reduce tive article to call into question, or 478 – 484.
cognition-based confounds and pro- advise against the use of, position 7 Goodwin GM, McCloskey DI, Matthews
PB. Proprioceptive illusions induced by
vide more accurate assessments of matching tasks in clinical research. muscle vibration: contribution by muscle
proprioceptive ability. Rather, it is hoped that the informa- spindles to perception? Science. 1972;175:
1382–1384.
tion provided will help enhance fu-
8 Burke D, Hagbarth KE, Lofstedt L, Wallin
Second, there appears to be a left- ture studies within the field of phys- BG. The responses of human muscle spin-
arm advantage for position matching ical therapy and allow for more dle endings to vibration of non-
contracting muscles. J Physiol. 1976;261:
(factor 2) that likely stems from an substantive breakthroughs to be 673– 693.
enhanced role for the right hemi- made. Indeed, it is more apparent 9 Roll JP, Vedel JP. Kinaesthetic role of mus-
sphere in proprioceptive feedback than ever that proprioceptive informa- cle afferents in man, studied by tendon
vibration and microneurography. Exp
processing. The implication of this tion is a key component of the plastic Brain Res. 1982;47:177–190.
finding for clinicians and researchers reorganization of central nervous sys- 10 Ferrell WR, Gandevia SC, McCloskey DI.
is that individuals with right- versus tem following injury.1,2 Adequate as- The role of joint receptors in human kin-
aesthesia when intramuscular receptors
left-hemisphere injury may be ex- sessment of proprioceptive acuity, cannot contribute. J Physiol. 1987;386:
pected to exhibit greater proprio- therefore, will be a critical indicator of 63–71.
ceptive deficits. treatment outcomes in future assess- 11 Edin BB. Cutaneous afferents provide in-
formation about knee joint movements in
ments of many disease conditions. humans. J Physiol. 2001;531(pt 1):289 –
Third, the position matching studies 297.
of the author and colleagues have The studies forming the basis for this article
12 Edin BB, Abbs JH. Finger movement re-
sponses of cutaneous mechanoreceptors
shown that the manner by which were conducted in the Motor Control Labo- in the dorsal skin of the human hand.
target positions are established (fac- ratory of Dr Susan Brown at the University of J Neurophysiol. 1991;65:657– 670.
tor 3) can have significant effects on Michigan. The author gives special thanks to 13 Rothwell JC, Traub MM, Day BL, et al.
Dr Brown and all of his former colleagues in Manual motor performance in a deaffer-
position matching performance. In ented man. Brain. 1982;105 (pt 3):515–
the Motor Control Laboratory for their con-
this case, clinician researchers are tributions to this work.
542.
advised to take care in ensuring that 14 Messier J, Adamovich S, Berkinblit M, et al.
Funding for the author was provided by fel- Influence of movement speed on accuracy
tested target amplitudes are of the and coordination of reaching movements
lowships from the Flemish Fund for Scientific
same magnitude and established in Research and the Canadian Institutes of
to memorized targets in three-dimensional
space in a deafferented subject. Exp Brain
the same manner (active versus pas- Health Research: Institute of Aging. Res. 2003;150:399 – 416.
sive) and at the same speed across 15 Sainburg RL, Ghilardi MF, Poizner H, Ghez
This article was submitted December 3, 2009,
all participants and experimental and was accepted April 21, 2010.
C. Control of limb dynamics in normal sub-
jects and patients without proprioception.
conditions. J Neurophysiol. 1995;73:820 – 835.
DOI: 10.2522/ptj.20090399
16 Sainburg RL, Poizner H, Ghez C. Loss of
Fourth, clinicians and researchers proprioception produces deficits in inter-
joint coordination. J Neurophysiol. 1993;
must be sure to account for natural References 70:2136 –2147.
shifts in proprioceptive acuity with 1 Nudo RJ. Postinfarct cortical plasticity and 17 Carey LM, Oke LE, Matyas TA. Impaired
age (factor 4), as clinical populations behavioral recovery. Stroke. 2007;38(2 limb position sense after stroke: a quanti-
suppl):840 – 845. tative test for clinical use. Arch Phys Med
often fall nearer to one end of the 2 Nudo RJ. Plasticity. NeuroRx. 2006;3:420 – Rehabil. 1996;77:1271–1278.
spectrum compared with other pop- 427. 18 Zia S, Cody FW, O’Boyle DJ. Identification
ulations (eg, people with stroke, 3 Xerri C. Post-lesional plasticity of somato- of unilateral elbow-joint position is im-
sensory cortex maps: a review [in French]. paired by Parkinson’s disease. Clin Anat.
older people). C R Acad Sci III. 1998;321:135–151. 2002;15:23–31.

