You are on page 1of 12

Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease 65 (2018) 977–988 977

DOI 10.3233/JAD-180344
IOS Press

Episodic Memory and Learning


Dysfunction Over an 18-Month
Period in Preclinical and Prodromal
Alzheimer’s Disease
Jenalle E. Bakera,b,∗ , Yen Ying Lima, # , Judith Jaegerc,d , David Amese,f , Nicola T. Lautenschlagerf ,
Joanne Robertsona , Robert H. Pietrzakg,h , Peter J. Snyderi , Victor L. Villemagnea,j,k ,
Christopher C. Rowej,k , Colin L. Mastersa and Paul Maruff a,l, #
a The Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
b Cooperative Research Centre for Mental Health, Carlton, Victoria, Australia
c CognitionMetrics, LLC., Wilmington, DE, USA
d Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Bronx, NY, USA
e National Ageing Research Institute, Parkville, VIC, Australia
f Department of Psychiatry, Academic Unit for Psychiatry of Old Age, The University of Melbourne,

St. George’s Hospital, Kew, VIC, Australia


g U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs National Center for Posttraumatic Stress Disorder,

Clinical Neurosciences Division, VA Connecticut Healthcare System, West Haven, CT, USA
h Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA
i Ryan Institute for Neuroscience, University of Rhode Island, Kingston, RI, USA
j Department of Nuclear Medicine and Centre for PET, Austin Health, Heidelberg, VIC, Australia
k Department of Medicine, Austin Health, The University of Melbourne, Heidelberg, VIC, Australia
l Cogstate Ltd., Melbourne, VIC, Australia

Handling Associate Editor: Alden Gross

Accepted 10 July 2018

Abstract. Recent meta-analyses suggest that episodic memory impairment associated with preclinical Alzheimer’s disease
(AD) equates to 0.15–0.24 standard deviations below that of cognitively healthy older adults. The current study aimed to
characterize impairments in verbal acquisition and recall detectable at a single assessment, and investigate how verbal learning
and episodic memory deteriorates in preclinical AD. A verbal list-learning task, the International Shopping List Test (ISLT),
was administered multiple times over an 18-month period, to three groups of participants: amyloid-beta negative healthy
older adults (A␤– CN; n = 50); A␤+ positive healthy older adults (preclinical AD; n = 25); and A␤+ positive individuals
diagnosed with mild cognitive impairment (prodromal AD; n = 22). At baseline, there was no significant difference between
the preclinical AD and control groups rate of acquisition, or total and delayed recall, however all indices were impaired in
prodromal AD. Performance on ISLT total score improved in the control group over the 18-month period, but showed a
moderate magnitude decline in the preclinical AD group (Cohen’s d = –0.63, [–1.12, –0.14]) and the prodromal AD group
(Cohen’s d = –0.36, [–0.94, 0.22]). No significant impairment in acquisition associated with preclinical AD was seen at

# Co-senior
authors.
∗ Correspondence to: Jenalle Baker, 30 Royal Parade, Parkville,
VIC 3052, Australia. Tel.: +61 614 23 271 223; E-mail: jenalle.
baker@florey.edu.au.

ISSN 1387-2877/18/$35.00 © 2018 – IOS Press and the authors. All rights reserved
978 J.E. Baker et al. / Impaired Learning in Early Alzheimer’s Disease

baseline. Individuals with preclinical AD showed a significantly different performance on the ISLT total score over an 18-
month period, compared to those without abnormal A␤. Individuals with prodromal AD showed substantial impairment on
the ISLT at baseline and declined to a greater extent over time.

Keywords: Alzheimer’s disease, amyloid-␤ protein, cognitive decline, learning curve, memory and learning tests, mild
cognitive impairment, neuropsychology, transfer of learning

INTRODUCTION For example, verbal list learning tests require individ-


uals to learn a set of stimuli over multiple (typically
Neuropsychological models of Alzheimer’s dis- 3–5) trials, with recall of these items required after
ease (AD) emphasize episodic memory dysfunction a brief delay [9]. By considering the rate of learn-
as a core clinical manifestation of both dementia and ing over trials as well as the related savings score,
its preclinical and prodromal stages. Consequently, impairments in episodic memory may become appar-
assessment of episodic memory is central to the detec- ent, which are not evident when summary scores
tion of the disease clinically, as well as for monitoring such as total and delayed recall are used. In older
its progression. In non-demented older adults, accu- adults with MCI, learning curves derived from ver-
mulation of amyloid-␤ (A␤) plaques and aggregation bal list learning tests are flattened compared to those
of hyperphosphorylated tau are pathological hall- from matched healthy controls [10–13]. In these MCI
marks of AD that are associated with brain volume groups, savings scores are typically 50% lower than
loss and subtle cognitive decline that can begin up to age-matched controls [14, 15]. Thus, consideration
30 years before dementia [1, 2]. This long preclinical of time-dependent learning curves and savings scores
period provides an opportunity for understanding the may provide greater sensitivity to A␤+ related mem-
clinical development of AD. As such, understanding ory impairment than is currently seen when using
how and when elevated A␤ disrupts episodic memory summary immediate and delayed recall scores.
is a central question to understanding the pathogene- Where appreciation of A␤+ related memory dis-
sis of AD. ruption requires verbal list learning tests be applied on
In preclinical AD, where abnormally high lev- multiple occasions, the extent to which performance
els of A␤ (A␤+) occur in the absence of clinical on these tests change with the repeated assess-
impairment, neuropsychological studies show that ments in A␤– adults also becomes important. For
while episodic memory is impaired, the magnitude is many standardized verbal list learning tests, repeated
relatively small (e.g., Cohen’s d of 0.15; [3]). By def- administration in older adults is associated with some
inition, episodic memory impairment is much greater improvement in performance (i.e., a practice effect),
in prodromal AD (A␤+ patients with mild cognitive with the magnitude of this varying as a function of the
impairment [MCI]), typically 1.5 standard devia- number of administrations, time between administra-
tions below age matched normative data [4]. In both tions, and number or equivalence of the alternative
preclinical and prodromal AD, episodic memory dys- forms [16–18]. Consequently, these practice effects
function becomes clearer over time, with progressive can obscure the presence and magnitude of A␤+
decline evident within prospective studies of preclin- related memory decline [18]. Therefore, to detect
ical and prodromal AD (d’s 0.20–0.50). In contrast, A␤+ related decline in episodic memory, it would
adults with low A␤ levels (A␤–) show no deteriora- be optimal to use measures of episodic memory for
tion, or even some improvement, in performance on which repeated administration does not give rise to
episodic memory tests over the same periods [5–8]. practice effects, rather, resulting in stable and reliable
The importance of time and of serial assessments estimates of cognition [19].
to detecting A␤+ related memory impairment in Recently we developed a computerized verbal list
non-demented older adults does raise the possibil- learning test with multiple parallel forms to enable
ity that consideration of time-dependent learning repeated administration over short periods of time
processes necessary for performance on episodic (International Shopping List Test; ISLT [19]). Unlike
memory tests in a single administration may increase other list learning tasks, for example, the Califor-
the sensitivity of those tests to early AD, compared to nia Verbal Learning Task (CVLT-second edition;
summary scores derived from the same performance. [20]), on each administration of the ISLT each word
J.E. Baker et al. / Impaired Learning in Early Alzheimer’s Disease 979

