You are on page 1of 20

Persuasion Knowledge in the Marketplace: A Meta-Analysis

Martin Eisend , and Farid Tarrahi


European University Viadrina
Accepted by Anirban Mukhopadhyay and Lauren Block, Editors; Associate Editor, Derek Rucker

Since the introduction of the persuasion knowledge model more than 25 years ago, many research studies
have investigated how consumers’ persuasion knowledge affects their reactions to persuasion attempts. While
most results have shown that persuasion knowledge increases coping responses and leads to less favorable
evaluations of marketer actions, the findings vary considerably, leaving researchers with a limited understand-
ing of the substance and structure of persuasion knowledge effects and the conditions that explain their vari-
ability. To develop a better understanding of persuasion knowledge effects in the marketplace, this study
builds on the concept of persuasion to predict responses to marketers’ attempts to persuade consumers with
different levels of persuasion knowledge. The study presents a meta-analysis of the findings in 148 papers
and 171 distinct data sets. Persuasion knowledge effects can be viewed as substantial compared with persua-
sion attempts, but persuasion knowledge cannot suppress or eliminate persuasion effects in the marketplace,
as it only reaches around 50% of the explanatory power of persuasion. Persuasion knowledge effects on evalu-
ations and coping depend on the characteristics of the persuasion process. All persuasion elements that help
consumers identify and better understand benefits not just for themselves, but also for marketers and how
marketers realize their benefits—such as the use of personal communication, communication about unfamiliar
products or products with experience attributes, and receiver experience—lead to less favorable effects for
marketers. This paper’s insights provide a new framework for persuasion knowledge effects in the market-
place, ideas for future research, and implications for researchers, consumers, policymakers, and marketers.
Keywords Persuasion knowledge; Persuasion; Consumer; Meta-analysis

Since the introduction of the persuasion knowledge limited understanding of persuasion knowledge
model more than 25 years ago (Friestad & Wright, effects’ substance and structure, as well as the con-
1994), plenty of research studies have investigated ditions that explain their variability.
how consumers develop persuasion knowledge As for persuasion knowledge effects’ substance
(e.g., Nelson, 2016; Wright, Friestad, & Boush, and structure, research provides the general insight
2005), when and how consumers use their persua- that persuasion knowledge increases consumers’
sion knowledge (e.g., Boerman, Van Reijmersdaal, coping responses and leads to less favorable evalua-
& Neijens, 2012; Campbell & Kirmani, 2000; Fries- tions (Campbell & Kirmani, 2008), but it does not
tad & Wright, 1995), and how their persuasion tell us how strong these effects are compared with
knowledge affects their reactions to persuasion the effects from persuasion in the marketplace: Are
attempts (e.g., Isaac & Grayson, 2017; Panic, Cau- persuasion knowledge effects strong enough to
berghe, & Pelsmacker, 2013). Insights about persua- reduce or even eliminate the influence from mar-
sion knowledge effects are of interest to researchers, keters’ persuasion attempts? Unfortunately, prior
marketers, consumers, and public policymakers research provides mixed findings on the direction,
because they carry major implications for marketing strength, and hierarchy of persuasion knowledge
actions’ effectiveness and consumers’ susceptibility effects on different consumer responses. For exam-
to these actions. However, most findings concern- ple, the direction of persuasion knowledge effects
ing persuasion knowledge effects vary considerably, on memory remains unknown (Cowley & Barron,
leaving both researchers and practitioners with a 2008; Matthes, Schemer, & Wirth, 2007). Prior
research also has found considerable variation in
Received 24 September 2019; accepted 6 June 2021 persuasion knowledge effects and indicates that
Available online 09 June 2021 under certain circumstances, persuasion knowledge
This research received financial support from a research grant
by the German Research Foundation (DFG: EI-508/10-1).
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Martin Eisend, Professor of Marketing, European University Via- © 2021 Society for Consumer Psychology
drina, Grosse Scharrnstr. 59, Frankfurt 15230, Germany. Elec- All rights reserved. 1057-7408/2021/1532-7663
tronic mail may be sent to eisend@europa-uni.de DOI: 10.1002/jcpy.1258
2 Eisend and Tarrahi

can even lead to positive evaluations of persuasion knowledge lies the concept of persuasion, and
agents (Isaac & Grayson, 2017; Kirmani & Camp- defining persuasion knowledge necessitates under-
bell, 2004). How can the variation in persuasion standing what persuasion entails. Despite a multi-
knowledge effects on either coping or evaluation be tude of definitions, conceptualizations, and
explained, and under which conditions does per- persuasion models, the common idea of persuasion
suasion knowledge lead to favorable or unfavorable is that it is an intentional effort through communi-
results for marketers and consumers? cation to influence a receiver who has some degree
In this study, we refer to the concept of persua- of freedom of choice (e.g., O’Keefe, 2002; Perloff,
sion to systematize consumer responses triggered 2017). This view is broader than equating persua-
by persuasion knowledge and to identify relevant sion with attitude change, which is a common
moderators of persuasion knowledge effects. In approach by attitude researchers and focuses on a
doing so, we answered the above questions, which smaller set of consumer response variables than the
are important for a better and more comprehensive present meta-analysis (Bohner & Dickel, 2011).
understanding of the concept of persuasion knowl- Persuasion in the marketplace benefits not only
edge and its effects and, thus, its role and impact in the receiver, but also the sender and influencer,
the marketplace—insights that provide implications which is a common assumption in persuasion mod-
concerning persuasion knowledge for future els, with the persuasion knowledge model suggest-
research, as well as for consumers, public policy- ing that the influencer harbors ulterior and self-
makers, and marketers. To this end, we meta- serving motives (Campbell & Kirmani, 2008; Fries-
analyzed data provided in 148 prior research tad & Wright, 1994). With increasing persuasion
papers with 171 distinct data sets. Specifically, we knowledge, receivers’ ability to recognize, reflect
compared persuasion knowledge effects’ strength to upon, understand, and evaluate influencers’ inten-
the strength of persuasion effects in the marketplace tions and efforts increases. Receivers with more per-
and decided to what extent persuasion knowledge suasion knowledge can understand not only
effects can reduce or even eliminate persuasion benefits for themselves, but also benefits for influ-
effects. We further resolved the ambiguities of encers and how influencers pursue their benefits.
effects in prior research and identified the direction, They compare these benefits, which influence per-
strength, and structure of the effects related to cop- suasion attempts’ outcomes. The definition of per-
ing responses, evaluations, intentions and behavior, suasion indicates that knowledge of persuasion can
and memory and learning. Finally, we demon- refer to and affect outcomes that are either intended
strated that persuasion elements that help con- or unintended by the influencer.
sumers identify and better understand persuasion Influencers’ intended outcomes in a marketing
attempts’ benefits, not just for themselves, but also context refer to brand-, channel-, and company-
for marketers and how marketers realize their bene- related evaluations, intentions, behavior, and mem-
fits—such as the use of personal communication, ories of consumers. If consumers gain insights into
messages about unfamiliar products or products persuasion that benefit not only the consumer, but
with experience and credence attributes, and recei- also the marketer, and if they see that marketers
vers’ experience—lead to less favorable effects for use potentially manipulative techniques, and might
marketers. These findings inform marketers about even intend to reduce consumers’ choices (e.g.,
conditions that jeopardize their investments’ effec- through building brand loyalty), they develop cop-
tiveness, and they inform consumers about condi- ing responses, as suggested by reactance theory
tions when they are more susceptible to marketers’ (Brehm, 1966). These coping responses are out-
persuasion attempts. They further provide a basis comes that are not intended by the marketer. The
for future research ideas on persuasion knowledge. conceptual framework in Figure 1 depicts both
types of outcomes, which are described and
explained in more in Table 1.
Theoretical Background Persuasion knowledge effects on outcome vari-
ables vary. Prior research has shown that increasing
Persuasion and Persuasion Knowledge
persuasion knowledge not only makes consumers
Persuasion knowledge refers to consumers’ more critical of persuasion and develops reactance,
knowledge and beliefs regarding marketers’ persua- but also may improve consumers’ skills in identify-
sion goals and attempts, as well as their underlying ing benefits for themselves from marketers’ persua-
motives and tactics, and how persuasion works sion attempts. Thus, they might even develop more
(Friestad & Wright, 1994). At the core of persuasion favorable evaluations with increasing persuasion
Persuasion Knowledge Meta-Analysis 3

Figure 1. Research on persuasion knowledge: overview and conceptual framework.

knowledge (Isaac & Grayson, 2017). Whether and that influence persuasion knowledge development
how consumers activate and apply their persuasion or on contextual triggers that make marketers’ ulte-
knowledge and how it affects their reactions rior motives accessible, such as sponsorship disclo-
depend on the cues that persuasion attempts pro- sures. Individual factors refer to variations in
vide to consumers. Because persuasion is a commu- expertise and cognitive resources, and as a variable
nication phenomenon, we can systematize the cues related to both factors, age is the most-often-
in line with the major elements of the communica- investigated individual variable. The findings indi-
tion or persuasion process (Lasswell, 1948): source; cate that persuasion knowledge starts developing in
message; channel; and receiver. For instance, if a childhood, starting at age 5; increases with age; and
source lacks transparency, consumers with more reaches a saturation level at senior age (Nelson,
persuasion knowledge are more likely to assume a 2016; Rozendaal, Buijzen, & Valkenburg, 2011; Van
hidden and ulterior motive that reduces the percep- Reijmersdaal, Rozendaal, & Buijzen, 2012). Contex-
tion of their benefits compared with the marketer’s tual triggers also can induce cognitive resources’
benefits, leading to more source-unfavorable out- availability (e.g., by distracting consumers; Camp-
comes than with a transparent source. Figure 1 bell & Kirmani, 2000) and ulterior motives’ accessi-
depicts persuasion process moderators besides bility. While many different triggers exist that
methodological and other moderators that we con- provide information about marketers’ motives, such
sider in our meta-analysis. Note that the different as information about a firm’s business status or tac-
types of knowledge, such as agent or topic knowl- tics (e.g., Brown & Krishna, 2004; Campbell & Kir-
edge, that have been suggested by Friestadt and mani, 2000; Morales, 2005), sponsorship disclosures
Wright (1994) correspond to communication ele- that are relevant from both a managerial and public
ments, but do not include channel or receiver char- policy perspective have been investigated most
acteristics as potential moderators of persuasion intensively (e.g., Boerman & Van Reijmersdal, 2016;
knowledge effects. Eisend, Reijmersdal, Boerman, & Tarrahi, 2020).
To provide a complete picture of persuasion Sponsorship disclosures can activate and increase
knowledge research, Figure 1 further depicts the consumers’ persuasion knowledge. Figure 1 pro-
main antecedents of persuasion knowledge as vides an overview of prior persuasion knowledge
investigated in prior research, although they are not research and depicts the conceptual framework for
empirically addressed in this meta-analysis. Most this meta-analysis that pertains to research in the
research has focused either on individual factors gray area.
4 Eisend and Tarrahi

