You are on page 1of 4

Journal ol Personality and Sochi Psychology

1974, Vol. 30, No. S, 654-657

ALPHABET AND ATTRACTION:


AN UNOBTRUSIVE MEASURE OF THE EFFECT OF
PROPINQUITY IN A FIELD SETTING1

MADY WECHSLER SEGAL 2


Department of Sociology, Eastern Michigan University

The relationship between propinquity and interpersonal attraction was studied


in a field setting. Place in alphabetical order was used as an unobtrusive
(and conservative) measure of propinquity. The effect of proximity on friend-
ship choice among 44 Maryland State Police trainees was found to be stronger
than the effects of a wide variety of other characteristics that have been
identified in the literature as determinants of interpersonal attraction.

Studies of interpersonal attraction have propinquity affects attraction, in a field set-


frequently found that attraction is affected by ting, using an unobtrusive measure of pro-
propinquity: The smaller the physical and pinquity.
functional distance between two people, the
METHOD
more likely they are to be attracted to each
other. Support for the propinquity-attrac- In 1966 a questionnaire was mailed to the $2
tion hypothesis is found in the research con- students in the Training Academy of the Maryland
State Police as part of a larger study of the
ducted by Byrne and Buehler (19SS), Fest- social organization and professionalization of the
inger, Schachter, and Back (1950), Kipnis force (Smith, 1968). The trainees (all of whom
(1957), Maisonneuve, Palmade, and Four- were male) had known each other for approxi-
ment (1952), and Priest and Sawyer (1967), mately six weeks at the time of the study. Seven of
these students dropped out of the Academy and
among others. therefore did not return their questionnaires. Only
Several theoretical approaches lead to pre- one student who remained in the Academy failed to
dictions of a positive relationship between respond to the questionnaire.
propinquity and interpersonal attraction. All but one of the 44 respondents were white.
Among these are exchange theory (Homans, They were all between the ages of 21 and 30 years
with a mean age of 23.4 and a median of 23. One
1961), balance and consistency theories man (2.3%) reported no religious preference, 31
(Heider, 1958; Newcomb, 1956), and rein- (70.5%) were Protestant, and 12 (27.3%) were
forcement theory (Berscheid & Walster, Catholic. There were 26 married men (59.1%) and
1969). The propinquity-attraction relation- 18 single men (40.9%). The modal family income
category was from $5,000 to $5,999 (n = 20 or
ship is often assumed to be mediated by in- 45.5%), with only 5 men (11.1%) reporting in-
teraction. On the other hand, Saegert, Swap, comes of $10,000 or more. The occupation of the
and Zajonc (1973) have demonstrated ex- head of the household in the respondent's parental
perimentally that "mere exposure" is a suf- family was most likely to be blue collar (n = 28 or
ficient condition for the enhancement of inter- 63.6%), with the majority of these men being skilled
workers (n = 21 or 47.7% of all respondents).
personal attraction. There were 14 white-collar parents (31.8%), mostly
The present study tests the hypothesis that proprietors or managers, 1 farmer (2.3%), and 1 man
did not answer the question. In sum, the men were
1
This article is based on a doctoral dissertation largely in their early 20s, of blue-collar backgrounds,
submitted to the Department of Sociology, Univer- Protestant, and married.
sity of Chicago. The author is indebted to Barry In addition to a large number of questions deal-
Schwartz and David McFarland for their generous ing with the attitudes and social characteristics of
advice and criticism, to John J. Mellinger for his the men, they were asked to name their three
excellent statistical advice, and to Joel M. Savell closest friends on the force. Of the 44 respondents,
for his suggestions for revision. 30 named at least one friend in the academy.
2
Requests for reprints should be sent to Mady Trainees are assigned to rooms and to seats in class-
Wechsler Segal, who is now at the Department of rooms on the basis of the alphabetical order of their
Sociology, University of Maryland, College Park, last names. Therefore, the closer two trainees are to
Maryland 20742. each other in alphabetical order, the more likely they
654
ALPHABET AND ATTRACTION 655

