You are on page 1of 17

733069

research-article2017
PSPXXX10.1177/0146167217733069Personality and Social Psychology BulletinChung et al.

Empirical Research Paper

Personality and Social

Friends With Performance Benefits:


Psychology Bulletin
1­–17
© 2017 by the Society for Personality
A Meta-Analysis on the Relationship and Social Psychology, Inc
Reprints and permissions:

Between Friendship and Group sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav


DOI: 10.1177/0146167217733069
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167217733069

Performance
pspb.sagepub.com

Seunghoo Chung1, Robert B. Lount, Jr.1, Hee Man Park2,


and Ernest S. Park3

Abstract
The current article examines if, and under which conditions, there exists a positive relationship between working with friends
and group performance. To do so, using data from 1,016 groups obtained from 26 studies, we meta-analyzed comparisons
of the performance of friendship groups versus acquaintance groups. Results show that friendship has a significant positive
effect on group task performance (Cohen’s d = 0.31). Furthermore, this relationship was moderated by group size (i.e., the
positive effect of friendship on performance increased with group size) and task focus (i.e., friendship groups performed
better than acquaintance groups on tasks requiring a high quantity of output, whereas there was no performance benefit on
tasks requiring a single or high-quality output). These results help to reconcile mixed findings and illustrate when friendship
groups are more likely to perform better than acquaintance groups.

Keywords
friendship, group performance, meta-analysis, relationships

Received June 21, 2016; revision accepted August 28, 2017

People prefer the company of friends over that of strangers to performance. On one hand, there may be clear benefits
or acquaintances. This should not be surprising as friend- associated with working with people who we know and like.
ships, by nature, are volitional relationships between two or On the other hand, a close affinity may detract from the
more people characterized by mutual liking and positive required work to be successful at the task (e.g., coworkers
regard. Importantly, this preference for spending time with who spend their time discussing their weekend activities/
friends applies not only to the social realm but also often plans together instead of doing their assigned task). A review
extends to choices regarding with whom we choose to work. of literature shows mixed findings with regard to the ques-
In the workplace, the benefits of working with friends are tion of whether groups of friends perform better or worse
often touted (Riordan, 2013), with managers having favor- than groups of acquaintances. Whereas some studies have
able attitudes about friendships among employees in the documented a positive effect of friendship on group perfor-
workplace (Berman, West, & Richter, 2002). In both educa- mance (e.g., Harrison, Mohammed, McGrath, Florey, &
tion and employment settings, people report greater satisfac- Vanderstoep, 2003; Jehn & Shah, 1997; Shah & Jehn, 1993),
tion when they work closely with their friends (Baldwin, other studies have not documented performance benefits of
Bedell, & Johnson, 1997; Morrison, 2002; Winstead, friendship groups (e.g., Andersson & Rönnberg, 1996;
Derlega, Montgomery, & Pilkington, 1995). Nibler & Harris, 2003; Peker & Tekcan, 2009; Swenson &
Despite the acknowledgment that friendships are impor- Strough, 2008).
tant, and the recognition that people generally like working
with friends more than strangers or acquaintances, it remains 1
The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA
unclear whether working with a friend(s) will be necessarily 2
Pennsylvania State University, State College, PA, USA
beneficial for group performance. That is, simply because 3
Grand Valley State University, Grand Rapids, MI, USA
employees and students may enjoy working with their
Corresponding Author:
friends, these positive feelings may not translate to actual Seunghoo Chung, The Ohio State University, 700 Fisher Hall, 2100 Neil
performance gains. One can imagine that there may be pros Avenue, Columbus, OH 43210, USA.
and cons to working with friends when it comes Email: chung.598@osu.edu
2 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 00(0)

In the current article, we seek to reconcile mixed findings on this conceptualization, participants who have just met and
and to better understand the relationship between friendship were introduced shortly before being assigned to work on a
and group performance. Considering the mixed findings in task together are an example of an acquaintance group. With
the literature, and the potentially high degrees of variability a brief exchange that provides a minimal foundation of famil-
that can arise when studying groups, a meta-analytic review iarity and ability to at least recognize one another, these indi-
can offer a more conclusive depiction of the effect of friend- viduals would no longer be complete strangers. Instead, they
ships on group performance. Conducting a meta-analysis would qualify as acquaintances, particularly when they still
helps move beyond a narrative review approach, which have neutral rather than strong positive or negative feelings
focuses on the number and the direction of significant find- about their relationship (Schnall, Harber, Stefanucci, &
ings and does not provide insight into the overall size of the Proffitt, 2008). The interpersonal aspects that distinguish
relationship. Moreover, given the logistic challenges associ- acquaintances and friends are worth noting, because they can
ated with recruiting existing friendship groups to the labora- presumably create important differences in how acquaintance
tory, and then comparing their performance with ad hoc versus friendship groups operate and, ultimately, perform.
acquaintance groups, it is feasible that some studies that In contrast to acquaintances, friendships are marked by
show null findings are simply underpowered. By meta-ana- their interpersonal closeness, warmth, and mutual regard
lytically integrating studies we are able to base our conclu- (Hartup & Stevens, 1997). Friendship groups are defined as
sions upon a large number of groups, offering more reliable groups with close, interpersonal ties and positive, amiable
insight into the relationship between friendship and group preexisting relationships (Jehn & Shah, 1997). These are
performance, and the existence of potential moderators. voluntary relationships that require reciprocity and mutual-
After introducing the construct of friendship, we describe ity in the sense that two individuals must affirm their rela-
how and why friendship may affect two processes which tionship to each other, and their affection must be
underlie effective group task performance: coordination and bidirectional (Ross, Cheyne, & Lollis, 1988). Friendships
motivation. We examined coordination and motivation as often involve individuals of equal status, who, because they
two primary processes, which scholars have regularly linked have chosen to care about each other, are particularly moti-
to the success/failure of groups (Hackman, 2002; Steiner, vated to learn about each other, consider each other’s wants
1972). Following this, we systemically integrate existing and desires, navigate conflicts effectively, and acknowledge
empirical findings to meta-analytically (Hedges & Olkin, the potential impact that their behaviors have on each other
1985; Lipsey & Wilson, 2001) estimate the overall effect of (Dunn, 2004). Accordingly, a defining feature of friendship
friendship on group performance. We also identify task and is the strong proclivity for attempts of empathy, understand-
group characteristics which we hypothesize will moderate ing, and validation.
the effect of friendship on group performance. Namely, we
first test the hypothesized moderating effects of group size,
What Makes Friendship Unique?
task independence (i.e., high vs. low interdependence), and
task focus (i.e., maximizing vs. optimizing tasks). We also While much research on social relationships often examines
conducted exploratory moderator analyses for task type (i.e., interpersonal interaction based on economic or social
physical vs. cognitive tasks) and participant population (i.e., exchange theories (Thibaut & Kelley, 1959), friendship does
children vs. adults). Ultimately, by testing our theoretically not begin with a planned agreement, statement, or announce-
derived hypotheses and exploratory analyses through meta- ment. For example, in business, producers rarely give prod-
analytic techniques, we hope to pave the way for a clearer ucts to customers without any monetary benefits in return.
and deeper understanding of when and why friendships Therefore, people should not desire to build relationships
affect group performance. when there are high explicit and implicit costs as compared
with the potential benefits that they may earn. However,
many suggest that friendships do not operate on a pure eco-
Literature Review and Theoretical nomic model of costs and benefits, but instead often utilize
Background more complex social exchange rules that consider more than
a reciprocation of benefits (Clark & Mills, 1979; Lin &
Acquaintance and Friendship Groups Rusbult, 1995). As such, friendship is primarily based on the
Much of the research on group performance has utilized ad positive experiences of interacting and working with others.
hoc or acquaintance groups. A group is defined as two or Friendship is also distinct from other constructs associ-
more interdependent individuals who influence one another ated with liking between individuals, such as cohesion,
through social interaction (Forsyth, 1999). Acquaintance familiarity, network density, closeness, and intimacy. For
groups are defined as groups with limited familiarity and con- example, on the surface, cohesion may seem to be similar
tact among members (Jehn & Shah, 1997). Acquaintances conceptually to friendship. While both cohesion and friend-
often do not have personally meaningful shared pasts, and do ship share interpersonal intimacy among group members
not possess deep, intimate knowledge of one another. Based (Evans & Dion, 1991; Shah & Jehn, 1993), not all cohesive
Chung et al. 3