August 2010 Volume 90 Number 8 Physical Therapy f 1183


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on March 18, 2013
Factors Affecting Position Matching

19 Zia S, Cody FW, O’Boyle DJ. Joint position 34 Juul-Kristensen B, Lund H, Hansen K, et al. 51 Roy EA, MacKenzie C. Handedness effects
sense is impaired by Parkinson’s disease. Test-retest reliability of joint position and in kinesthetic spatial location judgements.
Ann Neurol. 2000;47:218 –228. kinesthetic sense in the elbow of healthy Cortex. 1978;14:250 –258.
subjects. Physiother Theory Pract. 2008;
20 Leo KC, Soderberg GL. Relationship be- 52 Colley A. Spatial location judgements by
24:65–72.
tween perception of joint position sense right and left-handers. Cortex. 1984;20:
and limb synergies in patients with hemi- 35 Deshpande N, Connelly DM, Culham EG, 47–53.
plegia. Phys Ther. 1981;61:1433–1437. Costigan PA. Reliability and validity of an- 53 Nishizawa S. Different pattern of hemi-
kle proprioceptive measures. Arch Phys
21 Westlake KP, Wu Y, Culham EG. Sensory- sphere specialization between identical kin-
Med Rehabil. 2003;84:883– 889.
specific balance training in older adults: esthetic spatial and weight discrimination
effect on position, movement, and veloc- 36 Goble DJ, Hurvitz EA, Brown SH. Deficits tasks. Neuropsychologia. 1991;29:305–312.
ity sense at the ankle. Phys Ther. 2007;87: in the ability to use proprioceptive feed- 54 Nishizawa S, Saslow CA. Lateralization of
560 –568. back in children with hemiplegic cerebral kinesthetically guided spatial perception.
palsy. Int J Rehabil Res. 2009;32:267–269.
22 Kaufman LB, Schilling DL. Implementation Cortex. 1987;23:485– 494.
of a strength training program for a 5-year- 37 Goble DJ, Brown SH. Task-dependent 55 Goble DJ, Brown SH. Upper limb as-
old child with poor body awareness and asymmetries in the utilization of proprio- symetries in the matching of propriocep-
developmental coordination disorder. ceptive feedback for goal-directed move- tive versus visual targets [author reply].
Phys Ther. 2007;87:455– 467. ment. Exp Brain Res. 2007;180:693–704. J Neurophysiol. 2008;100:3459.
23 Rombaut L, De Paepe A, Malfait F, et al. 38 Goble DJ, Lewis CA, Brown SH. Upper 56 Goble DJ, Brown SH. The biological and
Joint position sense and vibratory percep- limb asymmetries in the utilization of pro- behavioral basis of upper limb asymme-
tion sense in patients with Ehlers-Danlos prioceptive feedback. Exp Brain Res. tries in sensorimotor performance. Neuro-
syndrome type III (hypermobility type). 2006;168:307–311. sci Biobehav Rev. 2008;32:598 – 610.
Clin Rheumatol. 2009 November 25 39 Goble DJ, Lewis CA, Hurvitz EA, Brown
[Epub ahead of print]. 57 Naito E, Roland PE, Grefkes C et al. Dom-
SH. Development of upper limb proprio- inance of the right hemisphere and role of
24 Hattori Y, Doi K, Sakamoto S, et al. Elbow ceptive accuracy in children and adoles- area 2 in human kinesthesia. J Neuro-
joint position sense following brachial cents. Hum Mov Sci. 2005;24:155–170. physiol. 2005;93:1020 –1034.
plexus palsy treated with double free mus- 40 Goble DJ, Brown SH. Upper limb asymme-
cle transfer. J Hand Surg Am. 2009;34: 58 Harris CM, Wolpert DM. Signal-dependent
tries in the matching of proprioceptive
1667–1673. noise determines motor planning. Nature.
versus visual targets. J Neurophysiol. 1998;394:780 –784.
25 Felson DT, Gross KD, Nevitt MC, et al. The 2008;99:3063–3074.
effects of impaired joint position sense on 59 Paillard J, Brouchon M. Active and passive
41 Goble DJ, Brown SH. Dynamic proprio-
the development and progression of pain movement in the calibration of position
ceptive target matching behavior in the
and structural damage in knee osteoarthri- sense. In: Freedman SJ, ed. The Neuro-
upper limb: effects of speed, task difficulty
tis. Arthritis Rheum. 2009;61:1070 –1076. physiology of Spatially Oriented Behav-
and arm/hemisphere asymmetries. Behav ior. Homewood, IL: Dorsey Press; 1968:
26 Wingert JR, Burton H, Sinclair RJ, et al. Brain Res. 2009;8;200:7–14. 37–55.
Joint-position sense and kinesthesia in ce- 42 Goble DJ, Noble BC, Brown SH. Proprio-
rebral palsy. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 60 Laufer Y, Hocherman S, Dickstein R. Accu-
ceptive target matching asymmetries in
2009;90:447– 453. racy of reproducing hand position when us-
left-handed individuals. Exp Brain Res. ing active compared with passive move-
27 Conti SF, Dazen D, Stewart G, et al. Pro- 2009;197:403– 408. ment. Physiother Res Int. 2001;6:65–75.
prioception after total ankle arthroplasty. 43 Kaas JH. The functional organization of