in the list is generated pseudo-randomly and with- where multiple assessments occur between routine
out replacement from a library of validated stimuli AIBL assessments. Recruitment, inclusion/exclusion
(n = 128). This means that at each assessment, partic- criteria, and measures used in the AIBL study
ipants receive lists consisting of different words, and have been described previously [23, 24]. Briefly,
any words used in an assessment are not repeated participants undergo neuropsychological, medical,
upon subsequent assessments. This method allows psychiatric and neurological examination at 18-
many alternate forms to be generated and controls month intervals [24, 25]. Clinical classification has
systematic error that can occur with different yet been described previously [24]; briefly, consensus
static word lists [19, 21, 22]. This use of random diagnoses were assigned to participants based on
word lists allows the ISLT to be given repeatedly at Winblad and Petersen criteria for MCI and NINCDS-
relatively short retest intervals without resulting in ADRDA criteria for AD [26, 27]. Clinical diagnoses
practice effects, and to generate data with high relia- were made blind to A␤ neuroimaging results. For the
bility (e.g., r’s for total score >0.80) in both healthy purposes of the current study, clinical groups were
younger and older adults and also in MCI and AD based on combined clinical and neuroimaging results
patients [22, 23]. Further, ISLT outcome measures of the AIBL ROCS sub-study, therefore 68 of the
can be separated into specific aspects of memory total sample were removed, as they had no imaging
(e.g., acquisition and retention; [22]). Quantitative data. Also excluded were 12 individuals with a diag-
and qualitative analysis of ISLT performance may nosis of dementia, 12 individuals with a diagnosis
therefore be useful to clarify the nature and magnitude of MCI who were A␤–, and healthy participants who
of episodic memory impairment in the earliest stages scored outside the bounds of clinically normal (n = 7).
of AD. However, the extent to which A␤+ disrupts Therefore, the sample comprised 50 A␤– cognitively
ISLT performance at baseline (over learning trials) normal (CN) older adults, 25 A␤+ CN older adults
or over repeated assessments in both preclinical and (preclinical AD), and 22 A␤+ adults MCI (prodro-
prodromal individuals remains unknown. mal AD). Demographic and clinical characteristics
The first aim of this study was to determine whether of these groups are shown in Table 1.
a greater focus on time dependent aspects of learn-
ing and memory could improve the sensitivity of Assessments
verbal list learning, over summary recall scores, to
A␤+ related memory impairment in early AD. The Demographic and clinical characteristics
second aim was to understand the nature and magni- Demographic information was collected at the
tude of A␤+ related disruption to verbal learning and baseline assessment. Age, gender, and medical his-
memory over 10 assessments of the ISLT over 18 tory were self-reported. The Wechsler Test of Adult
months. We hypothesized that A␤+ would be asso- Reading (WTAR; [28]) was used to estimate the pre-
ciated with disruption to the acquisition and delayed morbid intelligence of participants. Clinical disease
recall of information in early AD, with this impair- severity was rated using the Clinical Dementia Rating
ment greater than that seen using summary scores. scale (CDR; [29]). The Mini Mental State Exami-
We also hypothesized that A␤+ would be associated nation (MMSE; [30]) was used to screen for global
with a deterioration in verbal learning and memory cognitive function. APOE genotype was determined
over 18 months, while no change would be evident from genotyping of blood. Assessment of depressive
in A␤– individuals over the same period. and anxiety symptoms was conducted using Hospital
Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS; [31]).

METHODS Neuroimaging
A␤ imaging with positron emission tomography
Participants (PET) was conducted using one of three radioligands,
that is, Pittsburgh Compound B (PiB), florbetapir, or
Participants in the current sample were recruited flutemetamol. The acquisition protocol for each radi-
from the Australian Imaging Biomarkers and oligand has been detailed previously [25]. Briefly,
Lifestyle Rate of Change Sub-study (AIBL-ROCS), a 30-min acquisition was started 40 min after PiB-
a prospective cohort study of 196 older adults who injection, and 20-min acquisitions were performed
are cognitively normal or diagnosed with MCI or AD 50 min after florbetapir injection and 90 min after
980 J.E. Baker et al. / Impaired Learning in Early Alzheimer’s Disease

Table 1
Baseline demographic and clinical characteristics of each clinical group
A␤– CN (n = 50) Preclinical AD (n = 25) Prodromal AD (n = 22)
% Female 60% 52% 64%
% APOE ␧4 10% 24% 55%∗∗
Age 70.72 (5.81) 75.24 (9.47)∗ 79.45 (6.13)∗∗
Premorbid IQ 109.48 (5.02) 109.04 (7.50) 109.41 (5.84)
MMSE 29.44 (0.73) 28.96 (1.31) 27.36 (1.59)∗∗
CDR Sum of boxes 0 (0.5) 0 (0) 0.5 (3)∗∗
HADS Depression 2.44 (2.43) 1.55 (1.34) 3.95 (2.95)∗
HADS Anxiety 4.38 (2.42) 3.00 (2.56) 4.05 (2.28)
CVLT-II Total 55.30 (11.03) 53.38 (9.75) 36.34 (9.80)∗∗
CVLT-II Delayed 12.65 (4.31) 12.14 (3.80) 5.06 (3.85)∗∗
LM Immediate 13.71 (5.02) 13.81 (4.50) 8.97 (4.50)∗∗
LM Delay 12.44 (5.67) 12.44 (5.00) 6.07 (5.02)∗∗
Note. Means (SD). CDR-SB reported as median and range. Reference group for all pairwise comparisons
was the A␤– CN group. MMSE, Mini-Mental Status Examination; CDR-SB, Clinical dementia rating scale,
sum of boxes; HADS, Hospital anxiety and depression scale; CVLT-II, California Verbal Learning Test,
Second edition; LM, Wechsler Logical memory. ∗ p < 0.05; ∗∗ p < 0.001.