groups include friendships between team members. been documented to also affect motivation on group tasks.
Moreover, while friendship may be considered a type of Consistent with the predictions of the Collective Effort
“strong tie” (Krackhardt, 1992), not all strong ties are con- Model (Karau & Williams, 1993), individuals work harder
sidered friendships. A tie may be considered strong if it ful- on group tasks when they identify and care about the welfare
fills one or two of the elements of friendship, but friendship of the group (Ellemers, De Gilder, & Haslam, 2004). When
only exists when the relationship encompasses all three com- individuals identify and care about the welfare of the group,
ponents such as liking behaviors, shared experiences, and they are more inclined to work particularly hard for the group
reciprocity. As such, for the current study, we exclusively in an effort to help the group accomplish its goal (Karau &
focus on examining friendships within groups, excluding Williams, 1997).
groups that may be conceptualized or measured as cohesive As people interact and close interpersonal ties strengthen,
or contain social network ties, but do not fulfill all of the individuals begin to espouse aspects of each other into their
required elements of friendship. senses of self (i.e., self-expansion theory; Aron & Aron,
1997). Shared goals become synonymous with own goals,
and personal concern and accountability for the fate of
Group Processes: Friends Versus Nonfriends
friends rises as the concepts of self and other increasingly
It is well recognized that group processes, or the behaviors or merge. Presuming the emotional attachments that accom-
interactions among group members, often underlie how and pany friendships carry over and apply to their task groups
why some groups perform better or worse than other groups when friends work together, heightened commitment to the
(Hackman, 2002; Ilgen, Hollenbeck, Johnson, & Jundt, 2005; group is expected in such work settings. This is not trivial
Larson, 2010; Steiner, 1972). Steiner’s (1972) model of group because identification with and commitment to the group
performance suggests that groups often incur motivational or elevate personal accountability and task effort (Newcomb &
coordination process losses (i.e., where actual performance of Brady, 1982; Tesser, Millar, & Moore, 1988), which often
the group is less than the anticipated performance given the stimulate efforts toward helping the group perform well
individuals within the group). Building on Steiner’s frame- (Klein & Mulvey, 1995). Furthermore, work by Jehn and
work, scholars have noted that people working together in Shah (1997) supports this possibility, documenting that the
groups may experience motivational or coordination process increased commitment to friendship groups, relative to
gains (i.e., where actual performance of the group is greater acquaintance groups, helped account for the greater perfor-
than the anticipated performance given the individuals within mance of friendship groups.
the group; Hackman & Wageman, 1995; Kerr & Tindale, In addition to increased commitment, friendships may
2004; Larson, 2010; Nijstad, 2009). Specifically, motiva- facilitate goal-setting and increase persistence during goal
tional process losses (or gains) may occur when individuals in pursuit. Because friends derive a sense of understanding, car-
a group work less (or work more) than they would if they ing, and acceptance from their relationships, these positive
were working alone. Likewise, coordination process losses ties inherently satisfy the need to belong (Baumeister &
(or gains) may occur when groups of individuals perform Leary, 1995). When confronted with challenging tasks that
worse (or better) than the best individual member in the involve strain and possible ego-threats, the satisfaction of
group, due to the combined effective mixture of their needs (e.g., relatedness, autonomy, competence) has been
resources and efforts (i.e., coordination). found to be an important predictor of performance because
Below, we describe the underlying reasons for why one need satisfaction often fosters intrinsic motivation (Deci &
may anticipate an overall positive effect for motivation and Ryan, 2000). Intrinsically motivated individuals engage in
coordination to occur when friends work together. Namely, work from a source of genuine interest rather than pursuit of
we propose below that, relative to acquaintance groups, external rewards and are energized workers who are less sus-
when friends work together, coordination will be improved ceptible to exhaustion during goal pursuit (Kammeyer-
through increased collaboration, communication, and con- Mueller, Simon, & Judge, 2016). Past work demonstrates
flict management; motivation will be increased via increased that social cues that signal acceptance can fuel intrinsic moti-
commitment, goal-setting, and goal pursuit. vation when people work together (Carr & Walton, 2014),
which suggests that intrinsic motivation should be especially
Motivation.  A potential difference between acquaintance and high when tasks are assigned to friendship groups.
friendship groups that could have implications for group per- The psychosocial resources that friendships offer can be
formance involves the motivation levels of members. energizing, as well as confidence boosting. In a psychologi-
Decades of research have documented that social loafing cal landscape filled with subjectivity, uncertainty, and per-
(Latané, Williams, & Harkins, 1979) and free-riding (Kerr, ceptual ambiguity, the interpersonal similarity evidenced
1983) underlie reductions in motivation and harm group per- within friendships can serve as a vital source of validation
formance. Whereas research has identified structural factors, and affirmation. Combined with the acceptance that accom-
such as reducing anonymity or adding an incentive for good panies friendships, these close relationships exist as a natural
performance, to help reduce loafing, “social” factors have well-spring of self-efficacy. In fact, merely reminding
4 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 00(0)

participants of existing friendships decreases the perceived understanding one another’s preferences, groups are less
intensity of challenges (i.e., increases efficacy; Schnall et al., likely to experience problems with coordination (Deutsch,
2008). This work incorporated an established paradigm 1973).
where participants were escorted to the bottom of a hill, The defining features of friendship help explain why
asked to put on a backpack loaded with weights, and then interpersonal similarity promotes affiliation and why rela-
provided steepness estimates of the incline in front of them. tionship processes often function to strengthen commonali-
Prior research (Proffitt, Stefanucci, Banton, & Epstein, 2003) ties (Byrne, 1971; Pilialoha & Brewer, 2006). Friendships
has shown that the visual perception of the physical world is often form between individuals who share interests, atti-
systematically influenced by self-efficacy when confronting tudes, core values, backgrounds, and traits (Berscheid &
challenging tasks and threats. Specifically, to the degree self- Reis, 1998), making communication between parties easier
efficacy is low (e.g., burdened with a heavy backpack while and also preferred (vs. discourse with those who are not like-
looking up at a hill), the intensity of challenges (e.g., steep- minded). Friends’ heightened ability and willingness to com-
ness of a hill they anticipate climbing) becomes perceptually municate with each other provide an initial basis for the
exaggerated (e.g., hills subjectively appear steeper than they expectation that performance from friendship groups will be
objectively are). superior to that of acquaintance groups. This is founded on
Adopting these principles, Schnall et al. (2008) recruited the notion that group performance is in part a function of the
pairs of friends who were walking together by the experi- ability of members to coordinate their resources and actions
mental location (Study 1), or primed participants to think effectively (Steiner, 1972). So, group performance increases
about and visualize a friend who is important to them (Study to the extent that members are willing and able to communi-
2), and then asked these physically burdened participants to cate and share task-relevant information, provide critical
provide steepness estimates of the hill they were facing. evaluations, integrate knowledge and ideas into tangible
These judgments were compared with those of participants plans, and monitor and adjust ongoing actions accordingly
who were recruited while walking alone (Study 1) or primed (Larson, 2010). In addition, as compared with acquaintances,
to image an acquaintance relationship (Study 2). Results friends may have a greater awareness as to what each mem-
showed that the presence of friends and reminders of friend- ber already knows (i.e., sharing common knowledge).
ships led people to see the same hill as less steep in compari- Thomas et al. (2014) have highlighted that effective coordi-
son with control participants. Importantly, from both studies, nation between teammates can be dependent on knowing
relationship closeness ratings from those in the friendship what your partner already knows. As alluded to, these pro-
conditions were negatively related to hill steepness percep- cesses are expected to occur more frequently in friendship
tions, indicating a positive link between efficacy and friend- versus acquaintance groups for a variety of reasons.
ship ties. In a conceptual replication of this effect of Group members who hold similar values or beliefs, and
friendship on efficacy, others found that the mere presence of who understand, accept, and validate one another (e.g.,
friends (vs. being alone) led to reductions in the perceived friends), should communicate more efficiently, effectively,
physical formidability of a dangerous target (e.g., Fessler & and openly. As a result of the cognitive consistency and
Holbrook, 2013). social acceptance that have been established, coordination
Supporting the possibility that friendship will be benefi- losses should be relatively mild when friends work together.
cial for motivation, Karau and Williams (1997) documented Conversely, when members are unaware of or hold different
across two studies that groups of friends were less likely to values (e.g., nonfriends) and struggle to establish common
engage in social loafing as compared with acquaintance ground and consensus, the incongruity of perspectives is
groups. Moreover, Kerr and Seok (2011) showed that under likely to undermine coordination and performance. These
certain circumstances (i.e., when individuals’ contributions outcomes may also suffer when members who endorse con-
were instrumental for the team to perform well), participants flicting views are not willing to take perspectives nor be con-
displayed motivation gains (i.e., working harder in a group ciliatory, or when aversion to conflict or criticism generates
than when working alone) when working with a friend as reluctance in members to share ideas and useful information.
compared with an acquaintance. To be clear, this is not to say that conflict will necessarily be
less frequent in friendship versus acquaintance groups, or
Coordination. Effective coordination of group members even that conflict is something that should always be avoided.
depends on group members being able to share and integrate Rather, given the contrasts between acquaintances and
relevant information with one another, along with the aware- friends, the types of conflict and extent to which they inter-
ness of what others know (Thomas, DeScioli, Haque, & fere with group performance are presumed to differ across
Pinker, 2014). Inefficiencies and coordination losses com- group types. For example, when group members hold com-
monly documented show that group members’ lack of infor- peting viewpoints that pertain to the task (i.e., task conflict),
mation exchange often underlies coordination problems (Kerr disagreement can lead to mounting tensions. When members
& Tindale, 2004; Steiner, 1972). Importantly, when individu- feel a lack of voice and respect from their group, this per-
als communicate more effectively, and are better at ceived violation of social contracts may negate the desire to
Chung et al. 5