Table 1
Persuasion Knowledge Effects on Consumer Response Variables

Expected
Consumer response variable Definition effect

Marketer intended effects


Evaluations
Attitude toward the ad Evaluation of the ad and the advertised message Negative
Attitude toward the brand Evaluation of the sponsored brand
Company evaluation Evaluation of the sponsoring company and its representatives
Media evaluation Evaluation of the media or platform that transmits the sponsored content
Satisfaction Evaluation of purchasing outcomes compared with expectations
Intentions and behavior
Behavioral intention Consumers’ likelihood of engaging in brand-favoring behavior or in the behavior Negative
advised in the ad message
Choice and behavior Consumers’ brand-favoring choice and behavior or their actual choice and behavior in
line with advice given in the ad message
Memory and learning
Brand recall Consumers’ ability to retrieve a brand (name) from memory correctly when prompted ?
by a product category
Brand recognition Consumers’ ability to correct their discernment of a brand as having been seen or
heard before
Learning Consumers’ elaboration and knowledge acquisition
Other positive responses
Credibility The extent to which the source or message can be believed or trusted Negative
Positive cognitions Cognitive responses of positive valence
Positive feelings Emotional reactions of positive valence
Marketer unintended effects
Negative coping responses
Avoidance Actions taken to avoid and reduce persuasive influences Positive
Disapproval Disapproving evaluations and views of persuasion attempts
Negative cognitions Cognitive responses of negative valence
Negative feelings Emotional reactions of negative valence
Suspicion Consumers’ critical stance toward and processing of persuasion attempts and sources,
expressed by suspicion and skepticism
Other
Price estimate Estimate of consumers’ accepted price and/or sellers’ price advantage ?
Third-person perceptions Perceived influence on self (compared with others)

and managing persuasion attempts, including cog-


The Substance and Structure of Persuasion Knowledge
nitions, feelings, suspicions, or avoidance. Evalua-
Effects
tions relate to the assessment of a persuasion
Persuasion knowledge leads to outcomes object’s value, merit, or benefit, including variables
intended by the influencer (herein, marketer- such as brand attitudes, ad attitudes, or company
intended outcomes) that are investigated in most evaluations. Table 1 depicts the outcomes that have
marketing communication and persuasion studies, been investigated commonly in prior persuasion
as well as unintended outcomes, which are usually knowledge research and, thus, are selected for and
not investigated in this research stream. Similarly, included in the meta-analysis. While some authors
Campbell and Kirmani (2008) distinguished have included credibility, trustworthiness, or dislike
between coping responses that occur during a per- measures as indicators of persuasion knowledge
suasion episode and terminal outcomes related to activation or as evaluative dimensions of persua-
evaluations, intentions, and behaviors. Coping sion knowledge (e.g., Boerman et al., 2012; Rozen-
responses comprise all cognitive, emotional, or daal, Slot, Van Reijmersdal, & Buijzen, 2013), most
behavioral efforts by consumers in dealing with prior studies have emphasized the cognitive view
Persuasion Knowledge Meta-Analysis 5

suggested by Friestadt and Wright’s (1994) defini- optimize their efforts to channel these responses in
tion and have viewed evaluative constructs and meaningful ways, we do not know whether the
coping responses as persuasion knowledge out- responses to persuasion knowledge are primarily
comes (Ham, Nelson, & Das, 2015) besides learning emotional, cognitive, or behavioral. Regarding this,
and memory and intentions and behavior. In this we do not know whether persuasion knowledge
study, we follow the majority view of empirical might sometimes lead to positive emotions, as it
studies. might be possible that people feel elation after suc-
The outcomes in Table 1 are organized as evalu- cessfully activating persuasion knowledge (Camp-
ations, intentions and behavior, memory and learn- bell & Kirmani, 2008).
ing, other positive responses, negative coping Second, persuasion knowledge mostly leads to
responses, and other variables, following the classi- negative evaluations, but it is unclear whether it
fications from other meta-analyses dealing with per- affects all evaluation objects (e.g., company, brand,
suasion effects (e.g., Eisend & Tarrahi, 2016). ads) in the same way. Conceptually, persuasion
Although the outcomes are assumed to be related knowledge includes knowledge related to the per-
(e.g., strongly negative coping responses lead to less suasion process, including source and message ele-
positive evaluations), the present meta-analysis— ments; thus, the evaluation can affect various
similar to other meta-analyses—investigates persua- evaluation objects (Friestad & Wright, 1994). An
sion knowledge’s effects on each of the outcomes intriguing question is whether consumers who acti-
separately and independently. vate persuasion knowledge evaluate the source as
Considering that marketers are assumed to have the party responsible for the persuasion attempt
self-serving ulterior motives that they often hide–– more negatively than other communication objects.
and persuasion knowledge increases the likelihood The answer can provide insights to help marketers
that consumers not only understand the benefits for respond better to negative consumer responses due
themselves, but also recognize and understand mar- to persuasion knowledge.
keters’ motives––persuasion knowledge increases Third, researchers have begun to ask whether
the likelihood of negative coping responses to allow persuasion knowledge can exert positive effects
consumers to control unwanted persuasive influ- (e.g., Campbell & Kirmani, 2008; Isaac & Grayson,
ences, leading to negative evaluations, intentions, 2017). An interesting outcome in this regard is con-
and behaviors (e.g., Fransen, Verlegh, Kirmani, & sumers’ memory and learning persuasion-related
Smit, 2015). While these effects have been well cues, which have led to ambiguous findings in
established in the literature, we do not know how prior research (e.g., Cowley & Barron, 2008;
these effects are manifested compared with Matthes et al., 2007). Similar to other responses,
marketer-intended and successful persuasion any brand-favoring outcomes should be reduced;
effects, such as positive brand attitudes. In particu- thus, persuasion knowledge should affect memory
lar, we do not know to what extent persuasion and learning negatively. However, knowledge and
knowledge can reduce or even eliminate marketers’ memory are not necessarily correlated (DeMarie-
intended persuasive influence. If persuasion knowl- Dreblow, 1991), which also could imply a nonexis-
edge, indeed, can reduce persuasion effects by a tent relationship between persuasion knowledge
considerable amount or even eliminate them, then and memory and learning.
persuasion knowledge effects will be viewed as
substantial and important.
Explaining Variations in Persuasion Knowledge Effects
Aside from this major question regarding the
substance of persuasion knowledge effects, several Whether and how consumers apply their persua-
other open questions about the hierarchy and direc- sion knowledge and how it affects their responses
tion of responses triggered by persuasion knowl- depend on cues that the persuasion attempt and sit-
edge can be answered through our meta-analysis. uation provide to consumers, who, based on these
First, although the persuasion knowledge model is cues, can identify the persuasion attempt’s benefits
about knowledge, rather than emotions (Friestad & for themselves and for the marketers, compare and
Wright, 1994), the coping responses can refer to interpret these benefits, then react accordingly. The
consumers’ cognitive, emotional, or behavioral general idea is that with little or no persuasion
efforts in dealing with and managing persuasion knowledge, consumers mainly will identify benefits
attempts. Although it is important for marketers, for themselves, as they are salient and easy to pro-
consumers, and public policymakers to understand cess and understand in the context of a persuasion
what kind of responses they are dealing with to attempt. For instance, most brand ads clearly
6 Eisend and Tarrahi