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 111213 14 1516 1718 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 363940 4142 43 44 45

I 3 2 2 2 1 2 2 1 1 1 3 1 2 1 1 1 1 3 1 1 1 2 0 1 0 2 2 1 1 1 5 1 1 0 1 2 1 2 5 1 0 1 0 1|65

FIGURE 1. Matrix of friendship choices. (Numbers across the top of the matrix indicate place
in alphabetical order of chosen. Numbers down the left of the matrix indicate place in alpha-
betical order of chooser.)

are to room near each other and to be near each RESULTS


other in training sessions. Thus, place in alphabetical
order serves as a measure of propinquity and expo- Alphabetical ordering did indeed have a
sure, independent of any subjective judgments the powerful effect on friendship choices, as
trainees might make about whom they spend time
with. This proximity clearly occurs prior to the shown in Figure 1. Of the 65 friendship
formation of friendships and serves as the inde- choices made within the academy, 29 (or
pendent variable. Kendall (1960) has previously 44.6%) are given to others who are adjacent
noted an association between interpersonal choice to the chooser in alphabetical order. This is
and closeness in alphabetical ordering of last names
among medical students, who were assigned class- all the more impressive when we recognize
room sets on the basis of their names. It was that alphabetical ordering is a conservative
expected that the fewer the number of places in measure of propinquity. The assigned order-
alphabetical order between two police trainees, the
greater would be the likelihood that they would ing defines which men live across the hall
choose each other as friends. from each other as well as which men are
656 MADY WECHSLER SEGAL