flutemetamol injection. For PiB acquisition, stan- classification of clinical disease status for any partic-
dardized uptake value (SUV) data for key regions of ipant.
interest were summed and normalized to the cerebel-
lar cortex SUV. This resulted in a region-to-cerebellar AIBL verbal learning and memory tests
ratio which was termed SUV ratio (SUVR). For Classification of verbal episodic memory impair-
florbetapir, SUVR was generated using the whole ment in the AIBL cohort is based on performance
cerebellum as the reference region and for flutemeta- on the California Verbal Learning Test, Second Edi-
mol, the pons was used as the reference region [32]. tion (CVLT-II) and on a modified administration of
Consistent with previous studies, A␤ status was clas- the Logical Memory (LM) subtest of the Wechsler
sified as either low (A␤–) or high (A␤+). For PiB, an Memory Scale – Revised (WMS -R). The CVLT-II is
SUVR threshold ≥1.4 was used [32, 33]. For florbe- a 16-item verbal list learning task consisting of five
tapir and flutemetamol, an SUVR threshold of ≥1.1 learning trials, immediate free and cued recall trials,
and ≥0.62 were employed to discriminate between and delayed recall and recognition trials [20]. For the
A␤– and A␤+, in accord with results of phase III purposes of this study, the total of the five learning
studies. trials and delayed free recall trial were used. Higher
scores indicate better performance. The LM subtest is
a paragraph story recall task in which participants are
The International Shopping List Test (ISLT) read a short story and are required to repeat as much
The ISLT is a 12-item verbal list-learning task con- information as they can remember, both immediately
sisting of three learning trials and a delayed recall after being read the story (LM-I) and after a 30-min
trial [19]. Outcome measures include total number of delay (LM-II; [28]). Both tests were administered in
words recalled at each trial, delayed recall, and a sav- the context of the entire AIBL neuropsychological
ings score (delayed recall/trial 3), where higher scores battery [24] and both are used for clinical disease
equal better performance [22]. For each administra- staging in the study.
tion of the ISLT the computer program draws 12
words pseudo-randomly (without replacement) from Procedure
a set of validated stimuli (n = 128). Thus, participants
receive lists consisting of different words, with words The procedure for the AIBL-ROCS has been
used in one assessment, not repeated upon subse- described previously [23]. Briefly, participants from
quent assessments for that individual. The reliability the AIBL study were recruited for a series of repeated
of the total and delayed recall scores from the ISLT assessments over short test-retest intervals over an
in healthy populations, as well as those with MCI and 18-month period. Participants were assessed once per
AD is uniformly high (rICC = 0.83–0.94), while trial month for four months, then at six months, and then
by trial reliability is slightly lower (rICC = 0.69–0.92) every three months up to 18 months. This proce-
[19]. Performance on the ISLT was not used in the dure was utilized in part to mirror the designs used
J.E. Baker et al. / Impaired Learning in Early Alzheimer’s Disease 981

in clinical trials of new medicines for AD, where those measures showing sensitivity to A␤+ in the
in some cases repeated assessments occur at shorter learning curve analyses were compared between
retest intervals which then increase with time (e.g., groups using a series of planned comparisons set
[34, 35]). An initial training assessment was con- within a one-way ANOVA, i.e., between A␤– CN and
ducted to prepare participants for regular home visits preclinical AD, and between preclinical and prodro-
and to familiarize them with the computerized ver- mal AD. Measures of effect sizes (Cohen’s d) were
sion of a list-learning task, and therefore the second used to express the magnitude of difference from the
assessment was considered the baseline. Assessments comparison group for each measure.
were conducted either at the participants’ home, a
location nearby, or at the Mental Health Research Change in episodic memory over time
Institute, in Parkville, Australia. During the time To test the second hypothesis that assessing change
between the initial list presentation of the ISLT and in memory over time using multiple repeated assess-
the delayed recall trial (approximately 30 min), sev- ments would allow characterization of A␤+ related
eral other non-verbal computerized cognitive tasks memory change in preclinical AD, the second anal-
were administered. yses proceeded in two stages. First, the three main
performance measures for the ISLT, identified from
Data analysis the baseline analyses, were submitted to a series of
linear mixed model (LMM) analyses using maximum
All analyses were conducted in RStudio [36] using likelihood estimation and an unstructured covariance
R version 3.3.1 [37]. Packages used included “afex” matrix. In the LMM, group, time, and group x time
for analysis of repeated measures [38], “lme4” for interaction were entered as fixed factors; participant
linear mixed model analysis [39], and “ggplot2” for entered as a random factor; age, APOE ␧4 status, and
data visualization [40]. depression subscale of the HADS as covariates (for-
mula = score ∼ group*time + age + apoe + hadsd +
Differences in demographic or clinical (1|id)). From these analyses, the magnitude of dif-
characteristics ference between groups in the estimates of rate of
Differences between groups on relevant demo- change for each summary ISLT were expressed as
graphic and clinical characteristics were explored Cohen’s d. Due to the possibility of distribution of
using a series of one-way ANOVAs for continu- performance on verbal list learning tests to be biased
ous variables and chi-square tests for non-continuous towards ceiling effects within healthy populations
and categorical variables (e.g., CDR, APOE status, and floor effects within those with objective mem-
and sex). Characteristics for which group differences ory impairment when using parametric statistical
were identified at the p < 0.05 threshold were then methods, we repeated analyses using non-parametric
added to subsequent analyses as covariates. quantile regression. This allowed investigation of
the presence of distribution biases within the
Group differences in episodic memory at baseline ISLT performance measures (see Supplementary
To test the hypothesis that learning and recall in Material).
preclinical AD would be qualitatively similar but
quantitatively different from prodromal AD, while RESULTS
also different from A␤– CN adults, the analysis pro-
ceeded in two stages. First, learning curves on the Group differences in demographic and clinical
ISLT trials including the delayed recall trial were characteristics
compared between groups by submitting the num-
ber of words recalled on each trial to a 3 × 4 (group Statistically significant group differences were
[A␤– CN, preclinical AD, prodromal AD] × trial identified for proportion of APOE ␧4 carriers,
[One, Two, Three, Delayed]) mixed design ANOVA age, and the depression subscale of the HADS
(formula = score ∼ group + age + apoe + hadsd + (Table 1); thus, subsequent analyses included
Error(id/(trial)). Statistically significant group × trial these variables as covariates. As expected, groups
interactions were decomposed using interaction con- also differed significantly with respect to MMSE
trasts. Generalized eta-squared (␩2G ) was used to and CDR scores. Comparison of the A␤– CN
represent the magnitude of the interaction. Second, group to the preclinical AD group indicated
summary ISLT performance measures, derived from no statistically significant differences in perfor-
982 J.E. Baker et al. / Impaired Learning in Early Alzheimer’s Disease