communicate brands’ main benefits to consumers source and its intentions, and the commercial
(e.g., a comfortable car, a healthy drink) (Bruce, source cannot be identified easily (Campbell, Mohr,
Becker, & Reinartz, 2020). Even if benefits are sub- & Verlegh, 2013; Wei, Fischer, & Main, 2008). With
jective, persuasion in the marketplace commonly less persuasion knowledge, consumers are less
provides some benefits for the consumer, leading to likely to recognize covert marketing sources’ ulte-
positive evaluations by the consumer. However, rior motives. Instead, consumers focus on the bene-
with increasing persuasion knowledge, consumers fits for themselves provided in the persuasive
can see beyond the persuasion attempt’s salient ele- message, while with more persuasion knowledge,
ments and recognize, process, and interpret other consumers can identify the source, its intentions,
information about the persuasion process, mar- and applied tactics. Consumers can recognize mar-
keters, and the persuasion context to reveal infor- keters’ benefits and put their own benefits into per-
mation about the benefits for the marketer and how spective, thereby developing more coping responses
marketers try to achieve these benefits through, for and less favorable evaluations.
instance, manipulative tactics (Friestad & Wright, Note that our hypotheses are tested through a
1994). By better understanding the benefits for both meta-analysis by assessing effect sizes. A stronger
consumers and marketers, consumers can compare effect indicates a larger absolute value of an effect
their benefits to marketers’ benefits and put their size, thereby indicating more positive effects on
benefits into perspective, which can lead to more negative coping, but more negative effects on eval-
coping and less positive evaluations. By under- uations, and vice versa for weaker effects that are
standing that marketers tend to hide their inten- assessed by smaller effect sizes:
tions or sometimes use manipulative tactics,
consumers take a more critical stance. H1: The effect of persuasion knowledge on evalua-
Specific cues can help consumers with more per- tions and coping is stronger for covert sources
suasion knowledge better understand the benefits vs. noncovert sources.
for both themselves and marketers, as well as how
marketers try to realize their benefits. We system- Similar arguments have referred to personalized
atize the cues in line with major elements of the persuasive communication in the marketplace. With
communication or persuasion process (e.g., Lass- increasing persuasion knowledge, consumers are
well, 1948): source; message; channel; and receiver. more likely to identify the marketer’s aims and bene-
The persuasion process elements are moderators of fits, as well as the underlying tactics in using perso-
persuasion knowledge effects. Next to these persua- nalized communication over nonpersonalized
sion process elements, persuasion knowledge mea- communication. Understanding marketers’ applica-
sures are also important moderators, as several tion of personalized communication can increase
items from these measures are related to the per- privacy concerns and reactance, which can lead to
ception of marketers’ persuasive intentions (Wil- negative effects from personalized marketing commu-
liams, Fitzsimons, & Block, 2004). Note that the nication (Aguirre, Mahr, Grewal, Ruyter, & Wetzels,
variables that we ultimately have used to test per- 2015; Bleier & Eisenbeiss, 2015). As a result, greater
suasion process elements’ moderating effects also persuasion knowledge leads to more coping and less
are driven by methodological considerations and favorable evaluations when personalization is high.
the availability of information provided in the pri-
mary studies included in the meta-analysis. As is
common with a meta-analysis, all hypotheses are H2: The effect of persuasion knowledge on evalua-
predictions using an “all else equal” approach and tions and coping is stronger for personal
cannot consider conditional effects and moderators sources vs. nonpersonal sources.
that are not provided in the primary studies
included in the meta-analysis.
Message
The marketing communication message is cre-
Source
ated around a product. Messages for low-
A source characteristic that is likely to moderate involvement products apply basic persuasion tech-
persuasion knowledge effects is source trans- niques, such as the application of sex appeals or
parency. Covert marketing (e.g., product place- humor; provide basic information about benefits for
ments in movies, blogger sponsoring, and native consumers; and lead to shallower processing (Rossi-
advertising) is not transparent concerning the ter, Percy, & Donovan, 1991). While consumers
Persuasion Knowledge Meta-Analysis 7

with less persuasion knowledge mainly will focus H5: The effect of persuasion knowledge on evalua-
on salient benefits for themselves, consumers with tions and coping is stronger for services vs. goods.
greater persuasion knowledge are more likely to
identify marketers’ ulterior motives and see beyond H6: The effect of persuasion knowledge on evalua-
basic persuasion techniques applied in messages for tions and coping is stronger for products with
low-involvement products. Messages for high- experience or credence attributes vs. products
involvement products provide more detailed infor- with search attributes.
mation and are processed more intensively. Thus,
persuasion knowledge effects should be weaker, as
Channel
all consumers are more likely to identify both bene-
fits for themselves and benefits for marketers in Consumers also make judgments about persua-
messages for high-involvement products that are sive communication and media types based on their
processed more thoroughly. persuasion knowledge; they show more skepticism
toward new media channels and perceive them as
H3: The effect of persuasion knowledge on evalua- less credible compared with traditional ones (Moore
tions and coping is stronger for low-involvement & Rodgers, 2005). Persuasive communication in
products vs. high-involvement products. digital media, such as digital advertising, allows for
many variations, is less regulated than traditional
Similar arguments refer to unfamiliar products in advertising, and is, therefore, more likely to lead to
which consumers with less persuasion knowledge lack more suspicion and negative evaluations, such as
knowledge and information, and focus on the benefits the perception of privacy intrusion (Tucker, 2014).
for themselves, while consumers with more persuasion With increasing persuasion knowledge, consumers
knowledge are better able to identify marketers’ ulter- should be more aware of the differences between
ior motives concerning unfamiliar products’ persua- the use of online and offline channels in persuasion
sion attempts. As a result, they dismiss questionable or attempts; therefore, the difference in responses
exaggerated information from unfamiliar products between both channels should increase.
(Lee, 2014). In contrast, familiar products often are
associated with high credibility regarding both the H7: The effect of persuasion knowledge on evalua-
brand and the message (Hann & Berkey, 2002) and, tions and coping is stronger for online chan-
thus, are less likely to trigger suspicion and ulterior nels vs. offline channels.
motives among consumers, which should lead to an
attenuation of persuasion knowledge effects: Receiver
H4: The effect of persuasion knowledge on evalua- Persuasion knowledge gradually develops
tions and coping is stronger for unfamiliar pro- throughout life, evolving from simple to more
ducts vs. familiar products. sophisticated knowledge and beliefs about persua-
sion (Friestad & Wright, 1994; Wright et al., 2005),
Similar to messages for unfamiliar products, mes- that is, children and adolescents’ persuasion knowl-
sages about intangible products, such as services edge is less developed than that of adults (Hudders
(compared with goods) or products with primarily et al., 2017). Therefore, children and adolescents are
experience and credence attributes (e.g., organic less likely to understand marketers’ ulterior motives
fruits), rather than search attributes, are more difficult and manipulative actions, as their reactions are
to evaluate (Weathers, Sharma, & Wood, 2007). Con- weaker, they show fewer coping responses, and
sumers with less persuasion knowledge lack knowl- their evaluations are more favorable:
edge and information, and focus on the benefits for
themselves that are salient in the message, while con- H8: The effect of persuasion knowledge on evalua-
sumers with more persuasion knowledge are more tions and coping is stronger for adult samples
likely to identify marketers’ ulterior motives and vs. samples with children and/or adolescents.
manipulative efforts with persuasion attempts. Pro-
ducts with search attributes, as well as goods and Beyond the gradual development of persuasion
their benefits for both consumers and marketers, are knowledge throughout life, persuasion knowledge
easier to evaluate and, thus, the difference in persua- can increase significantly through education (Nel-
sion knowledge effects should be lower. son, 2016; Wright, 2002). The more educated
8 Eisend and Tarrahi

someone is, the higher their media and advertising situational measures (Briñol, Rucker, & Petty, 2015;
literacy and, thus, the higher their persuasion Ham et al., 2015). Chronic measures include knowl-
knowledge. Thus, in our meta-analysis, we distin- edge of persuasion tactics (Boush, Friestad, & Rose,
guished between studies with student samples and 1994), laypeople’s persuasion knowledge (Friestad
nonstudent samples. Student samples comprised & Wright, 1995), self-confidence in relation to per-
better-educated individuals than nonstudent sam- suasion knowledge (Bearden, Hardesty, & Rose,
ples. Therefore, they were more likely to have more 2001), and pricing tactic persuasion knowledge
persuasion knowledge, which leads to more nega- (Hardesty, Bearden, & Carlson, 2007). Situational
tive coping responses and evaluations. measures include ratings of persuasion-related
beliefs (Campbell & Kirmani, 2008), encompassing
H9: The effect of persuasion knowledge on evalua- scales that measure the inference of manipulative
tions and coping is stronger for samples with content (e.g., Campbell, 1995) or the understanding
students vs. nonstudent samples. of persuasive intent and motives (e.g., Tutai & Van
Reijmersdal, 2012). A chronic measure indicates gen-
The development of persuasion knowledge also eral persuasion knowledge, but does not refer to a
is influenced by how much practice consumers get specific persuasion attempt or situation, while a
during persuasion episodes and how much experi- situational measure refers to the particular persua-
ence they gain from encountering persuasion situa- sive communication attempt or situation. Other than
tions (Friestad & Wright, 1994). The prevalent and a chronic measure, a situational measure captures
intensive use of marketing in society provides sev- persuasion knowledge that allows consumers to use
eral information sources, such as mass media com- cues concerning persuasion from the specific persua-
mentary on marketing activities and advertising and sion context. Thus, a situational measure is more
sales expertise shared by marketing professionals in specific and likely to better explain the variance in
the media, which can increase consumers’ persua- responses (both negative coping responses and eva-
sion knowledge and lead to corresponding out- luations), that is, it leads to stronger responses com-
comes (Friestad & Wright, 1994). However, there pared with a chronic measure. Situational measures,
are two reasons why high-intensity marketing does but not chronic ones, often are used in studies with
not necessarily increase persuasion knowledge experimental manipulations, and because experi-
effects. First, persuasion knowledge might reach a mental manipulations lead to stronger effects (e.g.,
ceiling in high-intensity marketing contexts. Second, Crosno & Dahlstrom, 2008; Geyskens, Steenkamp, &
research on competitive interference has provided Kumar, 1998), this further underlines the assump-
strong evidence that increasing advertising clutter tion that effects should be stronger for situational,
can even reduce memory and learning (Keller, 1991; compared with chronic, persuasion knowledge.
Kent & Allen, 1994). Therefore, contexts with inten-
sive marketer actions that create more clutter can H11: The effect of persuasion knowledge on eva-
even reduce persuasion knowledge’s effectiveness luations and coping is stronger for situational
because consumers are less likely to identify market- measures vs. chronic measures.
ers’ ulterior motives and manipulative actions and,
thus, simply focus on their own benefits. The U.S. Research on the development of persuasion
has the highest marketing spending per capita (and, knowledge in children distinguishes further
thus, the highest probability of advertising clutter) between measures that reflect the different stages of
among the sample of countries in this meta-analysis, persuasion knowledge development (Hudders
which includes several European countries and et al., 2017; Nelson, 2016; Rozendaal et al., 2013;
others with comparatively low marketing spending Wright et al., 2005). These measures refer to the
per capita, such as India, China, Iran, and Thailand. recognition of persuasive communication and the
recognition of the persuasion source, as well as the
H10: The effect of persuasion knowledge on eva- understanding of persuasive intent, both of which
luations and coping is stronger in non-US are commonly used measures in the persuasion
samples vs. US samples. knowledge literature. They will be considered in
this meta-analysis, along with general persuasion
knowledge measures that encompass both recogni-
Measures
tion and understanding of intent. The recognition
In prior studies, persuasion knowledge has been of advertising is the first stage of persuasion knowl-
measured by either dispositional/chronic or edge development. Even if consumers recognize
Persuasion Knowledge Meta-Analysis 9