next door to each other, and it determines computed as follows: For each man who
who sits behind whom as well as who sits made at least one friendship choice, the mean
next to whom. Without data on the length absolute distance in alphabetical order be-
of barracks halls and classroom aisles, alpha- tween himself and all other men was com-
betical ordering does not tap these subtleties. puted ( X = 1 5 . 4 ) . Then, for each man, a
Thus, two men may in fact be physically difference score was computed as his actual
close and frequently interact even though mean minus the expected value. The mean of
they are not in adjacent places in alpha- these differences was then computed across
betical order. all choosers, .£=—11.22. A t test showed
Of the 65 friendship choices, 38 were given this mean to be highly significantly different
to others who named at least one friend in from zero (t = -10.02, df = 29, p < .001).
the academy. Of those 38, 22 were recipro- Similar results were obtained when the
cated, yielding a 57.9% reciprocation rate analysis was carried out from the point of
for friendship choices, regardless of propin- view of the 40 men who are chosen as
quity. Of the 29 adjacent choices, 19 could friends. The mean absolute distance from his
have been reciprocated and 14 actually were, choosers was computed for each person chosen
yielding a reciprocation rate of 73.7%. Thus, (^ = 4.5). Then, for each chosen man, a
choices to adjacent (propinquitous) others randomly expected absolute mean distance
are more likely to be reciprocated than those was computed by taking the mean absolute
to nonadjacent others (42.1% reciprocal). distance in alphabetical order between each
In the previous analyses, the unit of analy- chosen man and all other men (X — 15.3).
sis is the choice rather than the individual. The expected value was subtracted from
To perform other statistical tests, especially the observed value for each man chosen.
tests of significance, the data were trans- The mean of these difference scores was
formed in a number of ways. First, each — 10.82, which is significantly different from
man's friendship choices were combined by zero (t = -11.31, df = 39, p < .001).
computing the mean place in alphabetical The propinquity effect was the most power-
order of his friends. The correlation between ful determinant of interpersonal attraction
the chooser's place in alphabetical order and identified in the Maryland State Police Train-
the mean place in alphabetical order of the ing Academy. A variety of other possible
men he chooses as his friends was .917 determinants were examined. Although some
(w = 30 choosers, dj - 28, /><.001). An weak relationships were observed, there was
even stronger relationship existed between a no consistent tendency for friends to be simi-
man's place in alphabetical order and the lar with regard to social characteristics (e.g.,
mean place in alphabetical order of the men religion, frequency of church attendance,
who choose him as a friend (ra — .982, n — 40 age, marital status, ethnic background, par-
chosen men, dj = 38, p < .001). ents' education), organizational memberships,
Both of the above analyses may, however, leisure activity preferences, or certain atti-
misrepresent the proximity of a man's friends. tudes. All but one of the men were white, so
If a man is near the middle of the alpha- race was not a testable variable (the one
betical order and he chooses two men who black made two friendship choices and re-
are at the extremes (highly nonpropinqui- ceived none). None of these characteristics
tous), the mean place may be very close to can serve as an explanation of the rela-
his own, thus making the choices appear tionship observed between propinquity and
propinquitous. To solve this problem, the friendship choice.
mean absolute distance between a man's place
and those of his friends was computed
DISCUSSION
(X = 4.2). This was done for each chooser.
This was then compared to what would be The results presented here support the
expected if choices were distributed ran- proposition that propinquity contributes to
domly, that is, without regard to place in positive affect. In a field setting, uncontami-
alphabetical order. This expected value was nated by the effects of experimental demand
ALPHABET AND ATTRACTION 657
characteristics, using an unobtrusive (and Propinquity is also likely to be most highly
conservative) measure of proximity, we have related to attraction during the early stages
shown that proximity is highly related to of acquaintance. As people get to know each
friendship choice. Moreover, this relationship other, other characteristics may become im-
was stronger than that observed for a wide portant to friendship choice, but proximity is
range of other characteristics that have been still likely to remain a strong determinant.
identified in the literature as possible alter-
native determinants of interpersonal attrac- REFERENCES
tion. The fact stands out that the closer two Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. H. Interpersonal at-
Maryland State Police trainees are in the traction. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1969.
physical structure of the group, the more Byrne, D., & Buehler, J. A. A note on the influence
likely they are to choose each other as friends. of propinquity upon acquaintanceship. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1955, 51, 147-
Caution must be exercised in generalizing 148.
from the results found here to other groups. Festinger, L., Schachter, S., & Back, K. Social
The present subject population is relatively pressures in informal groups: A study of human
homogeneous with respect to a large number factors in housing. Stanford, Calif.: Stanford Uni-
versity Press, 19SO.
of variables. For example, there is not much Heider, F. The psychology of interpersonal relations.
variance in age among the trainees. It is New York: Wiley, 1958.
quite possible that the more homogeneous a Homans, G. C. Social behavior: Its elementary
group is on factors that are otherwise signifi- forms. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World, 1961.
cant for friendship formation, the greater the Kendall, P. Medical education as social process.
Paper presented at the meeting of the American
effect of propinquity on friendship. Con- Sociological Association, New York, August 1960.
versely, in a more heterogeneous group, mem- Kipnis, D. M. Interaction between members of
bers may be more willing to expend the bomber crews as a determinant of sociometric
greater cost required to become friends with choice. Human Relations, 1957, 10, 263-270.
Maisonneuve, J. A., Palmade, G., & Fourment, C.
someone who is physically further away. This Selective choices and propinquity. Sociometry,
would occur if the rewards produced by inter- 1952, 15, 135-140.
action with similar others are great enough Newcomb, T. M. The prediction of interpersonal
to more than offset the costs. Neither a study attraction. American Psychologist, 1956, 11, 575-
of police trainees nor studies of college 586.
Priest, R. F., & Sawyer, J. Proximity and peership:
sophomores provide us with populations that Bases of balance in interpersonal attraction.
are heterogeneous enough to evaluate this American Journal of Sociology, 1967, 72, 633-649.
possibility. Saegert, S., Swap, W., & Zajonc, R. B. Exposure,
It must be recognized, however, that many context, and interpersonal attraction. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 1973, 25,
of the groups within which people spend 234-242.
their time and with which they identify are Smith, T. S., Jr. Democratic controls and profes-
indeed homogeneous with respect to relevant sionalism in police work. Unpublished doctoral
variables. Thus, the importance of propin- dissertation, Department of Sociology, University
quity as a determinant of friendship must of Chicago, 1968.
not be discounted. (Received March 15, 1973)

You might also like