mance in CVLT-II total (d = –0.18[–0.66, 0.30]), contrasts. The first interaction contrast compared
CVLT-II delayed (d = –0.12[–0.60, 0.36]), LM-I learning across trials between the A␤– CN group and
(d = 0.02[–0.50, 0.46]) or LM-II (d = 0.00[–0.48, the preclinical AD group. No significant group × trial
0.48]) scores. As expected, compared to the interaction was observed (p = 0.24, d = 0.16) and for
A␤– CN group, the prodromal AD group per- both groups, total words recalled improved across
formed significantly worse on all aspects of the the three learning trials, F(2.66, 186.25) = 4.65,
CVLT-II and LM tests (CVLT-total d = –1.78[–2.35, p = 0.005, d = 0.29. Savings for the A␤– CNs were
–1.20], CVLT-delay d = –1.82[–2.40, –1.24]; LM- approximately 5% greater than in the preclinical AD
I d = –0.97[–1.50, –0.45], LM-II d = –1.16[–1.70, group, although the difference was not significant
–0.63]). Similarly, compared to the preclinical AD (Table 2). The second interaction contrast compared
group, the prodromal AD group performed signif- the rate of learning across trials between the pre-
icantly worse on CVLT-II total (d = –1.74[–2.41, clinical AD group to the prodromal AD group and
–1.07]), CVLT-II delayed recall (d = –1.85[–2.53, a significant group × trial interaction was observed,
–1.17]), LM-I (d = –1.08[–1.69, –0.46]), and LM-II F(3, 105) = 10.10, p < 0.001, d = 0.46. Compared to
(d = –1.27[–1.90, –0.65]). the preclinical AD group, the prodromal AD group
recalled fewer words on each trial (Fig. 1). The
Group differences in learning at baseline prodromal AD group had significantly less savings
(approximately –36%) compared to the preclinical
Mean words recalled on each of the three ISLT AD group (Table 2).
trials are shown in Fig. 1. The mixed ANOVA Table 2 summarizes the comparisons of the ISLT
revealed a statistically significant group × trial inter- learning trials, delayed recall, and savings scores
action, F(5.50, 231.16) = 6.71, p < 0.001, η2G = 0.04. between groups. ANOVAs indicated significant dif-
This effect was decomposed using two interaction ferences between groups on all three metrics, and
this effect was driven by the prodromal AD group
(Table 2). Cohen’s d effect sizes showed the magni-
tude of the difference between the preclinical AD and
A␤– CN groups were small for all summary metrics
(Cohen’s d’s < 0.25). In contrast, the magnitude of the
difference between the preclinical AD and prodromal
AD groups was very large (Cohen’s d’s > 1.50).

Group differences in rate of change in episodic


memory over 18 months

The results of the LMMs of ISLT performance


scores are summarized in Table 3 and shown graph-
Fig. 1. Mean words recalled for each clinical group at each ISLT ically in Figs. 2–4. Group mean slopes derived
learning trial and delayed recall at baseline. Error bars repre- from these models are also presented in Table 3.
sent 95% confidence intervals. Analyses adjusted for age, HADS A significant group × time interaction was observed
depression, and APOE ␧4 carriage. No significant differences
between the performance of the A␤– CN and preclinical AD group.
for ISLT total recall. Compared to the A␤– CN
Prodromal AD group significantly impaired across trials and at group, the preclinical AD group showed significantly
delayed recall. less change over time with the difference moder-

Table 2
Group mean performance on ISLT measures at baseline
Mean (SD) Cohen’s d [95% Confidence intervals]
Outcome A␤– CN Preclinical Prodromal A␤– CN versus Preclinical AD
AD AD Preclinical AD versus Prodromal AD
ISLT Total 26.92 (5.02) 26.78 (4. 55) 18.47 (4.55) –0.03 [–0.51, 0.45] –1.84 [–2.52, –1.16]∗∗
Delayed 9.47 (2.69) 8.78 (2.40) 3.81 (2.44) –0.27 [–0.75, 0.22] –2.05 [–2.76, –1.35]∗∗
Savings 90.88 (23.69) 85.40 (21.20) 49.13 (21.53) –0.24 [–0.72, 0.24] –1.70 [–2.36, –1.03]∗∗
Note. Means (SD) are adjusted for the covariates of age, HADS Depression, and APOE ␧4 status. ∗∗ p < 0.001.
J.E. Baker et al. / Impaired Learning in Early Alzheimer’s Disease 983

Table 3
Results of linear mixed model analyses examining the ISLT outcome measures over 18 months, and mean slopes (SD) for each group
Outcome Group Time Group × time A␤– CN Preclinical AD Prodromal AD
(df) F Slope (SD)
Total (122.32) 28.88∗∗ (596.53) 3.43 (596.14) 9.34∗∗ 0.07 (0.18)∗∗ –0.06 (0.19) –0.13 (0.19)∗∗
Delayed recall (112.23) 34.90∗∗ (595.22) 1.89 (594.89) 4.24∗ 0.02 (0.09) –0.01 (0.10) –0.05 (0.09)∗
Savings (138.52) 22.88∗∗ (597.17) 1.16 (596.69) 0.81 –0.04 (1.06) 0.02 (1.14) –0.39 (1.24)
Note. Analyses covaried for age, HADS Depression, and APOE ␧4 carriage. ∗ p < 0.05; ∗∗ p < 0.001.

Fig. 2. Mean words recalled on the ISLT total score for the A␤– Fig. 4. Percentage of savings on the ISLT savings score for A␤–
CN, preclinical AD, and prodromal AD groups from baseline to 18 CN, preclinical and prodromal AD groups from baseline to 18
months. Baseline is represented by the second assessment as the months. Baseline is represented by the second assessment as the
first was an initial training assessment. Shading represents 95% first was an initial training assessment. The shading represents
confidence intervals fitted via a linear smoothing function. Analy- 95% confidence intervals fitted via a linear smoothing function.
ses are adjusted for age, HADS depression, and APOE ␧4 carriage. Analyses are adjusted for age, HADS depression, and APOE ␧4
The preclinical and prodromal AD group slopes are significantly carriage. No change over the 18-month period was observed for
different to the A␤– CN group slope. A significant increase in any of the groups.
performance is also seen in the A␤– CN group.