engage in self-restraint (Colquitt, Scott, Judge, & Shaw, as compared with acquaintance groups. Examinations of
2006). In such cases, task conflict can spill over into the rela- group discussions showed that friendship was associated
tionship domain and heighten interpersonal friction and with reduced consensus seeking and deference to the leader,
incongruities (i.e., relationship conflict). When relationship along with an increase in a discussion of facts and request for
ties are fragile, and interpersonal differences versus similari- information from other group members.
ties are salient, decreases in the ability and willingness to Although concerns over maintaining a positive relation-
work collectively are then likely to hinder group perfor- ship may possibly hinder effective communication and infor-
mance (Jehn & Mannix, 2001; Yang & Mossholder, 2004). mation exchange, we ultimately see the motivational benefits
In contrast, when arousal from conflict is situated within along with other communication benefits detailed above to
a psychologically safe environment, divergent and critical outweigh the potential negatives when predicting whether or
forms of thinking can be adopted (Nemeth, 1986). So friend- not friendship will be beneficial for performance. Given that
ship groups with established interpersonal foundations of past work shows a positive relationship between efficacy and
trust and acceptance (vs. acquaintance groups) may, in par- performance (e.g., Bandura & Locke, 2003), the combined
ticular, have the ability to garner the positive aspects of task ability of friendships to increase efficacy, motivation, and
conflict (e.g., divergent thinking, consideration of diverse coordination provides a theoretical foundation for the fol-
perspectives) while effectively containing any sources of lowing prediction:
spillover that could accentuate relationship conflict (Simons
& Peterson, 2000; Tidd, McIntyre, & Friedman, 2004). Hypothesis 1: Groups composed of friends will perform
Thus, task conflict may be less likely to negatively affect the better than groups composed of nonfriends.
performance of friendship groups because friends (vs.
acquaintances) can more tactfully challenge each other, and Moderating Variables on the Effect of Friendship
the resultant sharing and consideration of varied perspec-
tives could benefit downstream performance (Peterson &
on Group Performance
Behfar, 2003). Consistent with contingency-based models of behavior (e.g.,
Despite the strong rationale underlying why friendship Galbraith, 1973; Lawrence & Lorsch, 1967), we anticipate
should aid in both coordination and motivation processes, we there to be contextual factors that will moderate the hypoth-
suspect that, at times, friendship groups may be prone to esized positive relationship between friendship and group
coordination losses which can detract from optimal perfor- performance. Namely, whereas we have proposed that
mance. Namely, although we have proposed that friendship friends’ abilities to minimize coordination and motivation
groups should be better coordinated than acquaintance losses help foster the proposed differences in performance
groups, the positive aspects of a friendship group (i.e., between friendship and acquaintance groups, contextual fea-
increased trust and positivity toward one another) can poten- tures that involve either the task or group can make the reli-
tially harm their potential to effectively coordinate informa- ance on these processes more or less important in predicting
tion and differing perspectives. When teammates need to performance. Specifically, we focus on variables commonly
identify and discuss conflicting opinions to identify a correct used to categorize tasks and groups as potential moderators:
solution, group members need to put aside relationship con- task interdependence, group size, and task focus (i.e., maxi-
cerns in favor of focusing on the task. As such, in part of their mizing vs. optimizing).
desire to have a positive relationship, it is conceivable that
friendship groups may be more susceptible to conformity Task interdependence. Task interdependence describes the
pressures that prompt unwanted confirmation biases, ulti- extent to which group members need to interact and depend
mately creating narrow and overconfident mind-sets. These on one another for tasks to be performed effectively (Van de
qualities could prematurely limit the searching for and shar- Ven & Ferry, 1980). For example, group tasks that are low in
ing of information, and reduce the drive to think critically interdependence allow members to perform separate func-
and carefully. For instance, Loyd, Wang, Phillips, and Lount tions individually and later pool their efforts into an aggregate
(2013) found that people working with similar teammates response. Tasks which require low levels of interdependence
(i.e., shared political affiliation) were more concerned about are common in workplace settings (e.g., Lount & Wilk, 2014)
getting along with their partner and less willing to engage in whereby a team needs to reach a certain goal but the work is
constructive task conflict, as compared with those working largely conducted by independent actors (e.g., team members
with dissimilar teammates. When performance depends on working toward reaching a team-level sales quota). Other
the integration of diverse perspectives, increased concerns group tasks involve increasing amounts of interdependence
over having a harmonious relationship ultimately can reduce and may require group members to engage in more sequential
performance of groups of like-minded people (Loyd et al., (i.e., contribution of one member needs to be shared for other
2013). Despite this possibility being a clear threat to friend- members to complete their responsibilities) or reciprocal (i.e.,
ship groups, Hogg and Hains (1998) found that friendship in contributions are shared and further developed jointly in an
groups was associated with reduced symptoms of groupthink ongoing, back-and-forth fashion) interactions (Thompson,
6 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 00(0)

1967). Highly interdependent tasks often occur when a dis- optimizing tasks, group members are able to typically see
cussion and information sharing is required for group mem- how their efforts are translating into greater performance.
bers to reach a decision (e.g., a selection committee making a This awareness that greater persistence is being associated
hiring decision). with greater performance should be particularly rewarding to
Of importance, task interdependence determines the friendship groups where greater value is placed upon the
degree to which members need to collaborate and coordinate benefit of a successful outcome (Karau & Williams, 1993).
to produce desirable task outcomes (Kelly & McGrath, 1985; On optimizing tasks, where people are often tasked with
Steiner, 1972), with higher interdependence necessitating working toward a single correct solution, there is often less
more frequent and effective communication and coordina- feedback directly linking efforts to performance along the
tion attempts. Under such settings, the coordination benefits way. Thus, we anticipate that the proposed benefits of friend-
associated with friendship groups should help provide an ship on group performance will be especially pronounced on
advantage over acquaintance groups. As such, friends (vs. maximizing tasks (i.e., ones that focus on quantity-based
nonfriends) should be predisposed to work more effectively outcome) because group members should be motivated to
with one another, so the impact of friendship on group per- promote and pursue the collective welfare (Karau &
formance should be pronounced when task interdependence Williams, 1997; Kerr & Seok, 2011), and intrinsically moti-
is high. However, for tasks requiring low levels of interde- vated to perform for the sake of enjoyment and derived satis-
pendence, the benefits of friendship groups should be less faction. Thus, we predict the following:
pronounced. When there are minimum requirements for
coordination to help facilitate performance, acquaintance Hypothesis 3: Task focus will moderate the relationship
groups should be just as equipped to effectively communi- between friendship and group performance, such that the
cate and coordinate actions as friendship groups. Thus, we positive relationship will be stronger on tasks that are
predict the following: maximizing versus optimizing.

Hypothesis 2: Task interdependence moderates the posi- Group size. As group size increases, so do the challenges
tive relationship between friendship and group perfor- groups face in avoiding threats to performance (Steiner,
mance, such that the positive relationship will be stronger 1972). Across a variety of tasks, group size has been found to
when task interdependence is high compared with low. affect performance (Bray, Kerr, & Atkin, 1978; Hackman &
Vidmar, 1970; Wheelan, 2009). Namely, an inherent chal-
Task focus.  A common way to distinguish group tasks is on a lenge of larger groups is the threats to individual member
basis of the criteria that are used to evaluate performance. motivation and the ability of group members to effectively
Steiner (1972) has argued that the task focus is one such communicate and coordinate information. Larger sized
important dimension. Maximizing tasks require a focus on groups display greater motivation losses (e.g., Ingham, Lev-
production, distance, or time (Baron & Kerr, 2003; Laughlin, inger, Graves, & Peckham, 1974; Karau & Williams, 1993;
2011), and criteria for evaluating performance are often Latané et al., 1979) and poorer group decision making (e.g.,
quantity-based (with higher amounts representing superior Lu, Yuan, & McLeod, 2012; Mullen, 1983) than smaller
performance). For instance, a group tasked with generating sized groups. With that said, the literature on the negative
as many ideas as possible to help solve a problem would be effect of group size on coordination and motivation has
performing under a maximizing task focus. Optimizing tasks largely been based on ad hoc acquaintance groups. In line
require a focus on the quality of output (Baron & Kerr, 2003; with our central assumptions that groups of friends are antic-
Laughlin, 2011), and criteria for evaluating performance are ipated to communicate and coordinate more effectively and
often based on the generation of a correct or optimal answer to be more motivated than nonfriends, increased group size
(or degree of closeness to these standards). For instance, a may not hamper their performance to the same degree as
group tasked with generating a single correct solution to groups of nonfriends. For instance, with regard to motivation
solve a problem would be performing under an optimizing losses, whereas feelings of anonymity often increase as
task focus. group size increases, such negative effects should be less
Performance on maximizing (relative to optimizing) tasks pronounced in a setting where everyone knows one another.
is likely to improve to the extent that group members are Moreover, the value associated with performing well, a cen-
willing to exert effort and sustain their drive and task focus tral ingredient to working hard on group tasks, may suffer
over time. As previously mentioned, the willingness to much more as group size increases with acquaintances, but
expend energy on behalf of the group should be positively may be less affected (or even increased), as a group of friends
related to members’ commitment to the group (Klein & grows larger in size. With regard to coordination and com-
Mulvey, 1995), and the willingness to sustain high levels of munication problems associated with larger groups, the
effort over time should be positively related to intrinsic inter- added comfort and acceptance provided by friendships
est (Deci & Ryan, 2000) and self-efficacy (Vroom, 1964) within a group may make people more willing to speak up
levels. Similarly, on maximizing tasks, as compared with and contribute their unique inputs, despite the group size.
Chung et al. 7