Foot Ankle Int. 2008;29:1069 –1073. 61 Stelmach GE, Kelso JA, Wallace SA. Prese-
somatosensory cortex in primates. Ann lection in short-term motor memory. J Exp
28 Lee AJ, Lin WH. Twelve-week biomechani- Anat. 1993;175:509 –518. Psychol Hum Learn. 1975;1:745–755.
cal ankle platform system training on pos- 44 Naito E, Nakashima T, Kito T, et al. Human
tural stability and ankle proprioception in 62 Granit R. Constant errors in the execution
limb-specific and non–limb-specific brain
subjects with unilateral functional ankle and appreciation of movement. Brain.
representations during kinesthetic illusory
instability. Clin Biomech (Bristol, Avon). 1972;95:451– 460.
movements of the upper and lower ex-
2008;23:1065–1072. tremities. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;25:3476 – 63 Kawato M, Wolpert D. Internal models for
29 Yang JL, Chen S, Jan MH, et al. Proprio- 3487. motor control. Novartis Found Symp.
ception assessment in subjects with idio- 1998;218:291–304.
45 Sperry R, Gazzaniga MS, Bogen JE. Inter-
pathic loss of shoulder range of motion: hemispheric relationships: the neocortical 64 Wolpert DM, Ghahramani Z. Computational
joint position sense and a novel proprio- commissures; syndromes of hemisphere principles of movement neuroscience. Nat
ceptive feedback index. J Orthop Res. disconnection. In: Handbook of Clinical Neurosci. 2000;3(suppl):1212–1217.
2008;26:1218 –1224. Neurology. Amsterdam, NY: North- 65 Helsen H. The Tau effect: an example of
30 Mir SM, Hadian MR, Talebian S, Nasseri N. Holland Publishing Co; 1969:273–290. psychological relativity. Science. 1930;71:
Functional assessment of knee joint posi- 46 Hellige JB. Cerebral Hemisphere Asymme- 536 –537.
tion sense following anterior cruciate lig- try: Method, Theory, and Application.
ament reconstruction. Br J Sports Med. 66 Helsen H, King SM. The Tau effect: an
New York, NY: Praeger Publishers; 1983.
2008;42:300 –303. example of psychological relativity. J Exp
47 Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis Psychol. 1931;14:202–217.
31 Sullivan JA, Hoffman MA, Harter RA. Shoul- of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory.
der joint position sense after thermal, 67 Goble DJ, Brown SH. Upper limb asymme-
Neuropsychologia. 1971;9:97–113.
open, and arthroscopic capsulorrhaphy tries in the perception of proprioceptively-
for recurrent anterior instability. J Shoul- 48 Gilbert AN, Wysocki CJ. Hand preference determined dynamic position sense. J Exp
der Elbow Surg. 2008;17:389 –394. and age in the United States. Neuropsycho- Psychol Hum Percept Perform. In press.
logia. 1992;30:601– 608.
32 Juul-Kristensen B, Lund H, Hansen K, et al. 68 Goble DJ, Coxon JP, Wenderoth N, et al.
Poorer elbow proprioception in patients 49 Vuoksimaa E, Koskenvuo M, Rose RJ, Proprioceptive sensibility in the elderly:
with lateral epicondylitis than in healthy Kaprio J. Origins of handedness: a nation- degeneration, functional consequences
controls: a cross-sectional study. J Shoulder wide study of 30,161 adults. Neuropsycho- and plastic-adaptive processes. Neurosci
Elbow Surg. 2008;17(1 suppl):72S– 81S. logia. 2009;47:1294 –1301. Biobehav Rev. 2009;33:271–278.
33 Nakasa T, Fukuhara K, Adachi N, Ochi M. 50 Coren S, Porac C. Fifty centuries of right- 69 Haggard P, Newman C, Blundell J, Andrew
The deficit of joint position sense in the handedness: the historical record. Science. H. The perceived position of the hand in
chronic unstable ankle as measured by in- 1977;198:631– 632. space. Percept Psychophys. 2000;62:
version angle replication error. Arch Or- 363–377.
thop Trauma Surg. 2008;128:445– 449.

1184 f Physical Therapy Volume 90 Number 8 August 2010


Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on March 18, 2013
Proprioceptive Acuity Assessment Via Joint Position
Matching: From Basic Science to General Practice
Daniel J. Goble
PHYS THER. 2010; 90:1176-1184.
Originally published online June 3, 2010
doi: 10.2522/ptj.20090399

References This article cites 64 articles, 20 of which you can access


for free at:
http://ptjournal.apta.org/content/90/8/1176#BIBL
Cited by This article has been cited by 1 HighWire-hosted articles:

http://ptjournal.apta.org/content/90/8/1176#otherarticles
Subscription http://ptjournal.apta.org/subscriptions/
Information
Permissions and Reprints http://ptjournal.apta.org/site/misc/terms.xhtml
Information for Authors http://ptjournal.apta.org/site/misc/ifora.xhtml

Downloaded from http://ptjournal.apta.org/ by guest on March 18, 2013

You might also like