group the difference in slopes was not signif-


icant, although moderate in magnitude (Cohen’s
d = –0.36[–0.94, 0.22]). A significant group x time
interaction was also observed for ISLT delayed recall.
Compared to the A␤– CN group, the preclinical
AD group did not show a significantly faster rate
of decline over time, but the effect size of differ-
ence in slopes was moderate in magnitude (Cohen’s
d = –0.37[–0.85, 0.11]). The prodromal AD group
showed significant decline over time, however, rel-
ative to the preclinical AD group the difference
in slopes was not significant, although of moder-
Fig. 3. Mean words recalled on ISLT delayed recall for A␤– ate magnitude (Cohen’s d = –0.42[–1.00, 0.16]). No
CN, preclinical, and prodromal AD groups from baseline to 18 significant group x time interaction was observed
months. Baseline is represented by the second assessment as the
first was an initial training assessment. Shading represents 95%
for the ISLT savings score (Table 3, Fig. 2c). Re-
confidence intervals fitted via a linear smoothing function. Analy- analysis of the prospective data for the ISLT using the
ses are adjusted for age, HADS depression, and APOE ␧4 carriage. non-parametric quantile regression yielded a pattern
No significant difference in the slopes between the A␤– CN and of outcomes consistent with the parametric meth-
preclinical AD groups. The prodromal AD group significantly
declined over the 18 months. ods (see Supplementary Table 1 and Supplementary
Figures 1–3). This indicates that outcomes of the
ate in magnitude (Cohen’s d = –0.63[–1.12, –0.14]). linear mixed models were not influenced by any
The prodromal AD group showed significant decline group-based biases in data distributions on the ISLT
over time, however, relative to the preclinical AD outcome measures (see Supplementary Material).
984 J.E. Baker et al. / Impaired Learning in Early Alzheimer’s Disease

DISCUSSION assessed on a single occasion, but do show decline


when assessed over time, indicates that while A␤+
The first hypothesis that the acquisition and recall related disruption to medial temporal lobe areas
of verbal information would be impaired in early begins very early in the AD process, this takes a long
AD in the presence of A␤+ was partially supported. time to manifest clinically [41, 42, 46–48]. Thus, this
At the baseline assessment, no differences in acqui- preclinical phase of AD is agreed upon to provide
sition, total recall, delayed recall, or the savings an opportunity to intervene so to prevent further neu-
scores on the ISLT were observed in the preclini- ronal loss and forestall the development of AD [49].
cal AD group. Furthermore, effect sizes reflecting These data also suggest that the slow course of A␤+
differences in performance between the groups for related memory loss continues through the preclini-
each of these measures were uniformly small (i.e., cal stage until it becomes severe enough to warrant
Cohen’s ds < 0.25), equivalent to estimates of mem- clinical classification of MCI, whereupon clinically
ory performance detected using the CVLT-II and LM meaningful memory impairment becomes obvious at
tests (i.e., Cohen’s ds < 0.20), and consistent with the a single assessment. The A␤+ related decline then
magnitude of impairments in memory in preclinical continues steadily throughout the prodromal phase.
AD observed in previous reports [7, 41, 42]. This Previous studies suggest that A␤+ related memory
indicates that when assessed on a single occasion, decline is correlated with loss of volume in the MTL
preclinical AD is not characterized by impairment in [50–52]. However, even with this decline in memory,
episodic memory even when the measures of mem- individuals with MCI remain able to live indepen-
ory are analyzed to consider both the acquisition dently and by definition have difficulty only with the
and retention of verbal information. In contrast, the most demanding of daily living tasks [4, 10, 53]. Stop-
prodromal AD group showed substantial impairment ping AD in the prodromal or preclinical stages could
(i.e., Cohen’s ds > 1.50) on all measures of the ISLT, therefore provide enormous benefit to individuals.
equivalent to impairment evident on the CVLT-II and Although the ISLT was designed to minimize
LM tests (Cohen’s ds > 1). Thus, for the preclinical practice effects from repeated administration, using
and prodromal AD groups, analyses of acquisition multiple parallel forms gained from randomization of
and retention did not provide estimates of memory word lists upon each assessment, the A␤– CN group
impairment that were greater than the ISLT summary did show a slight improvement, of approximately one
scores or the CVLT-II and LM scores. word, over the eight assessments conducted in the 18-
The second hypothesis that with repeated assess- months (Table 3). This slight improvement in ISLT
ment, episodic memory would deteriorate in A␤+ performance in A␤– CN adults was most likely a
individuals, but not change in those without A␤, consequence of the high frequency of ISLT admin-
was also partially supported. The preclinical AD istration in the study, as no improvements in ISLT
group showed a decline in the ISLT total recall score performance were observed in previous studies where
over the 18-months that, when compared to the A␤– it was administered four times over three months in
CN group, was moderate in magnitude (Cohen’s CN older adults and three times in a single setting in
d = –0.63). A decline of equivalent magnitude in ver- younger adults [19, 54]. Additionally, performance
bal list learning was evident in the prodromal AD on the ISLT has remained stable between two assess-
group on the ISLT measures of total and delayed ments over a period of one month in participants with
recall (Cohen’s ds –0.36 and –0.42, respectively). clinically diagnosed AD [22]. While the results of this
This decline in performance on the ISLT is con- study suggest that even with optimization for repeated
sistent with the decline on other tests of verbal use, eight administrations within an 18-month period
episodic memory in both preclinical and prodromal still results in practice effects in healthy individu-
AD observed previously in other natural history sam- als, the magnitude of the practice effect on the ISLT
ples [8, 43–45]. These data therefore indicate that (d = 0.10) was much less than in previous reports. For
estimates of cognitive dysfunction in the preclinical example, moderate improvements on the Neuropsy-
stage of AD are most obvious as change over time chological Assessment Battery (NAB) list learning
based on repeated assessments, when compared to task and the Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT)
individuals without A␤+. have been observed with fewer administrations, equal
The observation here, and elsewhere, that indi- to a 0.39 standard deviation annual increase [55], and
viduals with preclinical AD show no substantial an improvement of d = 0.51 after three assessments
impairment in verbal learning and memory when in 30 months, respectively [56]. Indeed, the absence
J.E. Baker et al. / Impaired Learning in Early Alzheimer’s Disease 985