Moreover, the lack of familiarity among group members may adults (Greenberg & Marvin, 1982). However, children may
only serve to exacerbate threats to group performance (e.g., make friends much quicker than adults and may experience a
conformity pressure) which occur as group size increases sense of closeness to another similar aged group member as
(Asch, 1951; Bond, 2005). Accordingly, it is possible that the compared with two adults meeting one another for the first
positive effects of friendships may be even exacerbated in time (Ladd, 1990). As such, it is an open question for what,
larger groups where members are all friends with one another. if any, impact participant population (i.e., children vs. adults)
Taken together, we anticipated that the difference in perfor- might have on the relationship between friendship and group
mance between friendship and acquaintance groups will be performance.
more pronounced among larger (vs. smaller) groups. Specifi-
cally, we predict the following:
Method
Hypothesis 4: Group size will moderate the effect of
friendship on group task performance, such that the posi-
Literature Search and Sample of Studies
tive relationship will be stronger for larger groups as com- To acquire articles for possible inclusion, we searched for
pared with smaller groups. empirical studies examining the effect of friends (vs. acquain-
tances) on group performance. Specifically, studies across
multiple disciplines were searched using various online data-
Exploratory Moderators of Group Performance bases (ABI/Inform, Business Source Complete, ERIC,
In addition to task interdependence, task focus, and group JSTOR, PsycINFO, ScienceDirect, Google Scholar, Web of
size, we also sought to explore whether other characteristics Science, Wiley Online Library). We searched for a combina-
(e.g., cognitive vs. physical tasks) and population (e.g., chil- tion of various keywords, such as friends*, friendship*, team
dren vs. adults) may moderate the relationship between task performance, group task performance, team perfor-
friendship and group performance. Group tasks can differ in mance*, friend/acquaintance, and friend/stranger. We also
the degree to which they require members to utilize more searched for articles that relate to structural ties (e.g., cohe-
cognitive versus physical skills. Cognitive tasks (e.g., deci- sion, affective ties, density, intimacy, familiarity) or group
sion making, brainstorming tasks) often involve memory size (e.g., “dyads” and “group/team”).
retrieval, problem solving, idea generation, and/or judgment To be thorough in our ability to identify studies applicable
and decision making. Arguably, positive performance out- to analyses, we also examined journals and conference pro-
comes on cognitive tasks are associated with members’ abili- ceedings across the fields of social psychology and organiza-
ties to know what and when to communicate, the development tional behavior (e.g., Academy of Management Journal,
of insights and ideas through the coordinated integration of Administrative Science Quarterly, Group Dynamics, Group
information and resources, and deliberate and divergent Processes and Intergroup Relations, Journal of Applied
forms of thinking. In contrast, tasks that are primarily physi- Psychology, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
cal in nature (e.g., lifting/moving heavy objects, sports and Journal of Management, Journal of Organizational Behavior,
exercise activities) can also contain coordination demands Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Organization
but are not typically centered on verbal communication. On Science, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
one hand, one may anticipate that friendship groups should Processes, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
perform better than acquaintance groups on cognitive tasks Personnel Psychology, Small Group Research, and Social
given such tasks often involve extensive verbal communica- Psychology and Personality Science). Moreover, given the
tion. On the other hand, on physical tasks one may anticipate interdisciplinary relevance of our research questions, a
that friendship groups may feel more comfortable operating search of articles published from outlets pertaining to educa-
within a close interpersonal space with their fellow group tion was also conducted (e.g., Child Development and
members, and thus, members may be less preoccupied and Psychology in the Schools).
concerned about violating norms of physical contact and dis-
tance while executing a task.
Furthermore, the participant population (i.e., children vs.
Inclusion Criteria for Studies
adults) is another potential moderator that warrants examina- There were several criteria to warrant inclusion in analyses.
tion. It is possible that children derive more comfort from First, we focused only on empirical research that employed
friendship groups compared with adults. Past work shows participants from established friendships, and also included
that children find the experience of working with friends nonfriends/acquaintances as a comparison group. We
more positive than do adults, partly due to the fact that chil- excluded studies that employed partners in a cohesive group
dren tend to engage in such collaborations more frequently (Zaccaro & Lowe, 1988; Zaccaro & McCoy, 2006),1 and
(Miell & MacDonald, 2000; Newcomb & Brady, 1982). romantic couples (Wegner, Erber, & Raymond, 1991), and
Moreover, children may have an increased aversion toward studies where friendship was manipulated via a scenario
working with strangers or nonfriends as compared with methodology (Perry-Smith, 2014).
8 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 00(0)

Table 1.  Summary of All Studies Included in the Meta-Analysis.

Work
Sample characteristicsa featuresb

Author(s) Effect size (d) SE n Population Group size (TI; TF; TT) Task type
Andersson (2001) 0.75 0.30 48 1 2 2; 1; 2 Picture recall
Andersson and Rönnberg (1995), −0.03 0.45 20 2 2 1; 1; 2 Word recall
Experiment 1
Andersson and Rönnberg (1995), 0.24 0.32 40 2 2 1; 1; 2 Detail recall
Experiment 2
Andersson and Rönnberg (1996) 0.02 0.32 40 2 2 1; 1; 2 Semantic and
episodic recall
Andersson and Rönnberg (1997) 0.65 0.30 48 2 2 2; 1; 2 Word recall
Azmitia and Montgomery (1993) 0.31 0.34 36 1 2 1; 2; 2 Problem solving
Bos, Buyuktur, Olson, Olson, and 1.26 0.49 20 2 9 1; 1; 2 Shape-building
Voida (2010) computer task
Brennan and Enns (2015) −0.12 0.33 37 2 2 1; 2; 2 Target recognition
Gruenfeld, Mannix, Williams, and −0.20 0.30 46 2 3 1; 2; 2 Hidden profile
Neale (1996)
Harrison, Mohammed, McGrath, 1.04 0.32 43 2 3 1; 1; 2 Problem solving
Florey, and Vanderstoep (2003)
Hope, Ost, Gabbert, Healey, and −0.26 0.41 24 2 2 1; 1; 2 Word recall
Lenton (2008)
Jehn and Shah (1997) 0.66 0.28 53 2 3 2; 1; 1 Model building
Karau and Williams (1997), 0.34 0.52 15 2 2 2; 1; 1 Typing
Experiment 1
Karau and Williams (1997), 0.20 0.33 38 2 2 2; 1; 2 Idea generation
Experiment 2
Kerr and Seok (2011) 0.44 0.31 44 2 2 2; 1; 1 Physical persistence
Krylow (2008) 0.14 0.30 46 2 3 1; 2; 2 Hidden profile
Kutnick and Kington (2005) −0.07 0.33 36 1 2 1; 2; 2 Scientific reasoning
Miell and MacDonald (2000) 0.65 0.46 20 1 2 1; 2; 2 Music composition
Newcomb and Brady (1982) 0.68 0.27 60 1 2 1; 1; 2 Problem solving
Nibler and Harris (2003) 0.14 0.20 100 2 5 1; 2; 2 Problem solving
Peker and Tekcan (2009) −0.40 0.38 29 2 3 2; 1; 2 Word recall
Shah and Jehn (1993) 0.56 0.46 20 2 3 2; 1; 1 Model building
Swenson and Strough (2008) −0.20 0.25 66 1 2 1; 2; 2 Problem solving
Thompson (2016) 0.73 0.38 30 2 2 2; 1; 1 Physical persistence
Van Dick, Stellmacher, Wagner, 0.18 0.32 39 2 3 2; 1; 2 Idea generation
Lemmer, and Tissington (2009)
Voida, Bos, Olson, Olson, and 1.44 0.53 18 2 8 1; 1; 2 Shape-building
Dunning (2012) computer task

Note. Positive effect sizes indicate that subjects who worked with friends perform better than strangers. Effect size d = Cohen’s d effect size (unbiased/
standardized).
a
n = Number of groups in a study; Population: Sample’s age (1 = children, 2 = adults); Group size: Number of subjects in a group.
b
TI = Task Interdependence: High (1) vs. Low (2); TF: Task Focus: Maximizing (1) vs. Optimizing (2); TT = Task Type: Physical (1) vs. Cognitive (2); Task:
Description of Task.

Second, to maintain a clear focus on group performance, These criteria yielded 24 papers, which presented the
studies were only included if task performance was reported. findings from 26 unique studies, thereby resulting in our
Studies that only reported outcomes tangentially related to ability to code 26 statistically independent effect sizes (Table
task performance were excluded (i.e., satisfaction: Mendelson 1). Outlier analysis was conducted with Huffcutt and Arthur’s
& Kay, 2003; Riordan & Griffeth, 1995; job involvement: (1995) sample-adjusted meta-analytic deviance approach.
Berndt & Keefe, 1995; Birch & Billman, 1986; engagement: No outliers were identified, and thus, all 26 independent
Sharabany & Hertz-Lazarowitz, 1981; cooperation: Hanham effect sizes were included in our analyses.
& McCormick, 2008; and fairness: Austin, 1980; Pataki, Given our focus on group task performance, all analyses
Shapiro, & Clark, 1994). were conducted at the group level. Altogether, the final
Chung et al. 9

sample included 1,016 groups (3,467 individuals), yielding variance weight calculation (Hedges & Olkin, 1985). The
26 independent effect sizes. inverse variance weight gives more weight to the overall
analysis to effect sizes that are more reliably estimated
(Lipsey & Wilson, 2001).
Coding of Study Characteristics
Following steps detailed by Lipsey and Wilson (2001), a
Analysis of Effect Sizes
coding scheme was developed for relevant study features and
moderators. For each study, we coded (a) study characteris- To test an overall relationship between friendships on group
tics (i.e., date of publication, number of subjects in the study, performance among the coded studies, we conducted a ran-
participant population, that is, children or adults), and (b) dom-effects analysis. The analyses were conducted in SPSS
group features and task characteristics (i.e., level of task with a Restricted Maximum Likelihood (REML) approach
interdependence, task focus, task type, group size,2 and using syntax provided by Lipsey and Wilson (2001). The
description of the task). random-effects approach attempts to make unconditional
To maintain statistical independence of data, when more inferences that go beyond the studies that are analyzed and
than one performance variable was reported from a study, a are an increasingly advocated approach for studies where the
single effect size was selected and coded. In cases where goal is to generalize the results beyond the studies included
multiple measures of performance on a single task were in the analysis (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001; Raudenbush, 2009;
available from a study, we selected the most objective perfor- Vevea & Coburn, 2015).
mance measure to reduce rater bias (De Jong, Dirks, & To conduct moderator analysis on categorical variables
Gillespie, 2016; Mesmer-Magnus & DeChurch, 2009). (i.e., task interdependence, task focus, task type, and popula-
When the multiple performance measures of differing tasks tion), we used a random-effects categorical model, analo-
were reported, when there was no difference in the level of gous to an ANOVA. Following Hedges and Olkin (1985), a
objectivity for performance outcomes, we randomly selected homogeneity Q statistic was computed to test the homoge-
a single performance measure (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). Two neous distribution of the effect sizes. The significant
graduate student coders read the papers and independently between-group variance statistic (Qb), which identifies the
provided ratings of the described characteristics for each of possible existence of a moderator(s), indicates that the mean
the studies. In the instances of disagreement between coders, effect size across groups differs by coded variables. A sig-
resolution was attempted via discussion (e.g., Kong, Dirks, nificant Qw statistic indicates within-group homogeneity of
& Ferrin, 2014). If consensus was still not reached, the sec- effect sizes in ANOVA and also indicates the appropriateness
ond author joined the discussion until agreement was of the categorical variable to explain the heterogeneity across
achieved. Cohen’s (1968) Kappa was calculated to assess the effect sizes (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). We also calculated
interrater reliability of coding variables. A Cohen’s Kappa of the variance (τ2) and the standard deviation between the stud-
90.6% was obtained, indicating a high degree of interrater ies in true effects (τ) in a random-effects model to estimate
agreement (Landis & Koch, 1977). the heterogeneity variance within each subgroup (Borenstein,
Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2009). Furthermore, we also
computed and report the I2 statistic (Higgins & Thompson,
Computation of Effect Sizes 2002), which is used to help quantify the degree of heteroge-
For each study, the unstandardized mean difference approach neity. The I2 statistic indicates the percentage of variation in
(Lipsey & Wilson, 2001) was used to calculate effect sizes point estimates derived from between-study heterogeneity
by comparing the means of outcome measures for experi- rather than sampling error, where .25 = low heterogeneity,
mental (e.g., friendship group performance) and control .50 = moderate heterogeneity, and .75 = high heterogeneity.
(e.g., acquaintance group performance) groups. Because To conduct moderator analysis on continuous variables
effect sizes using the unstandardized mean differences (i.e., group size), using SPSS syntax provided by Lipsey and
approach can be biased due to small sample sizes, we com- Wilson (2001), we used a random-effects meta-regression
puted Cohen’s d as an overall effect size from each study and approach. The meta-regression approach is analogous to an
then converted it to a standardized mean difference (Cohen, ordinary least squares (OLS) regression. However, the meta-
2013). Cohen’s d = 0.20 is considered to be a small effect regression approach is adapted for meta-analysis by incorpo-
size, d = 0.50 to be medium effect size, and d = 0.80 to be rating study-specific weighting into the analysis (Lipsey &
large effect size (Cohen, 2013). The standardized mean dif- Wilson, 2001).
ferences were derived by the pooled standard deviation to Last, we also conducted several analyses to examine the
adjust sample size bias (Hedges & Olkin, 1985). This proce- potential impact of publication bias (i.e., the file-drawer
dure helps to minimize the upward biases of effect sizes problem). To assess publication bias, we utilized a number
when sample sizes from individual studies are small. Thus, of different techniques using Comprehensive Meta-
we used unbiased (i.e., standardized) mean differences effect Analysis software (Version 3.0; Borenstein, Hedges,
sizes and derived weighted mean effect sizes from the inverse Higgins, & Rothstein, 2014). This software evaluated the
10 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 00(0)