advertising or marketing communication, they do Censorship due to authors, editors, or reviewers


not necessarily understand its persuasive intent is related to the size of effects reported in studies
and, therefore, are less likely to elicit strong reac- (Rust, Lehman, & Farley, 1990). Leading journals’
tions (Eisend et al., 2020). Thus, while recognition is editors and reviewers are more likely to select stu-
a prerequisite for understanding, recognition alone dies with strong effect sizes, signaling a theory’s
should trigger weaker coping responses and more usefulness and the rigorous application of methods
positive evaluations than the understanding of per- (Aguinis, Dalton, Bosco, Pierce, & Dalton, 2011).
suasive intent or a general persuasion knowledge Additionally, authors are more likely to select
measure that includes both recognition and under- strong findings for submission to a leading jour-
standing: nal. If the meta-analysis suffers from a selection
bias, the effect sizes in leading journals would be
H12: The effect of persuasion knowledge on eva- larger than the effect sizes in other publication
luations and coping is stronger for measures outlets.
of understanding persuasive intent or general
persuasion knowledge measures vs. recogni-
tion measures. Method
Study Retrieval
Method and others (controls)
The meta-analysis compiled papers that pro-
To control for common variations between stu- vided statistical information regarding persuasion
dies and effect sizes, as well as potential biases, knowledge effects on consumer response variables.
we considered the time, study design, publication To this end, we conducted an exhaustive search of
status, and publication outlet as control variables, published and unpublished papers that empirically
like other meta-analyses (e.g., Carlson, Vincent, measured and/or tested persuasion knowledge
Hardesty, & Bearden, 2008; Neumann, Böckenholt, effects. First, we performed keyword searches of
& Sinha, 2016; Scheibehenne, Greifeneder, & Todd, electronic databases (such as Business Source Com-
2010). The progression of knowledge in a research plete, JSTOR, PsycINFO, ProQuest Dissertations &
area suggests that effect sizes can change over Theses Global, and Google Scholar) and relevant con-
time because scientific progress can be continuous, ference proceedings (the conferences by the Ameri-
discontinuous, or static (Eisend, 2015). Further- can Advertising Academy, Association for Consumer
more, each research topic has a life cycle. How Research, and European Academy of Advertising)
long and how intensively researchers have dealt using the following keywords: “persuasion knowl-
with a specific topic can influence the explanatory edge”; “persuasion tactic*” + “knowledge”;
power that any additional study provides. There- “manipulative intent*”; “persuasive intent*”; “per-
fore, time (as assessed by the year of the collected suasive motive”; “selling motive”; “inference” +
data) can influence the size of the effects reported “manipulat*”; “suspicion” + “motive”; “identifica-
in a study. tion”/ “recognition” + “commercial source;”
Experimental studies can control the levels of the “understanding” + “advertising process”; and
factor to which a participant is exposed and can “advertising tactic*”/“advertis*” + “truth”/
offer the control necessary to eliminate potential “advertis*” + “bias.” Second, we searched review
confounds (e.g., Field & Hole, 2003). Similar to articles (Campbell & Kirmani, 2008; Ham et al.,
other meta-analyses (e.g., Crosno & Dahlstrom, 2015; Wright et al., 2005), examining their refer-
2008; Geyskens et al., 1998), experimental settings ence lists and applying an ancestry tree search in
are expected to lead to larger effect sizes than other the Web of Science database and on Google Scholar
study designs. for all papers referring to the review papers and
Furthermore, significant findings are more likely the seminal paper by Friestad and Wright (1994).
to be submitted to peer-reviewed journals, and Third, we performed a manual search of the jour-
they are more likely to be published than insignifi- nal outlets that turned out to be major sources for
cant findings, leading to publication bias (Sutton, articles dealing with persuasion knowledge.
2009). Therefore, the effect size estimates in Fourth, we reviewed the reference lists in all the
unpublished papers are expected to be lower than obtained papers. The compilation procedure
those in published papers, in case the literature aligned to that of prior consumer research meta-
included in the meta-analysis suffers from publica- analyses (e.g., van Laer, de Ruyter, Visconti, &
tion bias. Wetzels, 2014; Motyka et al., 2014; Neumann
10 Eisend and Tarrahi

et al., 2016) and recommendations in the literature Effect Size Computation and Coding
(Grewal, Puccinelli, & Monroe, 2018; Hunter &
We retrieved and computed an effect size that
Schmidt, 2004), and it included all papers that
describes the relationship between persuasion
were available by July 2019.
knowledge and any of the consumer response vari-
After identifying papers for potential inclusion in
ables. The effect size metric selected was the corre-
the meta-analytic database, we applied inclusion
lation coefficient, which was an easily interpretable
and exclusion criteria to determine which papers to
effect size. A positive sign in the correlation coeffi-
retain. We included all papers that reported on
cient indicated that persuasion knowledge increased
empirical studies based on samples of consumers
the consumer response variable, and vice versa for
and that quantitatively investigated persuasion
a negative sign in the correlation coefficient. Higher
knowledge effects, as defined above, on any consu-
absolute values of the correlation coefficient indi-
mer response variables. We excluded any paper
cated a stronger influence from persuasion knowl-
that lied beyond this scope. We also excluded
edge on a consumer response variable. For studies
papers that did not provide enough data for the
that reported other measures (e.g., Student’s t,
purposes of our meta-analysis, such as those that
mean differences, other effect size measures), these
lacked sufficient statistical information to calculate
measures were converted to correlation coefficients.
an effect size and for which the necessary informa-
The correlations were adjusted for measurement
tion could not be retrieved from the authors. Apart
error following the procedure proposed by Hunter
from these exclusions, we considered any papers
and Schmidt (2004). When a study did not report
written in English that provided the appropriate
on reliability, we used the mean reliability for that
empirical data.
variable across all studies. Out of 726 effect sizes,
To avoid duplicates in our database and to 32.1% were based on single item scales, and 75% of
address data dependencies, we proceeded in the the remaining scales reported a reliability coeffi-
following manner. A document with original ana- cient. Overall, 726 relevant effect sizes were
lyses and findings by the authors (e.g., journal retrieved from 171 data sets provided in 148
article, working paper, conference paper) was papers. The combined sample size for these effect
called a “paper.” Some papers analyzed more sizes included 28,944 consumers. The consumer
than one distinct data set (e.g., a paper with sev- response variables were categorized according to
eral experiments), while other data sets are ana- the variable definitions in Table 1. Two researchers
lyzed in more than one paper (e.g., an empirical independently assigned all the consumer response
study published as a conference paper and a jour- variables in the data sets to these categories. The
nal paper). We considered data sets to be nested agreement rate was 94.2% (Krippendorff’s α =.937),
within a paper. If results were spread across and inconsistencies were resolved through discus-
papers based on the same data set, they were sion. In addition, a student coder who was external
coded as results from a single data set nested to the research coded the consumer response vari-
within a paper. We coded the paper characteris- ables. In 4% of the cases, the student assistant pro-
tics (publication status, publication outlet) based vided different coding that was reconciled through
on the most recent publication. Papers with discussion.
changes in author teams that used the same data
set were identified by identical sample sizes and
overlapping results. Each data set can provide Integration of Correlation-Based Effect Sizes
single or multiple effects. Our final database To capture persuasion knowledge’s overall
included 148 unique papers that used 171 distinct effects on consumer response variables, the
data sets (see the Web Appendix S1 for a detailed correlation-based effect sizes were integrated, that
list). The database for this meta-analysis included is, an average estimate was computed. We first
journal articles, book chapters, working papers, transformed the reliability-corrected correlations
unpublished theses, and conference proceedings, using Fisher’s z transformation to benefit from its
thereby reducing the risk of a biased representa- variance-stabilizing property. We performed the
tion of the state of research due to the publication analysis using this index, then converted the sum-
source. Overall, 19 data sets came from unpub- mary values back to correlations for presentation.
lished sources, including 11% of the effect sizes We dealt with integrating dependencies between
(n = 80), comprising a sample size of 3,071 consu- effect sizes as follows. When a data set provided
mers. findings for different consumer response variables,
Persuasion Knowledge Meta-Analysis 11

we treated the findings as independent, because expressed by our hypotheses, were measured on all
we integrated and analyzed the estimates for each three levels. Thus, the estimated model was
consumer response variable separately. Some data expressed as follows:
sets reported multiple relevant tests for the same
consumer response variable. We accounted for the zijk ¼ γ 000 þ γ 001 ∗PUBSTATUSk þ γ 002 ∗TOPJOURk
dependencies of the effects sizes and our data’s þ γ 010 ∗COVERTjk þ γ 020 ∗PERSONALjk
nested nature (first level: effect sizes; second level: þ γ 030 ∗INVOLVEMENTjk þ γ 040 ∗FAMILIARjk
data set; third level: paper) by using multilevel
(i.e., three-level) models (Raudenbush & Bryk, þ γ 050 ∗SERVICEjk þ γ 060 ∗EXPERIENCEjk
2002). Following the procedure from other þ γ 070 ∗ONLINEjk þ γ 080 ∗AGEjk
multilevel meta-analyses (e.g., Chernev, þ γ 090 ∗STUDENTjk þ γ 0100 ∗USjk
Böckenholt, & Goodman, 2015; Kranzbühler, þ γ 0110 ∗YEARjk þ γ 0120 ∗STUDYDESIGNjk
Zerres, Kleijnen, & Verlegh, 2020), we used the
following equation: þ γ 100 ∗CHRONICijk þ γ 200 ∗RECOGNITIONijk
þeijk þ u0jk þ a00k ,
zi ¼ γ i þ ei þ ui þ ai (1) (2)