of practice effects from repeated administration of population, as the study utilizes detailed inclusion and
memory tests has been proposed to be characteristic exclusion criteria to ensure minimal untreated comor-
of early AD. While the current study aimed to mini- bidities. Third, given that many AIBL participants
mize practice effects through the use of the ISLT in have had prior exposure to cognitive testing, replica-
order to gain reliable estimates of A␤+ associated tion in a sample naı̈ve to cognitive testing would help
cognitive change, previous work has shown that the to increase generalization to the broader population.
degree of practice effects can provide an understand- Fourth, due to the schedule of testing and necessary
ing of disease progression. For example, reduced home visits for the current study, the sample size was
practice effects were five times more likely in individ- small and as such, replication of the current results in
uals with A␤+ compared to those without A␤ [57]. a larger sample is warranted.
Similarly, the magnitude of practice effects across Notwithstanding these limitations, our findings
three consecutive memory assessments was signifi- highlight an issue with current models attempting
cantly less in those who progressed to clinical disease to understand subtle cognitive dysfunction in pre-
than in those that did not (d = 0.07 and 0.33, respec- clinical AD populations using neuropsychological
tively) [58]. However, in each case, these estimates methods created for the detection of impairment.
of A␤+ related dysfunction were based on a reduc- They underscore the importance of shifting from
tion in the magnitude of practice effects, while still in examination of neuropsychological performance at
the presence of performance gains, rather than from one timepoint to characterizing change in cognitive
observation of a declining trajectory associated with processes over time. Ultimately, the ability to conduct
A␤+, as was shown in the current study (e.g., Fig. 2). repeated assessments over shorter time periods will
In the context of the hypothesis that memory dys- need to become the standard assessment protocol for
function in preclinical AD is more evident upon individuals at risk of AD to demonstrate meaning-
repeated assessment, it is worth considering that the ful change in cognitive function. Our results suggest
analysis of learning curves from the individual ISLT that even when using cognitive tasks with high test-
trials on the first administration could be consid- retest reliability, practice effects can be seen in A␤–
ered an index of practice, albeit over minutes rather CN adults but not in those with preclinical AD. As
than over days, months, or years. As no difference such, a potential way forward is to deliberately seek
in learning curves was observed between A␤– CN to improve performance in order to separate individ-
and preclinical AD groups at baseline (Fig. 1), it is uals who are able to improve from those who cannot,
likely that information about memory dysfunction in potentially leading to an increase in the reliability of
preclinical AD, here shown over 18 months, and in prediction of disease progression.
previous studies over a period of one week [57] or sev-
eral years [58], requires a time interval longer than
minutes to become evident. The reduction in practice ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
effects from high-frequency assessment in preclini-
cal AD groups comparative to CN A␤– older adults Funding for the study was provided by
suggests that memory dysfunction could be concep- AstraZeneca Pharmaceuticals LP, the CSIRO
tualized as a deficit in learning from repetition. As Flagship Collaboration Fund and the Science and
such, the assessment of memory utilizing primarily Industry Endowment Fund (SIEF) in partnership
learning-based episodic memory paradigms may pro- with Edith Cowan University (ECU), The Mental
vide a more effective method for characterization of Health Research Institute (MHRI), Alzheimer’s
this proposed deficit in preclinical AD. Australia (AA), National Ageing Research Institute
There are some caveats to the generalization of the (NARI), Austin Health, CogState Ltd, Hollywood
current results. First, the AIBL-ROCS sample was Private Hospital, Sir Charles Gairdner Hospital. The
selected specifically to undergo high frequency test- study also receives funding from the National Health
ing, and as such the repeated assessment schedule and Medical Research Council (NHMRC), the
employed in AIBL-ROCS does not reflect that typ- Dementia Collaborative Research Centres program
ically used in clinical practice. Future research will (DCRC), The McCusker Alzheimer’s Research
need to administer repeated assessments in a similar Foundation, and Operational Infrastructure Sup-
context to that of clinical practice to provide greater port from the Government of Victoria. The authors
generalizability of results. Second, the AIBL sam- acknowledge the financial support of the Cooperative
ple is unlikely to be representative of the general Research Centre (CRC) for Mental Health. The CRC
986 J.E. Baker et al. / Impaired Learning in Early Alzheimer’s Disease

programme is an Australian Government Initiative. [7] Doherty BM, Schultz SA, Oh JM, Koscik RL, Dowling
YYL reports grants from the National Health and NM, Barnhart TE, Murali D, Gallagher CL, Carlsson CM,
Bendlin BB, LaRue A, Hermann BP, Rowley HA, Asthana
Medical Research Council (GNT1111603, GNT S, Sager MA, Christian BT, Johnson SC, Okonkwo OC
1147465). The ROCS team wishes to thank the (2015) Amyloid burden, cortical thickness, and cognitive
participants in the ROCS study for their commitment function in the Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer’s Preven-
and dedication to helping advance research into tion. Alzheimers Dement (Amst) 1, 160-169.
[8] Donohue MC, Sperling RA, Petersen RC, Sun CK, Weiner
the early detection and causation of AD and the MW, Aisen PS; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Ini-
clinicians who referred patients to the study. tiative (2017) Association between elevated brain amyloid
Authors’ disclosures available online (https:// and subsequent cognitive decline among cognitively normal
www.j-alz.com/manuscript-disclosures/18-0344r1). persons. JAMA 317, 2305-2316.
[9] Strauss E, Sherman EMS, Spreen O (2006) A compendium
of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and
SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL commentary, Third Edition, Oxford University Press.
[10] Petersen RC (2004) Mild cognitive impairment as a diag-
nostic entity. J Intern Med 256, 183-194.
The supplementary material is available in the [11] Greenaway MC, Lacritz LH, Binegar D, Weiner MF, Lipton
electronic version of this article: http://dx.doi.org/ A, Munro Cullum C (2006) Patterns of verbal memory per-
10.3233/JAD-180344. formance in mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer disease,
and normal aging. Cogn Behav Neurol 19, 79-84.
[12] Ribeiro F, Guerreiro M, De Mendonça A (2007) Verbal
REFERENCES learning and memory deficits in mild cognitive impairment.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 29, 187-197.
[1] Jack CR Jr, Knopman DS, Jagust WJ, Petersen RC, [13] Perri R, Carlesimo GA, Serra L, Caltagirone C; Early Diag-
Weiner MW, Aisen PS, Shaw LM, Vemuri P, Wiste HJ, nosis Group of the Italian Interdisciplinary Network on
Weigand SD, Lesnick TG, Pankratz VS, Donohue MC, Tro- Alzheimer’s Disease (2005) Characterization of memory
janowski JQ (2013) Tracking pathophysiological processes profile in subjects with amnestic mild cognitive impairment.
in Alzheimer’s disease: An updated hypothetical model of J Clincial Exp Neuropsychol 27, 1033-1055.
dynamic biomarkers. Lancet 12, 207-216. [14] Libon DJ, Bondi MW, Price CC, Lamar M, Eppig J,
[2] Sperling RA, Aisen PS, Beckett LA, Bennett DA, Craft Wambach DM, Nieves C, Delano-Wood L, Giovannetti T,
S, Fagan AM, Iwatsubo T, Jack CR Jr, Kaye J, Montine Lippa C, Kabasakalian A, Consentino S, Swenson R, Pen-
TJ, Park DC, Reiman EM, Rowe CC, Siemers E, Stern Y, ney DL, Cosentino S, Swenson R, Penney DL (2011) Verbal
Yaffe K, Carrillo MC, Thies B, Morrison-Bogorad M, Wag- serial list learning in mild cognitive impairment: A profile
ster MV, Phelps CH (2011) Toward defining the preclinical analysis of interference, forgetting, and errors. J Int Neu-
stages of Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the ropsychol Soc 17, 905-914.
National Institute on Aging and the Alzheimer’s Associ- [15] Moulin CJ, James N, Freeman JE, Jones RW (2004) Defi-
ation workgroup on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s cient acquisition and consolidation: Intertrial free recall
disease. Alzheimers Dement 7, 280-292. performance in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive
[3] Baker JE, Lim YY, Pietrzak RH, Hassenstab J, Snyder impairment. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 26, 1-10.
PJ, Masters CL, Maruff P (2016) Cognitive impairment [16] Beglinger LJ, Gaydos B, Tangphao-Daniels O, Duff K,
and decline in cognitively normal older adults with high Kareken DA, Crawford J, Fastenau PS, Siemers ER (2005)
amyloid-␤: A meta-analysis. Alzheimers Dement (Amst) 6, Practice effects and the use of alternate forms in serial
108-121. neuropsychological testing. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 20,
[4] Albert MS, DeKosky ST, Dickson D, Dubois B, Feldman 517-529.
HH, Fox NC, Gamst A, Holtzman DM, Jagust WJ, Petersen [17] Calamia M, Markon K, Tranel D (2012) Scoring higher
RC, Snyder PJ, Carrillo MC, Thies B, Phelps CH (2011) The the second time around: Meta- analyses of practice effects
diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s in neuropsychological assessment. Clin Neuropsychol 26,
disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on 543-570.
Aging and Alzheimer’s Association workgroup on diagnos- [18] Goldberg TE, Harvey PD, Wesnes KA, Snyder PJ, Schnei-
tic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement der LS (2015) Practice effects due to serial cognitive
7, 270-279. assessment: Implications for preclinical Alzheimer’s dis-
[5] Lim YY, Snyder PJ, Pietrzak RH, Ukiqi A, Villemagne ease randomized controlled trials. Alzheimers Dement
VL, Ames D, Salvado O, Bourgeat P, Martins RN, Mas- (Amst) 1, 103-111.
ters CL, Rowe CC, Maruff P (2015) Sensitivity of composite [19] Lim YY, Harrington K, Ames D, Ellis KA., Lachovitzki
scores to amyloid burden in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: R, Snyder PJ, Maruff P (2012) Short term stability of
Introducing the Z-scores of Attention, Verbal fluency, and verbal memory impairment in mild cognitive impairment
Episodic memory for Nondemented older adults composite and Alzheimer’s disease measured using the Interna-
score. Alzheimers Dement (Amst) 2, 19-26. tional Shopping List Test. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 34,
[6] Lim YY, Maruff P, Pietrzak RH, Ames D, Ellis KA, Harring- 853-863.
ton K, Lautenschlager NT, Szoeke C, Martins RN, Masters [20] Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA (2000) CVLT-II:
CL, Villemagne VL, Rowe CC; AIBL Research Group California verbal learning test: Adult version, Psychologi-
(2014) Effect of amyloid on memory and non-memory cal Corporation.
decline from preclinical to clinical Alzheimer’s disease. [21] Lim YY, Prang KH, Cysique L, Pietrzak RH, Snyder PJ,
Brain 137, 221-231. Maruff P (2009) A method for cross-cultural adaptation of
J.E. Baker et al. / Impaired Learning in Early Alzheimer’s Disease 987