Table 2.  Effects of Friendship on Group Performance With Between- and Within-Homogeneity Tests Across Hypothesized/
Exploratory Categorical Variables (Random-Effects Model).

Homogeneity tests

Variables k Effect size (d) 95% CI Q(b) Q(w) I2


Overall effect of friendship (H1) 26 0.31 [0.15, 0.48] 40.97* 38.98
Theory-relevant moderators
  Task interdependence (H2) 1.50  
  High 16 0.24 [0.05, 0.43] 21.67 30.78
  Low 10 0.43 [0.18, 0.67] 6.71 0.00
  Task focus (H3) 10.54**  
  Maximizing 18 0.46 [0.30, 0.62] 25.70 33.85
  Optimizing 8 0.04 [−0.16, 0.24] 4.73 0.00
Exploratory variables
  Task type 2.22  
  Physical 5 0.57 [0.19, 0.94] 0.49 0.00
  Cognitive 21 0.26 [0.09, 0.42] 27.14 26.31
 Population 0.01  
  Children 6 0.32 [0.01, 0.64] 5.71 12.43
  Adults 20 0.31 [0.12, 0.49] 21.20 10.38

Note. Positive effect size indicates that friendship groups performed better than acquaintance groups; k indicates the number of independent samples.
CI = confidence interval.
*p < .05. **p < .01.

publication bias by funnel plots (i.e., a scatterplot of each Hypothesized Moderator Analyses
effect size against its standard error), Egger’s test of
regression (Egger, Smith, Schneider, & Minder, 1997), and To examine whether the benefit of friendship on group per-
Begg’s rank correlation test (Begg & Mazumdar, 1994). formance would be more pronounced for highly interde-
We also conducted Duval and Tweedie’s (2000) “Trim and pendent tasks (Hypothesis 2), we conducted a categorical
Fill” analysis, which is based on the assumption that a moderator analysis. Results did not support Hypothesis 2,
complete set of possible studies will be normally distrib- as the level of task interdependence did not significantly
uted around a true mean. The method seeks to trim outly- moderate the effect of friendship on group task perfor-
ing studies, fill in estimated studies, and then recalculate a mance (Qb = 1.50, p= .22). In other words, the benefit of
newly estimated overall effect size if publication bias is friendship on group performance did not differ between
detected. levels of task interdependence. Friendship groups were
documented to perform better than acquaintance groups on
both high interdependent tasks (d = 0.24; k = 16; 95% CI =
Results [0.05, 0.43]) and low interdependence tasks (d = 0.43; k =
Main Effect: Friendship and Group Task = 10; 95% CI = [0.18, 0.67]).
We next tested whether the benefit of friendship on group
Performance performance would be more pronounced on maximizing
The overall effect of friendship on group performance was than optimizing tasks (Hypothesis 3). When comparing the
significant and positive: Cohen’s d = 0.31; k = 26; 95% con- two different types of task focus, moderator analyses yielded
fidence interval (CI) = [0.15, 0.48], indicating that friendship a significant difference, Qb = 10.54, p = .001, supporting
groups performed better than acquaintance groups, support- Hypothesis 3. Namely, task focus was a boundary condition
ing Hypothesis 1. Table 2 presents results for both the overall for the positive effects of friendship on group performance,
main effect and the categorical moderator analyses. with a significant positive relationship occurring for perfor-
Moreover, as shown in Table 2, there was significant het- mance on maximizing tasks (d = 0.46, k = 18, 95% CI =
erogeneity among effect sizes (τ2 = 0.13; τ = 0.36; Q(w) = [0.30, 0.62]) but not for performance on optimizing tasks (d
40.97; p = .02), suggesting that it is likely that there are = 0.04, k = 8, 95% CI = [−0.16, 0.24]). Put another way, these
moderators of the main effect (Hedges & Olkin, 1985; results indicate that friends perform better when working on
Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). Further supporting evidence of maximizing tasks as compared with optimizing tasks.
heterogeneity, I2 statistic indicated a low-to-moderate To test our prediction that the benefit of friendship would
degree of heterogeneity, 38.98% (Higgins, Thompson, be more pronounced as group size increased (Hypothesis 4),
Deeks, & Altman, 2003). we conducted a continuous moderator analysis (Table 3).
Chung et al. 11

Table 3.  Continuous Model for Effect Sizes (Random-Effects Discussion


Model).
The current findings offer a generalizable view of the link
Linear regression between friendship and group performance, and provide
Predictor b SE 95% CI additional evidence that reliably depicts conditions that
moderate this effect in substantive ways. Overall, the pro-
Group size (H4) 0.11 0.05 [0.003, 0.216] posed main effect describing the benefits of friendship on
R2 .15 group performance was supported, and despite the mixed
Note. b = unstandardized regression coefficient; nature of the prior literature, our results indicate that there
CI = confidence interval. are performance benefits associated with friendship, which
are generalizable across a variety of task characteristics and
populations.
Supporting Hypothesis 4, the meta-regression analysis
The relative performance superiority of friendship groups
yielded a positive and significant effect of group size on
can be interpreted through a variety of explanations, none of
group performance (b = 0.11, SE = 0.05, k = 26, 95% CI =
which are mutually exclusive. Integrating theory and past
[0.003, 0.216], R2 = .15). This positive relationship indicates
research, we speculated that the benefits of friendship groups
that the benefits of friendship on group performance became
may stem from their members’ heightened abilities to coordi-
larger as group size increased.
nate resources and actions through shared knowledge and
effective communication, and elevated levels of motivation.
Exploratory Moderator Analyses Some of these presumed characteristics of friendship groups
have been documented in individual research studies (e.g.,
We next examined what impact, if any, task type would have Harrison et al., 2003; Jehn & Shah, 1997), and the robust
on the relationship between friendship and group perfor- nature of main effect that we report is consistent with the
mance. A categorical moderator analysis did not show a dif- notion that friendships allow members to bring to the table
ference in how friends versus nonfriends groups perform many of the group dynamics and processes that underlie pro-
across physical and cognitive tasks (Qb = 2.22, p = .14). ductivity and effective performance.
Friends performed significantly better than nonfriends Interestingly, while the relative performance advantage of
groups when working on a physical task (d = 0.57; k = 5; friendship groups was found to be stable, the magnitude of
95% CI = [0.19, 0.94]) and on a cognitive task (d = 0.26; k = benefits from this type of group composition effect did not
21; 95% CI = [0.09, 0.42]). occur uniformly. As hypothesized, we found support for the
Last, we also conducted an exploratory moderator analy- expectation that task focus (i.e., maximizing vs. optimizing
sis for participant population (children vs. adults). The rela- tasks) and group size moderated the positive effect of friend-
tionship between friendship and group performance did not ship on performance. Our general depiction of friendship
differ between populations (Qb = 0.01, p = .93). Friends per- groups as social contexts that intensify commitment, self-
formed better than nonfriends in both groups of children (d = efficacy, and intrinsic motivation also provides bases for
0.32, k = 6, 95% CI = [0.01, 0.64]) and groups of adults (d = understanding the relative performance superiority of friend-
0.31, k = 20, 95% CI = [0.12, 0.49]).3 ship groups when tasks are maximizing versus optimizing.
Maximizing tasks often require effortful persistence under
conditions of strain, where workers have to continuously
Publication Bias Analyses
combat unwanted conditions like fatigue and potential bore-
In addition to the overall main effect analysis and moderator dom from behavioral redundancy. Because performance on
analysis, we used multiple techniques to examine the poten- maximizing tasks are largely a function of members’ abilities
tial impact of publication bias. First, we conducted Egger’s to self-regulate, our research suggests that friendship groups
regression analysis (Egger et al., 1997) and Begg’s rank cor- offer psychosocial resources that can ameliorate these aver-
relation test (Begg & Mazumdar, 1994) to statistically test for sive forces, namely, those that we used to describe friendship
publication bias. Neither Egger’s regression test, p = .10, nor groups. Moreover, it is worth noting that friendship groups
Begg’s rank correlation approach, p = .19, yielded evidence were not found to outperform acquaintance groups on opti-
that publication bias affected the results. We next conducted mizing tasks. This highlights an important boundary condi-
Duval and Tweedie’s (2000) Trim and Fill analysis and found tion for the purported benefits of friendship for group
no adjustment for missing studies. That is, the Trim and Fill performance, and one that has important practical implica-
method imputed no “missing studies,” and yielded an tions for managers and practitioners alike. As such, on opti-
observed point estimate of d = 0.31 (95% CI = [0.15, 0.48]), mizing tasks, where success is based on a group’s ability to
which was identical to the value from our random-effects effectively coordinate, share information, and figure out a
analysis. Taken together, these results suggest that publication correct solution, friendship groups do not appear to have an
bias is unlikely to account for our findings. advantage over ad hoc groups. In addition, on optimizing
12 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 00(0)