in which zijk denotes the ith Fisher’s z trans-


In this equation, observed effect sizes (zi) are formed correlation reported within the jth data
predicted from the average population effect (γi), set reported within the kth paper. The equation de-
as well as the sampling variance component (ei), scribes the effects from the moderator variables
a between-data-set (within-paper) error (ui), and a such that:
between-paper error (ai). The equation is an PUBSTATUSk = 1 if the paper was unpublished
“intercept-only” model in that no moderator is and 0 if it was published. TOPJOURk = 1 if the
included as a predictor. We integrated the effect paper was published in a top journal (papers in the
sizes and estimated the model in HLM (using the meta-analysis were published in the following top
HLM 8.0 software) and performed analyses for all journals: Journal of Consumer Research; Journal of
consumer response variables separately and for Marketing Research; and Journal of Personality and
combined response categories. Furthermore, we Social Psychology) and 0 if otherwise. COVERTjk = 1
calculated the statistical power for the significant if the source was covert and 0 if it was noncovert.
summary effect size of each consumer PERSONALjk = 1 if the communication was per-
response variable (Valentine, Pigott, & Rothstein, sonal, and 0 if the communication was nonpersonal.
2010). INVOLVEMENTjk = 1 if the message referred to a
high-involvement product, and 0 if it referred to a
low-involvement product. FAMILIARjk = 1 if the
Meta-Regression
message referred to an unfamiliar product, and 0 if
We assessed moderator variables’ influence on it referred to a familiar product. SERVICEjk = 1 if
Fisher’s z transformed correlation-based effect the message referred to a service, and 0 if it
sizes, which refer to either negative coping referred to goods. EXPERIENCEjk = 1 if the mes-
responses or evaluations. We applied a power sage referred to products with experience/credence
analysis to ensure that we had a sufficient number attributes, and 0 if it referred to products with
of effect sizes for a desired power level of .8, a search attributes. ONLINEjk = 1 if the channel was
given number of predictors, and the anticipated online, and 0 if the channel was offline. AGEjk = 1
effect size (Faul, Erdfelder, Buchner, & Lang, if respondents were children/adolescents and 0 if
2009). The number of correlations and data sets they were adults. STUDENTjk = 1 if the respon-
for both consumer response variables was suffi- dents were students and 0 if they were non-
cient to produce robust results. students. USjk = 1 if the respondents were from the
A multilevel model that includes influencing U.S. and 0 if they were from other countries.
variables is termed a conditional model, which is a YEARjk = data collection year (continuous variable).
mixed-effects model, as fixed effects for the influ- STUDYDESIGNjk = 1 if the study was nonexperi-
encing variables are considered in addition to ran- mental and 0 if it was experimental. CHRONICijk =
dom components. The moderator variables, as 1 if the measure referred to chronic persuasion
12 Eisend and Tarrahi

Table 2
Persuasion Knowledge Effects: Integration of Correlation-Based Effect Sizes

Expected No. No. data No. effect Sample Corrected Lower Upper
Consumer response variable effect papers sets sizes size mean r 95% CI 95% CI Power

Marketer intended effects


Evaluations Negative 96 119 296 20,460 −0.098 −0.152 −0.043 >.999
Attitude toward the ad 28 32 49 8,108 −0.041 −0.127 0.046 –
Attitude toward the brand 60 75 168 10,732 −0.100 −0.161 −0.037 .956
Company evaluation 11 13 31 2,927 −0.208 −0.464 0.079 –
Media evaluation 16 17 28 4,061 −0.057 −0.120 0.007 –
Satisfaction 6 9 20 971 −0.087 −0.202 0.030 –
Intentions and behavior Negative 56 67 112 13,719 −0.123 −0.197 −0.048 .999
Behavioral intention 52 60 99 12,523 −0.133 −0.210 −0.054 .999
Choice and behavior 6 7 13 1,196 −0.015 −0.293 0.266 –
Memory and learning ? 23 25 49 3,666 0.206 −0.012 0.405 –
Brand recall 13 13 19 1,846 0.116 −0.257 0.459 –
Brand recognition 4 4 14 457 0.371 0.145 0.561 .800
Learning 8 10 16 1,736 0.271 0.093 0.432 .978
Other positive responses Negative 26 36 81 5,021 −0.196 −0.311 −0.076 .999
Credibility 20 28 59 4,468 −0.183 −0.313 −0.047 .990
Positive cognitions 2 3 3 448 −0.503 −0.875 0.241 –
Positive feelings 6 7 19 499 −0.207 −0.358 −0.046 .631
Marketer unintended effects
Negative coping responses Positive 46 64 152 11,891 0.256 0.174 0.334 >.999
Avoidance 10 13 22 3,489 0.195 0.083 0.302 .982
Disapproval 10 10 23 2,068 0.041 −0.063 0.144 –
Negative cognitions 4 4 8 984 0.074 −0.074 0.219 –
Negative feelings 4 5 8 1,038 0.398 0.225 0.546 .994
Suspicion 25 39 91 6,264 0.327 0.213 0.432 >.999
Other ?
Price estimate 4 7 17 574 −0.211 −0.290 −0.129 .715
Third-person perceptions 5 8 19 1,843 −0.003 −0.099 0.094 –

The corrected mean correlation coefficients (r) are variance-weighted and reliability-corrected estimates. Before integrating the correla-
tions, they were transformed using Fisher’s z transformation. After performing the analysis with this index, the summary values were
converted back to correlations for presentation.

knowledge and 0 if otherwise. RECOGNITIONijk = standard deviations) for the models, but did not
1 if the measure referred to recognition and 0 if find any.
otherwise.
Before estimating a multilevel model with HLM
software (HLM 8.0), we conducted several checks Results
to ensure the model’s robustness, particularly to
Persuasion Knowledge Effects: Integration of Effect Sizes
reduce collinearity as a major issue in meta-
regression. First, we examined the bivariate correla- Table 2 presents an overview of the integrated
tions among the potential covariates. Second, we correlations. The confidence interval indicates sig-
computed the variance inflation factors (VIFs). nificant findings, which show that, on average, per-
Third, we performed sensitivity analyses by omit- suasion knowledge increased negative coping
ting each of the covariates with at least one correla- responses, as well as memory and learning (at p
tion greater than .5, one at a time, as proposed in <.10), while it decreased positive coping responses
previous meta-analyses (Bijmolt, van Heerde, & and led to less favorable evaluations, intentions,
Pieters, 2005). As a result, we omitted three vari- and behavior. Most of the mean effect sizes related
ables from the moderator model related to evalua- to each consumer response variable were small to
tions (FAMILIAR, SERVICE, and STUDENT). moderate, and some did not reach significance (i.e.,
Furthermore, we checked for outliers (outside three attitude toward the ad, company evaluation, media
Persuasion Knowledge Meta-Analysis 13

evaluation, satisfaction, choice and behavior, brand we identified that unfamiliar products and products
recall, third-person perceptions, disapproval, nega- with experience/credence attributes increased cop-
tive cognitions, positive cognitions). The power ing responses, while messages for high-involvement
analysis indicated enough power for all significant products led to fewer negative evaluations. These
mean correlations, except for positive feelings. The findings supported H3, H4, and H6, but rejected
findings in Table 2 remain robust when estimating H5. We did not find any differences for channel
the figures without variance weights, thus ignoring characteristics, thereby rejecting H7. We elucidated
the sample sizes (see Web Appendix S1). The bare- that adults show more negative coping responses
bone integration without reliability-correction and and less favorable evaluations than children or ado-
weighting (Web Appendix S1) shows smaller inte- lescents, supporting H8. Furthermore, negative cop-
grated effect sizes, which is expected when effect ing responses were lower in the U.S., supporting
sizes are not corrected for artifacts; however, the H10. We found no differences in effect sizes for stu-
significance of the effect sizes remains the same as dent samples compared with nonstudent samples,
indicated by the confidence intervals. thereby rejecting H9. Furthermore, chronic mea-
Concerning the three research questions related sures led to less negative coping responses and
to persuasion knowledge effects’ structure and hier- more positive evaluations than situational mea-
archy, we found the following. First, negative cop- sures, supporting H11. We noted no differences for
ing responses were primarily behavioral and recognition vs. other measures, rejecting H12. We
emotional because we found no significant effect on tested further differences between measures and
cognition. As indicated by the confidence intervals scales by adding variables that indicated specific
around the mean correlation, the effect on negative scales (Bearden et al., 2001; Boush et al., 1994;
feelings was significantly stronger than on negative Campbell, 1995; Hardesty et al., 2007). The main
cognitions and disapproval. The significant and findings remained the same, and we found only
negative effect on positive feelings, as indicated by one significant effect from the Boush et al., (1994)
the negative confidence interval, showed that per- scale in the coping response model. As for the con-
suasion knowledge did not lead to positive emo- trol variables, none exerted a significant effect on
tions. Second, the evaluations seemed to affect the the findings.
brand primarily, while the effects related to the
message and the agents were not significant. How-
ever, the confidence intervals indicated overlap;
Discussion
thus, it is not possible to state that evaluations of
different objects differ. Third, memory and learning This meta-analysis summarizes, organizes, and
effects did not follow other terminal outcomes’ neg- extends prior research on persuasion knowledge
ative effect patterns, but increased with persuasion and provides an updated and more comprehensive
knowledge. We will discuss persuasion knowledge perspective on persuasion knowledge’s role and
effects’ strength compared with persuasion effects importance in the marketplace. By referring to the
in the marketplace in the discussion section below. concept of persuasion, the meta-analysis identifies
marketer-intended and -unintended effects, and
measures persuasion knowledge effects’ strength in
Persuasion Knowledge Effects’ Moderators: Meta-
consumer responses. Therefore, a basis is provided
Regression
for the interpretation of how strong persuasion
Table 3 provides the meta-regression models’ knowledge effects are compared with persuasion
results. The unstandardized regression coefficients effects in marketing (and as described below). Fur-
and standard errors are provided in parentheses, thermore, the meta-analysis investigates several per-
along with the predicted values for the results that suasion process variables’ moderating influence on
were at least marginally significant. Although persuasion knowledge effects. The findings provide
heterogeneity remained at all levels, the deviance a better understanding of the concept of persuasion
statistic indicated that both models significantly knowledge to interpret its effects and explain the
explained the variation in effect sizes. While we variation in its effects on coping and evaluations.
found no effect from covert marketing, rejecting These insights provide several contributions for
H1, we did find that coping responses increased researchers, consumers, policymakers, and mar-
and evaluations decreased for personal communica- keters. The meta-analysis further provides insights,
tion compared with nonpersonal communication, ideas, and perspectives for future research on per-
supporting H2. As for message-related moderators, suasion knowledge.
14 Eisend and Tarrahi