a verbal memory assessment. Behav Res Methods 41, 1190- [34] Pietrzak RH, Maruff P, Snyder PJ (2009) Methodological
1200. improvements in quantifying cognitive change in clini-
[22] Thompson TA, Wilson PH, Snyder PJ, Pietrzak RH, Darby cal trials: An example with single-dose administration of
D, Maruff P, Buschke H (2011) Sensitivity and test-retest donepezil. J Nutr Health Aging 13, 268-273.
reliability of the international shopping list test in assessing [35] Atri A, Frölich L, Ballard C, Tariot PN, Molinuevo JL,
verbal learning and memory in mild Alzheimer’s disease. Boneva N, Windfeld K, Raket LL, Cummings JL (2018)
Arch Clin Neuropsychol 26, 412-424. Effect of idalopirdine as adjunct to cholinesterase inhibitors
[23] Lim YY, Jaeger J, Harrington K, Ashwood T, Ellis KA, on change in cognition in patients with Alzheimer disease:
Stöffler A, Szoeke C, Lachovitzki R, Martins RN, Ville- Three randomized clinical trials. JAMA 319, 130-142.
magne VL, Bush A, Masters CL, Rowe CC, Ames D, Darby [36] RStudio Team (2015) RStudio: Integrated development for
D, Maruff P (2013) Three-month stability of the CogState R. RStudio, Inc., Boston, MA.
brief battery in healthy older adults, mild cognitive impair- [37] R Core Team (2015) R: A language and environment for
ment, and Alzheimer’s disease: Results from the Australian statistical computing. RStudio, Inc., Boston, MA.
imaging, Biomarkers, and Lifestyle-rate of change substudy [38] Singman H, Bolker B, Westfall J, Aust F (2017) afex: Anal-
(AIBL-ROCS). Arch Clin Neuropsychol 28, 320-330. ysis of factorial experiments.
[24] Ellis KA, Bush AI, Darby D, De Fazio D, Foster J, Hud- [39] Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S (2015) Fitting
son P, Lautenschlager NT, Lenzo N, Martins RN, Maruff P, linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw 67,
Masters C, Milner A, Pike K, Rowe C, Savage G, Szoeke 1-48.
C, Taddei K, Villemagne V, Woodward M, Ames D; AIBL [40] Wickham H (2009) ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data
Research Group (2009) The Australian Imaging, Biomark- Analysis. Springer-Verlag, New York.
ers and Lifestyle (AIBL) study of aging: Methodology and [41] Lim YY, Ellis KA, Harrington K, Kamer A, Pietrzak RH,
baseline characteristics of 1112 individuals recruited for a Bush AI, Darby D, Martins RN, Masters CL, Rowe CC,
longitudinal study of Alzheimer’s disease. Int Psychogeriatr Savage G, Szoeke C, Villemagne VL, Ames D, Maruff P
21, 672-687. (2013) Cognitive consequences of high A␤ amyloid in mild
[25] Rowe CC, Ellis KA, Rimajova M, Bourgeat P, Pike KE, cognitive impairment and healthy older adults: Implications
Jones G, Fripp J, Tochon-Danguy H, Morandeau L, O’Keefe for early detection of Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology
G, Price R, Raniga P, Robins P, Acosta O, Lenzo N, Szoeke 27, 322-332.
C, Salvado O, Head R, Martins R, Masters CL, Ames D, [42] Hassenstab J, Chasse R, Grabow P, Benzinger TL, Fagan
Villemagne VL (2010) Amyloid imaging results from the AM, Xiong C, Jasielec M, Grant E, Morris JC (2016) Certi-
Australian Imaging, Biomarkers and Lifestyle (AIBL) study fied normal: Alzheimer’s disease biomarkers and normative
of aging. Neurobiol Aging 31, 1275-1283. estimates of cognitive functioning. Neurobiol Aging 43, 23-
[26] Winblad B, Palmer K, Kivipelto M, Jelic V, Fratiglioni L, 33.
Wahlund LO, Nordberg A, Bäckman L, Albert M, Almkvist [43] Clark LR, Racine AM, Koscik RL, Okonkwo OC, Engel-
O, Arai H, Basun H, Blennow K, de Leon M, DeCarli C, man CD, Carlsson CM, Asthana S, Bendlin BB, Chappell R,
Erkinjuntti T, Giacobini E, Graff C, Hardy J, Jack C, Jorm Nicholas CR, Rowley HA, Oh JM, Hermann BP, Sager MA,
A, Ritchie K, van Duijn C, Visser P, Petersen RC (2004) Christian BT, Johnson SC (2016) Beta-amyloid and cogni-
Mild cognitive impairment–beyond controversies, towards tive decline in late middle age: Findings from the Wisconsin
a consensus: Report of the International Working Group on Registry for Alzheimer’s Prevention study. Alzheimers
Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Intern Med 256, 240-246. Dement 12, 805-814.
[27] Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos [44] Farrell ME, Kennedy KM, Rodrigue KM, Wig G, Bischof
EG, Kokmen E (1999) Mild cognitive impairment: Clinical GN, Rieck JR, Chen X, Festini SB, Devous MD Sr, Park DC
characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol 56, 303-308. (2017) Association of longitudinal cognitive decline with
[28] Wechsler D (1997) Wechsler memory scale - Third edition, amyloid burden in middle-aged and older adults: Evidence
The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX. for a dose-response relationship. JAMA Neurol 74, 830-838.
[29] Morris JC (1993) The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): [45] Petersen RC, Wiste HJ, Weigand SD, Rocca WA, Roberts
Current version and scoring rules. Neurology 43, 2412- RO, Mielke MM, Lowe VJ, Knopman DS, Pankratz VS,
2414. Machulda MM, Geda YE, Jack CR Jr (2016) Association
[30] Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR (1975) “Mini-mental of elevated amyloid levels with cognition and biomarkers
state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of in cognitively normal people from the community. JAMA
patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12, 189-198. Neurol 73, 85-92.
[31] Snaith RP, Zigmond AS (1986) The hospital anxiety and [46] Mielke MM, Machulda MM, Hagen CE, Christianson TJ,
depression scale. Br Med J (Clin Res Ed) 292, 344. Roberts RO, Knopman DS, Vemuri P, Lowe VJ, Kremers
[32] Villemagne VL, Burnham S, Bourgeat P, Brown B, Ellis WK, Jack CR Jr, Petersen RC (2016) Influence of amyloid
KA, Salvado O, Szoeke C, Macaulay SL, Martins R, Maruff and APOE on cognitive performance in a late middle-aged
P, Ames D, Rowe CC, Masters CL; Australian Imaging cohort. Alzheimers Dement 12, 281-291.
Biomarkers and Lifestyle (AIBL) Research Group (2013) [47] Papp KV, Mormino EC, Amariglio RE, Munro C, Dagley A,
Amyloid ␤ deposition, neurodegeneration, and cognitive Schultz AP, Johnson KA, Sperling RA, Rentz DM (2016)
decline in sporadic Alzheimer’s disease: A prospective Biomarker validation of a decline in semantic processing in
cohort study. Lancet Neurol 12, 357-367. preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology 30, 624-
[33] Jack CR Jr, Lowe VJ, Senjem ML, Weigand SD, Kemp BJ, 630.
Shiung MM, Knopman DS, Boeve BF, Klunk WE, Mathis [48] Insel PS, Donohue MC, Mackin RS, Aisen PS, Hans-
CA, Petersen RC (2008) 11C PiB and structural MRI pro- son O, Weiner MW, Mattsson N; Alzheimer’s Disease
vide complementary information in imaging of Alzheimer’s Neuroimaging Initiative (2016) Cognitive and functional
disease and amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Brain 131, changes associated with A␤ pathology and the progression
665-680. to mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol Aging 48, 172-181.
988 J.E. Baker et al. / Impaired Learning in Early Alzheimer’s Disease