tasks, it may also be the case that motivation is a much less small to medium effect sizes observed. As such, future work
critical component for success as compared with its impact examining the impact of friendship on group performance
on maximizing tasks. Although our meta-analysis does not may require larger samples than those typically examined in
provide evidence on why friendship groups do not outper- prior studies to be best equipped to detect significant effects.
form nonfriends groups on optimizing tasks, we suspect that
optimizing tasks may increase relational concerns which can
Limitations and Future Directions
accompany certain types of relationships between team
members (Loyd et al., 2013) and ultimately limit the benefits We believe the results of this meta-analysis are important for
of working with friends. a number of reasons. In recent years, concern about the valid-
With regard to the greater benefit of friendship on perfor- ity and reliability of individual research publications has
mance as group size increased, we propose that these benefits been intensifying, along with a growing appreciation for the
stem from friendship groups’ ability, relative to acquaintance value of replication research. The numerous advantages that
groups, to limit the threats to motivation and coordination meta-analyses offer over single publications and replication
which increase with group size. Studies have regularly docu- studies for documenting the robustness of effects are particu-
mented that motivation and coordination problems decrease larly salient in the present climate, and calls for increased use
the effectiveness of larger groups, often leading practitioners of such analyses have been raised (Fabrigar & Wegener,
and group scholars to conclude that group size needs to be 2016; Stroebe, 2016). We believe that this work provides an
limited. Our findings suggest that friendships between group exemplary and timely response to such calls in part given
members may be an important caveat to consider as group size that prior research on friendship and group performance has
increases. Namely, larger groups of friends may not succumb commonly relied upon small sample sizes and, as a literature,
to the same motivation and coordination threats which limit has yielded mixed findings.
the performance of larger ad hoc groups. It is important to rec- As with any work though, the current study is not without
ognize that our findings only focused on the performance of limitations. For our meta-analysis, some of these are particu-
these groups, and we cannot show which process(es) underlies larly reflective of the construct we chose to study. For
these differences. instance, one possible limitation concerns the relatively low
Taken together, the results from our meta-analysis con- number of aggregated independent studies (i.e., independent
firm the relative performance superiority of friendship effect sizes). The current analysis included effect sizes from
groups and provide informative details about the conditions 26 independent studies, which may seem low in comparison
under which this effect is especially likely to occur. The cur- with other published meta-analyses. However, we suspect
rent findings add to the large literature on the array of bene- that the logistical challenges that are involved in recruiting
fits that surround close relationships, but help to extend this groups of intact friends, and the need to form appropriate ad
area by emphasizing the benefit of friendships on task per- hoc groups for purposes of comparison, fostered the modest
formance, rather than the ways in which friendships promote number of existing studies.
the psychological or physical well-being of a given individ- Although the study of friendships and group performance
ual. In this way, the current findings have implications rele- is important to advance, research that involves either, and
vant to work settings. For example, our results suggest that it nonetheless research that incorporates both, is challenging to
may be advisable to facilitate friendship development during conduct because of the need to access and obtain sets of par-
team building or corporate training, to encourage employees ticipants who meet the necessary criteria. For these reasons,
socialize with teams, and to perhaps explicitly recognize the the sample sizes of studies that involve group-level outcomes
value of socioemotional leaders. can be relatively small, as a multitude of applicable partici-
Last, this work helps inform possible reasons for mixed pants are required to produce a single datum point. To address
findings in the literature. In addition to identifying that opti- these obstacles, some researchers may simply accept the use
mizing tasks are a boundary condition, we identified other of small sample sizes, choose to rely on scenario-based
moderators which affected the positive relationship on per- methods, or create imagined friendships and groups through
formance. Considering that our significant effects ranged the use of priming. As mentioned in the “Method” section,
from “small” to “medium” in size, it may be the case that we chose not to include the latter two cases in our meta-anal-
some of the null effects in the prior literature are attributable ysis. Thus, within the empirical research that utilized intact
to underpowered studies. Given the logistic challenges asso- friendship groups and interacting group members who per-
ciated with bringing groups of friends to the lab and then formed a collective task, the average sample sizes per study
comparing them with the performance of acquaintance ad were small. This can raise concerns that the overall effect
hoc groups, the average sample size of groups that were size of a meta-analysis may be upwardly biased (Hedges,
included in the typical study in this meta-analysis was not 1981; Lipsey & Wilson, 2001), but given the methodological
large (M = 39.08). We suspect that the number of groups obstacles that researchers of friendship groups face, we
being studied in the typical study may have made it difficult strongly believe in the merits of a meta-analysis to character-
to detect significant effects given the variability and overall ize more reliable and replicable depictions of group
Chung et al. 13

phenomena over a single study, and believe this is consistent relative performance advantages of friendship groups so that
with the current call for such publications. groups can be composed more effectively through consider-
Given the potential for friendship ties to produce a range of ation of such relevant variables. The current findings con-
interesting and applicable effects, the directions for future tribute to the group composition literature by featuring
research are many. For instance, with the presumed ability of effects from variables that are interpersonal (e.g., friendship
friendship ties to induce numerous types of commitment and ties) rather than intrapersonal (e.g., individual differences,
motivation, outcomes such as satisfaction (Winstead et al., expertise level) in nature. To advance the literature on group
1995), intentions to leave the group (Riordan & Griffeth, 1995), performance, it is critical to integrate interpersonal concepts
and prosocial behaviors could be measured and compared because close ties often develop from group work, and these
across group types. Although we expect that communication meaningful relationships are likely to produce a cascading
processes are enhanced within friendship groups, friends range of effects on how group members think, feel, and
clearly enjoy socializing. Therefore, it is conceivable that behave. In the current article, we hoped to stimulate increased
friendship groups display performance superiority when they interest in variables with an interpersonal nature by summa-
are granted ample time for task completion, yet perform rather rizing ways in which the close and unifying bonds that exist
poorly when faced with challenging time constraints that between persons can positively affect the performance of a
require a strict task focus. The social exchange rules that gov- collective.
ern friendships can also differ, and it is conceivable that these
and other norms exert interesting and informative effects on Acknowledgments
how friends think, feel, and behave when they work together. The authors thank Fabrizio Butera, Sarah Doyle, Howard Klein,
Last, we believe this work has implications for managers Ray Noe, and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful feedback
and practitioners interested in how to compose workgroups. A on earlier versions of this article.
common concern among those who are responsible for form-
ing groups (e.g., teachers and managers) may be that having Declaration of Conflicting Interests
people work with their friends will lead to lower performance The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
as group members may be more interested in enjoying their to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
time together rather than focusing on performing the task at
hand. Although we suspect there are likely individuals who
Funding
may be inclined to encourage one another to take it easy when
working with friends, our results show there are clear perfor- The author(s) received no financial support for the research, author-
ship, and/or publication of this article.
mance benefits associated with working with friends. In addi-
tion, for teams composed of acquaintances as compared with
teams composed of friends, there may need to be more atten- Notes
tion given to rules that help teams overcome the potential 1. Whereas we included studies which recruited friends and
limitations associated with motivation and coordination referred to them as “cohesive groups,” we excluded studies
threats. We have suggested that these threats are more pro- which manipulated cohesion, as a group can be cohesive without
nounced in acquaintance groups than friendship groups. As being composed of friends.
such, acquaintance groups may benefit greater from actions 2. Although there have been debates about the minimum number
of people for a unit to be called a “group” (see Moreland, 2010;
aimed at reducing threats to motivation (e.g., making indi-
Williams, 2010), two people (i.e., a dyad) is often considered
vidual performance contributions visible) and those that can the smallest sized group (Forsyth, 1999). There are important
improve coordination/communication (e.g., assigning a dev- effects that emerge in both larger groups and dyads, and some
il’s advocate). Moreover, consistent with the oft-stated phrase, of the fundamental work on motivation and communication in
“The more the merrier,” composing larger groups of friends groups has been documented to reliably occur in both dyads and
seems to only enhance this benefit. With that said, a remain- larger groups (Williams, 2010).
ing question is how many group members or what proportion 3. We additionally conducted an exploratory moderator analy-
of the group needs to be friends with one another for such sis for publication year. There was no significant association
benefits to occur? Future work will be needed to answer these between year of publication and the performance of friendship
questions, and we see unpacking the dynamics of larger groups (b = −0.007, SE = 0.010, 95% confidence interval [CI] =
friendship groups, and their possible compositions, as being [−0.03, 0.01]).
an important and worthwhile area for future work which will
have basic and applied contributions. References
References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the
Conclusion meta-analysis.
*Andersson, J. (2001). Net effect of memory collaboration: How is
Given the ubiquitous employment of groups and teams collaboration affected by factors such as friendship, gender and
across domains in life, it is important to acknowledge the age? Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 42, 367-375.
14 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 00(0)