Table 3
Moderators of Persuasion Knowledge Effects: Meta-Regression Models

Negative coping responses Evaluations

Hypothesis β (SE) Predicteda β (SE) Predicted

Intercept .111 (.132) .025 (.101)


Source
Covert (0 = noncovert, 1 = covert) H1 (+/−)b −.031 (145) .044 (.064)
Personal (0 = nonpersonal, 1 = personal) H2 (+/−) .191 (.089)* .204 vs..379 −.322 (.120)** −.076 vs. −.378
Message
Involvement (0 = low, 1 = high−involvement product) H3 (−/+) .077 (.075) .110 (.062)* −.160 vs. −.050
Familiar (0 = familiar, 1 = unfamiliar product) H4 (+/−) .149 (.073)* .209 vs..347 Cc
Service (0 = goods, 1 = service) H5 (+/−) −.192 (.104) C
Experience (0 = search, 1 = experience/credence) H6 (+/−) .182 (.092)* .152 vs..323 −.073 (.054)
Channel
Online (0 = offline, 1 = online) H7 (+/−) .187 (.133) .026 (.065)
Receiver
Age (0 = children/adolescents, 1 = adults) H8 (+/−) .210 (.107)* .081 vs..282 −.169 (.085)* .025 vs. −.144
Student (0 = non−students, 1 = students) H9 (+/−) −.041 (.080) C
U.S. (0 = other, 1 = U.S.) H10 (−/+) −.339 (.119)** .452 vs..146 .059 (.064)
Measures
Chronic (0 = situational, 1 = chronic) H11 (−/+) −.152 (.048)*** .293 vs. 149 .280 (.059)*** −.148 vs..129
Recognition (0 = other, 1 = recognition) H12 (−/+) −.136 (.103) −.151 (.096)
Method/Other
Year (continuous) (?) −.006 (.007) .005 (.004)
Study design (0 = experiment, 1 = other) (−/+) −.036 (.106) −.051 (.075)
Pub. status (0 = published, 1 = unpublished) (−/+) .103 (.122) .103 (.084)
Top journal (0 = other, 1 = top journal) (+/−) −.024 (.115) −.012 (.105)
Unexplained variance
Level-1/-2 heterogeneity (χ2/df) 71.538/6*** 85.439/14***
Level-3 heterogeneity (χ2/df) 123.122/43*** 366.467/93***
Deviance/df 28.055/16** 46.482/13***

a
The dependent variable is the Fisher-z-transformed correlation. Predicted values are retransformed into correlations.
b
The first sign refers to the expected effect for the negative coping response model, and the second sign refers to the expected effect for
the evaluation model.
c
C indicates that the moderator has been excluded due to collinearity.
*p < .10, **p < .05, ***p < .01.

increasing persuasion knowledge leads to stronger


Interpreting Persuasion Knowledge Effects
brand recognition and increases the likelihood of
Persuasion knowledge increases negative coping acquiring brand knowledge. This demonstrates that
responses and leads to less favorable evaluations of memory and learning effects do not follow the pat-
persuasion attempts, as suggested in the literature tern of other marketing-intended effects that
(Campbell & Kirmani, 2008). The meta-analysis become more unfavorable with increasing persua-
herein provides several new insights beyond these sion knowledge. Persuasion knowledge lies in a
established findings. For instance, it showed that particular domain and includes knowledge of
persuasion knowledge leads to primarily emotional brands and ads. The positive relationship between
and behavioral coping responses, rather than cogni- persuasion knowledge and memory and learning,
tive ones. We did not find significant differences in as revealed by the meta-analysis, can be explained
negative evaluations related to different communi- by associative knowledge networks: Knowledge
cation objects, indicating that persuasion knowledge about persuasion and brands is connected strongly,
leads to similar evaluative responses for different and this connection eases information retrieval from
objects. The findings further revealed a positive memory (Anderson & Reder, 1979). This finding
effect on memory and learning, such as that questions the persuasion knowledge model’s
Persuasion Knowledge Meta-Analysis 15

premise that increasing persuasion knowledge leads exactly persuasion works in the marketplace. These
to effects that are generally unfavorable for mar- insights should encourage public policymakers to
keters. act to further increase consumers’ persuasion
Most of the effect sizes of persuasion knowl- knowledge or focus on alternative measures that
edge’s influence were small to moderate, but the can restrict consumers’ susceptibility to marketing
question remains as to how strong they are when actions, such as regulations. The 50% figure in this
compared with persuasion in the marketplace. If meta-analysis can provide a benchmark for assess-
persuasion knowledge, indeed, can reduce or even ing persuasion knowledge’s capacity to handle per-
eliminate persuasion effects, it is considered sub- suasion in the marketplace.
stantial and important. To answer the question, we
compared the effects in this meta-analysis to per-
Explaining Variations in Persuasion Knowledge Effects
suasion effects as provided by a meta-meta-analysis
on Coping and Evaluations
of the effect sizes in advertising (Eisend & Tarrahi,
2016). As a caveat, it should be noted that a com- While the literature suggests that persuasion
parison of persuasion knowledge effects with knowledge positively affects coping and negatively
advertising effects and the suggested explanations affects evaluations, the variation in effects is consid-
assume that persuasion knowledge is exogenous. erable. Some studies even have shown that under
However, persuasion knowledge might be endoge- certain conditions, consumers who cope success-
nous and related to advertising intensity, that is, fully can better comprehend and carve out their
more advertising might increase persuasion knowl- benefits from a persuasion attempt, leading to posi-
edge, as consumers can gain experience and prac- tive evaluations via persuasion knowledge (Isaac &
tice from encountering persuasion situations Grayson, 2017). Whether and how consumers apply
(Friestad & Wright, 1994). The sizes of persuasion their persuasion knowledge to identify persuasion
knowledge effects on evaluations (−.098) or inten- benefits and their effects depend on the cues that
tions and behavior (−.122) were significantly smal- persuasion process elements provide and that can
ler than advertising’s general effects on attitudes help consumers identify marketers’ ulterior motives
(.214) or behavior (.199). Comparing persuasion and manipulative actions.
knowledge effects’ explanatory power with that of Except for channel characteristics, we found sig-
advertising effectiveness showed that persuasion nificant moderating effects in the source, message,
knowledge effects reached around 50% of advertis- receiver characteristics, and measures of persuasion
ing’s explanatory power over consumer responses. knowledge. Every time these elements helped con-
This implies that these effects can be viewed as sub- sumers understand the benefits for both themselves
stantial because they can counteract and reduce and for marketers, and helped identify hidden ulte-
advertising influences on consumers’ evaluations rior motives and persuasion tactics as manipulative,
and behaviors considerably, by 50%. Additionally, persuasion knowledge effects became unfavorable
the findings indicated that persuasion knowledge is for marketers, weakening their intended effects (here:
a factor that can contribute to and partially explain evaluations) and strengthening unintended effects
lesser advertising effectiveness, which has attracted (here: negative coping). Figure 2 provides an over-
substantial attention in the literature (e.g., Dahlén & view of persuasion elements that explained varia-
Rosengren, 2016). The literature has discussed sev- tions in persuasion knowledge effects. The figure
eral explanations as to why marketing communica- summarizes which kinds of sources, messages, recei-
tion efforts go awry and advertising fails, for vers, and measures moderated persuasion knowl-
example, inattention, miscomprehension, or ineffec- edge effects. Persuasion processes with sources not
tive actions on the part of advertisers (e.g., over- deemed transparent nor obtrusive, messages that
advertising or lack of field tests and tracking; Tellis, make only consumer benefits salient, experienced
2004). However, the literature has neglected persua- recipients, and specific persuasion knowledge mea-
sion knowledge as an explanation. Our findings sures increase persuasion knowledge effects that
show that the development of persuasion knowl- were unfavorable for marketers. Some of the moder-
edge generally does not imply that consumers fully ators even revealed conditions under which persua-
control (i.e., by 100%) advertising’s influence, sug- sion knowledge did not harm marketers, but might
gesting that despite developing persuasion knowl- have led to outcomes that were favorable for mar-
edge, consumers cannot fully understand all the keters. We found that negative persuasion knowl-
details of persuasion tactics and interpret how edge effects on evaluations disappeared for children
16 Eisend and Tarrahi

Figure 2. Explaining the variation in persuasion knowledge effects: a persuasion process perspective.