[49] Sperling RA, Rentz DM, Johnson KA, Karlawish J, Dono- memory: A comparison of international shopping list task
hue M, Salmon DP, Aisen P (2014) The A4 study: Stopping and rey auditory verbal learning test on build-up of proactive
AD before symptoms begin? Sci Transl Med 6, 228fs13. interference. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 27, 790-795.
[50] Marks SM, Lockhart SN, Baker SL, Jagust WJ (2017) Tau [55] Gavett BE, Gurnani AS, Saurman JL, Chapman KR, Stein-
and ␤-amyloid are associated with medial temporal lobe berg EG, Martin B, Chaisson CE, Mez J, Tripodis Y, Stern
structure, function, and memory encoding in normal aging. RA (2016) Practice effects on story memory and list learning
J Neurosci 37, 3192-3201. tests in the neuropsychological assessment of older adults.
[51] Mattsson N, Insel PS, Aisen PS, Jagust W, Mackin S, Weiner PLoS One 11, e0164492.
M; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (2015) [56] Machulda MM, Hagen CE, Wiste HJ, Mielke MM, Knop-
Brain structure and function as mediators of the effects of man DS, Roberts RO, Vemuri P, Lowe VJ, Jack CR Jr,
amyloid on memory. Neurology 84, 1136-1144. Petersen RC (2017) Practice effects and longitudinal cog-
[52] Wang L, Benzinger TL, Hassenstab J, Blazey T, Owen C, nitive change in clinically normal older adults differ by
Liu J, Fagan AM, Morris JC, Ances BM (2015) Spatially Alzheimer imaging biomarker status. Clin Neuropsychol 31,
distinct atrophy is linked to ␤-amyloid and tau in preclinical 99-117.
Alzheimer disease. Neurology 84, 1254-1260. [57] Duff K, Foster NL, Hoffman JM (2014) Practice effects
[53] Petersen RC, Parisi JE, Dickson DW, Johnson KA, Knop- and amyloid deposition: Preliminary data on a method for
man DS, Boeve BF, Jicha GA, Ivnik RJ, Smith GE, Tangalos enriching samples in clinical trials. Alzheimer Dis Assoc
EG, Braak H, Kokmen E (2006) Neuropathologic features Disord 28, 247-252.
of amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol 63, [58] Hassenstab J, Ruvolo D, Jasielec M, Xiong C, Grant E,
665-672. Morris JC (2015) Absence of practice effects in preclinical
[54] Rahimi-Golkhandan S, Maruff P, Darby D, Wilson P (2012) Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology 29, 940-948.
Barriers to repeated assessment of verbal learning and

You might also like