*Andersson, J., & Rönnberg, J. (1995). Recall suffers from collabo- Work (pp. 89-96). New York, NY: Association for Computing
ration: Joint recall effects of friendship and task complexity. Machinery.
Applied Cognitive Psychology, 9, 199-211. Bray, R. M., Kerr, N. L., & Atkin, R. S. (1978). Effects of group size,
*Andersson, J., & Rönnberg, J. (1996). Collaboration and memory: problem difficulty, and sex on group performance and member
Effects of dyadic retrieval on different memory tasks. Applied reactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36,
Cognitive Psychology, 10, 171-181. 1224-1240.
*Andersson, J., & Rönnberg, J. (1997). Cued memory collabora- *Brennan, A. A., & Enns, J. T. (2015). What’s in a friendship?
tion: Effects of friendship and type of retrieval cue. European Partner visibility supports cognitive collaboration between
Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 273-287. friends. PLoS ONE, 10, e0143469.
Aron, E. N., & Aron, A. (1997). Sensory-processing sensitivity Byrne, D. E. (1971). The attraction paradigm (Vol. 11). New York,
and its relation to introversion and emotionality. Journal of NY: Academic Press.
Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 345-368. Carr, P. B., & Walton, G. M. (2014). Cues of working together
Asch, S. E. (1951). Effects of group pressure upon the modification fuel intrinsic motivation. Journal of Experimental Social
and distortion of judgments. In H. Guetzkow (Ed.), Groups, lead- Psychology, 53, 169-184.
ership, and men (pp. 177-190). Pittsburgh, PA: Carnegie Press. Clark, M. S., & Mills, J. (1979). Interpersonal attraction in exchange
Austin, W. (1980). Friendship and fairness effects of type of rela- and communal relationships. Journal of Personality and Social
tionship and task performance on choice of distribution rules. Psychology, 37, 12-24.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6, 402-408. Cohen, J. (1968). Weighted kappa: Nominal scale agreement pro-
*Azmitia, M., & Montgomery, R. (1993). Friendship, transactive vision for scaled disagreement or partial credit. Psychological
dialogues, and the development of scientific reasoning. Social Bulletin, 70, 213-220.
Development, 2, 202-221. Cohen, J. (2013). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sci-
Baldwin, T. T., Bedell, M. D., & Johnson, J. L. (1997). The social ences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
fabric of a team-based M.B.A. program: Network effects on Colquitt, J. A., Scott, B. A., Judge, T. A., & Shaw, J. C. (2006).
student satisfaction and performance. Academy of Management Justice and personality: Using integrative theories to derive
Journal, 40, 1369-1397. moderators of justice effects. Organizational Behavior and
Bandura, A., & Locke, E. A. (2003). Negative self-efficacy and goal Human Decision Processes, 100, 110-127.
effects revisited. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 87-99. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal
Baron, R. S., & Kerr, N. L. (2003). Group process, group decision, pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior.
group action. Philadelphia, PA: Open University Press. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227-268.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: De Jong, B. A., Dirks, K. T., & Gillespie, N. (2016). Trust and
Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human team performance: A meta-analysis of main effects, mod-
motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497-529. erators, and covariates. Journal of Applied Psychology, 101,
Begg, C. B., & Mazumdar, M. (1994). Operating characteristics 1134-1150.
of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics, 50, Deutsch, M. (1973). The resolution of conflict: Constructive and
1088-1101. destructive processes. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Berman, E. M., West, J. P., & Richter, M. N., Jr. (2002). Workplace Dunn, J. (2004). Children’s friendships: The beginning of intimacy.
relations: Friendship patterns and consequences (according to Malden, MA: Blackwell.
managers). Public Administration Review, 62, 217-230. Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000). Trim and Fill: A simple funnel-
Berndt, T. J., & Keefe, K. (1995). Friends’ influence on adolescents’ plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias
adjustment to school. Child Development, 66, 1312-1329. in meta-analysis. Biometrics, 56, 455-463.
Berscheid, E., & Reis, H. T. (1998). Interpersonal attraction and Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias
close relationships. In S. Fiske, D. Gilbert, G. Lindzey, & E. in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. British
Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., 193- Medical Journal, 315, 629-634.
281). New York, NY: Random House. Ellemers, N., De Gilder, D., & Haslam, S. A. (2004). Motivating
Birch, L. L., & Billman, J. (1986). Preschool children’s food shar- individuals and groups at work: A social identity perspective on
ing with friends and acquaintances. Child Development, 57, leadership and group performance. Academy of Management
387-395. Review, 29, 459-478.
Bond, R. (2005). Group size and conformity. Group Processes and Evans, C. R., & Dion, K. L. (1991). Group cohesion and perfor-
Intergroup Relations, 8, 331-354. mance a meta-analysis. Small Group Research, 22, 175-186.
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J., & Rothstein, H. R. Fabrigar, L. R., & Wegener, D. T. (2016). Conceptualizing and
(2009). Publication bias. In Introduction to meta-analysis (pp. evaluating the replication of research results. Journal of
277-292). Chichester, UK: John Wiley. Experimental Social Psychology, 66, 68-80.
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J., & Rothstein, H. R. Fessler, D. M. T., & Holbrook, C. (2013). Friends shrink foes: The
(2014). Comprehensive meta-analysis (Version 3). Englewood, presence of comrades decreases the envisioned physical formi-
NJ: Biostat. dability of an opponent. Psychological Science, 24, 797-802.
*Bos, N. D., Buyuktur, A., Olson, J. S., Olson, G. M., & Voida, Forsyth, D. R. (1999). Group dynamics (3rd ed.). Belmont, CA:
A. (2010, November). Shared identity helps partially distrib- Wadsworth.
uted teams, but distance still matters. In Proceedings of the Galbraith, J. R. (1973). Designing complex organizations. Reading,
16th ACM International Conference on Supporting Group MA: Addison-Wesley Longman.
Chung et al. 15

Greenberg, M. T., & Marvin, R. S. (1982). Reactions of preschool Kammeyer-Mueller, J. D., Simon, L. S., & Judge, T. A. (2016). A
children to an adult stranger: A behavioral systems approach. head start or a step behind? Understanding how dispositional
Child Development, 53, 481-490. and motivational resources influence emotional exhaustion.
*Gruenfeld, D. H., Mannix, E. A., Williams, K. Y., & Neale, M. A. Journal of Management, 42, 561-581.
(1996). Group composition and decision making: How mem- Karau, S. J., & Williams, K. D. (1993). Social loafing: A meta-ana-
ber familiarity and information distribution affect process and lytic review and theoretical integration. Journal of Personality
performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision and Social Psychology, 65, 681-706.
Processes, 67(1), 1-15. *Karau, S. J., & Williams, K. D. (1997). The effects of group cohe-
Hackman, J. R. (2002). Leading teams: Setting the stage for great siveness on social loafing and social compensation. Group
performances. Boston, MA: Harvard Business Press. Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1, 156-168.
Hackman, J. R., & Vidmar, N. (1970). Effects of size and task type Kelly, J. R., & McGrath, J. E. (1985). Effects of time limits and
on group performance and member reactions. Sociometry, task types on task performance and interaction of four-person
33(1), 37-54. groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 395-
Hackman, J. R., & Wageman, R. (1995). Total quality management: 407.
Empirical, conceptual, and practical issues. Administrative Kerr, N. L. (1983). Motivation losses in small groups: A social
Science Quarterly, 40, 309-342. dilemma analysis. Journal of Personality and Social
Hanham, J., & McCormick, J. M. (2008). Relationships between Psychology, 45, 819-828.
self-processes and group processes with friends and acquain- *Kerr, N. L., & Seok, D. H. (2011). “. . . with a little help from my
tances. Issues in Educational Research, 18, 118-137. friends”: Friendship, effort norms, and group motivation gain.
*Harrison, D. A., Mohammed, S., McGrath, J. E., Florey, A. T., Journal of Managerial Psychology, 26, 205-218.
& Vanderstoep, S. W. (2003). Time matters in team perfor- Kerr, N. L., & Tindale, R. S. (2004). Group performance and deci-
mance: Effects of member familiarity, entrainment, and task sion making. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 623-655.
discontinuity on speed and quality. Personnel Psychology, 56, Klein, H. J., & Mulvey, P. W. (1995). Two investigations of the
633-669. relationships among group goals, goal commitment, cohe-
Hartup, W. W., & Stevens, N. (1997). Friendships and adaptation in sion, and performance. Organizational Behavior and Human
the life course. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 355-370. Decision Processes, 61, 44-53.
Hedges, L. V. (1981). Distribution theory for Glass’s estimator of Kong, D. T., Dirks, K. T., & Ferrin, D. L. (2014). Interpersonal
effect size and related estimators. Journal of Educational and trust within negotiations: Meta-analytic evidence, critical
Behavioral Statistics, 6, 107-128. contingencies, and directions for future research. Academy of
Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical method for meta-anal- Management Journal, 57, 1235-1255.
ysis. Princeton, NJ: Academic Press. Krackhardt, D. (1992). The strength of strong ties: The importance
Higgins, J. P., & Thompson, S. G. (2002). Quantifying heterogene- of philos in organizations. In N. Nohria & R. Eccles (Eds.),
ity in a meta-analysis. Statistics in Medicine, 21, 1539-1558. Networks and organizations: Structure, form, and action (pp.
Higgins, J. P., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., & Altman, D. G. 216-239). Boston, MA: Harvard Business School Press.
(2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. British *Krylow, M. R. (2008). One step beyond being “familiar”: Comparing
Medical Journal, 327, 557-560. the engagement of friends and strangers in a hidden profile study
Hogg, M. A., & Hains, S. C. (1998). Friendship and group identi- (Master’s thesis). The University of Delaware, Newark.
fication: A new look at the role of cohesiveness in groupthink. *Kutnick, P., & Kington, A. (2005). Children’s friendships and
European Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 323-341. learning in school: Cognitive enhancement through social
*Hope, L., Ost, J., Gabbert, F., Healey, S., & Lenton, E. (2008). interaction? British Journal of Educational Psychology, 75,
“With a little help from my friends . . .”: The role of co-wit- 521-538.
ness relationship in susceptibility to misinformation. Acta Ladd, G. W. (1990). Having friends, keeping friends, making
Psychologica, 127, 476-484. friends, and being liked by peers in the classroom: Predictors
Huffcutt, A. I., & Arthur, W. (1995). Development of a new outlier of children’s early school adjustment? Child Development, 61,
statistic for meta-analytic data. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1081-1100.
80, 327-334. Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer
Ilgen, D. R., Hollenbeck, J. R., Johnson, M., & Jundt, D. (2005). agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33, 159-174.
Teams in organizations: From input-process-output models to Larson, J. R., Jr. (2010). In search of synergy in small group perfor-
IMOI models. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 517-543. mance. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Ingham, A. G., Levinger, G., Graves, J., & Peckham, V. (1974). Latané, B., Williams, K., & Harkins, S. (1979). Many hands make
The Ringelmann effect: Studies of group size and group per- light the work: The causes and consequences of social loafing.
formance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 10, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 822-832.
371-384. Laughlin, P. R. (2011). Group problem solving. Princeton, NJ:
Jehn, K. A., & Mannix, E. A. (2001). The dynamic nature of con- Princeton University Press.
flict: A longitudinal study of intragroup conflict and group per- Lawrence, P., & Lorsch, J. (1967). Organization and environ-
formance. Academy of Management Journal, 44, 238-251. ment. Boston, MA: Division of Research, Harvard Business
*Jehn, K. A., & Shah, P. P. (1997). Interpersonal relationships and School.
task performance: An examination of mediation processes in Lin, Y. H. W., & Rusbult, C. E. (1995). Commitment to dating
friendship and acquaintance groups. Journal of Personality relationships and cross-sex friendships in America and China.
and Social Psychology, 72, 775-790. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 12, 7-26.
16 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 00(0)

Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Raudenbush, S. W. (2009). Analyzing effect sizes: Random-effects
Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE. models. In H. Cooper, L. V. Hedges, & J. C. Valentine (Eds.),
Lount, R. B., Jr., & Wilk, S. L. (2014). Working harder or hardly The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis (pp.
working? Posting performance eliminates social loafing and 295-315). New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.
promotes social laboring in workgroups. Management Science, Riordan, C. M. (2013). We all need friends at work. Harvard
60, 1098-1106. Business Review. Retrieved from https://hbr.org/2013/07/we-
Loyd, D. L., Wang, C. S., Phillips, K. W., & Lount, R. B., Jr. all-need-friends-at-work
(2013). Social category diversity promotes premeeting elabo- Riordan, C. M., & Griffeth, R. W. (1995). The opportunity for
ration: The role of relationship focus. Organization Science, friendship in the workplace: An underexplored construct.
24, 757-772. Journal of Business and Psychology, 10, 141-154.
Lu, L., Yuan, Y. C., & McLeod, P. L. (2012). Twenty-five years Ross, H. S., Cheyne, J. A., & Lollis, S. P. (1988). Defining and
of hidden profiles in group decision making: A meta-analysis. studying reciprocity in young children. In S. Duck, D. F.
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16, 54-75. Hale, S. E. Hobfoll, W. Ickes, & B. M. Montgomery (Eds.),
Mendelson, M. J., & Kay, A. C. (2003). Positive feelings in friend- Handbook of personal relationships: Theory, research and
ship: Does imbalance in the relationship matter? Journal of interventions (pp. 143-160). Oxford, UK: John Wiley.
Social and Personal Relationships, 20, 101-116. Schnall, S., Harber, K. D., Stefanucci, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R.
Mesmer-Magnus, J. R., & DeChurch, L. A. (2009). Information (2008). Social support and the perception of geographical slant.
sharing and team performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1246-1255.
Applied Psychology, 94, 535-546. *Shah, P. P., & Jehn, K. A. (1993). Do friends perform better than
*Miell, D., & MacDonald, R. (2000). Children’s creative collabora- acquaintances? The interaction of friendship, conflict, and task.
tions: The importance of friendship when working together on Group Decision and Negotiation, 2, 149-165.
a musical composition. Social Development, 9, 348-369. Sharabany, R., & Hertz-Lazarowitz, R. (1981). Do friends share and
Moreland, R. L. (2010). Are dyads really groups? Small Group communicate more than non-friends? International Journal of
Research, 41, 251-267. Behavioral Development, 41, 45-59.
Morrison, E. W. (2002). Newcomers’ relationships: The role Simons, T. L., & Peterson, R. S. (2000). Task conflict and rela-
of social network ties during socialization. Academy of tionship conflict in top management teams: The pivotal role of
Management Journal, 45, 1149-1160. intragroup trust. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 102-111.
Mullen, B. (1983). Operationalizing the effect of the group Steiner, I. D. (1972). Group process and productivity. New York,
on the individual: A self-attention perspective. Journal of NY: Academic Press.
Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 295-322. Stroebe, W. (2016). Are most published social psychological find-
Nemeth, C. J. (1986). Differential contributions of majority and ings false? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 66,
minority influence. Psychological Review, 93, 23-32. 134-144.
*Newcomb, A. F., & Brady, J. E. (1982). Mutuality in boys’ friend- *Swenson, L. M., & Strough, J. (2008). Adolescents’ collabora-
ship relations. Child Development, 53, 392-395. tion in the classroom: Do peer relationships or gender matter?
*Nibler, R., & Harris, K. L. (2003). The effects of culture and cohe- Psychology in the Schools, 45, 715-728.
siveness on intragroup conflict and effectiveness. The Journal Tesser, A., Millar, M., & Moore, J. (1988). Some affective con-
of Social Psychology, 143, 613-631. sequences of social comparison and reflection processes: The
Nijstad, B. A. (2009). Group performance. New York, NY: pain and pleasure of being close. Journal of Personality and
Psychology Press. Social Psychology, 54, 49-61.
Pataki, S. P., Shapiro, C., & Clark, M. S. (1994). Children’s acqui- Thibaut, J. W., & Kelley, H. H. (1959). The social psychology of
sition of appropriate norms for friendships and acquaintances. groups. Oxford, UK: John Wiley.
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 11, 427-442. Thomas, K. A., DeScioli, P., Haque, O. S., & Pinker, S. (2014). The
*Peker, M., & Tekcan, A. İ. (2009). The role of familiarity among psychology of coordination and common knowledge. Journal
group members in collaborative inhibition and social conta- of Personality and Social Psychology, 107, 657-676.
gion. Social Psychology, 40, 111-118. Thompson, J. D. (1967). Organizations in action: Social science
Perry-Smith, J. E. (2014). Social network ties beyond nonredun- bases of administration. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
dancy: An experimental investigation of the effect of knowl- *Thompson, N. S. (2016). “The Friend Zone”—Friendship moder-
edge content and tie strength on creativity. Journal of Applied ates the impact of a web-based group dynamics application on
Psychology, 99, 831-846. group cohesion: A randomized trail (Master’s thesis). Kansas
Peterson, R. S., & Behfar, K. J. (2003). The dynamic relationship State University, Manhattan.
between performance feedback, trust, and conflict in groups: Tidd, S. T., McIntyre, H. H., & Friedman, R. A. (2004). The
A longitudinal study. Organizational Behavior and Human importance of role ambiguity and trust in conflict perception:
Decision Processes, 92, 102-112. Unpacking the task conflict to relationship conflict linkage.
Pilialoha, B. R., & Brewer, M. B. (2006). Motivated entitativity: International Journal of Conflict Management, 15, 364-380.
Applying balance theory to group perception. Group Processes Van de Ven, A. H., & Ferry, D. L. (1980). Measuring and assessing
and Intergroup Relations, 9, 235-247. organizations. New York, NY: John Wiley.
Proffitt, D. R., Stefanucci, J., Banton, T., & Epstein, W. (2003). *Van Dick, R., Stellmacher, J., Wagner, U., Lemmer, G., &
The role of effort in perceived distance. Psychological Science, Tissington, P. A. (2009). Group membership salience and task
14, 106-112. performance. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 24, 609-626.
Chung et al. 17

Vevea, J. L., & Coburn, K. M. (2015). Maximum-likelihood meth- Williams, K. D. (2010). Dyads can be groups (and often are). Small
ods for meta-analysis: A tutorial using R. Group Processes & Group Research, 41, 268-274.
Intergroup Relations, 18, 329-347. Winstead, B. A., Derlega, V. J., Montgomery, M. J., & Pilkington,
*Voida, A., Bos, N., Olson, J., Olson, G., & Dunning, L. (2012, C. (1995). The quality of friendships at work and job satis-
May). Cross-cutting faultlines of location and shared identity faction. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 12,
in the intergroup cooperation of partially distributed groups. 199-215.
In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors Yang, J., & Mossholder, K. W. (2004). Decoupling task and rela-
in Computing Systems (pp. 3101-3110). Association for tionship conflict: The role of intragroup emotional processing.
Computing Machinery. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 25, 589-605.
Vroom, V. H. (1964). Work and motivation. New York, NY: John Zaccaro, S. J., & Lowe, C. A. (1988). Cohesiveness and perfor-
Wiley. mance on an additive task: Evidence for multidimensionality.
Wegner, D. M., Erber, R., & Raymond, P. (1991). Transactive The Journal of Social Psychology, 128, 547-558.
memory in close relationships. Journal of Personality and Zaccaro, S. J., & McCoy, M. C. (2006). The effects of task and
Social Psychology, 61, 923-929. interpersonal cohesiveness on performance of a disjunc-
Wheelan, S. A. (2009). Group size, group development, and group tive group task. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 18,
productivity. Small Group Research, 40, 247-262. 837-851.

You might also like