or adolescents, and even became positive when per-


Future Research
suasion knowledge measures were chronic.
These findings carry several implications. Con- While a meta-analysis should not be viewed as a
cerning consumers and public policy, persuasion substitute for new primary research (Cooper &
knowledge effects can be activated or improved in Hedges, 1994), it can help ensure that the next
such a way that they support consumers by wave of primary research is headed in the most
increasing receiver expertise, providing information illuminating research direction by identifying
on sources’ obtrusiveness or lack of transparency, unsolved research problems and less-researched,
or discounting messages as beneficial only for con- but important, effects, and by suggesting factors
sumers. As for researchers, the results regarding that explain variations in effects. In this spirit, we
persuasion knowledge measures show that persua- suggest some areas that would benefit from further
sion knowledge research findings cannot be com- investigation.
pared unless identical measures are used. First, by explaining variations in effects, this
Comparisons across studies with different mea- meta-analysis used communication process cues
sures are misleading. These insights also relate to that could be retrieved from primary studies and
marketers, who need to understand what kind of from which sufficient and specific data were avail-
consumer persuasion knowledge they are dealing able. Further moderators relating to the communi-
with to predict what kind of outcomes from their cation process elements could be considered in
persuasion attempts they can expect. Because per- future primary research. The idea is that some com-
suasion knowledge, on average, can decrease per- munication elements help consumers develop more
suasion effects by 50%, conditions in which or less critical reactions, which influence whether
persuasion knowledge effects become even more they can identify the persuasion attempt’s benefits
unfavorable for marketers (e.g., messages for unfa- for both the consumer and marketer, as well as
miliar products or products with experience and compare and evaluate these benefits. As for
credence attributes, as well as personalized com- sources, endorsers and their fit with a product or
munication) can jeopardize marketing investments endorser authenticity have not been investigated
in persuasion attempts considerably. A solution for sufficiently as a cue that could increase or decrease
marketers is to adopt their marketing communica- critical thinking. Persuasion knowledge effects
tion practices to consumers with different degrees barely have been investigated, as they relate to
of persuasion knowledge. For instance, they message elements used commonly in advertising,
should avoid personalization when communicating such as humor, sex appeal, and creativity. These
with consumers who possess much persuasion elements might be able to change the likelihood of
knowledge. critical thinking, for example, humor decreasing
Persuasion Knowledge Meta-Analysis 17

critical thinking (Attardo, 1994). While the meta- whether and how they can contribute to persuasion
analysis does not indicate differences between knowledge development. The prevalent and inten-
online and offline sources, comparisons of particu- sive use of marketing and advertising in society
lar sources (e.g., TV vs. newspapers, banner ads vs. provides several information sources—such as mass
social media advertising) might be more informa- media commentary on marketing activities, adver-
tive, as they may trigger different degrees of critical tising, and sales expertise shared by marketing pro-
thinking. As for receivers, the meta-analysis pro- fessionals in the media—that can increase
vides some intriguing differences that could be consumers’ persuasion knowledge (Friestad &
extended to consumers’ other characteristics, such Wright, 1994). Simultaneously, too many marketing
as gender or education, which can moderate the actions can lead to situations (e.g., advertising clut-
likelihood of critical thinking. ter) in which consumers are inhibited in developing
Second, the meta-analysis relied on a cognitive persuasion knowledge. As mentioned above, mar-
view of persuasion knowledge and distinguished keter actions are not fully exogenous and also can
between chronic and situational measures. The dif- be influenced by persuasion knowledge, that is,
ferences in effects between different persuasion marketer actions can be a consequence of persua-
knowledge measures indicate that researchers need sion knowledge development. Marketers have
to be careful and clearly distinguish between per- knowledge and beliefs regarding consumers’ per-
suasion knowledge concepts in which they are suasion knowledge and about consumers’ under-
interested. Distinguishing between chronic and situ- standing of persuasion practices (Moreau, Krishna,
ational persuasion knowledge at the operational & Harlam, 2001). They might change their market-
level – as well as between recognition, the under- ing actions and investments, depending on their
standing of intent, and general persuasion knowl- perception of how changes in persuasion knowl-
edge – can provide the first steps in developing edge relate to marketing or advertising effectiveness
more general persuasion knowledge measures (Nyilasy & Reid, 2009). Further research is needed
(Boerman, Van Reijmersdal, Rozendaal, & Dima, to investigate the relationship between marketer
2017). Considering that persuasion is a dynamic actions or investments on one hand and persuasion
phenomenon and that persuasion applications and knowledge development on the other. Such find-
methods change over time, the concept and mea- ings can provide important insights for marketers,
sures of persuasion knowledge should be under- as they might need to account for the diminishing
stood as dynamic, too. More than 25 years of persuasion effect through increasing persuasion
persuasion knowledge research have shown that it knowledge when planning for increased marketing
is difficult to develop a generalizable and stable and advertising budgets.
measure of persuasion knowledge. We might better Finally, while the multilevel approach of the cur-
understand persuasion knowledge as a dynamic rent meta-analysis accounts for the covariation
concept that requires continuous and systematic among observations from the same data set,
monitoring and development (see Bergkvist & thereby following other recent meta-analytic appli-
Eisend, 2021). cations, McShane and Böckenholt (2018) have sug-
Third, while the meta-analysis provided a frame- gested an approach that accounts for the differing
work encompassing antecedents and consequences levels of variation among multiple dependent vari-
of persuasion knowledge, it empirically summa- ables and that captures the variation and covaria-
rized the consequences. A recent meta-analysis has tion that is induced by the nested structure on all
examined disclosures’ effects on persuasion knowl- levels of the analysis. This approach is better able
edge (Eisend et al., 2020), but the effects from the to account for the complexity of research data as
second major antecedent variable, consumer age, often found in consumer psychology and should be
have not been analyzed systematically. While we considered in future meta-analyses applied to such
know that children have developed less persuasion data.
knowledge than adults, we do not know whether
the relationship between persuasion knowledge and
age reaches a ceiling or follows a curvilinear rela-
Conclusion
tionship, and how the strength of this relationship
depends on context factors, such as cultural differ- The current meta-analysis presented a persuasion
ences in education or media consumption. Related (process) perspective to answer several open
to this, another important, but neglected, determi- research questions in the persuasion knowledge lit-
nant is marketer actions and the question of erature, as well as broaden our understanding of
18 Eisend and Tarrahi

persuasion knowledge in the marketplace, includ- content. Paper presented at the International Conference
ing its substance and effects. All these insights pro- on Research in Advertising (ICORIA), Ghent, Belgium.
vide opportunities for further research, which will Bohner, G., & Dickel, N. (2011). Attitudes and attitude
help develop an up-to-date understanding of per- change. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 391–417.
Boush, D. M., Friestad, M., & Rose, G. M. (1994). Adoles-
suasion knowledge that truly benefits both con-
cent skepticism toward tv advertising and knowledge
sumers and marketers.
of advertiser tactics. Journal of Consumer Research, 31,
165–175.
Brehm, J. W. (1966). A theory of psychological reactance.
Conflict of Interest New York: Academic Press.
Briñol, P., Rucker, D. D., & Petty, R. E. (2015). Naı̈ve the-
The authors have no conflict of interest to declare.
ories about persuasion: Implications for information
processing and consumer attitude change. International
Journal of Advertising, 34, 85–106.
References
Brown, C. L., & Krishna, A. (2004). The skeptical shopper: A
Aguinis, H., Dalton, D. R., Bosco, F. A., Pierce, C. A., & metacognitive account for the effects of default options on
Dalton, C. M. (2011). Meta-analytic choices and judg- choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 31, 529–539.
ment calls: Implications for theory building and testing, Bruce, N. I., Becker, M., & Reinartz, W. (2020). Communi-
obtained effect sizes, and scholarly impact. Journal of cating brands in television advertising. Journal of Mar-
Management, 37, 5–38. keting Research, 57, 236–256.
Aguirre, E., Mahr, D., Grewal, D., Ruyter, K. D., & Wet- Campbell, M. C. (1995). When attention-getting advertis-
zels, M. (2015). Unraveling the personalization paradox: ing tactics elicit consumer inferences of manipulative
The effect of information collection and trust-building intent: The importance of balancing benefits and invest-
strategies on online advertisement effectiveness. Journal ments. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 4, 225–254.
of Retailing, 91, 34–49. Campbell, M. C., & Kirmani, A. (2000). Consumers’ use
Anderson, J. R., & Reder, L. M. (1979). An elaborative of persuasion knowledge: The effects of accessibility
processing explanation of depth of processing. In L. S. and cognitive capacity on perceptions of an influence
Cermak, & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Levels of processing in agent. Journal of Consumer Research, 27, 69–83.
human memory (pp. 385–403). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Campbell, M. C., & Kirmani, A. (2008). I know what
Erlbaum Associates. you’re doing and why you’re doing it. The use of per-
Attardo, S. (1994). Linguistic theories of humor. New York: suasion knowledge model in consumer research. In C.
Mouton de Gruyter. P. Haugtvedt, P. M. Herr, & F. R. Kardes (Eds.), Hand-
Bearden, W. O., Hardesty, D. M., & Rose, R. L. (2001). book of consumer psychology (pp. 549–573). New York:
Consumer self-confidence: Refinements in conceptual- Taylor and Francis.
ization and measurement. Journal of Consumer Research, Campbell, M. C., Mohr, G. S., & Verlegh, P. W. J. (2013).
28, 121–134. Can disclosures lead consumers to resist covert persua-
Bergkvist, L., & Eisend, M. (2021). The dynamic nature of sion? The important roles of disclosure timing and type
marketing constructs. Journal of the Academy of Market- of response. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 23, 438–495.
ing Science, 49, 521–541. Carlson, J. P., Vincent, L. H., Hardesty, D. M., & Bearden,
Bijmolt, T., van Heerde, H. J., & Pieters, R. G. M. (2005). W. O. (2008). Objective and subjective knowledge rela-
New empirical generalizations on the determinants of tionships: A quantitative analysis of consumer research
price elasticity. Journal of Marketing Research, 42, 141– findings. Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 864–876.
156. Chernev, A., Böckenholt, U., & Goodman, J. (2015).
Bleier, A., & Eisenbeiss, M. (2015). The importance of Choice overload: A conceptual review and meta-
trust for personalized online advertising. Journal of analysis. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 25, 333–358.
Retailing, 91, 390–409. Cooper, H., & Hedges, L. V. (1994). Potentials and limita-
Boerman, S. C., Van Reijmersdaal, E. A., & Neijens, P. C. tions of research synthesis. In H. Cooper, & L. V.
(2012). Sponsorship disclosure: Effects of duration on Hedges (Eds.), The handbook of research synthesis (pp.
persuasion knowledge and brand responses. Journal of 521–529). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Communication, 62, 1047–1064. Cowley, E., & Barron, C. (2008). When product placement
Boerman, S. C., & Van Reijmersdal, E. A. (2016). Inform- goes wrong. The effects of program liking and place-
ing consumers about “hidden” advertising: A literature ment prominence. Journal of Advertising, 37, 89–98.
review of the effects of disclosing sponsored content. In Crosno, J. L., & Dahlstrom, R. (2008). A meta-analytic
P. D. Pelsmacker (Ed.), Advertising in new formats and review of opportunism in exchange relationships. Jour-
media (pp. 115–146). Bingley, UK: Emerald. nal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 36, 191–201.
Boerman, S., Van Reijmersdal, E. A., Rozendaal, E., & Dahlén, M., & Rosengren, S. (2016). If advertising won’t
Dima, A. (2017). Development of the persuasion knowledge die, what will it be? Towards a working definition of
scale (pks): A measure of consumers’ knowledge of sponsored advertising. Journal of Advertising, 45, 334–345.
Persuasion Knowledge Meta-Analysis 19

DeMarie-Dreblow, D. (1991). Relation between knowl- Keller, K. L. (1991). Memory and evaluation effects in
edge and memory: A reminder that correlation does competitive advertising environments. Journal of Con-
not imply causality. Child Development, 62, 484–498. sumer Research, 17, 463–476.
Eisend, M. (2015). Have we progressed marketing knowl- Kent, R. J., & Allen, C. T. (1994). Competitive interference
edge? A meta-meta-analysis of effect sizes in marketing effects in consumer memory for advertising: The role of
research. Journal of Marketing, 79, 23–40. brand familiarity. Journal of Marketing, 58, 97–105.
Eisend, M., Reijmersdal, E. V., Boerman, S., & Tarrahi, F. Kirmani, A., & Campbell, M. C. (2004). Goal seeker and
(2020). A meta-analysis of the effects of disclosing persuasion sentry: How consumer targets respond to
sponsored content. Journal of Advertising, 49, 344–366. interpersonal marketing persuasion. Journal of Consumer
Eisend, M., & Tarrahi, F. (2016). The effectiveness of Research, 31, 573–582.
advertising: A meta-meta-analysis of advertising inputs Kranzbühler, A.-M., Zerres, A., Kleijnen, M. H. P., & Ver-
and outcomes. Journal of Advertising, 45, 519–531. legh, P. W. J. (2020). Beyond valence: A meta-analysis
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). of discrete emotions in fimr-customer encounters. Jour-
Statistical power analysis using g*power 3.1: Tests for nal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 48, 478–498.
correlation and regression analysis. Behavior Research Lasswell, H. D. (1948). The structure and function of com-
Methods, 41, 1149–1160. munication in society. In W. Schramm, & D. F. Roberts
Field, A., & Hole, G. (2003). How to desgin and report (Eds.), The process and effects of mass communication (pp.
experiments. Los Angeles: Sage. 84–99). Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.
Fransen, M. L., Verlegh, P. W. J., Kirmani, A., & Smit, E. Lee, S. Y. (2014). When do consumers believe puffery
G. (2015). A typology of consumer strategies for resist- claims? The moderating role of brand familiarity and
ing advertising, and a review of mechanisms for repetition. Journal of Promotion Management, 20, 219–
countering them. International Journal of Advertising, 34, 239.
6–16. Matthes, J., Schemer, C., & Wirth, W. (2007). More than
Friestad, M., & Wright, P. (1994). The persuasion knowl- meets the eye: Investigating the hidden impact of brand
edge model: How people cope with persuasion placements in television magazines. International Journal
attempts. Journal of Consumer Research, 21, 1–31. of Advertising, 26, 477–503.
Friestad, M., & Wright, P. (1995). Persuasion knowledge: McShane, B. B., & Böckenholt, U. (2018). Multilevel multi-
Lay people’s and researchers’ beliefs about the psychol- variate meta-analysis with application to choice over-
ogy of advertising. Journal of Consumer Research, 22, 62– load. Psychometrika, 83, 255–271.
74. Moore, J. J., & Rodgers, S. L. (2005). An examination of
Geyskens, I., Steenkamp, J. B. E. M., & Kumar, N. (1998). advertising credibility and skepticism in five different media
Generalizations about trust in marketing channel rela- using the persuasion knowledge model. Paper presented at
tionships using meta-analysis. International Journal of the Annual Conference of the American Advertising
Research in Marketing, 15, 223–248. Academy, Houston, TX.
Grewal, D., Puccinelli, N., & Monroe, K. B. (2018). Meta- Morales, A. C. (2005). Giving firms an ’e’ for effort: Con-
analysis: Integrating accumulated knowledge. Journal of sumer responses to high-effort firms. Journal of Con-
the Academy of Marketing Science, 46, 9–30. sumer Research, 31, 806–812.
Ham, C.-D., Nelson, M. R., & Das, S. (2015). How to mea- Moreau, P., Krishna, A., & Harlam, B. (2001). The
sure persuasion knowledge. International Journal of manufacturer-retailer-consumer triad: Differing percep-
Advertising, 34, 17–53. tions regardings price promotions. Journal of Retailing,
Hann, P., & Berkey, C. (2002). A study of the believability 77, 547–569.
of the forms of puffery. Journal of Marketing Communica- Motyka, S., Grewal, D., Puccinelli, N. M., Roggeveen, A.
tions, 8, 243–256. L., Avnet, T., Daryanto, A., . . . Wetzels, M. (2014). Reg-
Hardesty, D. M., Bearden, W. O., & Carlson, J. P. (2007). ulatory fit: A meta-analytic synthesis. Journal of Con-
Persuasion knowledge and consumer reactions to pric- sumer Psychology, 24, 394–410.
ing tactics. Journal of Retailing, 83, 199–210. Nelson, M. R. (2016). Developing persuasion knowledge
Hudders, L., De Pauw, P., Cauberghe, V., Panic, K., by teaching advertising literacy in primary school. Jour-
Zarouali, B., & Rozendaal, E. (2017). Shedding new nal of Advertising, 45, 169–182.
light on how advertising literacy can affect children’s Neumann, N., Böckenholt, U., & Sinha, A. (2016). A
processing of embedded advertising formats: A meta-analysis of extremness aversion. Journal of Con-
future research agenda. Journal of Advertising, 46, sumer Psychology, 26, 193–212.
333–349. Nyilasy, G., & Reid, L. N. (2009). Agency practitioner the-
Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (2004). Methods of meta- ories of how advertising works. Journal of Advertising,
analysis. Correcting error and bias in research findings, 2nd 38, 81–96.
ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. O’Keefe, D. J. (2002). Persuasion: Theory and research, 2nd
Isaac, M., & Grayson, K. (2017). Beyond skepticism: Can ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
accessing persuasion knowledge bolster credibility? Panic, K., Cauberghe, V., & Pelsmacker, P. D. (2013).
Journal of Consumer Research, 43, 895–912. Comparing tv ads and advergames targeting children:
20 Eisend and Tarrahi

The impact of persuasion knowledge on behavioral statistical power for meta-analysis. Journal of Educational
responses. Journal of Advertising, 42, 264–273. and Behavioral Statistics, 35, 215–247.
Perloff, R. M. (2017). The dynamics of persuasion: Communi- van Laer, T., de Ruyter, K., Visconti, L. M., & Wetzels, M.
cation and attitudes in the twenty-first century, 6th ed. (2014). An extended transportation-imagery model: A
Milton Park: Routledge. meta-analysis of the antecedents and consequences of
Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical lin- consumers’ narrative transportation. Journal of Consumer
ear models. Application and data analysis methods, 2nd ed. Research, 40, 797–817.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Van Reijmersdaal, E. A., Rozendaal, E., & Buijzen, M.
Rossiter, J. R., Percy, L., & Donovan, R. J. (1991). A better (2012). Effects of prominence, involvement, and persua-
advertising planning grid. Journal of Advertising sion knowledge on children’s cognitive and affective
Research, 31, 11–21. responses to advergames. Journal of Interactive Market-
Rozendaal, E., Buijzen, M., & Valkenburg, P. (2011). Chil- ing, 26, 33–42.
dren’s understanding of advertisers’ persuasive tactics. Weathers, D., Sharma, S., & Wood, S. L. (2007). Effects of
International Journal of Advertising, 30, 329–350. online communication practices on consumer percep-
Rozendaal, E., Slot, N., Van Reijmersdal, E. A., & Buijzen, tions of performance uncertainty for search and experi-
M. (2013). Children’s responses to advertising in social ence goods. Journal of Retailing, 83, 393–401.
games. Journal of Advertising, 42, 142–154. Wei, M.-L., Fischer, E., & Main, K. J. (2008). An examina-
Rust, R. T., Lehman, D. R., & Farley, J. U. (1990). Estimat- tion of the effects of activating persuasion knowledge
ing publication bias in meta-analysis. Journal of Market- on consumer response to brands engaging in covert
ing Research, 27, 220–226. marketing. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 27, 34–
Scheibehenne, B., Greifeneder, R., & Todd, P. M. (2010). 44.
Can there be too many options? A meta-analytic review Williams, P., Fitzsimons, G. J., & Block, L. G. (2004).
of choice overload. Journal of Consumer Research, 37, When consumers do not recognize "benign" intention
409–425. questions as persuasion attempts. Journal of Consumer
Sutton, A. J. (2009). Publication bias. In H. Cooper, L. V. Research, 31, 540–550.
Hedges, & J. C. Valentine (Eds.), The handbook of Wright, P. (2002). Marketplace metacognition and social
research synthesis and meta-analysis, 2nd ed. (pp. 435– intelligence. Journal of Consumer Research, 28, 677–682.
452). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Wright, P., Friestad, M., & Boush, D. M. (2005). The
Tellis, G. J. (2004). Effective advertising: How, when, and development of marketplace persuasion knowledge in
why advertising works. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publi- children, adolescents, and young adults. Journal of Pub-
cations. lic Policy & Marketing, 24, 222–233.
Tucker, C. E. (2014). Social networks, personalized adver-
tising, and privacy controls. Journal of Marketing
Research, 51, 546–562. Supporting Information
Tutai, K., & Van Reijmersdal, E. A. (2012). Effects of
online advertising format and persuasion knowledge Additional supporting information may be found in
format on audience reactions. Journal of Marketing Com- the online version of this article at the publisher’s
munications, 18, 5–18. website:
Valentine, J. C., Pigott, T. D., & Rothstein, H. H. (2010). Appendix S1. Persuasion knowledge in the mar-
How many studies do you need?: A primer on ketplace: a meta-analysis.

You might also like